Skip to main content
eLife logoLink to eLife
. 2018 Sep 11;7:e37125. doi: 10.7554/eLife.37125

The influence of the descending pain modulatory system on infant pain-related brain activity

Sezgi Goksan 1,2, Luke Baxter 1,2, Fiona Moultrie 1,2, Eugene Duff 1,2, Gareth Hathway 3, Caroline Hartley 1,2, Irene Tracey 2, Rebeccah Slater 1,2,
Editors: Peggy Mason4, Eve Marder5
PMCID: PMC6133549  PMID: 30201093

Abstract

The descending pain modulatory system (DPMS) constitutes a network of widely distributed brain regions whose integrated function is essential for effective modulation of sensory input to the central nervous system and behavioural responses to pain. Animal studies demonstrate that young rodents have an immature DPMS, but comparable studies have not been conducted in human infants. In Goksan et al. (2015) we used functional MRI (fMRI) to show that pain-related brain activity in newborn infants is similar to that observed in adults. Here, we investigated whether the functional network connectivity strength across the infant DPMS influences the magnitude of this brain activity. FMRI scans were collected while mild mechanical noxious stimulation was applied to the infant’s foot. Greater pre-stimulus functional network connectivity across the DPMS was significantly associated with lower noxious-evoked brain activity (p = 0.0004, r = -0.86, n = 13), suggesting that in newborn infants the DPMS may regulate the magnitude of noxious-evoked brain activity.

Research organism: Human

Introduction

In adults, pain perception is modulated by the descending pain modulatory system (DPMS), allowing environmental, contextual and cognitive factors to influence our pain experiences (McMahon et al., 2013; Ossipov et al., 2010; Tracey and Mantyh, 2007). The DPMS comprises a network of cortical and subcortical brain regions that can facilitate or inhibit nociceptive afferent brain input via brainstem nuclei (Ossipov et al., 2010; Tracey, 2010; Zhuo and Gebhart, 1997). The functional connectivity of the DPMS is altered in adult chronic pain conditions such as migraine, back pain, fibromyalgia and painful diabetic neuropathy (Jensen et al., 2012; Mainero et al., 2011; Segerdahl et al., 2018; Yu et al., 2014), and transient alterations in DPMS connectivity influences pain perception. For example, pre-stimulus functional connectivity between the anterior insula (AI) and the periaqueductal gray (PAG) relates to whether or not a noxious stimulus is perceived as painful (Ploner et al., 2010), and pre-stimulus activity in the insular and anterior cingulate cortices (ACC) is predictive of subsequent pain intensity ratings (Boly et al., 2007). Furthermore, anticipatory brainstem activity in adults has been shown to predict changes in insula activity evoked by noxious thermal stimulation (Fairhurst et al., 2007).

Evidence from animal studies suggests that infant descending pain modulation is immature (Hathway et al., 2009). During the first 3 postnatal weeks, anatomical descending projections to the dorsal horn are physically present; however, physiological inhibition of nociceptive input is ineffective or absent in rat pups (Hathway et al., 2009; Fitzgerald and Koltzenburg, 1986; Hathway et al., 2006). Moreover, the brainstem nuclei in the rostral ventromedial medulla (RVM), which are the principle source of these projections, exclusively facilitate nociceptive spinal activity, rather than exerting more adult-like biphasic inhibitory and facilitatory nociceptive control (Hathway et al., 2009; Schwaller et al., 2017). In the human infant, spinal reflexes are uncoordinated, exaggerated and prolonged (Andrews and Fitzgerald, 1994; Cornelissen et al., 2013; Hartley et al., 2016). Nociceptive reflexes are refined postnatally in infants born prematurely, and by term age, infant reflexes have lower amplitude and shorter duration compared with premature infants (Cornelissen et al., 2013; Hartley et al., 2016). During this developmental period, the postnatal refinement of spinal cord excitability is concomitant with the maturation of nociceptive brain activity (Hartley et al., 2016), leading to the possibility that in the newborn term infant, the brain regions involved in descending pain modulation may be influential in modifying pain behaviour and experience.

In our previous paper, we used fMRI to demonstrate that patterns of noxious-evoked brain activity in the infant are similar to those observed in the adult, and include both sensory and affective brain regions (Goksan et al., 2015). Given we cannot measure subjective pain experience in non-verbal infants, we are reliant on objective surrogate measures such as changes in noxious-evoked BOLD activity to make inferences about pain experiences (Baumgärtner et al., 2010; Lee et al., 2008; Maihöfner and Handwerker, 2005). Using this approach, provides the opportunity to investigate whether the network connectivity strength between brain regions involved in descending pain modulation modifies infant pain. The aim of this study was to test the hypothesis that in the human infant, the magnitude of noxious-evoked brain activity recorded using fMRI in response to a standardised nociceptive stimulus is related to the pre-stimulus functional connectivity of brain regions known to comprise the DPMS.

Results and discussion

Pre-stimulus functional connectivity in the infant DPMS

Mild experimental noxious stimulation was applied to the infant's foot using a 128 mN PinPrick stimulator. To ascertain the pre-stimulus functional connectivity, we extracted the demeaned BOLD signal from the three volumes recorded immediately prior to the application of the stimulus, which were acquired within the 10 s pre-stimulus period (see Materials and methods and Figure 3—figure supplement 2A). We extracted these time courses for the DPMS Network, and for brain regions in two control networks - the first, referred to as the ‘Control Network’ has similar topography to the DPMS Network, and the second network is a well-recognised resting state network (the Default Mode Network). The DPMS Network comprised the bilateral AI, ACC, amygdala (AMY), RVM, PAG, and the middle frontal gyri (mFG) situated within the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (Figure 1A). This includes the main brain structures identified in the adult DPMS (McMahon et al., 2013; Schweinhardt and Bushnell, 2010). The Control Network comprised a set of brain regions that are not reported to be involved in descending pain modulation, but included distinct cortical, subcortical and brainstem structures, and had similar topographic distribution to the DPMS brain regions. Whilst the Control Network is not a known functional network within the brain, this network controls for global signal confounds for example respiratory or cardiovascular signals. The brain regions in the Control Network are the bilateral calcarine cortices (CAL), caudate (CAU), hippocampus (HIP), pontine nuclei (PON), recti gyri (RGY) and the supplementary motor areas (SMA) (Figure 1C). As an additional control, the Default Mode Network (an established network that has been identified in adults and term infants) (Doria et al., 2010; Raichle, 2015) allowed us to test the specificity of the relationship between the pre-stimulus functional connectivity of the DPMS and the noxious-evoked BOLD activity. The Default Mode Network included the posterior cingulate cortex (PCC), the inferior parietal lobules (IPL) and the medial superior frontal gyrus (mSFG) situated within the medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC) (Figure 1E).

Figure 1. Connectivity between brain regions in the DPMS and control networks.

Schematic representation showing approximate locations of brain regions in sagittal and coronal slices in the (A) DPMS Network, (C) Control Network and (E) Default Mode Network. Each anatomical region of interest is identified in Figure 1—figure supplement 1 and the source data is provided in Figure 1—source data 1. Figure 1—figure supplement 2 shows the registration of two example masks from template to functional space and example time series. Network schematics of the mean pre-stimulus functional connectivity between pairs of regions in the (B) DPMS Network, (D) Control Network and (F) Default Mode Network. For abbreviations see main text.

Figure 1—source data 1. All region-of-interest masks in standard space.
DOI: 10.7554/eLife.37125.006

Figure 1.

Figure 1—figure supplement 1. Masks of regions included in the DPMS, Control Network and Default Mode Network.

Figure 1—figure supplement 1.

Numbers by the top left of each transverse image represent coordinate locations in infant template space. The location of each transverse slice is demonstrated (red lines) on the sagittal template brain on the right. The source data (Figure 1—source data 1) contains all the brain regions in standard space for all networks.
Figure 1—figure supplement 2. Registration and time series data.

Figure 1—figure supplement 2.

Registration of masks from (A) template to (B) structural and finally to (C) functional space (blue mask = ACC, yellow mask = mFG). (D) Examples of the resulting time series within the ACC (blue) and mFG (yellow). Black circles highlight the pre-stimulus data points.
Figure 1—figure supplement 3. Pre-stimulus connectivity is stable.

Figure 1—figure supplement 3.

Average pre-stimulus connectivity in the DPMS Network across infants prior to each of the 10 stimuli. Error bars indicate mean and standard deviation.

The overall mean pre-stimulus functional connectivity was calculated for each network (Figure 1B,D,F). This was not significantly different between the DPMS Network and the Control Network (mean pre-stimulus functional connectivity: DPMS Network = 0.08 ± 0.10; Control Network = 0.15 ± 0.12). Unsurprisingly, given that the Default Mode Network is a canonical network that has been identified in both adult and infant resting state data (Doria et al., 2010; Raichle, 2015), the functional connectivity of the Default Mode Network was significantly greater (mean pre-stimulus functional connectivity: Default Mode Network = 0.24 ± 0.18) than connectivity within the DPMS and the Control Network (p = 0.0014, repeated measures ANOVA, Tukey post-hoc comparison of DPMS and Control Network: p = 0.06, Default Mode Network and DPMS: p < 0.001, Default Mode Network and Control Network: p = 0.047).

Characterisation of noxious-evoked brain activity in infants

Consistent with previous reports (Goksan et al., 2015; Williams et al., 2015), we identified positive clusters of noxious-evoked BOLD activity in the bilateral postcentral gyrus (somatosensory cortices), thalamus, anterior cingulate cortex and contralateral posterior insular cortex (Figure 2, Table 1). We report a reduction in the number of active brain regions compared with our previous publication (Goksan et al., 2015), and demonstrate more highly localised clusters of significant activity within distinct anatomical regions (Figure 2). For example, clusters of activity can now be identified in the medial surface of the somatosensory cortex, which encodes the somatotopic foot representation (Figure 2 and Figure 2—source data 1). These differences have arisen due to improvements in the data analysis pipeline to incorporate recent recommendations and methodological advances. Importantly, the statistical cluster-defining threshold has increased from z = 2.3 to z = 3.1, to account for potential inflation in family wise error rates that have been observed across a broad range of MRI studies (Eklund et al., 2016). Improved filtering of head motion parameters using FIX (Griffanti et al., 2014; Salimi-Khorshidi et al., 2014) and an infant-specific haemodynamic response function (Arichi et al., 2012) were also used (see Materials and methods). The brain regions identified in this more stringent analysis represent the most robustly activated clusters of noxious-evoked brain activity in the infant, and are consistent with those most commonly reported in adults (Tracey and Mantyh, 2007). Our previous report that the infant pattern of pain-related brain activity is similar to that observed in adults is reconfirmed here (Goksan et al., 2015).

Figure 2. Group noxious-evoked brain activity.

Figure 2.

(A) Sagittal and coronal views of the significant group activity from the 13 infants. Red lines indicate how the two images (and the transverse image at z = 42, in B) relate to one another. (B) Transverse images showing significant group activity. The source data is provided in Figure 2—source data 1). Numbers by the top left of each image represent coordinate locations in infant template space. The location of each transverse slice is demonstrated (red lines) on the sagittal template brain in the top right. The activity map is overlaid on a standard template of an infant brain at 40 weeks’ gestational age (Serag et al., 2012). Letters in italics depict axis labels: L = left, R = right, = posterior. Statistical maps are of cluster thresholded z-statistics (z > 3.1, cluster significance threshold p < 0.05).

Figure 2—source data 1. Thresholded group activity map.
DOI: 10.7554/eLife.37125.008

Table 1. Significant positive clusters of noxious-evoked brain activity, observed across the whole group (n = 13, cluster forming threshold: z = 3.1, cluster significance threshold, p = 0.05).

This table provides an anatomical description and the location of the peak z-statistic within each active brain region. The group activity reported consisted of 14 distinct clusters, some of which spanned multiple brain regions.

Anatomical description of location of activity Maximum z-statistic within cluster Coordinates of maximum z-statistic in infant template space
X Y Z
Post-central gyrus Contra 4.8 7.7 −27.5 50.7
Ipsi 4.7 −23.2 −27.5 43.8
Posterior cingulate sulcus Contra 4.8 6.9 −23.2 36.0
Ipsi 3.8 −6.0 −20.6 34.3
Superior parietal lobule Contra 4.8 12.9 −44.6 47.2
Ipsi 4.7 −14.6 −43.8 43.8
Thalamus Contra 4.7 16.3 −19.7 12.0
Ipsi 4.6 −13.8 −24.0 16.3
Supra-marginal gyrus Ipsi 4.7 −25.8 −29.2 37.8
Contra 4.6 30.1 −24.9 28.3
Superior frontal sulcus Contra 4.7 16.3 −4.3 44.6
Middle frontal gyrus Ipsi 4.7 −27.5 0.9 30.0
Contra 4.0 24.1 5.2 33.5
Cuneus Contra 4.7 7.7 −53.2 26.6
Ipsi 4.2 −5.1 −50.7 23.2
Superior parietal lobe / Precuneus Contra 4.7 5.2 −35.2 39.5
Ipsi 4.0 −0.9 −43.8 41.2
Pre-central gyrus / Central sulcus Contra 4.6 24.1 −14.6 45.5
Ipsi 3.9 −8.6 −23.2 53.2
Posterior insula Contra 4.6 19.0 −19.7 24.0
Parietal operculum Ipsi 4.6 −34.4 −21.4 18.0
Superior temporal gyrus / Posterior operculum Contra 4.6 31.0 −27.5 22.3
Occipital gyrus Ipsi 4.6 −12.0 −63.5 21.4
Anterior cingulate cortex Ipsi 4.2 −1.7 14.7 24.0
Contra 3.9 4.3 12.1 25.7
Superior temporal gyrus Ipsi 4.2 −27.5 −28.3 12.8

Relationship between the pre-stimulus functional connectivity of the DPMS and noxious-evoked brain activity

For each infant, the mean pre-stimulus functional connectivity across the DPMS Network and the control networks were calculated, and related to the mean percentage change in BOLD activity evoked by the noxious stimulation (calculated for each individual participant across all the voxels where significant group activity was identified). There was a significant inverse relationship between the magnitude of pre-stimulus DPMS functional connectivity and the percentage change in noxious-evoked BOLD activity (Pearson correlation coefficient (r) = -0.86, p = 0.0004, parameter estimate (β) = -0.74, linear model also included gestational age in weeks as an explanatory variable, Figure 3A). Infants with greater functional connectivity across their DPMS Network prior to noxious stimulation had lower noxious-evoked brain activity. In contrast, the mean functional connectivity in the Control Network and in the Default Mode Network were not related to the mean change in noxious-evoked BOLD activity (Control Network: r = -0.36, p = 0.26, β = -0.25; Default Mode Network: r = 0.06, p = 0.88, β= -0.03 Figure 3B,C). The absence of a significant relationship between the functional connectivity of the Default Mode Network and the noxious-evoked BOLD activity suggests that the influence of the DPMS on the noxious-activity is not generalisable across all established brain networks.

Figure 3. Relationship between noxious-evoked brain activity and pre-stimulus functional connectivity in the DPMS and control networks.

Linear regression models (blue lines) were used to compare pre-stimulus functional connectivity (psFC) with the percentage change in BOLD activity in the (A) DPMS Network, (B) Control Network and (C) the Default Mode Network (DMN). Noxious-evoked brain activity for each infant (calculated within a mask of the group activity, see Figure 2) was adjusted for gestational age (in weeks) at the time of study. Coloured circles represent data from individual infants within the DPMS (red) and control networks (light blue). Figure 3—source data 1 provides the individual PAG and RVM functional masks for each infant. Figure 3—figure supplement 1 shows the relationship between the percentage change in BOLD activity and the psFC in the DPMS Network and Control Network with the brainstem regions removed. (D) The brain schematic highlights the pairs of brain regions where psFC was significantly correlated with percentage change in the BOLD response (dashed yellow lines). (E,F,G) The three pairs of regions within the DPMS Network which demonstrated strong correlations between mean psFC and noxious-evoked brain activity.

Figure 3—source data 1. Individual DPMS brainstem masks in functional space.
DOI: 10.7554/eLife.37125.013

Figure 3.

Figure 3—figure supplement 1. Relationship between percentage change in noxious-evoked brain activity and pre-stimulus functional connectivity in the DPMS Network and Control Network with the brainstem regions removed.

Figure 3—figure supplement 1.

Linear regression model (blue lines) comparison of the pre-stimulus functional connectivity (psFC) with the percentage change in BOLD activity in the (A) DPMS Network and (B) Control Network with the brainstem regions removed. The DPMS psFC was calculated between the AI, ACC, amygdala and middle frontal gyri, and the Control Network psFC was calculated in the bilateral calcarine cortices, caudate, hippocampus, recti gyri and the supplementary motor areas. Coloured circles represent data from individual infants within the DPMS Network (red) and Control Network (light blue) .
Figure 3—figure supplement 2. Example data from individual infants.

Figure 3—figure supplement 2.

Data from an individual infant showing (A) the mean time series within all DPMS brain regions and (B) the resulting connectivity matrix. Black circles overlaid on the time series indicate pre-stimulus points. Vertical blue lines indicate the point of stimulation. The minimum inter-stimulus interval was 25 s. (C) Examples of statistical COPE values related to the magnitude of noxious-evoked brain activity and (D) the change in BOLD signal within a single voxel (red) plotted over the expected model fit (black). The statistical COPE map has been masked with the group activity mask (see Figure 2); therefore, coloured regions represent voxels that survive cluster thresholding at the group level. The baseline (light blue) is the temporal mean used to calculate percentage change in BOLD.

To explore the relative contribution of different brain regions within the DPMS Network, the relationship between the functional connectivity and the mean change in noxious-evoked BOLD activity was calculated for each pair of brain regions. Increased pre-stimulus functional connectivity between the ACC and PAG was associated with a substantial reduction in noxious-evoked BOLD activity (adjusted for age, p = 0.0012, β = -0.22, Figure 3E). Functional connectivity between the AI-mFG and ACC-AI were also strongly related to the change in noxious-evoked BOLD activity (p = 0.02, β = -0.23 and p = 0.03, β = -0.17 respectively, Figure 3F,G). For all other pairs of brain regions, the functional connectivity strength did not influence the magnitude of noxious-evoked brain activity. The observation that a high degree of functional connectivity between the ACC and PAG is strongly associated with a reduction in pain-related brain activity in the infant is interesting in light of observations in adults where greater co-variation in the functional activity of the rostral ACC and PAG relates to an increase in the efficacy of endogenous analgesia elicited by placebo treatment (Petrovic et al., 2002). Anticipation of placebo has been associated with greater pre-stimulus activity in the PAG, and leads to a placebo-induced reduction in evoked brain activity in the thalamus and rostral ACC (Wager et al., 2004). The importance of the PAG, as part of the DPMS, has also been demonstrated in animal studies, where direct stimulation of the PAG is associated with a reduction in incoming nociceptive information from the peripheral nervous system (Reynolds, 1969). In adult rodents, descending modulation (evidenced by PAG activation) preferentially modulates C-fibre input (McMullan and Lumb, 2006; Waters and Lumb, 2008), whereas the noxious stimulus applied in this study likely preferentially activates A-delta fibres, which may be differentially modulated compared with C fibre input. However, it is not known how other supraspinal components of the DPMS respond to activity in subclasses of nociceptors in humans. Further work is needed to understand the developmental trajectory of the PAG-RVM axis in humans and the maturation of its connections to the spinal cord.

Human and non-human infants display heightened sensitivity to noxious stimulation, which has long been attributed to hyper-excitable spinal reflex networks (Fitzgerald, 2005). In the neonatal rodent, brain structures within the DPMS facilitate, rather than inhibit, spinally-mediated nocifensive behaviours (Hathway et al., 2009). During human preterm development, these exaggerated reflexes are refined with shorter durations, lower magnitudes, and higher response thresholds, and patterns of noxious-evoked brain activity concomitantly mature (Hartley et al., 2016; Cornelissen et al., 2013; Fabrizi et al., 2011). The data presented here in term infants suggest that the functional DPMS brain networks have an inhibitory function at the level of the brain, similar to that observed in the adult (Ossipov et al., 2010). There is, however, a potential contradiction with observations in neonatal rodents where facilitation at the level of the spinal cord has been observed in electrophysiological recordings. This could reflect maturational differences in the connectivity of supraspinal DPMS regions and the connectivity of the descending pathways from the RVM to the spinal nociceptive dorsal horn network in newborn infants. Nevertheless, this interpretation relies on a comparison across species, which is based on a theoretical age-equivalence between human infants and neonatal rat pups. Some networks will likely have a different developmental trajectory in rodents compared with humans and the maturation of the CNS is not a coordinated linear process as different networks likely mature at different rates (Clancy et al., 2001). As we have not measured activity in the spinal cord, we cannot determine the relationship between functional connectivity in the DPMS and spinal activity in the term-aged infant.

Collecting functional imaging data in the brainstem is challenging, both in adults and infants, due to head motion, and cardiac-related and respiratory-related motion (Harita and Stroman, 2017). In this study, we identified and regressed out physiological noise using independent component analysis (Salimi-Khorshidi et al., 2014; McKeown et al., 2005; McKeown et al., 1998). As further confirmation of the results, we re-assessed the strength of pre-stimulus functional connectivity in the DPMS Network and Control Network excluding brainstem regions; namely the PAG and RVM for the DPMS Network and the PON from the Control Network. The strength of the DPMS Network within the remaining cortical and subcortical regions, the ACC, AI, mFG and AMY, was still significantly inversely related to noxious-evoked brain activity (r = -0.61, p = 0.04, β = -0.37, Figure 3—figure supplement 1). As before, the Control Network excluding the PON, was not significantly correlated with noxious-evoked brain activity (r = -0.29, p = 0.36, β = -0.15). This suggests that our results are unlikely to be driven by noise within the brainstem. While there are inherent limitations in this study in terms of the spatial resolution that can be achieved when imaging small structures within the brainstem, we believe that the PAG and RVM masks that we individually defined for each infant are well localised within these anatomical structures. Figure 3—source data 1 gives the individual PAG and RVM functional masks for each infant.

It is possible that application of the noxious stimulus could influence the pre-stimulus data; however, the stimulus presentation was not predictable, and the time-period between stimuli was always greater than 25 s. The pre-stimulus functional connectivity of the DPMS was not dependent on stimulus number (p = 0.33, repeated measures ANOVA, see Figure 1—figure supplement 3), suggesting that the functional connectivity of this network is relatively stable. In adults, functional brain networks are also thought to be dominated by stable individual features, and only modestly influenced by evoked factors and day-to-day variability (Gratton et al., 2018). To further understand the relationship between infant noxious-evoked brain activity and the DPMS, functional connectivity analysis of resting state data, and underlying structural connectivity and white matter fibre integrity between DPMS regions using diffusion MRI is warranted (Gratton et al., 2018; Friston, 2011). Neuroimaging studies in both humans and animals suggest that functional connectivity measures can be used to better understand how networks of brain regions are involved in complex functions (Cole et al., 2016; Fox et al., 2005; Smith et al., 2013), including pain (Baliki et al., 2014). While these measures may represent direct or indirect communication between these brain regions (Fox et al., 2005; Smith et al., 2013), they may also reflect underlying changes in the amplitude of the neural signals, which are unrelated to neural synchrony (Friston, 2011; Cole et al., 2016; Duff et al., 2018) – further investigation of DPMS structural and functional connectivity may elucidate these underlying mechanisms.

In summary, this study suggests that in term infants the DPMS may be influential in regulating the magnitude of noxious-evoked brain activity. In adults, greater pre-stimulus activity in brain regions within the DPMS network are coupled with lower behavioural pain reports (Ploner et al., 2010; Boly et al., 2007; Fairhurst et al., 2007). Therefore, a possible interpretation of our results is that when regions within the DPMS are more strongly functionally connected, infants have a greater ability to regulate their pain experience and dampen the magnitude of their brain activity in response to incoming nociceptive input. Surgical injury in the neonatal period is known to lead to whole-body changes in pain sensitivity that persist into childhood (Walker et al., 2009), and this may be dependent upon changes in the maturation of the DPMS, especially the RVM (Walker et al., 2015). To understand how the DPMS develops during early life, and how it is influenced by early life experiences, further investigation of the DPMS is required in both younger preterm infants and older infants. For example, it has been suggested that development of aberrant DPMS function in early life may lead to long-term vulnerability towards chronic pain states (Denk et al., 2014). The presence of a functional supraspinal modulation system in a term-aged human infant is consistent with the proposal that the emergence of top-down inhibitory pathways develop in early life (Hartley et al., 2016). We conclude that the DPMS network can influence the magnitude of pain-related brain activity in term-aged infants.

Materials and methods

Participants

Seventeen newborn term-aged infants were recruited from the Maternity Unit at the John Radcliffe Hospital, Oxford, UK. All infants completed the full study protocol. The National Research Ethics Service provided ethical approval: REC reference 12/SC/0447. Informed written parental consent was obtained prior to each study. The study was carried out in accordance with the standards set by the Declaration of Helsinki and Good Clinical Practice guidelines.

Data from four infants were excluded from the analysis because the most caudal region of interest, the rostral ventral medulla in the brainstem, fell outside of the field of view. Therefore, 13 term infants (average gestational age (GA) at study = 40 weeks, range 38 to 43 weeks) were included in this analysis. The average postnatal age at the time of the study was 4 days (range 1 to 8 days). Eight of the 13 infants included in this analysis were also included in our previous publication (Goksan et al., 2015).

Study protocol

Infant recruitment criteria, experimental study design and MRI study protocol were identical to that described previously by Goksan et al., 2015. In brief, all infants were scanned at the Centre for Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging of the Brain (FMRIB), John Radcliffe Hospital, Oxford. Prior to scanning infants were fed and swaddled and provided with three levels of ear protection: ear putty (Mack’s Kids size earplugs, McKeon Products Inc., MI), ear muffs (Minimuffs, Natus Medical Inc., Galway, Ireland) and hearing defenders (Em's 4 Bubs Baby Earmuffs, Em's 4 Kids, Brisbane, Australia), with noise reduction ratings of 22 dB, 7 dB, and 22 dB, respectively. Infants were then placed in a vacuum-positioning mattress and all scanning was done when infants were settled or asleep.

During all MRI sessions, T2-weighted structural images were collected prior to acquisition of functional echo planar imaging (EPI) scans. During individual infant’s functional scans, acute experimental noxious stimulation was applied using a calibrated nociceptive stimulator (force: 128 mN, PinPrick Stimulators, MRC Systems). Noxious stimulation was applied 10 times to the heel of the left foot by the same experimenter and with a minimum inter-stimulus interval of 25 s. The interval was chosen based on the neonatal term infant haemodynamic response function (HRF) described by Arichi et al. (2012) and the interval was extended if necessary to ensure the infant was settled at the time of stimulation.

MRI acquisition

Images were collected using a Siemens 3-Tesla Magnetom Verio scanner (Erlangen, Germany) with a 32-channel adult head coil. T2-weighted turbo spin echo structural scans were acquired for each infant (sequence parameters: repetition time/echo time (TR/TE) = 14740/88 ms; flip angle 150 °; resolution 1 mm3; slices = 85, field of view (FOV) = 192×192 mm, acceleration = GRAPPA 2, slice order = interleaved, with no slice overlap). BOLD images were acquired using a T2*-weighted EPI acquisition (sequence parameters: TR/TE = 2500/40 ms; flip angle = 90°; FOV = 192×192 mm; imaging matrix 64×64; resolution 3×3×3 mm; slices = 33, collected in descending order; average total volumes = 142). Prospective Acquisition Correction for head motion (PACE) was applied during all EPI scans (Thesen et al., 2000), as described previously in Goksan et al., 2015). Field map images were obtained for post-acquisition correction of gradient field effects (sequence parameters: TR = 400 ms; TE1/TE2 = 5.19/7.65 ms; flip angle = 60°; FOV = 192×192 mm; imaging matrix 64×64; resolution 3×3 ×3 mm; slices = 36, slice order = interleaved; inter-slice gap = 0.75 mm). The noxious stimuli were time-locked to the fMRI recording using Neurobehavioural Systems (Presentation, www.neurobs.com) software; coded to detect an experimenter’s button-press each time an experimental stimulus was applied to the participant's foot.

Data analysis

MR data processing

All MR data pre-processing were done using FMRIB Software Library (FSL) (www.fmrib.ox.ac.uk/fsl), Versions 5.0.10 and 4.1.9. Version 5.0.10 was used to prepare the structural and field map images. FSL’s Brain Extraction Tool (BET) was used in order to extract brain-tissue signal from the non-brain structures in each infant’s structural image (Smith, 2002). The fractional intensity threshold and threshold gradient parameters within BET were adjusted in order to obtain the most accurate brain extraction per subject. A mask of each infant’s brain-extracted structural scan was registered to the fieldmap and used to guide fieldmap preparation. All fMRI data registrations were done using FMRI Expert Analysis Tool (FEAT) Version 5.98 (FSL Version 4.1.9) to avoid boundary-based registration (BBR), due to hard coding of the adult-appropriate BBR-slope parameter, which is unsuitable for infant fMRI data. Functional images were registered to a standard average infant template (40 week GA template; downloaded from www.brain-development.org). Each EPI was initially registered to the infant’s structural image (FLIRT: rigid body transformation with six DOF [Jenkinson et al., 2002; Jenkinson and Smith, 2001]). Subsequently, images in structural space were non-linearly registered to the neonatal-specific template image, which corresponded to the GA of the infant at the time of the study (Serag et al., 2012) and then to the standard infant 40-week gestation template (FNIRT: non-linear transformation with twelve DOF).

FEAT (Version 5.98) was used to run functional data pre-processing steps implemented within FSL; which included motion correction of the functional data using MCFLIRT (Jenkinson et al., 2002), distortion correction using FUGUE, brain extraction using BET, high pass temporal filtering at 0.01 Hz (100 s period), and grand mean scaling. Spatial smoothing is a common preprocessing step that typically filters the data with a smoothing kernel extent (measured in full width at half maximum) larger than one voxel. Given our spatial resolution, the voxel size relative to the neonatal brain, and our use of small brainstem ROIs, spatial smoothing was deemed inappropriate and thus omitted. MELODIC (model-free fMRI analysis using probabilistic independent component analysis) was used to decompose functional data into spatially independent components, which were subsequently manually labelled as signal or noise (Griffanti et al., 2017). FIX (FMRIB's ICA-based Xnoiseifier, v1.065) was then applied to regress out the noise component time series and 24 head motion parameter time series (Griffanti et al., 2014; Salimi-Khorshidi et al., 2014). While spatial ICA-based denoising does not remove global signal artefacts, we did not include a pre-processing step to address this, such as GSR (global signal regression). We addressed the potential issue of global signal cofounds in the main analysis by including our Control Network.

MR data statistical analysis was conducted using FSL (Version 5.0.10). Time-series statistics were generated using general linear modelling (GLM) in FEAT (Version 6.00). The experimental model was created using an event-related design, where each input represented the timing of each noxious stimulus (duration: approximately 1 s), recorded during the scanning session via the Presentation code. The experimental design was convolved with three term-infant-specific optimal basis functions generated by Arichi and colleagues (Arichi et al., 2012). Motion outlier variables were included in the model by identifying volumes where large deviations in head position occurred. Each motion variable was generated by FSL’s motion outliers command using the DVARS option; which calculated the rate of change of the variance between volumes (Power et al., 2012). Cluster thresholding (with a cluster defining threshold of p = 0.001 (z = 3.1) and a cluster significance threshold of p = 0.05) was used to identify significant increases in BOLD following experimental noxious stimulation.

Group analysis was run in FSL (Version 5.0.10), using mixed effects FLAME 1 and 2 in FEAT (Version 6.00), with automatic outlier detection. The first contrast of the parameter estimate (COPE) statistical image of each participant was input into the higher group analysis, therefore only taking into account the first basis function described by Arichi et al. (2012). This function closely resembles a double gamma function with a peak at 7 s and an undershoot to positive peak ratio of 0.49. Two neonatal-specific atlases, the University of North Carolina’s (UNC) atlas (Shi et al., 2011) and an Imperial College London (ICL) atlas (Serag et al., 2012), and an adult atlas (Mai et al., 2008) were used to guide description of the resulting group activity (in Table 1). Three atlases were required because each atlas provided varying levels of anatomical specificity. The ICL atlas was the most general, describing all the lobes of the brain, as well as some deep brain nuclei and maps of the CSF, grey and white matter. Despite this broad labelling, the ICL atlas also provided the most accurate partition between anatomical boundaries. The UNC infant atlas was used as it contained more specific anatomical masks. However, the partitions between anatomical boundaries were less good; therefore the ICL atlas was used in conjunction to define boundaries. Finally when describing the anatomical location of peaks within the group activity (see Table 1), an adult atlas was used as it provided further guidance for labelling specific gyri and sulci. Clusters that extended across more than one brain region were described separately only when a region of activity with a separate local peak voxel was observed within the adjacent brain region. For all regions named in the table, masks were hand drawn and aimed to include all the active voxels within each region, however given the subjective nature of this task it is possible that small regions of activity that formed part of the same cluster may have been overlooked. The function Cluster, available within FSL, was then used to obtain maximum z-statistics and their coordinate locations.

Pre-stimulus functional connectivity (psFC)

Mean time series were calculated in 15 brain regions. Six regions were identified as key regions within the DPMS: anterior cingulate cortex (ACC), amygdala (AMY), anterior insula (AI), middle frontal gyrus (mFG) (a region within the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex - dlPFC); assessed using the following papers (Rajkowska and Goldman-Rakic, 1995; Sallet et al., 2013; Stagg et al., 2013), periaqueductal grey (PAG) and rostal ventral medulla (RVM). A further nine brain regions, without a known role in the DPMS, were included within the two control networks. The Control Network included the calcarine cortex (CAL), caudate (CAU), hippocampus (HIP), pons (PON), recti gyri (RGY) and the supplementary motor area (SMA). Three regions were identified in the Default Mode Network – the posterior cingulate cortex (PCC), inferior parietal lobules (IPL) and the medial superior frontal gyrus (mSFG). The mSFG was chosen in place of the medial prefrontal cortex (commonly reported as part of the DMN) because a medial prefrontal cortex mask was not available as part of the UNC or ICL infant atlases; therefore, the mSFG was taken as the representative of this region.

Brain regions commonly reported to be involved in descending pain modulation (Schweinhardt and Bushnell, 2010), were included in the DPMS Network. However, this network did not include all DPMS regions, as for example, the hypothalamus also plays a key role in descending pain modulation (Denk et al., 2014; Dafny et al., 1996). The DPMS Network reported here therefore includes core regions within the adult DPMS network that could be confidently identified and masked in the infant.

Using the two neonatal atlases described above (UNC and ICL), region of interest (ROI) masks were created. ACC, AMY, CAU, HIP and PCC masks were taken directly from the ICL atlas at 40 week GA. CAL, IPL, mFG, mSFG, RGY and SMA were regions within the UNC infant atlas. Following registration of the UNC atlas to the 40-week template brain, masks of all six aforementioned UNC atlas regions were isolated. Subsequently, UNC atlas masks were carefully inspected to ensure that each mask fell within the appropriate boundaries of the ICL atlas (as this atlas is more accurately registered with the anatomy of the template brain). The mFG mask was taken directly from the UNC atlas. For the CAL mask, voxels that fell within the occipital lobe mask from the ICL atlas were included, and individual voxels falling outside of the calcarine cortex were manually removed. For the RGY and mSFG masks, only voxels that fell within the frontal lobe mask from the ICL atlas were included. For the IPL, voxels that fell within the parietal lobe mask from the ICL atlas were included. For the SMA, voxels labelled as CSF by the ICL atlas were removed.

Four brain regions were not included as independent brain regions in either atlas and were therefore hand-drawn. AI, PAG, PON and RVM masks were manually drawn in FSLeyes (FSL, Version 5.0.10). Adult masks of the AI (Wiech et al., 2014) and PAG (Ezra et al., 2015) were used to guide drawing of masks over the infant template brain. PON and RVM masks were drawn with reference to the Duvernoy Atlas (Duvernoy, 2012). The RVM mask fell within the region of the ventromedial nucleus of the solitary tract, while the PON mask consisted of the pontine nuclei and the basilar part of the pons (Figure 1—figure supplement 1).

Mean time series were calculated in EPI space by (i) registering masks of each region from the standard neonatal (40 week GA) template to EPI space (via structural scans) (Figure 1—figure supplement 2A–C), (ii) checking that each ROI mask fell within the field of view and was appropriately registered, (iii) using the function fslmeants, available in FSL, to generate a mean time series within each specific ROI (Figure 1—figure supplement 2D). Note, for the RVM (as this is a small ROI), we extracted a weighted time series using the mask weights in functional space. While masks were in structural space (Figure 1—figure supplement 2B), segmentations of individual participant’s structural images (generated using a beta release of the developing Human Connectome Project (dHCP) pipeline) were used to remove voxels classified as CSF. Finally, voxels that fell within regions of signal dropout (i.e. where there was greater than 10 % signal loss in the functional image from maximum signal intensity) were also rejected. This was done by using a mask - automatically generated in FEAT - to identify and subtract voxels of large signal loss from the ROI masks.

All mean time series were demeaned and the three pre-stimulus data points were selected by identifying the volumes in which the noxious stimulus occurred and selecting the data from the three volumes immediately prior to each stimulus (see Figure 3—figure supplement 2A). The TR was 2.5 s so the pre-stimulus period had a duration of 7.5 s. As the stimuli could be applied at any time within a single volume the start point of the pre-stimulus period occurred between 7.5 and 10 s prior to the application of the stimulus. This resulted in 10 sets of 3 data points per mean time series per infant. Next, pairs of ROIs were taken from each infant’s data (let these be ROI1 and ROI2). The overall pre-stimulus correlation between ROI1 and ROI2 was calculated by averaging the 10 pre-stimulus correlations (one per stimulus), which were the correlations between the three pre-stimulus points from ROI1 and the equivalent set of pre-stimulus points from ROI2. This method was repeated for all combinations of ROIs resulting in a connectivity matrix displaying all correlations per infant (Figure 3—figure supplement 2B). Finally, the mean pre-stimulus functional connectivity was calculated per infant by averaging the below diagonal values of the connectivity matrix. The mean pre-stimulus connectivity was compared with the average post-stimulus percentage change in BOLD (calculated using the function Featquery, available in FSL) within a mask of all significantly active voxels (z > 3.1, p < 0.05) from the group analysis.

Regression analysis was carried out using MATLAB (Mathworks, version R2017a). Post-stimulus percentage change in BOLD in the group activity mask was input as the response variable into a linear regression model, which included the mean pre-stimulus functional connectivity within the network and infant’s GA at study (in weeks and days) as the first and second explanatory variables respectively. The parameter estimates and p-values from the model are reported in the results. Finally, the r-value (Pearson’s Correlation Coefficient) was calculated between the percentage change in BOLD and pre-stimulus functional connectivity (adjusted for age and obtained following the regression model fit).

Acknowledgements

We thank the infants and their parents for taking part in this study. We also thank Olivia Faull, Jon Campbell and Michael Sanders for their thoughtful advice on the study design and analysis. This work was funded by the Wellcome Trust. Eugene Duff is a University of Oxford Excellence Fellow in Paediatric Neuroscience, supported by the SSNAP ‘Support for the Sick Newborn and their Parents’ Medical Research Fund.

Funding Statement

The funders had no role in study design, data collection and interpretation, or the decision to submit the work for publication.

Contributor Information

Rebeccah Slater, Email: rebeccah.slater@paediatrics.ox.ac.uk.

Peggy Mason, University of Chicago, United States.

Eve Marder, Brandeis University, United States.

Funding Information

This paper was supported by the following grants:

  • Wellcome 102176 to Fiona Moultrie.

  • National Institute for Health Research Clinical Doctoral Fellowship to Fiona Moultrie.

  • Wellcome Wellcome Centre for Integrative Neuroimaging, 203139/Z/16/Z to Irene Tracey.

  • Wellcome Senior Research Fellowship, 207457/Z/17/Z to Rebeccah Slater.

Additional information

Competing interests

No competing interests declared.

Author contributions

Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Writing—original draft, Writing—review and editing.

Formal analysis, Validation, Investigation, Methodology, Writing—review and editing.

Validation, Investigation, Methodology, Writing—review and editing.

Formal analysis, Supervision, Validation, Investigation, Methodology, Writing—review and editing.

Writing—original draft, Writing—review and editing.

Supervision, Validation, Methodology, Writing—original draft, Writing—review and editing.

Conceptualization, Supervision, Investigation, Methodology, Writing—review and editing.

Conceptualization, Supervision, Funding acquisition, Methodology, Writing—original draft, Writing—review and editing.

Ethics

Human subjects: The National Research Ethics Service provided ethical approval: REC reference 12/SC/0447. Informed written parental consent, and consent to publish, was obtained. The study was carried out in accordance with the standards set by the Declaration of Helsinki and Good Clinical Practice guidelines.

Additional files

Transparent reporting form
DOI: 10.7554/eLife.37125.014

Data availability

The group brain activity data file is provided in the Source Data files. Raw data for individual infants is not provided as consent was not obtained for this data to be made publicly available.

References

  1. Andrews K, Fitzgerald M. The cutaneous withdrawal reflex in human neonates: sensitization, receptive fields, and the effects of contralateral stimulation. Pain. 1994;56:95–101. doi: 10.1016/0304-3959(94)90154-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Arichi T, Fagiolo G, Varela M, Melendez-Calderon A, Allievi A, Merchant N, Tusor N, Counsell SJ, Burdet E, Beckmann CF, Edwards AD. Development of BOLD signal hemodynamic responses in the human brain. NeuroImage. 2012;63:663–673. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.06.054. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Baliki MN, Mansour AR, Baria AT, Apkarian AV. Functional reorganization of the default mode network across chronic pain conditions. PLoS ONE. 2014;9:e106133. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0106133. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Baumgärtner U, Iannetti GD, Zambreanu L, Stoeter P, Treede RD, Tracey I. Multiple somatotopic representations of heat and mechanical pain in the operculo-insular cortex: a high-resolution fMRI study. Journal of Neurophysiology. 2010;104:2863–2872. doi: 10.1152/jn.00253.2010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Boly M, Balteau E, Schnakers C, Degueldre C, Moonen G, Luxen A, Phillips C, Peigneux P, Maquet P, Laureys S. Baseline brain activity fluctuations predict somatosensory perception in humans. PNAS. 2007;104:12187–12192. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0611404104. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Clancy B, Darlington RB, Finlay BL. Translating developmental time across mammalian species. Neuroscience. 2001;105:7–17. doi: 10.1016/S0306-4522(01)00171-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Cole MW, Yang GJ, Murray JD, Repovš G, Anticevic A. Functional connectivity change as shared signal dynamics. Journal of Neuroscience Methods. 2016;259:22–39. doi: 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2015.11.011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Cornelissen L, Fabrizi L, Patten D, Worley A, Meek J, Boyd S, Slater R, Fitzgerald M. Postnatal temporal, spatial and modality tuning of nociceptive cutaneous flexion reflexes in human infants. PLoS ONE. 2013;8:e76470. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0076470. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Dafny N, Dong WQ, Prieto-Gomez C, Reyes-Vazquez C, Stanford J, Qiao JT. Lateral hypothalamus: site involved in pain modulation. Neuroscience. 1996;70:449–460. doi: 10.1016/0306-4522(95)00358-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Denk F, McMahon SB, Tracey I. Pain vulnerability: a neurobiological perspective. Nature Neuroscience. 2014;17:192–200. doi: 10.1038/nn.3628. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Doria V, Beckmann CF, Arichi T, Merchant N, Groppo M, Turkheimer FE, Counsell SJ, Murgasova M, Aljabar P, Nunes RG, Larkman DJ, Rees G, Edwards AD. Emergence of resting state networks in the preterm human brain. PNAS. 2010;107:20015–20020. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1007921107. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Duff EP, Makin T, Cottaar M, Smith SM, Woolrich MW. Disambiguating brain functional connectivity. NeuroImage. 2018;173:540–550. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2018.01.053. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Duvernoy HM. The Human Brain Stem and Cerebellum: Surface, Structure, Vascularization, and Three-Dimensional Sectional Anatomy, with MRI. 2012. [Google Scholar]
  14. Eklund A, Nichols TE, Knutsson H. Cluster failure: Why fMRI inferences for spatial extent have inflated false-positive rates. PNAS. 2016;113:7900–7905. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1602413113. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Ezra M, Faull OK, Jbabdi S, Pattinson KT. Connectivity-based segmentation of the periaqueductal gray matter in human with brainstem optimized diffusion MRI. Human Brain Mapping. 2015;36:3459–3471. doi: 10.1002/hbm.22855. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Fabrizi L, Slater R, Worley A, Meek J, Boyd S, Olhede S, Fitzgerald M. A shift in sensory processing that enables the developing human brain to discriminate touch from pain. Current Biology. 2011;21:1552–1558. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2011.08.010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Fairhurst M, Wiech K, Dunckley P, Tracey I. Anticipatory brainstem activity predicts neural processing of pain in humans. Pain. 2007;128:101–110. doi: 10.1016/j.pain.2006.09.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Fitzgerald M, Koltzenburg M. The functional development of descending inhibitory pathways in the dorsolateral funiculus of the newborn rat spinal cord. Developmental Brain Research. 1986;389:261–270. doi: 10.1016/0165-3806(86)90194-X. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Fitzgerald M. The development of nociceptive circuits. Nature reviews. Neuroscience. 2005;6:507–520. doi: 10.1038/nrn1701. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Fox MD, Snyder AZ, Vincent JL, Corbetta M, Van Essen DC, Raichle ME. The human brain is intrinsically organized into dynamic, anticorrelated functional networks. PNAS. 2005;102:9673–9678. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0504136102. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Friston KJ. Functional and effective connectivity: a review. Brain Connectivity. 2011;1:13–36. doi: 10.1089/brain.2011.0008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Goksan S, Hartley C, Emery F, Cockrill N, Poorun R, Moultrie F, Rogers R, Campbell J, Sanders M, Adams E, Clare S, Jenkinson M, Tracey I, Slater R. fMRI reveals neural activity overlap between adult and infant pain. eLife. 2015;4:e06356. doi: 10.7554/eLife.06356. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Gratton C, Laumann TO, Nielsen AN, Greene DJ, Gordon EM, Gilmore AW, Nelson SM, Coalson RS, Snyder AZ, Schlaggar BL, Dosenbach NUF, Petersen SE. Functional brain networks are dominated by stable group and individual factors, not cognitive or daily variation. Neuron. 2018;98:439–452. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2018.03.035. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Griffanti L, Douaud G, Bijsterbosch J, Evangelisti S, Alfaro-Almagro F, Glasser MF, Duff EP, Fitzgibbon S, Westphal R, Carone D, Beckmann CF, Smith SM. Hand classification of fMRI ICA noise components. NeuroImage. 2017;154:188–205. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.12.036. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Griffanti L, Salimi-Khorshidi G, Beckmann CF, Auerbach EJ, Douaud G, Sexton CE, Zsoldos E, Ebmeier KP, Filippini N, Mackay CE, Moeller S, Xu J, Yacoub E, Baselli G, Ugurbil K, Miller KL, Smith SM. ICA-based artefact removal and accelerated fMRI acquisition for improved resting state network imaging. NeuroImage. 2014;95:232–247. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.03.034. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Harita S, Stroman PW. Confirmation of resting-state BOLD fluctuations in the human brainstem and spinal cord after identification and removal of physiological noise. Magnetic Resonance in Medicine. 2017;78:2149–2156. doi: 10.1002/mrm.26606. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Hartley C, Moultrie F, Gursul D, Hoskin A, Adams E, Rogers R, Slater R. Changing balance of spinal cord excitability and nociceptive brain activity in early human development. Current Biology. 2016;26:1998–2002. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2016.05.054. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Hathway G, Harrop E, Baccei M, Walker S, Moss A, Fitzgerald M. A postnatal switch in GABAergic control of spinal cutaneous reflexes. European Journal of Neuroscience. 2006;23:112–118. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2005.04529.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Hathway GJ, Koch S, Low L, Fitzgerald M. The changing balance of brainstem-spinal cord modulation of pain processing over the first weeks of rat postnatal life. The Journal of physiology. 2009;587:2927–2935. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.2008.168013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Jenkinson M, Bannister P, Brady M, Smith S. Improved optimization for the robust and accurate linear registration and motion correction of brain images. NeuroImage. 2002;17:825–841. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2002.1132. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Jenkinson M, Smith S. A global optimisation method for robust affine registration of brain images. Medical Image Analysis. 2001;5:143–156. doi: 10.1016/S1361-8415(01)00036-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Jensen KB, Loitoile R, Kosek E, Petzke F, Carville S, Fransson P, Marcus H, Williams SC, Choy E, Mainguy Y, Vitton O, Gracely RH, Gollub R, Ingvar M, Kong J. Patients with Fibromyalgia display less functional connectivity in the brain's pain inhibitory network. Molecular Pain. 2012;8:1744-8069-8-32. doi: 10.1186/1744-8069-8-32. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Lee MC, Zambreanu L, Menon DK, Tracey I. Identifying brain activity specifically related to the maintenance and perceptual consequence of central sensitization in humans. Journal of Neuroscience. 2008;28:11642–11649. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2638-08.2008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Mai JK. Paxinos G. Voss T. Atlas of the Human Brain. third ed. Academic Press; 2008. [Google Scholar]
  35. Maihöfner C, Handwerker HO. Differential coding of hyperalgesia in the human brain: a functional MRI study. NeuroImage. 2005;28:996–1006. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.06.049. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Mainero C, Boshyan J, Hadjikhani N. Altered functional magnetic resonance imaging resting-state connectivity in periaqueductal gray networks in migraine. Annals of Neurology. 2011;70:838–845. doi: 10.1002/ana.22537. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. McKeown M, Hu YJ, Jane Wang Z. ICA denoising for Event-Related fMRI studies. Conference Proceedings : ... Annual International Conference of the IEEE Engineering in Medicine and Biology Society. IEEE Engineering in Medicine and Biology Society. Annual Conference. 2005;1:157–161. doi: 10.1109/IEMBS.2005.1616366. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. McKeown MJ, Makeig S, Brown GG, Jung TP, Kindermann SS, Bell AJ, Sejnowski TJ. Analysis of fMRI data by blind separation into independent spatial components. Human Brain Mapping. 1998;6:160–188. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1097-0193(1998)6:3&#x0003c;160::AID-HBM5&#x0003e;3.0.CO;2-1. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. McMahon S, Koltzenburg M, Tracey I, Turk DC. Wall & Melzack's Textbook of Pain. Elsevier Health Sciences; 2013. [Google Scholar]
  40. McMullan S, Lumb BM. Midbrain control of spinal nociception discriminates between responses evoked by myelinated and unmyelinated heat nociceptors in the rat. Pain. 2006;124:59–68. doi: 10.1016/j.pain.2006.03.015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Ossipov MH, Dussor GO, Porreca F. Central modulation of pain. Journal of Clinical Investigation. 2010;120:3779–3787. doi: 10.1172/JCI43766. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Petrovic P, Kalso E, Petersson KM, Ingvar M. Placebo and opioid analgesia-- imaging a shared neuronal network. Science. 2002;295:1737–1740. doi: 10.1126/science.1067176. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Ploner M, Lee MC, Wiech K, Bingel U, Tracey I. Prestimulus functional connectivity determines pain perception in humans. PNAS. 2010;107:355–360. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0906186106. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Power JD, Barnes KA, Snyder AZ, Schlaggar BL, Petersen SE. Spurious but systematic correlations in functional connectivity MRI networks arise from subject motion. NeuroImage. 2012;59:2142–2154. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.10.018. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Raichle ME. The brain's default mode network. Annual Review of Neuroscience. 2015;38:433–447. doi: 10.1146/annurev-neuro-071013-014030. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Rajkowska G, Goldman-Rakic PS. Cytoarchitectonic definition of prefrontal Areas in the normal human cortex: ii. variability in locations of Areas 9 and 46 and relationship to the talairach coordinate system. Cerebral Cortex. 1995;5:323–337. doi: 10.1093/cercor/5.4.323. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Reynolds DV. Surgery in the rat during electrical analgesia induced by focal brain stimulation. Science. 1969;164:444–445. doi: 10.1126/science.164.3878.444. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Salimi-Khorshidi G, Douaud G, Beckmann CF, Glasser MF, Griffanti L, Smith SM. Automatic denoising of functional MRI data: combining independent component analysis and hierarchical fusion of classifiers. NeuroImage. 2014;90:449–468. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.11.046. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Sallet J, Mars RB, Noonan MP, Neubert FX, Jbabdi S, O'Reilly JX, Filippini N, Thomas AG, Rushworth MF. The organization of dorsal frontal cortex in humans and macaques. Journal of Neuroscience. 2013;33:12255–12274. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5108-12.2013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Schwaller F, Kanellopoulos AH, Fitzgerald M. The developmental emergence of differential brainstem serotonergic control of the sensory spinal cord. Scientific Reports. 2017;7:3779. doi: 10.1038/s41598-017-02509-2. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Schweinhardt P, Bushnell MC. Pain imaging in health and disease--how far have we come? Journal of Clinical Investigation. 2010;120:3788–3797. doi: 10.1172/JCI43498. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Segerdahl AR, Themistocleous AC, Fido D, Bennett DL, Tracey I. A brain-based pain facilitation mechanism contributes to painful diabetic polyneuropathy. Brain. 2018;141:357–364. doi: 10.1093/brain/awx337. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Serag A, Aljabar P, Ball G, Counsell SJ, Boardman JP, Rutherford MA, Edwards AD, Hajnal JV, Rueckert D. Construction of a consistent high-definition spatio-temporal atlas of the developing brain using adaptive kernel regression. NeuroImage. 2012;59:2255–2265. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.09.062. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Shi F, Yap PT, Wu G, Jia H, Gilmore JH, Lin W, Shen D. Infant brain atlases from neonates to 1- and 2-year-olds. PLoS ONE. 2011;6:e18746. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0018746. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Smith SM, Vidaurre D, Beckmann CF, Glasser MF, Jenkinson M, Miller KL, Nichols TE, Robinson EC, Salimi-Khorshidi G, Woolrich MW, Barch DM, Uğurbil K, Van Essen DC. Functional connectomics from resting-state fMRI. Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 2013;17:666–682. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2013.09.016. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Smith SM. Fast robust automated brain extraction. Human Brain Mapping. 2002;17:143–155. doi: 10.1002/hbm.10062. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Stagg CJ, Lin RL, Mezue M, Segerdahl A, Kong Y, Xie J, Tracey I. Widespread modulation of cerebral perfusion induced during and after transcranial direct current stimulation applied to the left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex. Journal of Neuroscience. 2013;33:11425–11431. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3887-12.2013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Thesen S, Heid O, Mueller E, Schad LR. Prospective acquisition correction for head motion with image-based tracking for real-time fMRI. Magnetic Resonance in Medicine. 2000;44:457–465. doi: 10.1002/1522-2594(200009)44:3&#x0003c;457::AID-MRM17&#x0003e;3.0.CO;2-R. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Tracey I, Mantyh PW. The cerebral signature for pain perception and its modulation. Neuron. 2007;55:377–391. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2007.07.012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Tracey I. Getting the pain you expect: mechanisms of placebo, nocebo and reappraisal effects in humans. Nature Medicine. 2010;16:1277–1283. doi: 10.1038/nm.2229. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Wager TD, Rilling JK, Smith EE, Sokolik A, Casey KL, Davidson RJ, Kosslyn SM, Rose RM, Cohen JD. Placebo-induced changes in FMRI in the anticipation and experience of pain. Science. 2004;303:1162–1167. doi: 10.1126/science.1093065. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Walker SM, Fitzgerald M, Hathway GJ. Surgical injury in the neonatal rat alters the adult pattern of descending modulation from the rostroventral medulla. Anesthesiology. 2015;122:1391–1400. doi: 10.1097/ALN.0000000000000658. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  63. Walker SM, Franck LS, Fitzgerald M, Myles J, Stocks J, Marlow N. Long-term impact of neonatal intensive care and surgery on somatosensory perception in children born extremely preterm. Pain. 2009;141:79–87. doi: 10.1016/j.pain.2008.10.012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  64. Waters AJ, Lumb BM. Descending control of spinal nociception from the periaqueductal grey distinguishes between neurons with and without C-fibre inputs. Pain. 2008;134:32–40. doi: 10.1016/j.pain.2007.03.025. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  65. Wiech K, Jbabdi S, Lin CS, Andersson J, Tracey I. Differential structural and resting state connectivity between insular subdivisions and other pain-related brain regions. Pain. 2014;155:2047–2055. doi: 10.1016/j.pain.2014.07.009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  66. Williams G, Fabrizi L, Meek J, Jackson D, Tracey I, Robertson N, Slater R, Fitzgerald M. Functional magnetic resonance imaging can be used to explore tactile and nociceptive processing in the infant brain. Acta Paediatrica. 2015;104:158–166. doi: 10.1111/apa.12848. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  67. Yu R, Gollub RL, Spaeth R, Napadow V, Wasan A, Kong J. Disrupted functional connectivity of the periaqueductal gray in chronic low back pain. NeuroImage. Clinical. 2014;6:100–108. doi: 10.1016/j.nicl.2014.08.019. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  68. Zhuo M, Gebhart GF. Biphasic modulation of spinal nociceptive transmission from the medullary raphe nuclei in the rat. Journal of Neurophysiology. 1997;78:746–758. doi: 10.1152/jn.1997.78.2.746. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Decision letter

Editor: Peggy Mason1

In the interests of transparency, eLife includes the editorial decision letter and accompanying author responses. A lightly edited version of the letter sent to the authors after peer review is shown, indicating the most substantive concerns; minor comments are not usually included.

Thank you for submitting your article "The influence of the descending pain modulatory system on infant pain-related brain activity" for consideration by eLife. Your article has been reviewed by 3 peer reviewers, including Peggy Mason as the Reviewing Editor and Reviewer #1, and the evaluation has been overseen by Sabine Kastner as the Senior Editor.

The reviewers have discussed the reviews with one another and the Reviewing Editor has drafted this decision to help you prepare a revised submission.

The reviewers agree that these data are valuable, even precious, and that the manuscript is well written. The following aspects could be clarified or better justified.

– The control network. The reviewers discussed this "control" extensively. For the non-aficionados of fMRI, the inclusion of this network does not appear justified. What makes this collection of structures into a control? What is the functional connection between the structures or did you simply choose structures not involved in the pain matrix? The "control" network choices are further confusing given the group's previous work on hippocampus and pain (Ploghaus et al., 2001). In sum, this strategy needs to be better explained and justified.

– A different approach may be to replace the control network with a control time – a baseline period when activity is unrelated to the stimulus.

– There was skepticism regarding the resolution and accuracy of the brainstem activations. The authors may consider deleting this from the manuscript.

– The data are at odds with previous findings that facilitation predominates over inhibition in the neonate and that, consistent with this, the newborn human is exquisitely sensitive to nociceptor stimulation. Please address this discrepancy in the Results and Discussion.

– Descending modulation preferentially affects C-fiber inputs over A-deltas. A-delta inputs are often unaffected or facilitated by descending modulation. Again, this is at odds with the present results and the discrepancy should be discussed.

Reviewer #1:

This fMRI study in human infants suggests that functional connectivity within the pain matrix activates descending modulation resulting in less activation in the pain matrix. This study depends on highly processed fMRI data and a secondary analysis of this processed data from which connectivity is inferred. This reviewer is unable to assess the validity of these methods or the sufficiency or lack thereof of the sample size (13 subjects). Yet the ideas are intriguing, and the story is interesting and exciting.

Reviewer #2:

This fMRI study in human infants describes an inverse relationship between functional connectivity strength in networks associated with the descending pain modulatory system (DPMS) and nociceptor evoked brain activity. The authors suggest that in newborn infants the DPMS may contribute an inhibitory influence on nociceptor evoked brain activity.

I am not best placed to critically evaluate details of the fMRI design and methodology. However, I do have comments on the interpretation of the data.

1) Importantly the study includes a comparison between the relationship of DPMS network connectivity and a 'control' network to nociceptor evoked brain activity. However, it was unclear if there was a known functional relationship in the control network or whether these were just selected as brain regions not involved in the DPMS network.

2) The balance between facilitatory and inhibitory influences of DPMS on spinal nociceptive processing is dynamic and changes during the progression of chronic pain, in different behavioural and emotional states, and during development. There is a significant body of evidence to suggest that in neonatal rodents, facilitation predominates over inhibitory control and, consistent with this, the newborn human is exquisitely sensitive to nociceptor stimulation. However, this seems to be at odds with the data presented in this manuscript which imply that it is descending inhibition that is inversely related to the noxious-evoked brain activity i.e. in the newborn, the stronger the functional connectivity in components of the DPMS network the lower the evoked brain activity. The authors need to address this apparent anomaly.

3) For understandable ethical reasons, monitoring of brain activity is limited to responses to pin prick stimuli of cutaneous tissues, which will preferentially activate A-delta nociceptors. However, descending inhibitory control from the brain targets spinal neuronal responses to C-nociceptor stimulation, whereas responses to A-delta nociceptive input may be unaffected or even facilitated. The authors need to discuss the impact of this on the interpretation of their data.

Reviewer #3:

This manuscript details the results of an fMRI investigation examining noxious-stimulus evoked activation in response to pin-prick stimuli in a group of term born neonates. The authors examined pre-stimulus activation in brain regions mediating pain modulation. The activation in these brain regions was compared to the noxious-stimulus evoked activation and to activation in brain regions not involved in pain perception or its descending modulation.

The manuscript is part of new emerging literature examining the development of nociceptive pathways in infants using in vivo MRI. While the sample size is modest, the data obtained in the experiment are rare and were likely difficult to acquire.

This work is important to the field, the approach and results are novel and the manuscript is well written and straightforward. Some methodological issues affect the interpretation of results that I have detailed below. Further, the manuscript could be strengthened by adding to the Results and Discussion section to expand upon the interpretation of the findings.

Essential to the understanding of the results is the definition of the pre-stimulus period. While this information is included in the Materials and methods section and displayed in Figure 3—figure supplement 2, making mention of the timing and duration of the pre-stimulus period at the outset would be beneficial to the reader. Additionally, in the previous work by the group (Ploner et al., 2010), the pre-stimulus period was 3 secs before the noxious stimulus was administered. In the current work, what was the rationale for choosing the timing for the pre-stimulus period? Was there an indicator of when the noxious stimuli would be applied?

Related to the pre-stimulus event, was a pre-stimulus period modeled in the analysis for each stimulus including the first stimulus? Of interest would be to report on pre-stimulus activation throughout the course of the fMRI scanning run.

While it would not be possible to obtain pain ratings in the context of the current experiment with infants, the authors noted in their previous work (Goksan et al., 2015) that a foot withdrawal was often elicited in response to the stimulus. Did the authors record foot withdrawals during the course of the experiment?

What was the baseline (no pain condition) that the authors used for subtraction from the activation associated with the noxious stimuli? Figure 3—figure supplement 2A could be updated to highlight the baseline condition.

Imaging reliable activation in the brainstem in adults is challenging in the context of an fMRI experiment not involving noxious stimulation. As the authors note, obtaining reliable activation in the brainstem of an infant receiving noxious stimuli is not only difficult to due movement-related artifact but also by respiration. The authors note that they addressed motion in the current experiment by regressing out movement-related activation using ICA. The authors note that FIX was employed in the Materials and methods section. For FIX to be effective in identifying good and bad components, a training data set should be provided. Was this performed for the current analysis using a previous data set? Did the authors consider global signal reduction or "scrubbing" methods to address motion in the study?

Related to this issue of activation in the brainstem, the choice of blurring kernel of 4.5mm FWHM to perform spatial smoothing may be considered large in relation to the anatomical size of the brainstem nuclei. The size of the rostral ventral medulla (RVM) in adults is likely to be on the order of a few cubic millimetres. Did the authors consider the overall size of the RVM in the neonate during the preprocessing of the data? Was the activation reliable in this region across participants?

Of note is that no stimulus-evoked activation was seen in the anterior insula while instead activation in several frontal and temporal lobe regions. Could the authors add to the Results and Discussion section concerning the stimulus-evoked activation in the infant brain in relation to previously published experimental pain studies in the adult literature?

eLife. 2018 Sep 11;7:e37125. doi: 10.7554/eLife.37125.018

Author response


The reviewers have discussed the reviews with one another and the Reviewing Editor has drafted this decision to help you prepare a revised submission

The reviewers agree that these data are valuable, even precious, and that the manuscript is well written. The following aspects could be clarified or better justified.

– The control network. The reviewers discussed this "control" extensively. For the non-aficionados of fMRI, the inclusion of this network does not appear justified. What makes this collection of structures into a control? What is the functional connection between the structures or did you simply choose structures not involved in the pain matrix? The "control" network choices are further confusing given the group's previous work on hippocampus and pain (Ploghaus et al., 2001). In sum, this strategy needs to be better explained and justified.

Choosing a control network in fMRI studies is challenging. In this case, we chose a combination of brain regions that are not thought to be involved in descending pain modulation, and we ensured that they had a similar topographic distribution to the brain regions included in the DPMS network, consisting of cortical, subcortical and brainstem structures. Whilst we acknowledge that the hippocampus is involved in pain-related anxiety, our previous studies have not identified a role for the hippocampus in the DPMS (Ploghaus et al., 2001). The rationale for choosing this combination of regions was two-fold. First, to the best of our knowledge, this set of brain regions does not form a typical functional network, so there should be no relationship between the functional connectivity of this ‘network’ and the magnitude of the stimulus response, thus acting as a ‘negative’ control. Second, BOLD fMRI data is known to contain ‘global signals’ present across the entire brain that can originate from neurologically uninteresting sources, such as motion and respiration. Our data preprocessing included ICA denoising, which dramatically reduced the effects of both motion and respiration but cannot remove global effects. Inclusion of this ‘control network’ was intended to address the potential risk of an artefactual global signal driving the correlations we report – if a non-BOLD-related global signal were driving our results, this effect should be present in the Control Network – but this was not observed. We therefore consider that the inclusion of this network robustly demonstrates that general increased functional connectivity across the brain does not necessarily influence the noxious-evoked changes in BOLD activity. The rationale for this approach has been explained in more detail in the revised Results and Discussion section.

We do however appreciate that these brain regions do not form part of an established network. Therefore, we have included an additional control, the Default Mode Network, which is an established network that has been identified in adults and term infants (Doria et al., 2010; Raichle, 2015). This allows us to demonstrate the specificity of the relationship between the prestimulus functional connectivity of the DPMS and the noxious-evoked BOLD activity. The Default Mode network included the posterior cingulate cortex (PCC), medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC) and inferior parietal lobules (IPL).

These additional analyses have been added to the Results and Discussion section, where we now show that there is no evidence that the prestimulus functional connectivity of the Default Mode Network influences noxious-evoked BOLD activity. We would like to thank the Reviewers and Editors for raising this important point – we believe it has substantially improved the validity of our reported observations.

– A different approach may be to replace the control network with a control time – a baseline period when activity is unrelated to the stimulus.

We did not record resting state brain activity in this study so there is limited scope to investigate the relationship between the functional connectivity of the DPMS in an ‘independent’ time-period and the noxious-evoked BOLD activity. This is an important question and it forms a central direction for future research in this area, which we now discuss in the manuscript (subsection “Relationship between the pre-stimulus functional connectivity of the DPMS and noxious-evoked brain activity”).

– There was skepticism regarding the resolution and accuracy of the brainstem activations. The authors may consider deleting this from the manuscript.

We very carefully considered the inclusion of brainstem activations and the accuracy of localising them in the RVM and PAG and discussed this extensively with experienced adult brainstem imagers (Faull – included in the Acknowledgements), in addition to our authors who have published extensively on the adult brainstem and DPMS. To ensure that the results we report do not entirely rely upon the connectivity of these brainstem regions, we conducted the analysis both with and without their inclusion, confirming that the inclusion of brainstem data did not significantly influence the results.

To address the reviewers concerns further, we have now provided the anatomical RVM and PAG masks in functional space for each infant (Supplementary file 3), and an example time course for both regions from one infant can be seen in Figure 3—figure supplement 2A. While we acknowledge the inherent limitations of studying small brain regions, we have carefully considered the activity identified to ensure that it is localised within these anatomical structures. Data from four infants where brainstem data was not adequately localised were excluded from all analyses in the original manuscript.

We acknowledge that application of spatial smoothing, and variations in approaches of transforming small ROIs from standard to functional space, could potentially affect the results from these small regions. In order to address this, we have revised the Materials and methods section and now report data without spatial smoothing and using a weighted mean timecourse from the RVM ROI, with minimal impact on the results reported in the manuscript.

These limitations have now been specifically addressed in the Results and Discussion section.

– The data are at odds with previous findings that facilitation predominates over inhibition in the neonate and that, consistent with this, the newborn human is exquisitely sensitive to nociceptor stimulation. Please address this discrepancy in the Results and Discussion.

This is an extremely important point, which we did not adequately discuss in the original manuscript but is now explained in detail in our revision (Results and Discussion section). In brief, both human and non-human neonates are substantially more sensitive to noxious stimulation than adults, which has long been regarded as being the result of hyper-excitable spinal reflex networks. In the neonatal rodent, DPMS brain structures facilitate (rather than inhibit) spinally-mediated nocifensive behaviours. Similarly, in premature human infants, spinally-mediated reflex withdrawals are exaggerated. During human preterm development however, it has been shown that these reflex responses are refined, with shorter duration, lower amplitude and higher response thresholds (Cornelissen et al., 2013; Hartley et al., 2016). The data presented here in term infants suggest that by term gestation DPMS networks are present and appear to have an inhibitory function, evidenced by a correlative reduction in noxious-evoked brain activity, similar to that observed in the more mature adult brain. While it is tempting to draw comparisons between this study and those in neonatal rodents that have measured spinal responses to PAG activation, it must be acknowledged that our study has not measured activity in the spinal cord, and we do not know how increased functional connectivity in the DPMS impacts spinal activity in the term-aged infant. In order to address the neonatal rodent literature in our manuscript, we have added some caveats to the Discussion about assumptions regarding age equivalence between human infants and neonatal rat pups and assumptions regarding CNS maturation as a linear and uniform process. Different parts of the CNS clearly mature at different rates and the developmental trajectory may differ between rodents and man.

– Descending modulation preferentially affects C-fiber inputs over A-deltas. A-delta inputs are often unaffected or facilitated by descending modulation. Again, this is at odds with the present results and the discrepancy should be discussed.

As highlighted by the reviewer, in the adult rodent, PAG activation differentially modulates C- and A-delta sensory fibre dorsal horn input (Waters and Lumb, 2008), but how other supraspinal components of the DPMS respond to activity in subclasses of nociceptors is unknown, especially in man. Equally, the age at which descending fibres from the PAG-RVM axis and dorsal horn neurons form is unknown. This is now discussed in detail in the manuscript (Results and Discussion section).

Reviewer #2:

This fMRI study in human infants describes an inverse relationship between functional connectivity strength in networks associated with the descending pain modulatory system (DPMS) and nociceptor evoked brain activity. The authors suggest that in newborn infants the DPMS may contribute an inhibitory influence on nociceptor evoked brain activity.

I am not best placed to critically evaluate details of the fMRI design and methodology. However, I do have comments on the interpretation of the data.

Reviewer 2 has raised several important points regarding the interpretation of our data. These points were all highlighted by the Editors as key points to address. Each of these points have been revised in the manuscript and addressed directly in our response to the Editors (above). We thank the reviewer for this insightful review.

1) Importantly the study includes a comparison between the relationship of DPMS network connectivity and a 'control' network to nociceptor evoked brain activity. However, it was unclear if there was a known functional relationship in the control network or whether these were just selected as brain regions not involved in the DPMS network.

This has been discussed in detail in the revised manuscript and in the response to the Editor (see above).

2) The balance between facilitatory and inhibitory influences of DPMS on spinal nociceptive processing is dynamic and changes during the progression of chronic pain, in different behavioural and emotional states, and during development. There is a significant body of evidence to suggest that in neonatal rodents, facilitation predominates over inhibitory control and, consistent with this, the newborn human is exquisitely sensitive to nociceptor stimulation. However, this seems to be at odds with the data presented in this manuscript which imply that it is descending inhibition that is inversely related to the noxious-evoked brain activity i.e. in the newborn, the stronger the functional connectivity in components of the DPMS network the lower the evoked brain activity. The authors need to address this apparent anomaly.

This has been discussed in detail in the revised manuscript and in the response to the Editor (see above).

3) For understandable ethical reasons, monitoring of brain activity is limited to responses to pin prick stimuli of cutaneous tissues, which will preferentially activate A-delta nociceptors. However, descending inhibitory control from the brain targets spinal neuronal responses to C-nociceptor stimulation, whereas responses to A-delta nociceptive input may be unaffected or even facilitated. The authors need to discuss the impact of this on the interpretation of their data.

This has been discussed in detail in the revised manuscript and in the response to the Editor (see above).

Reviewer #3:

This manuscript details the results of an fMRI investigation examining noxious-stimulus evoked activation in response to pin-prick stimuli in a group of term born neonates. The authors examined pre-stimulus activation in brain regions mediating pain modulation. The activation in these brain regions was compared to the noxious-stimulus evoked activation and to activation in brain regions not involved in pain perception or its descending modulation.

The manuscript is part of new emerging literature examining the development of nociceptive pathways in infants using in vivo MRI. While the sample size is modest, the data obtained in the experiment are rare and were likely difficult to acquire.

This work is important to the field, the approach and results are novel and the manuscript is well written and straightforward. Some methodological issues affect the interpretation of results that I have detailed below. Further, the manuscript could be strengthened by adding to the Results and Discussion section to expand upon the interpretation of the findings.

Many thanks for this insightful review. The Results and Discussion section has been substantially edited to provide a more in-depth interpretation of our results. In particular, we have discussed the value of including the Default Mode Network and Control Network in our analyses; discussed the difference between observations in the human and rodent literature; and expanded on the limitations in brainstem imaging. We have also provided more supporting data in Supplementary figures and Supplementary data files. All the text edits have been highlighted in the revised manuscript.

Essential to the understanding of the results is the definition of the pre-stimulus period. While this information is included in the Materials and methods section and displayed in Figure 3—figure supplement 2, making mention of the timing and duration of the pre-stimulus period at the outset would be beneficial to the reader. Additionally, in the previous work by the group (Ploner et al., 2010) the pre-stimulus period was 3 secs before the noxious stimulus was administered. In the current work, what was the rationale for choosing the timing for the prestimulus period? Was there an indicator of when the noxious stimuli would be applied?

The manuscript has been revised so that the key experimental details about the pre-stimulus period have now been included in the main body of the text (Introduction) and are further explained in the Materials and methods section. We have clarified that the pre-stimulus period included the three volumes prior to each stimulus. Given that the TR was 2.5 seconds, the duration of the pre-stimulus period was 7.5 seconds. The stimuli could be applied at any time within a volume, therefore the start of the pre-stimulus period ranged from 7.5 to 10 seconds prior to the application of the stimulus.

In this study, we chose a longer pre-stimulus period than previously used in the Ploner study to provide a more stable estimation of the time series correlations in the pre-stimulus period. Ten seconds represented the maximum number of time-points we could include without markedly encroaching on the recovery of the HRF, and allowed sufficient time for the stimulus evoked HRF to return to a baseline state (Arichi et al., 2012).

We have also clarified details concerning the application of stimuli in the Materials and methods section. Infants were unable to anticipate the stimulus as the experimenter received a visual cue 25 seconds after the stimulus and then waited for the infant to be still prior to applying the next stimulus. The application of stimuli was therefore not entirely regular or predictable.

Related to the pre-stimulus event, was a pre-stimulus period modeled in the analysis for each stimulus including the first stimulus? Of interest would be to report on pre-stimulus activation throughout the course of the fMRI scanning run.

An HRF model of the pre-stimulus period was not used in this study, but instead the mean time courses were extracted in the pre-stimulus period and the mean functional connectivity across the DPMS in the pre-stimulus period was calculated. We have extended our analysis to investigate whether the functional connectivity of this network was dependent on the stimulus number. We found that the pre-stimulus functional connectivity of the DPMS was stable and did not vary significantly. This result has been added to Figure 1—figure supplement 3 and included in the Results and Discussion section.

While it would not be possible to obtain pain ratings in the context of the current experiment with infants, the authors noted in their previous work (Goksan et al., 2015) that a foot withdrawal was often elicited in response to the stimulus. Did the authors record foot withdrawals during the course of the experiment?

During the experiment, we visually observed whether reflexes were evoked by the stimuli however we did not quantify these observations. Video footage of reflexes was not collected in this study, although in our current work we are now aiming to video infant reflexes throughout the scanning period. We have previously investigated the relationship between brain activity and reflex activity in infants using EEG and EMG, and we plan to investigate this further using fMRI. The importance of understanding this relationship is critical if we are to bridge our understanding from neonatal rat pup studies to human observations. This topic has been addressed in our revised Results and Discussion section.

What was the baseline (no pain condition) that the authors used for subtraction from the activation associated with the noxious stimuli? Figure 3—figure supplement 2A could be updated to highlight the baseline condition.

Our response to the stimuli was modelled with respect to the rest periods in between applications of the noxious stimuli (Figure 3—figure supplement 2). The inter-stimulus interval was a minimum of 25 seconds. This was not highlighted in the original figure and has now been added to the figure legend. The temporal mean that was used to calculate the percentage change in BOLD has been added to Figure 3—figure supplement 2D.

Imaging reliable activation in the brainstem in adults is challenging in the context of an fMRI experiment not involving noxious stimulation. As the authors note, obtaining reliable activation in the brainstem of an infant receiving noxious stimuli is not only difficult to due movement-related artifact but also by respiration. The authors note that they addressed motion in the current experiment by regressing out movement-related activation using ICA. The authors note that FIX was employed in the Methods. For FIX to be effective in identifying good and bad components, a training data set should be provided. Was this performed for the current analysis using a previous data set? Did the authors consider global signal reduction or "scrubbing" methods to address motion in the study?

We revised the Materials and methods section to include more comprehensive details regarding how we implemented FIX. In brief, the components were manually classified (Griffanti et al., 2017). FIX was then used to regress out both the noise ICA components and the 24 motion parameter time series. Thus, FIX was used to implement manual ICA clean-up, and did not need to be trained on any prior data. Also, using this approach manual FIX denoising can remove noise due to respiration and cardiac pulsatility, not just head motion. We did not use Scrubbing to remove time points that were influenced by motion artefact, as we found that implementing FIX provided a robust approach to reduce motion-related confounds. Similarly, while global signal regression (GSR) can be helpful in reducing motion artefacts, it also introduces confounds and can alter correlation structure and remove BOLD-related signal. We acknowledge that the data will contain global signals that cannot be removed by any of the preprocessing steps that we implemented, and therefore included the Control Network to address any global signal confounds (see response to Editor).

Related to this issue of activation in the brainstem, the choice of blurring kernel of 4.5mm FWHM to perform spatial smoothing may be considered large in relation to the anatomical size of the brainstem nuclei. The size of the rostral ventral medulla (RVM) in adults is likely to be on the order of a few cubic millimetres. Did the authors consider the overall size of the RVM in the neonate during the preprocessing of the data? Was the activation reliable in this region across participants?

We very carefully considered the inclusion of brainstem activations and the accuracy of localising them in the RVM and PAG and discussed this extensively with experienced adult brainstem imagers (Faull – included in the Acknowledgements), in addition to our authors with expertise in this field. To ensure that the results we report do not entirely rely upon the connectivity of these brainstem regions, we conducted the analysis both with and without their inclusion, confirming that the inclusion of brainstem data did not significantly influence the results.

To address the reviewers concerns further, we have now provided the anatomical RVM and PAG masks in functional space for each infant (Supplementary file 3) and an example time course for both regions from one infant can be seen in Figure 3—figure supplement 2A. While we acknowledge the inherent limitations of studying small brain regions, we have carefully considered the activity identified to ensure that it is localised within these anatomical structures. Data from four infants where brainstem data was not adequately localised were excluded from all analyses in the original manuscript.

We acknowledge that application of spatial smoothing, and variations in approaches of transforming small ROIs from standard to functional space, could potentially affect the results from these small regions. In order to address this, we have revised the Materials and methods section and report data without spatial smoothing and using a weighted mean timecourse from the RVM ROI, with minimal impact on the results reported in the manuscript.

Of note is that no stimulus-evoked activation was seen in the anterior insula while instead activation in several frontal and temporal lobe regions. Could the authors add to the Results and Discussion section concerning the stimulus-evoked activation in the infant brain in relation to previously published experimental pain studies in the adult literature?

We previously described that activity in the insular cortices was restricted to the posterior section (Goksan et al., 2015) and this observation is again reported in the present study, using our optimised data analysis protocols. As highlighted by the reviewer, highly localised clusters of activity were recorded in frontal and temporal lobe regions. In adults, attention and the threat of pain increase pain perception, and is associated with increased neural activity in the anterior insular cortex (Ploner et al., 2011, Wiech et al., 2010). We could postulate that infants do not evaluate or contextualise the nociceptive input in the same way as adults, which may contribute to the lack of activity within these regions. However, we cannot make statistical inferences about subthreshold activity.

Associated Data

    This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

    Supplementary Materials

    Figure 1—source data 1. All region-of-interest masks in standard space.
    DOI: 10.7554/eLife.37125.006
    Figure 2—source data 1. Thresholded group activity map.
    DOI: 10.7554/eLife.37125.008
    Figure 3—source data 1. Individual DPMS brainstem masks in functional space.
    DOI: 10.7554/eLife.37125.013
    Transparent reporting form
    DOI: 10.7554/eLife.37125.014

    Data Availability Statement

    The group brain activity data file is provided in the Source Data files. Raw data for individual infants is not provided as consent was not obtained for this data to be made publicly available.


    Articles from eLife are provided here courtesy of eLife Sciences Publications, Ltd

    RESOURCES