Skip to main content
PLOS ONE logoLink to PLOS ONE
. 2018 Sep 12;13(9):e0203000. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0203000

The relationship between greenspace and the mental wellbeing of adults: A systematic review

Victoria Houlden 1,*, Scott Weich 2, João Porto de Albuquerque 3,#, Stephen Jarvis 4, Karen Rees 5,#
Editor: C Mary Schooling6
PMCID: PMC6135392  PMID: 30208073

Abstract

Introduction

The view that interacting with nature enhances mental wellbeing is commonplace, despite a dearth of evidence or even agreed definitions of ‘nature’. The aim of this review was to systematically appraise the evidence for associations between greenspace and mental wellbeing, stratified by the different ways in which greenspace has been conceptualised in quantitative research.

Methods

We undertook a comprehensive database search and thorough screening of articles which included a measure of greenspace and validated mental wellbeing tool, to capture aspects of hedonic and/or eudaimonic wellbeing. Quality and risk of bias in research were assessed to create grades of evidence. We undertook detailed narrative synthesis of the 50 studies which met the review inclusion criteria, as methodological heterogeneity precluded meta-analysis.

Results

Results of a quality assessment and narrative synthesis suggest associations between different greenspace characteristics and mental wellbeing. We identified six ways in which greenspace was conceptualised and measured: (i) amount of local-area greenspace; (ii) greenspace type; (iii) visits to greenspace; (iv) views of greenspace; (v) greenspace accessibility; and (vi) self-reported connection to nature. There was adequate evidence for associations between the amount of local-area greenspace and life satisfaction (hedonic wellbeing), but not personal flourishing (eudaimonic wellbeing). Evidence for associations between mental wellbeing and visits to greenspace, accessibility, and types of greenspace was limited. There was inadequate evidence for associations with views of greenspace and connectedness to nature. Several studies reported variation in associations between greenspace and wellbeing by life course stage, gender, levels of physically activity or attitudes to nature.

Conclusions

Greenspace has positive associations with mental wellbeing (particularly hedonic wellbeing), but the evidence is not currently sufficient or specific enough to guide planning decisions. Further studies are needed, based on dynamic measures of greenspace, reflecting access and uses of greenspace, and measures of both eudaimonic and hedonic mental wellbeing.

Introduction

Background

Urbanisation is increasing at an unprecedented rate, and with over half the world’s population now residing in cities [1], many people may not have access to the green landscapes in which the human species evolved [2, 3]. Greenspace may provide human benefits, such as facilitating exercise, social activities and connecting with nature [4], and it is suggested that urban greenspaces are critical to healthy living, both physically [5, 6] and mentally [7, 8]. There may also be salutogenic effects on mental health and wellbeing, such as increased attention, feelings of happiness and reduced stress [9, 10].

The United Nations Sustainable Development Goals emphasise the importance of greenspace provision “to foster prosperity and quality of life for all” [11]. The World Health Organisation stated that urban greenspaces (including parks, woodlands, and sports facilities) are a “necessary component for delivering healthy, sustainable, liveable conditions” [12], while highlighting the dearth of evidence to support planning advice [12]. In the UK, local authorities are responsible for providing access to the natural environment [13], and guidelines recommend that all residents should live within 300m of at least 2 hectares of greenspace [14, 15], despite limited evidence for the wellbeing benefits of these recommendations.

Measuring greenspace

One of the reasons for this dearth of evidence is the lack of consensus regarding the definition of the terms ‘nature’ and ‘natural’ [10, 16], and features that may appear ‘natural’ are often artificially constructed [8]. Hartig et al. provide the most detailed definition of nature, as the “physical features and processes of nonhuman origin…, the ‘living nature’ of flora and fauna” [8].

Furthermore, ‘nature’ and ‘greenspace’ are often used interchangeably [1721]; ‘greenspace’ is more inclusive, referring to areas of grass, trees or other vegetation [22], and can be used to describe both surrounding greenness in the countryside, and spaces managed or reserved in urban environments [14]. Greenspace was therefore chosen as the focus of this review. We chose not to include studies of water (blue space), as this is generally considered separately to greenspace [5, 2325].

Mental wellbeing and greenspace

Mental wellbeing comprises happiness and life satisfaction (hedonic wellbeing) and fulfilment, functioning and purpose in life (eudaimonic wellbeing) [26, 27]. It is therefore a multi-dimensional measure of positive mental health, reflecting more than an absence of mental distress, in which those with the best mental wellbeing are able to realise their potential, cope well with everyday stressors, and flourish mentally. It is increasingly recognised as an indicator of national prosperity [28], due to its associations with productivity, longevity and societal functioning [2830]. While theories suggest that mental wellbeing may be improved by exposure to greenspace, there is limited evidence for clear benefits; many studies use unvalidated measures or proxies such as mental distress or quality of life [7]. Additionally, measures of nature and greenspace vary widely [8, 12, 22].

Previous reviews have examined the relationship between greenspace (/nature) and general health [7, 8, 12], or mental health [31], although the latter has generally been defined in terms of mental distress, rather than mental wellbeing. While Douglas et al. describe their recent scoping review as focussing on “green space benefits for health and well-being”, they include no studies measuring mental wellbeing per se, but provide further evidence for reduced mental distress in greener neighbourhoods [7]. Similarly, Gascon et al.’s review of “Mental Health Benefits” of long-term greenspace exposure includes some studies of aspects of mental wellbeing, but focusses mainly on measures of mental distress, rather than positive mental health [31]. We therefore believe this is the first review to examine greenspace associations specifically with mental wellbeing, in adults.

The aim of this review was therefore to synthesise quantitative evidence for associations between greenspace and mental wellbeing. We were able to identify varying evidence for associations between different characterisations of greenspace and mental wellbeing, while highlighting key areas for future research, and subsequent implications for policy and practice.

Materials and methods

Search strategy and selection criteria

The review was registered with PROSPERO (available at https://www.crd.york.ac.uk/prospero/, ID: CRD42016041377). We followed guidance from York’s Centre for Research and Dissemination and the Cochrane Handbook for Systematic Reviews [32, 33]. A search strategy was developed with an information specialist, undertaken by one reviewer (VH), supported by a second, independent reviewer (SW). The following databases were searched: Applied Social Sciences Index and Abstracts (ASSIA), American Psychological Association (PsychInfo), National Center for Biotechnology Information (PubMED), Elsevier’s Scopus, and Web of Science (WOS). Common keywords relating to greenspace and mental wellbeing were derived from the literature, refined following a trial search in each database; this created a final set of terms for greenspace (greenspace(s), green space(s), open space(s), green, greener, nature, natural, landscape) and mental wellbeing (wellbeing, well-being, wellbeing, happiness, happy, happier, life satisfaction, satisfaction with life). We restricted searches to studies in English, relating to humans, published after 01/01/1980. Searches were run from 07/07/2016 to 31/01/2018. The full electronic searches are shown in Table 1.

Table 1. Database search strategy.

Database Search
ASSIA ti(green?space OR "open space" OR green* OR natur* OR landscape) AND ti(wellbeing OR well?being OR "mental health" OR happy OR happi* OR life NEAR/5 satisfaction)
PubMed (((((((greenspace[Title] OR "green space"[Title] OR "open space"[Title] OR green*[Title] OR nature[Title] OR natural[Title] OR landscape[Title])) AND (well-being[Title] OR wellbeing[Title] OR "well being"[Title] OR "mental health"[Title] OR happy[Title] OR happier[Title] OR happiness[Title] OR "life satisfaction"[Title])) AND ("1980/01/01"[PDat]: "2018/01/31"[PDat]) AND Humans[Mesh] AND English[lang])))
PsychInfo ti(green?space OR "open space" OR green* OR natur* OR landscape) AND ti(wellbeing OR well?being OR "mental health" OR happy OR happi* OR life NEAR/5 satisfaction) AND la.exact("English")
Scopus ((TITLE (greenspace OR (open space) OR (green space) OR green OR greener OR nature OR natural OR landscape) AND TITLE (well?being OR wellbeing OR (mental health) OR happy OR happier OR happiness OR (life W/5 satisfaction)))) AND PUBYEAR > 1979) AND ORIG-LOAD-DATE AFT 1529266261 AND ORIG-LOAD-DATE BEF 1529871076 AND PUBYEAR AFT 2016 AND (LIMIT-TO (LANGUAGE, "English"))
WOS TITLE: (("green space*" OR greenspace* OR "open space*" OR greener OR green OR nature OR natural OR landscape)) <i>AND</i> TITLE: ((well?being OR wellbeing OR "mental health" OR happy OR happiness OR happier OR life NEAR/5 satisfaction)) Refined by: *LANGUAGES:* (ENGLISH)

Using the in-built database functions, an auto-search was timed to re-run each query on a weekly basis to detect any further publications within the review duration. All articles recovered from initial searches were recorded in Endnote, and duplicates removed. Titles and Abstracts were screened for potential relevance by two reviewers independently, and full texts of shortlisted studies retrieved for formal inclusion/exclusion. It was agreed that any disputed studies would be cautiously retained for full text evaluation.

Study eligibility criteria

Criteria for inclusion were: (a) Population: adults aged over 16 (or all ages, but not wholly or mainly children); (b) Exposure: any measure of greenspace, defined as areas of grass, trees or other vegetation. Studies measuring personal connectedness to nature were included. As we were interested in all greenspace characteristics, we included both urban and rural studies; (c) Control: Comparators must include a control group which differed in the type/degree of exposure to greenspace, or direct comparison before and after an intervention; (d) Outcome: mental wellbeing, ascertained using a validated measure of hedonic and/or eudaimonic mental wellbeing, or one or more aspects of these (e.g. life satisfaction, happiness, quality of life. The General Health Questionnaire (GHQ) is designed to measure psychological distress, but includes several positive items, and is prevalent in the literature; studies using this outcome were therefore included. Instruments designed to capture only symptoms of mental distress were not included; (e) No study designs were explicitly excluded.

Evaluation of evidence

After identifying eligible papers, one reviewer (VH) evaluated study contents by extracting: authors, publication date, country, study design, age of participants, sample size, greenspace measures, methods, outcomes, confounders, and a results summary, including effect sizes (regression coefficients/risk ratio and confidence interval/standard error).

For quality appraisal, risk of bias was assessed using Cochrane-recommended criteria [32]: the Newcastle-Ottawa Scale (NOS), adapted for longitudinal and cross-sectional studies, alongside the Cochrane Risk of Bias (RoB) tool for controlled studies [34, 35]. The criteria cover potential risk of bias arising from: representativeness of the sample, participant awareness of the intervention, control factors, and selection of reported results.

We used established Quality Assessment thresholds to categorise each article [36]. For those assessed using the Cochrane RoB tool, a Good quality study met all criteria (low RoB), while those of Fair quality had moderate RoB not meeting one criterion; Poor quality studies had high RoB, not meeting multiple criteria. More complex scoring criteria were used for papers analysed using the NOS, across three domains: Selection (representativeness of sample, treatment of non-respondents), Comparability (between exposure groups) and Outcome (assessment, soundness). Good studies scored at least 3 for Selection, 1 for Comparability and 2 for Outcome; Fair studies scored at least 2, 1 and 2, respectively. Poor papers scored 1 or less for each category. A final quality rating was given according to the lowest rating for any category.

Stratification by characterisation of greenspace

We identified six types of study, according to the characterisation of greenspace: (a) amount of local-area greenspace, most commonly the proportion of local areas covered by greenspace; (b) greenspace type; (c) views of greenspace; (d) visits to greenspace; (e) accessibility, in terms of proximity to greenspaces and self-reported ‘access’; and (f) subjective connection to nature.

We conducted a narrative review of evidence, as methodological heterogeneity precluded meta-analysis. Evidence for associations between each type of greenspace characteristic and mental wellbeing was classified according to the consistency, strength and methodological quality of the findings, and study design. Evidence of association was categorised using established guidelines used by other studies in the field [37]: Adequate (most studies, at least one Good quality, reported an association between greenspace and mental wellbeing); Limited (more than one study, at least one Good, reported an association, but with inconsistent findings); Inadequate (associations reported in one or more studies, but none Good quality); and No association (several Good quality studies reported an absence of a statistically significant association between greenspace and mental wellbeing).

Results

Titles and abstracts of 485 records were screened, and 75 chosen for full-text evaluation; 42 were found to be eligible. During this process, 10 additional papers were found via Auto-Searching the databases and recommendations. Therefore, 52 papers were finally included in this review (Fig 1).

Fig 1. Study selection process.

Fig 1

Among these, 4 were controlled case studies and a further 6 were longitudinal cohort studies; there was one ecological analysis, 4 uncontrolled case studies, the remaining 37 were cross-sectional surveys. Two studies were international, 31 were restricted to Europe, 15 just in the UK; 5 were based in the USA with another 6 in Canada, 10 in Australia. Analyses were confined to urban areas in 22 cases, 9 included only rural greenspace. Sample size ranged from 25 to 30,900 participants, but was not specified in 3 cases. Age ranges were fairly consistent, covering young adults to past retirement age, although 1 focused on ‘youths’ (aged 16–25), 3 studies recruited university students and two included mainly people aged over 55; however, 11 studies did not specify participants’ age. After quality assessment, the majority of studies (n = 27) were determined to be Good, 13 were Fair, and 12 Poor. For Poor studies, Table 2 provides further justification. For full details of the risk of bias for each study, heat maps are presented in S1 and S2 Tables. Table 3 provides further detail on the typologies of greenspace measures implemented for each study.

Table 2. Main characteristics and results of included studies.

Authors, Year, Country Study Design Age of Participants Sample Size Greenspace Measure Mental Wellbeing Tool Mental Wellbeing Confounders Methods Statistically Significant Associations** Effect Size**
(C: Correlation Coefficient, SE: Standard Error, CI: Confidence Interval)
Interaction Effects Quality
a) Amount of Local- Area Greenspace
Alcock et al., 2015, England [38] Longitudinal Cohort Study under 25- over 75 2,020
214 movers
% area of each LSOA*,
10 land-cover types
Rural areas only
GHQ-12 Psychological Distress Individual: Demographic, Marital, SES, Living Conditions, Health Commuting. Local: IMD Multilevel Linear Regression Cross-sectional differences: no association.
Longitudinal differences for movers: significant, positive associations with increase access individually to Arable, Improved Grassland, Semi-natural Grassland, Mountain, Heath and Bog, and Coastal land cover.
C, SE: Within-individual:
Arable: 0.083, 0.037
Improved Grassland: 1.351, 0.040
Semi-natural Grassland: 0.152, 0.062
Mountains/Heath: 1.667, 0.074
N/A Good
Alcock et al., 2014, England [23] Longitudinal Cohort Study 16–55+ 1,064 residents of BHPS who relocated during survey % greenspace in each LSOA, including private gardens,
Urban areas only
GHQ-12 Psychological Distress Individual: Demographic, Marital, SES, Living Conditions, Health, Pre-move GHQ, Commuting. Local: IMD Linear Regression Movers to greener areas: significantly lower GHQ scores post-move.
Movers to less green areas: GHQ decreased in year preceding the move but no significant difference post-move.
C, SE: Movers to greener areas
T+1: 0.369, 0.152
T+2: 0.378, 0.158
T+3: 0.431, 0.162
N/A Good
Ambrey and Fleming, 2014, Australia [39] Cross-Sectional Survey 15–60+ NOT GIVEN % public greenspace in each CD*
Urban areas only
Life Satisfaction Life Satisfaction Individual: Demographic, Language, Marital, SES, Living Conditions, Health, Commuting, Hours Worked Linear Regression More greenspace: higher life satisfaction C, SE: 0.003, 0.002 N/A Good
Ambrey, 2016, Australia [40] Cross-Sectional Survey NOT GIVEN 3,288 Greenspace per capita, in each CD
Urban areas only
SF-36 Mental Component Survey Mental Health Individual: Physical Activity Linear Regression More greenspace: better mental health, only for those engaged in physical activity C, SE: Greenspace Physical Activity Interaction: 4.392, 1.702 Positive interaction between greenspace and physical activity Good
Ambrey, 2016, Australia [41] Cross-Sectional Survey NOT GIVEN 6,082 Greenspace per capita, in each CD
Urban areas only
Life Satisfaction, SF-36 Life Satisfaction, Quality of Life Individual: Physical Activity Logistic Regression More greenspace: better life satisfaction and quality of life Odds, CI: Life Satisfaction: 0.942, 0.920–0.990.
Quality of Life: 0.974, 0.912–1.039
N/A Good
Ambrey, 2016, Australia [42] Cross-Sectional Survey NOT GIVEN 6,077 Amount of greenspace in each CD
Urban areas only
SF-36 Quality of Life Individual: Demographic, Ethnicity, Marital, SES, Free Time, Social Interaction, Household Members Engaged in Physical Activity, Personality. Local: Proximity to Lake, River, Coastline, SES Linear Regression More greenspace: better quality of life, only for those engaged in physical activity C, SE: 0.553, 0.229 Positive interaction between greenspace and physical activity Good
Astell-Burt et al., 2014, UK [17] Longitudinal Cohort Study 15–75+ 65,407 person-years % greenspace in each ward, excluding water and private gardens
Urban areas only
GHQ-12 Psychological Distress Individual: Demographic, Marital, SES, Living Conditions, Smoking Linear Regression More greenspace: lower GHQ scores among men. Variation in associations across life course and gender. C, SE: ‘High’ Greenspace: 0.300, 0.370 Interactions for life course and gender Good
Bos et al., 2016, The Netherlands [43] Cross-Sectional Survey 18–87 4,924 % greenspace within 1km and 3km buffers Manchester Short Assessment of Quality of Life Quality of Life Individual: Demographic, Country of Origin, Marital, SES Linear Regression More greenspace within 3km: better quality of life, significant interactions for age and gender.
For middle aged men, inverse association
Greenspace within 1km: no association
C, SE: 1km: 5.200, 5.500.
3km: 6.300, 4.500
Interactions for life course and gender Poor
Limited Statistical reporting
De Vries et al., 2003, The Netherlands [5] Cross-Sectional Survey All ages (including children) 10,179 % greenspace in local area, % bluespace in local area, presence of a garden GHQ-12 Psychological Distress Individual: Demographic, SES, Living Conditions, Health Insurances, Life Events in Last Year Multilevel Linear Regression More greenspace: lower GHQ scores
Access to agricultural space: lower GH
Only for lower educated groups
Results only significant for whole sample, not for individual urban categories
Having a garden: significant only in very urban municipalities
C, SE:
%green within 3km: -0.100, 0.003
Interaction with level of urbanity Good
De Vries et al., 2013,
The Netherlands [44]
Cross- Sectional Survey NOT GIVEN 1,641 Quantity and quality of streetscape greenery,
Urban areas only
SF-36 Quality of Life Individual: Demographic, SES, Living Conditions, Health, Life Events in Last Year, Multilevel Linear Regression Higher amounts of greenspace: higher QOL, but not statistically significant after quality is added to the model.
High quality of greenspace: higher quality of life.
C, SE:
Quantity: 0.007, 0.036 (not statistically significant)
Quality: 0.0153, 0.069
Both Quantity and Quality show positive interactions with stress, social cohesion, and green activity Good
Dzhambov et al., 2018, Bulgaria [45] Cross- Sectional Survey 15–25 399 Amount of green land within 500m of home, perceived neighbourhood greenness and quality
Urban areas only
GHQ-12 Psychological Distress Individual: Demographic, SES, Living Conditions, Noise. Local: Population Density Linear Mixed Models and Linear Mediation Models Perceived greenness and quality: lower GHQ scores.
No statistically significant associations for objective greenspace measures.
C, CI:
Perceived greenness: -0.59, -0.85- -0.32
Greenspace quality: -0.08, -0.12 - -0.04
Higher perceived restorative quality was associated with more physical activity and social cohesion, which was associated with lower GHQ scores. For objective measures, this held for all but the greenspace quality measure. Fair
Houlden et al., 2017, England [46] Cross-Sectional Survey 16–65+ 30,900 % greenspace in each LSOA, excluding gardens SWEMWBS Mental Wellbeing Individual: Demographic, Marital, SES, Living Conditions, Health, Commuting. Local: IMD Linear Regression Greater amounts of greenspace: higher SWEMWBS scores. Reduced to null after adjustment No statistically significant associations to report N/A Good
Maas et al., 2009, The Netherlands [47] Cross-sectional Survey 12–65+ 10,089 %greenspace within 1 and 3km buffers GHQ-12 Psychological Distress Individual: Demographic, Ethnicity, SES, Living Conditions, Health Insurance, Life Events in Last Year. Local: Level of Urbanity Multilevel Linear Regression More surrounding greenspace: lower GHQ score. Stronger association for 1km than 3km C, SE:
1km: -0.005, 0.002
3km: -0.004, 0.002
N/A Good
Taylor et al., 2018, Australia and New Zealand [48] Cross-Sectional Survey 18–75+ 1,819 Amount of greenspace in postcode
Urban areas only
WHO-5 Hedonic Wellbeing NO Linear Regression Higher amounts of greenspace: higher WHO-5 scores. Only for 2 sample cities, remaining 2 insignificant C:
Melbourne: 1.410
Sydney: 2.470
N/A Poor
No controls
Triguero-Mas et al., 2015, Spain [49] Cross-Sectional Survey NOT GIVEN 8,793 Amount of greenspace within 300m buffer
Sensitivity analysis with other buffers
GHQ-12 Psychological Distress Individual: Demographic, Birth Place, Marital, SES, Health Insurance. Local: SES Logistic Regression Higher amounts of greenspace: lower odds of higher GHQ score
Consistent results for all buffers
Odds, CI:
Males: 0.820, 0.700–0.980
Females: 0.770, 0.670–0.880
Stronger association for males than females Fair
Triguero-Mas et al., 2017, Europe [50] Cross-Sectional Survey 18–75 403 Amount of greenspace within 300m buffer,
Urban areas only
SF-36 Mental Component Survey Mental Health Individual: Demographic Linear Regression No association for surrounding greenspace. No Statistical Results to report Stronger association for males than females Fair
Vemuri and Costanza, 2006, International [51] Ecological Analysis NOT GIVEN 172 Countries Ecosystem services product (ESP), per square kilometre for each country, normalised. From amount of each land-cover and multiplied by ecosystem services per country. Life Satisfaction Life Satisfaction NO Linear Regression Better natural capital: higher life satisfaction Odds, SE: 2.453, 0.739 N/A Poor
No controls, high-level analysis
Ward Thompson et al., 2014, Scotland [52] Cross-Sectional Survey NOT GIVEN 305 Amount of greenspace “around each home”, perceptions of local greenspace,
Urban areas only
SWEMWBS Mental Wellbeing Individual: Demographic, Income, Deprivation Linear Regression Perceptions of having sufficient local greenspace: better mental wellbeing
Satisfaction with quality: higher mental wellbeing
No Statistical Results to Report N/A Fair
White et al., 2013, England [24] Cross-Sectional Survey Under 25-over75 12,818 (GHQ)
10,168 (Life Satisfaction)
% greenspace in each LSOA, including private gardens,
Urban areas only
Life Satisfaction, GHQ Life Satisfaction, Psychological Distress Individual: Demographic, Marital, SES, Living Conditions, Health, Commuting. Local: IMD Linear Regression Higher percentage of greenspace: decreased GHQ, increased Life Satisfaction C, SE:
GHQ: -0.004, 0.001
Life Satisfaction: 0.002, 0.001
N/A Good
White et al., 2013, England [25] Cross-Sectional Survey Under 25-over75 15,361 % greenspace in each LSOA, including private gardens Life Satisfaction, GHQ Life Satisfaction, Psychological Distress Individual: Demographic, Marital, SES, Living Conditions, Health, Commuting. Local: IMD Linear Regression Higher percentage of greenspace: decreased GHQ C, SE:
GHQ (reversed):
Greenspace: 0.003, 0.001
N/A Good
Wood et al., 2017, Australia [53] Cross-Sectional Survey NOT GIVEN 492 Amount and number of public greenspaces within 1.6km buffer, type of greenspace: sports, recreational, natural
Urban areas only
SWEMWBS Mental Wellbeing Individual: Demographic, SES Linear Regression Number of parks: higher mental wellbeing. Strongest association for largest parks, decreasing with size.
Greater area of parks: higher mental wellbeing scores
Strongest association for sports spaces
C, SE:
Number of parks: 0.110, 0.050
Hectare increase of park area: 0.070, 0.020
Number of sports spaces: 0.430, 0.210
Number of recreational spaces: 0.110, 0.050
Number of natural spaces: 0.110, 0.050
N/A Fair
b) Greenspace Types
Alcock et al., 2015, England [38] Longitudinal Cohort Study under 25- over 75 2,020
214 movers
10 land-cover types
Rural areas only
GHQ-12 Psychological Distress Individual: Demographic, Marital, SES, Living Conditions, Health Commuting. Local: IMD Multilevel Linear Regression Cross-sectional differences: no association.
Longitudinal differences for movers: significant, positive associations with increase access individually to Arable, Improved Grassland, Semi-natural Grassland, Mountain, Heath and Bog, and Coastal land cover.
C, SE: Within-individual:
Arable: 0.083, 0.037
Improved Grassland: 1.351, 0.040
Semi-natural Grassland: 0.152, 0.062
Mountains/Heath: 1.667, 0.074
N/A Good
Annerstedt et al., 2012, Sweden [54] Longitudinal Cohort Study 18–80 7,549 residents who did not relocate during survey Presence of 5 green qualities within 300m buffer: Serene, Wild, Lush, Spacious, Culture
Rural areas only
GHQ-12 Psychological Distress Individual: Demographic, Country of Origin, Marital, Financial Strain, Physical Activity Logistic Regression Presence of Serene: lower GHQ score, only for those engaged in physical activity
Presence of Spacious: lower GHQ, only for women engaged in physical activity
Odds, CI: Women with Access to Serene: 0.200, 0.060–0.900 Positive interaction between being physical activity and serene greenspace
Positive interaction between being physical activity and serene greenspace, for women
Good
Bjork et al., 2008, Sweden [18] Cross-Sectional Survey 19–76 24,819 Number of 5 green qualities within 100 and 300m buffers: Serene, Wild, Lush, Spacious, Culture
Rural areas only
SF-36 Vitality Component Survey Vitality Individual: Demographic, SES, Financial Strain, Smoking Logistic Regression More green qualities within 300m: better vitality, only for women
More green qualities within 100m: no association
Individual qualities: no association
Odds and CI, women with access to number of qualities:
4–5: 1.070, 0.880–1.290
3: 1.220, 1.060–1.410
2: 1.060,0.940–1.190
Interactions with gender Good
Luck et al., 2011, Australia [55] Cross-sectional Survey All ages 1,043 Residential neighbourhood greenspace aspects:, vegetation cover, vegetation density,
Urban areas only
Subjective Wellbeing Subjective Wellbeing Individual: Demographic, SES, Living Conditions, General Activity Multilevel Linear Regression Higher levels of species richness, species abundance, vegetation cover, vegetation density: better subjective wellbeing, strongest for vegetation C, SE:
Vegetation Cover: 0.560, 0.260
Vegetation Density: 0.800, 0.390
N/A Good
MacKerron and Mourato, 2013, UK [56] Cross-Sectional Survey All ages 21,947 Land cover types Happiness Happiness NO Linear Regression All outdoor land cover types: better happiness than continuous urban areas. Marine and coastal areas have happiest scores. C, SE:
Mountains/moors: 2.710, 0.870
Woodland: 2.120, 0.340
Semi-natural grassland: 2.040, 0.350
Suburban/rural: 0.880, 0.160
N/A Fair
Sugiyama et al., 2008, Australia [57] Cross-Sectional Survey 20–65 1,895 Neighbourhood Environment Walkability Scale,
Urban areas only
SF-36 Mental Component Survey Mental Health Individual: Demographic, Marital, SES, Walking, Social Interaction Logistic Regression Higher reported greenness: better mental health Odds, CI:
High Perceived Greenness: 1.270, 0.990–1.620
N/A Good
Van den Bosch et al., 2015, Sweden [58] Longitudinal Cohort Study 18–80 1,419 residents who relocated during survey Amount and presence of greenspace within 300m buffer: Serene, Wild, Lush, Spacious, Culture,
Rural areas only
GHQ-12 Psychological Distress Individual: Deprivation, Marital, Education Logistic Regression Gained access to Serene greenspace: improved mental health among women. No other associations Odds, CI:
Access to Serene: 2.800, 1.110–7.040
Associations only for females, not males Good
Vemuri et al., 2011, USA [59] Cross-sectional Survey 18–65+ 1,361 Neighbourhood satisfaction, quality of neighbourhood natural environment, amount of tree cover per census block,
Urban areas only
Life Satisfaction Life Satisfaction Individual: Demographic, Ethnicity, Marital, Living Conditions, Social Capital Logistic Regression Stronger perceived environmental quality: improved life satisfaction
Perceived shows stronger association than objective measures
C, SE: 0.276, 0.514 N/A Good
Weimann et al., 2015, Sweden [60] Longitudinal Cohort Study 18–80 9,444 Number of 5 green qualities within local 1km2 area: Serene, Wild, Lush, Spacious, Culture GHQ-12 Psychological Distress Individual: Demographic, Marital, SES, Living Conditions BMI, Smoking Multilevel Logistic Regression Within-individual difference of higher neighbourhood greenness: lower psychological distress Odds, CI:
Within-Individual: 1.030, 1.000–1.160
Between-Individuals:1.070, 1.000–1.140
N/A Good
Wood et al., 2017, Australia [53] Cross-Sectional Survey NOT GIVEN 492 Amount and number of public greenspaces within 1.6km buffer, type of greenspace: sports, recreational, natural
Urban areas only
SWEMWBS Mental Wellbeing Individual: Demographic, SES Linear Regression Number of parks: higher mental wellbeing. Strongest association for largest parks, decreasing with size.
Greater area of parks: higher mental wellbeing scores
Strongest association for sports spaces
C, SE:
Number of parks: 0.110, 0.050
Hectare increase of park area: 0.070, 0.020
Number of sports spaces: 0.430, 0.210
Number of recreational spaces: 0.110, 0.050
Number of natural spaces: 0.110, 0.050
N/A Fair
c) Views of Greenspace
Gilchrist et al., 2015, Scotland [61] Cross-Sectional Survey 16–55+ 366 Workplace view naturalness, view satisfaction, extent of features in view
Urban areas only
SWEMWBS Mental Wellbeing Individual: Demographic, Job Type, Greenspace Use in Leisure Time. Local: Location Linear Regression No association for view naturalness
Satisfaction with view, views of trees/bushes/flowering plants: higher SWEMWBS score
Types strongest predictors
C, SE:
View of Trees: 0.616, 0.198
View bushes/flowers: 0.610, 0.312
View Satisfaction: 0.802, 0.215
N/A Good
Pretty et al., 2005, UK [20] Controlled Case Study 18–60 100 Running while exposed to photographs: urban/rural pleasant and unpleasant Rosenberg Self-Esteem Questionnaire, Profile of Mood States Self-Esteem, Mood NO N/A Viewing pleasant scenes: increase in self-esteem No Statistical Results to Report N/A Fair
Vemuri et al., 2011, USA [59] Cross-sectional Survey 18–65+ 1,361 Number of trees visible from residence
Urban areas only
Life Satisfaction Life Satisfaction Individual: Demographic, Ethnicity, Marital, Living Conditions, Social Capital Logistic Regression Perceived shows stronger association than objective measures No Statistical Results to Report N/A Good
d) Visits to Greenspace
Duvall and Kaplan, 2014, USA [62] Uncontrolled Case Study 20–50+ 73 Wilderness Expedition,
Rural areas only
AFI, PANAS Attention, Affect Individual: Demographic, SES, Physical and Mental Health History, Veteran History Linear Mixed Models Post expedition: more positive affect and better attentional functioning
Follow-up: better positive affect
Score Change:
AFI: 0.340
Affect: 0.270
N/A Poor
Small sample, allocation based on intervention
Dzhambov et al., 2018, Bulgaria [45] Cross- Sectional Survey 15–25 399 Amount of green land within 500m of home, Euclidean distance to nearest greenspace, perceived neighbourhood greenness and quality, travel time to and time spent in neighbourhood greenspace
Urban areas only
GHQ-12 Psychological Distress Individual: Demographic, SES, Living Conditions, Noise. Local: Population Density Linear Mixed Models and Linear Mediation Models Perceived greenness and quality, and travel time to greenspace: lower GHQ scores.
No statistically significant associations for objective greenspace measures.
C, CI:
Perceived greenness: -0.59, -0.85- -0.32
<5min to greenspace: -2.54, -3.96 - -1.12
Greenspace quality: -0.08, -0.12 - -0.04
Higher perceived restorative quality was associated with more physical activity and social cohesion, which was associated with lower GHQ scores. For objective measures, this held for all but the greenspace quality measure. Fair
Gilchrist et al., 2015, Scotland [61] Cross-Sectional Survey 16–55+ 366 Workplace greenspace visit frequency, weekly use duration
Urban areas only
SWEMWBS Mental Wellbeing Individual: Demographic, Job Type, Greenspace Use in Leisure Time. Local: Location Linear Regression No association for use frequency
Time spent in workplace greenspace, satisfaction with view, views of trees/bushes/flowering plants: higher SWEMWBS score
Types strongest predictors
C, SE:
Use Duration: 0.431, 0.191
N/A Good
Herzog and Stevey, 2008, USA [63] Cross-Sectional Survey University Students 823 Self-reported typical contact with nature Ryff’s Scales of Psychological Well-Being, Attention, PANAS Mental Wellbeing, Attention, Affect Individual: Sense of humour Linear Regression Greater contact with nature: better personal development, effective functioning. C:
Personal Development: 0.090
Effective Functioning: 0.230
N/A Fair
Jakubec et al., 2016, Canada
[64]
Uncontrolled Case Study Adults 37 Visits to greenspace,
Rural areas only
Quality of Life Inventory Quality of Life NO Score Change Post-Intervention: improved quality of life, not statistically significant Score Change:
Satisfaction with love: +1.000
Satisfaction with life: -1.000
N/A Poor
No controls, participants aware of intervention
Kamitsis and Francis, 2013, Australia [65] Cross-Sectional Survey 18–69 190 Nature Exposure, CNS WHOQOL-BREF Quality of Life Individual: Spirituality Linear Regression Higher nature exposure or connection to nature: better quality of life C:
Exposure: 0.280
CNS: 0.330
N/A Poor
Minimal controls
Marselle et al., 2013, UK [66] Controlled Case Study Adults, mostly over 55 708 Group walks in different environments: natural and semi-natural, green corridors, farmland, parks and gardens, urban, coastal, amenity greenspace, allotments, outdoor sports facilities, other WEMWBS, PANAS Mental Wellbeing, Affect Individual: Demographic, Marital, Education, Deprivation Multilevel Linear Regression Walks in farmland: better mental wellbeing
No associations with other greenspace types
C, SE:
Walks in farmland: 2.790, 0.003
N/A Fair
Marselle et al., 2015, UK [67] Cross-Sectional Survey Adults, mostly over 55 127 Walking: environment type, perceived naturalness, perceived biodiversity, perceived restorativeness, duration of walk, perceived walk intensity Happiness, PANAS Happiness, Affect NO Multilevel Linear Regression Perceived restorativeness, perceived walk intensity: positively associated with affect and happiness. C, SE:
Affect: 0.126, 0.014
Happiness: 0.029, 0.003
N/A Poor
No controls, participants aware of intervention
Mitchell, 2013, Scotland [19] Cross-sectional Survey 16+ 1,890 Frequency of use of different environment types for physical activity WEMWBS, GHQ Mental Wellbeing, Psychological Distress Individual: Demographic, Income, Physical Activity. Local: Level of Urbanity Linear Regression Regular use of open space/park or woods/forest: lower GHQ score
Regular use of natural environments: no clear association with mental wellbeing
Regular use of non-natural environments: better mental wellbeing
Odds, CI:
GHQ:
Park >1 a week: 0.570, 0.369–0.881
Woods >1 a week: 0.557, 0.323–0.962
WEMWBS:
Park <1 a week: 2.442, 0.769–4.115
N/A Good
Molsher and Townsend, 2016, Australia [68] Uncontrolled Case Study 14–71 32 Engagement with 10 week Environmental Volunteering Project,
Rural areas only
General Wellbeing Scale, PANAS Wellbeing, Affect NO Score Change Post-intervention and Follow-up: improved wellbeing and mood state scores Score Change: Wellbeing: +11.600 N/A Poor
No controls, participants aware of intervention
Nisbet and Zekenski, 2011, Canada [69] Controlled Case Study 16–48 150 Walking indoors or outdoors in nature, Nature Relatedness
Urban areas only
Happiness, PANAS Happiness, Affect NO T-Tests Walking outdoors: more positive affect, relaxation and fascination T-Test:
Outdoor Walk:
Affect: 4.860
Relaxation: 4.570
Fascination: 4.800
N/A Fair
Panno et al., 2017, Italy [70] Cross-Sectional Survey NOT GIVEN 115 Self-reported greenspace visit frequency WHO-5 Hedonic Wellbeing Individual: Demographics, SES Hierarchical Regression Higher reported frequency of greenspace visits: greater wellbeing scores. Not statistically significant. No Statistically Significant Results to Report N/A Fair
Richardson et al., 2016, UK [71] Uncontrolled Case Study 18–71 613 Nature in Self, Engagement with “30 Days Wild” Programme Happiness Happiness NO T-Tests Post-intervention, increased nature connection, increased general happiness T-Tests: 6.650 N/A Fair
Triguero-Mas et al., 2017, Europe [50] Cross-Sectional Survey 18–75 403 Frequency of contact with greenspace in terciles
Urban areas only
SF-36 Mental Component Survey Mental Health Individual: Demographic Linear Regression Lower frequency of greenspace visits: poorer mental health. Stronger associations for males C, CI for “low” contact
Males: -9.140, -14.420 - -3.860
Females: -5.000, -9.790- -0.021
Stronger association for males than females Fair
Van den Berg et al., 2016, Spain, The Netherlands, Lithuania, UK [21] Cross-Sectional Survey 18–75 3,748 Reported hours of greenspace visits in last month,
Urban areas only
SF-36 Mental Component Survey Mental Health Individual: Demographic, SES, Living Conditions, Childhood Nature Experience Multilevel Linear Regression Higher visits to greenspace: better mental health C, CI:
0.030, 0.020–0.040
N/A Good
Ward Thompson et al., 2014, Scotland [52] Cross-Sectional Survey NOT GIVEN 305 Patterns of greenspace use
Urban areas only
SWEMWBS Mental Wellbeing Individual: Demographic, Income, Deprivation Linear Regression No association between greenspace use and mental wellbeing No Statistical Results to Report N/A Fair
White et al., 2017, England [72] Cross-Sectional Survey NOT GIVEN 7,272 Did the individual visit greenspace yesterday. Amount of time spent outdoors
Urban areas only
ONS4 Mental Wellbeing Individual: Demographic, Marital, SES, Living Conditions, Health, Commuting. Local: IMD Logistic Regression Visiting a greenspace yesterday: higher happiness
Spending time outdoors: more frequently associated with higher worth, decreasing with frequency
C, CI:
Visited greenspace yesterday, happiness: 1.660, 1.320–2.080
Spending time outdoors everyday day, compared to never, worth: 1.960, 1.490–2.580
N/A Good
e) Greenspace Accessibility
Bjork et al., 2008, Sweden [18] Cross-Sectional Survey 19–76 24,819 Number of 5 green qualities within 100 and 300m buffers: Serene, Wild, Lush, Spacious, Culture
Rural areas only
SF-36 Vitality Component Survey Vitality Individual: Demographic, SES, Financial Strain, Smoking Logistic Regression More green qualities within 300m: better vitality, only for women
More green qualities within 100m: no association
Individual qualities: no association
Odds and CI, women with access to number of qualities within 300m:
4–5: 1.070, 0.880–1.290
3: 1.220, 1.060–1.410
2: 1.060,0.940–1.190
Interactions with gender Good
Bos et al., 2016, The Netherlands [43] Cross-Sectional Survey 18–87 4,924 % greenspace within 1km and 3km buffers Manchester Short Assessment of Quality of Life Quality of Life Individual: Demographic, Country of Origin, Marital, SES Linear Regression More greenspace within 3km: better quality of life, significant interactions for age and gender.
For middle aged men, inverse association
Greenspace within 1km: no association
C, SE: 1km: 5.200, 5.500.
3km: 6.300, 4.500
Interactions for life course and gender Poor
Limited Statistical reporting
Dadvand et al., 2016, Spain [73] Cross-Sectional Survey 18–65+ 3461 % greenspace within 100m, 250m and 500m buffers, subjective presence of greenspace within 10 minute walk, objective presence of greenspace within 200m of minimum 5000m2
Urban areas only
GHQ-12 Psychological Distress Individual: Demographic, SES, Social Support, Physical Activity
Local: Deprivation
Logistic Regression More greenspace nearer to home: lower GHQ score. Effect sizes decreasing with distance.
Greater subjective and objective proximity to greenspace: lower GHQ scores
Odds, CI:
100m: 1.320, 1.160–1.510
250m: 1.250, 1.100–1.400
500m: 1.170, 1.040–1.320
Subjective proximity: 1.300, 1.040–1.630
Objective proximity: 1.200, 0.970–1.480
N/A Good
Dzhambov et al., 2018, Bulgaria
[45]
Cross- Sectional Survey 15–25 399 Amount of green land within 500m of home, Euclidean distance to nearest greenspace, perceived neighbourhood greenness and quality, travel time to greenspace
Urban areas only
GHQ-12 Psychological Distress Individual: Demographic, SES, Living Conditions, Noise. Local: Population Density Linear Mixed Models and Linear Mediation Models Travel time to greenspace: lower GHQ scores.
No statistically significant associations for objective greenspace measures.
C, CI:
<5min to greenspace: -2.54, -3.96 - -1.12
Lower travel time to greenspace was associated with more physical activity and social cohesion, which was associated with lower GHQ scores.. Fair
Krekel et al., 2015, Germany [74] Cross-sectional Survey 17–99 NOT GIVEN Euclidean distance from home to green and abandoned areas
Urban areas only
SF-36 Mental Component Survey Mental Health Individual: Demographic, Country of Origin, Marital, SES, Living Conditions, Disabilities Linear Regression Access to urban greenspaces: better mental health
Access to abandoned areas: poorer mental health
C:
Greenspace: 0.007
N/A Good
Maas et al., 2009, The Netherlands [47] Cross-sectional Survey 12–65+ 10,089 %greenspace within 1 and 3km buffers GHQ-12 Psychological Distress Individual: Demographic, Ethnicity, SES, Living Conditions, Health Insurance, Life Events in Last Year. Local: Level of Urbanity Multilevel Linear Regression More surrounding greenspace: lower GHQ score. Stronger association for 1km than 3km C, SE:
1km: -0.005, 0.002
3km: -0.004, 0.002
N/A Good
Sugiyama et al., 2008, Australia [57] Cross-Sectional Survey 20–65 1,895 Neighbourhood Environment Walkability Scale,
Urban areas only
SF-36 Mental Component Survey Mental Health Individual: Demographic, Marital, SES, Walking, Social Interaction Logistic Regression Higher reported greenness: better mental health Odds, CI:
High Perceived Greenness: 1.270, 0.990–1.620
N/A Good
Triguero-Mas et al., 2015, Spain [49] Cross-Sectional Survey NOT GIVEN 8,793 Amount of greenspace within 100m, 300m, 500m and 1km buffers, presence of green and blue spaces within buffer
Sensitivity analysis with other buffers
GHQ-12 Psychological Distress Individual: Demographic, Birth Place, Marital, SES, Health Insurance. Local: SES Logistic Regression Higher amounts of greenspace: lower odds of higher GHQ score
Consistent results for all buffers
Odds, CI:
Males: 0.820, 0.700–0.980
Females: 0.770, 0.670–0.880
Stronger association for males than females Fair
f) Subjective Connection to Nature
Cervinka et al., 2012, Austria [75] Cross-Sectional Survey 15–87 547 CN-SI* SF-36 Component Surveys, SWLS, WHOQOL-BREF Quality of Life, Life Satisfaction Individual: Demographic Linear Regression Higher CN-SI Score: better meaningfulness, mental health, vitality and emotional-role function C:
Meaningfulness: 0.210
Mental Health: 0.180
Vitality: 0.230
Emotions: 0.190
N/A Poor
Limited sampling description
Howell et al., 2011, Canada [76] Cross-Sectional Survey University Students 452 CNS* Keyes’ Index of Well-Being and Mindful Attention Awareness Scale Mental Wellbeing, Attention NO Linear Regression Greater connection to nature: greater psychological wellbeing and social wellbeing. Not associated with emotional wellbeing or mindfulness C:
Psychological Wellbeing: 0.150
Social Wellbeing: 0.200
N/A Poor
No controls, minimal reporting
Howell et al., 2013, Canada [77] Cross-Sectional Survey University Students 311 CNS, Nature Relatedness Scale* Emotional Wellbeing, Steen Happiness Index, Meaning in Life Questionnaire, Meaningful Life Measure, General Life Purpose Scale Mental Wellbeing, Happiness, Meaning in Life NO Linear Regression Greater connection to nature: better reported wellbeing, meaning in life C:
Meaning: 0.310
Purpose: 0.250
Happiness: 0.220
Emotional Wellbeing: 0.200
Psychological Wellbeing: 0.250
Social Wellbeing: 0.260
N/A Poor
No controls, minimal reporting
Kamitsis and Francis, 2013, Australia [65] Cross-Sectional Survey 18–69 190 Nature Exposure, CNS WHOQOL-BREF Quality of Life Individual: Spirituality Linear Regression Higher nature exposure or connection to nature: better quality of life C:
Exposure: 0.280
CNS: 0.330
N/A Poor
Minimal controls
Nisbet et al., 2011, Canada [78] Cross-Sectional Survey Adults, student subgroup 184, 145,in two studies Nature Relatedness Scale, New Ecological Consciousness Scale Ryff’s Psychological Well-Being Inventory, SWLS, PANAS Mental Wellbeing, Life Satisfaction, Affect NO Linear Regression Higher nature relatedness: better wellbeing, positive affect, purpose in life. No association for life satisfaction. C:
Study 1:
Affect: 0.330
Purpose: 0.230
Study 2:
Affect: 0.220
Purpose: 0.240
N/A Fair
Zelenski et al., 2014, Canada [79] Cross-Sectional Survey NOT GIVEN 950 Nature Relatedness Scale, Inclusion of Nature in Self Ryff’s PWBI, SWLS, Subjective Happiness Scale (SHS), PANAS Mental Wellbeing, Life Satisfaction, Happiness, Affect NO Linear Regression Stronger connection to nature: improved wellbeing, happiness, life satisfaction and affect C:
Wellbeing: 0.250
Happiness: 0.360
Life Satisfaction: 0.310
Affect: 0.380
N/A Poor
No controls
Zhang et al., 2014, USA [80] Cross-Sectional Survey 18–88 1,108 CNS, Engagement with Natural Beauty Scale SWLS Life Satisfaction Individual: Demographic, Personality Multilevel Linear Regression Higher connectedness with nature: improved life satisfaction, only for those reporting being attuned to nature’s beauty C, CI:
Connectedness: 0.1000, -0.990–0.109
Engagement: 0.155, 0.121–0.344
ConnectednessXENGAGEMENT: 0.080, 0.170–0.151
Positive interaction between connectedness to nature and being attuned to nature’s beauty Good

*LSOA, Lower-Layer Super Output Area, a census-based spatial unit. CD, Census District, a census-based spatial unit.

*CNS, Connectedness to Nature Scale, measure of individuals’ trait levels of feeling emotionally connected to the natural world. CN-SI, single-item version of CNS. Nature Relatedness Scale, affective, cognitive, and experiential aspects of individual’s connection to nature

**All associations described in this table are statistically significant, unless otherwise specified

Table 3. Greenspace measures employed in included studies.

Study Greenspace Type Measure Type Metrics Used Spatial Scale
a) Amount of Local-Area Greenspace
Alcock et al., 2015 [38] Natural Land Cover Land Cover Map Proportion of area that is greenspace LSOA
Alcock et al., 2014 [23] Greenspace and Private Gardens Generalised Land Use Database (GLUD) Proportion of area that is greenspace LSOA
Ambrey and Fleming, 2014 [39] Public Greenspace (including public parks, community gardens cemeteries, sports fields, national parks and wilderness areas) GIS Proportion of area that is greenspace Census District
Ambrey, 2016 [40] Public Greenspace (including public parks, community gardens cemeteries, sports fields, national parks and wilderness areas) GIS Amount of greenspace per Capita Census District
Ambrey, 2016 [41] Public Greenspace (including public parks, community gardens cemeteries, sports fields, national parks and wilderness areas) GIS Amount of greenspace per Capita Census District
Ambrey, 2016 [42] Public Greenspace (including public parks, community gardens cemeteries, sports fields, national parks and wilderness areas) GIS Amount of greenspace per Capita Census District
Astell-Burt et al., 2014 [17] Green and Natural Environment (excluding water and private gardens) Land Use Database Proportion of area that is greenspace Ward
Bos et al., 2016 [43] Greenspace (urban green including vegetable gardens, sports areas >0.5ha, parks >1ha; and rural green including agricultural and natural green) Dutch Land Use Database and GIS Proportion of area that is greenspace 1km and 3km buffers of postcode centroid
De Vries et al., 2003 [5] Greenspace (urban green, agricultural green, forests and nature areas) National Land Use Classification Database and GIS Proportion of area that is greenspace 3km around centre of neighbourhood unit
De Vries et al., 2013 [44] All types of visible vegetation, and quality based on variation, maintenance, orderly arrangement, absence of litter and general impression of greenspace On-street Audit Level of greenness (1- the street does not make a very green impression, to 5- the street makes a very green impression) Average street greenness of neighbourhood unit
Dzhambov et al., 2018 [45] Green land cover NDVI Proportion of area that is greenspace 500m Euclidean buffer of home
Greenspace (parks, gardens, street trees) Self-reported Perceived neighbourhood greenness and quality, travel time to and time spent in neighbourhood greenspace, green views from home Self-reported neighbourhood
Houlden et al., 2017 [46] Greenspace Generalised Land Use Database (GLUD) Proportion of area that is greenspace LSOA
Maas et al., 2009 [47] Greenspace (urban green, agricultural green, forests and nature areas) National Land Use Classification Database and GIS Proportion of area that is greenspace 1km and 3km buffer around individual’s home
Taylor et al., 2018 [48] Green land cover NDVI NDVI value Postcode
Triguero-Mas et al., 2015 [49] Green land cover NDVI Amount of greenspace 300m Euclidean buffer of postcodes
Triguero-Mas et al., 2017 [50] Green land cover NDVI Amount of greenspace 300m Euclidean buffer of postcodes
Vemuri and Costanza, 2006 [51] Land Cover Types Land Cover Map Ecosystem Services Product (amount of each land cover, multiplied by ecosystem services per country) Country
Ward Thompson et al., 2014 [52] Greenspace (parks, woodlands, scrub and other publicly accessible natural environments) GIS Amount of Greenspace Neighbourhood unit
White et al., 2013 [24] Greenspace and Private Gardens Generalised Land Use Database (GLUD) Proportion of area that is greenspace LSOA
White et al., 2013 [25] Greenspace and Private Gardens Generalised Land Use Database (GLUD) Proportion of area that is greenspace LSOA
Wood et al., 2017 [53] Greenspace (parks and other areas of green public open spaces) Land Cover Map Amount and number of parks 1.6km road network buffer
b) Greenspace Types
Alcock et al., 2015 [38] Land Cover Types (broadleaf woodland, coniferous woodland, arable, improved grassland, semi-natural grassland, mountain, heath and bog, saltwater, freshwater, coastal, built-up areas including gardens) Land Cover Map Proportion of area of each type LSOA
Annerstedt et al., 2012 [54] 5 qualities: Serene (place of peace, silence and care), Wild (place of fascination with wild nature), Lush (place rich in species), Spacious (place offering a restful feeling of entering another world), Culture (the essence of human culture) CORINE Land Cover and GIS Presence of each type 3km Euclidean buffer from home
Bjork et al., 2008 [18] 5 qualities: Serene, Wild, Lush, Spacious, Culture CORINE Land Cover and GIS Presence of each type 100 and 300m Euclidean buffers from home
Luck et al., 2011 [55] Vegetation Cover (woody and non-woody vegetation) Advanced Land Observation Satellite Proportion of vegetation Census District
Vegetation Density (understory, mid-story and over-story cover) Field Survey Proportion of vegetation Census District
MacKerron and Mourato, 2013 [56] Land Cover Classes (marine and coastal, freshwater and wetlands, mountains and moors and heathland, semi-natural grasslands, farmland, coniferous woodland, broadleaf woodland, bare ground, suburban/rural development, continuous urban) Land Cover Map Type Current GPS location
Sugiyama et al., 2008 [57] Neighbourhood Greenness Self-Reported Level of greenness Neighbourhood unit
Van den Bosch et al., 2015 [58] 5 qualities: Serene, Wild, Lush, Spacious, Culture CORINE Land Cover and GIS Amount and presence of each type 300m Euclidean buffer from home
Vemuri et al., 2011 [59] Natural environment quality and satisfaction Self-Reported Perceptions of neighbourhood Neighbourhood
Weimann et al., 2015 [60] 5 qualities: Serene, Wild, Lush, Spacious, Culture CORINE Land Cover and GIS Presence of each type 5–10 minute walk from homes
Wood et al., 2017 [53] Sports, recreational, and natural green spaces Land Cover Map Amount and presence of each type 1.6km network buffer of homes
Views of Greenspace
Gilchrist et al., 2015 [61] Workplace greenspace Self–Reported Perceptions of view of greenspace naturalness and extent Workplace
Pretty et al., 2005 [20] Rural pleasant and unpleasant scenes
Urban pleasant and unpleasant scenes
Lab environment setting Photographs Photographs of views
Vemuri et al., 2011 [59] Number of trees visible from home Self-Reported Perceptions of neighbourhood Individual
c) Visits to Greenspace
Duvall and Kaplan, 2014 [62] Wilderness Objective Exposure through expedition Individual
Dzhambov et al., 2018 [45] Parks and gardens Self-Reported Time spent in greenspace Self-reported Neighbourhood
Gilchrist et al., 2015 [61] Workplace greenspace Self–Reported Frequency and duration of greenspace exposure Workplace
Herzog and Stevey, 2008 [63] Nature Self-Reported Typical contact Individual
Jakubec et al., 2016 [64] Wilderness Objective Exposure through expedition Individual
Kamitsis and Francis, 2013 [65] Nature Self-Reported Level of exposure Individual
Marselle et al., 2013 [66] Natural and semi-natural, green corridors, farmland, parks/gardens, urban, coastal, amenity green space, allotments, outdoor sports facilities, other Land Use Database Walking while exposed to different environments Individual
Marselle et al., 2015 [67] Natural and semi-natural, green corridors, farmland, parks/gardens, urban, coastal, amenity green space, allotments, outdoor sports facilities, other Land Use Database, Duration of walk and environment type Individual
Natural and semi-natural, green corridors, farmland, parks/gardens, urban, coastal, amenity green space, allotments, outdoor sports facilities, other Self-Reported Perceived naturalness, biodiversity, restorativeness, walk intensity Individual
Mitchell, 2013 [19] Woodland/forest, open space/park, country paths, beach/river, sports field/courts, swimming pool, gym/sports centre, pavements, home/garden, other, none Self-Reported Frequency of use of different greenspace types for physical activity Individual
Molsher and Townsend, 2016 [68] Rural nature Objective Engagement with 10-week Environmental Volunteering Project Individual
Nisbet and Zekenski, 2011 [69] Outdoors (in nature) Objective Walking indoors vs outdoors Individual
Panno et al., 2017 [70] Greenspace Self-Reported Greenspace visit frequency Individual
Richardson et al., 2016 [71] Nature Self-Reported Engagement with 100 days wild programme Individual
Triguero-Mas et al., 2017 [50] Natural outdoor environment Urban Atlas, CORINE Land Cover and GIS Duration of exposure to nature Individual
Van den Berg et al., 2016 [21] Greenspace (Public and private open spaces that contain “green” and/or “blue” natural elements such as street trees, forests, city parks and natural parks/reserves) Self-Reported Duration of visits to greenspace Individual
Ward Thompson et al., 2014 [52] Greenspace (parks, woodlands, scrub and other publicly accessible natural environments) Self-Reported Frequency of greenspace visits Individual
White et al., 2017 [72] Greenspace Self-Reported Having visited a greenspace yesterday Individual
d) Greenspace Accessibility
Bjork et al., 2008 [18] 5 qualities: Serene, Wild, Lush, Spacious, Culture CORINE Land Cover and GIS Presence of each type 100 and 300m Euclidean buffer of home
Bos et al., 2016 [43] Greenspace (urban green including vegetable gardens, sports areas >0.5ha, parks >1ha; and rural green including agricultural and natural green) Dutch Land Use Database and GIS Proportion of area that is greenspace 1km and 3km Euclidean buffers of postcode centroid
Dadvand et al., 2016 [73] Green land cover NDVI Proportion of area that is greenspace
Presence of 5000m2 greenspace within 200m
100m, 250m and 500m Euclidean buffer of home
Greenspace Self-Reported Proximity to greenspace 10 minute walk from home
Dzhambov et al., 2018 [45] Greenspace (park, allotment, or recreational grounds) OpenStreetMap and GIS Proximity to greenspace Euclidean distance from home
Krekel et al., 2015 [74] Urban green areas (greens, forests, and waters), and abandoned urban areas European Urban Atlas Proximity to greenspace Euclidean distance from home
Maas et al., 2009 [47] Greenspace (urban green, agricultural green, forests and nature areas) National Land Use Classification Database and GIS Proportion of area that is greenspace 1km and 3km Euclidean buffer of home
Sugiyama et al., 2008 [57] Neighbourhood Greenness Self-Reported Access to park or nature reserve Neighbourhood
Triguero-Mas et al., 2015 [49] Green land cover NDVI Amount of greenspace 100m, 300m, 500m, 1km Euclidean buffer of home
e) Subjective Connection to Nature
Cervinka et al., 2012 [75] Nature Self-Reported Connectedness to nature Individual
Howell et al., 2011 [76] Nature Self-Reported Connectedness to nature Individual
Howell et al., 2013 [77] Nature Self-Reported Connectedness to nature
Nature relatedness
Individual
Kamitsis and Francis, 2013 [65] Nature Self-Reported Connectedness to nature Individual
Nisbet et al., 2011 [78] Nature Self-Reported Nature relatedness
Ecological consciousness
Individual
Zelenski et al., 2014 [79] Nature Self-Reported Nature relatedness
Inclusion of nature in self
Individual
Zhang et al., 2014 [80] Nature Self-Reported Connectedness to nature
Engagement with natural beauty
Individual

Mental wellbeing measures

Only 14 studies were found to measure both hedonic and eudaimonic mental wellbeing, of which the most commonly used measure was the Warwick-Edinburgh Mental Well-Being Scale (WEMWBS) [19, 46, 52, 53, 61, 66]. WEMWBS includes 14 positively worded questions, regarding individual feelings over the past 2 weeks, including “feeling relaxed”, “interested in new things”, and “close to others” [81]; there is also a reduced 7-item version, known as SWEMWBS (Shortened-WEMWBS) [82]. The recent Personal Wellbeing ONS4, applied in to one study [72], measures individuals’ life satisfaction, happiness and anxiety (hedonic wellbeing) and sense of worth (eudaimonic wellbeing) [83].

The remaining 32 studies assessed outcomes considered to be aspects of mental wellbeing, such as quality of life, life satisfaction, and affect, but did not report both hedonic and eudaimonic wellbeing. The WHO-5 Well-Being Index, used in 2 studies [48, 70], asks how frequently individuals have felt “cheerful and in good spirits” and “calm and relaxed”, over the previous 2 weeks, but focusses on hedonic rather than eudaimonic wellbeing [84].

Quality of life was measured in 6 studies, two using the WHOQOL-BREF [65, 75], a 26-item questionnaire covering physical and psychological health, social relationships and personal environment [85]. The SF-36 instrument measures quality of life with 36 physical, emotional and psychological health questions [86], and was used in 4 studies [41, 42, 44, 75]. A brief 12-item version (SF-12) has three subscales: mental health, vitality [18], and emotional-role functioning. The mental component summary (MCS), derived from a subset of emotional problems, wellbeing and social functioning questions, was used in 6 papers [21, 40, 50, 57, 74, 75], asking how often the individual recently felt “full of energy”, “nervous” and “happy” [86].

Single-item Life Satisfaction was used in 6 studies [24, 25, 40, 41, 51, 87]. The Satisfaction With Life Scale (SWLS) was applied to 4 studies [75, 7880], and includes a more thorough 5 life-evaluation questions, which ask how ideal and satisfying the individual’s life is, and if they have “gotten the important things… in life” [88].

Happiness was measured with one question in 4 studies [56, 67, 69, 71]. The Attentional Functioning Index (AFI), which assesses daily functioning, was used in one study [62, 89].

Eight studies reported affect scores [62, 63, 6669, 78, 79], which include positive feelings (happiness, interest), and negative emotions (anger, sadness), using the 20-item Positive and Negative Affect Scale (PANAS) [90]. Similarly, The Profile of Mood States (POMS) asks about experiences of 65 different emotions, including some positive items, such as “lively” and “relaxed” [91], and was used in one study [20]. The General Health Questionnaire (GHQ) was used in 14 studies [5, 17, 19, 2325, 38, 45, 47, 49, 54, 58, 60, 73]. It contains some positively worded items (“In the last 2 weeks I have… been able to concentrate”, “felt I have been playing a useful part” and “feeling reasonably happy”) but was designed and validated as a screening tool for psychiatric disorders, with higher scores indicative of greater distress [92]. Other studies which measured on poor mental health were excluded from this review.

Full details of the included studies are presented in Table 2, which is ordered by greenspace characteristic. Where articles cover multiple characteristics, the study appears under multiple headings.

Greenspace characteristics

Amount of local-area greenspace

21 studies examined associations between quantities of local-area greenspace and mental wellbeing, 2 of which were longitudinal. Most calculated the proportion of greenspace for each Lower-Layer Super Output Area (LSOA, a geographic area generated for being as consistent in population size as possible, with a minimum population of 1000 and the mean of 1500), Census District (CD), or within a defined radius of residents. Two articles measured greenspace area per capita. Of 15 studies, one was restricted to public greenspace [39], and 14 included only urban areas.

Only four (cross-sectional) studies measured hedonic and eudaimonic mental wellbeing (SWEMWBS and ONS4). No statistically significant association was reported between greenspace and mental wellbeing in three studies [46, 52, 72], although urban residents who reported “sufficient local greenspace” showed significantly higher SWEMWBS scores [52]. However, Wood et al.’s study found that a 1ha increase in park area within a 1.6km walk of an individual’s home showed a 0.070-point increases in SWEMWBS score [53]; this suggests that examining greenspace around individuals, rather than aggregating to local area, may better detect associations.

Five studies, 4 of which were Good quality and based in urban areas, found that life satisfaction was significantly higher in areas with more greenspace [24, 39, 41, 51], albeit with small linear effect sizes of 0.002–0.003 [24, 39]. The study by White et al. included a large sample, over 10,000 individuals, demonstrating a slight but significant association between LSOA greenspace proportions and life satisfaction. Another large study by the same authors found no significant association between mental wellbeing and the amount of rural local-area greenspace [25], suggesting that associations may differ between urban and rural environments.

An ecological analysis of over 172 countries measured the amount of green land cover per km2, adjusted for the nation’s size, finding a significant association with better life satisfaction. Despite the large sample size and strong odds ratios (2.450), the study was of poor methodological quality, due to its ecological design and hence inability to adjust for individual-level confounding [51]. Four studies also found the quantity of urban greenspace was associated with quality of life or mental health, characterised by the SF-36 scale and its sub-components [4143, 74]; however, three others, which included only public urban greenspace, found no association [39, 44, 50]. Taylor et al. observed mixed results: the amount of urban greenspace was positively and significantly associated with hedonic wellbeing for two cities in Australia, but not two others in New Zealand [48].

Based on these Good quality studies, we conclude that there is adequate evidence for an association between local-area urban greenspace and life satisfaction, but not rural greenspace. Mixed results provide inadequate evidence for associations with quality of life, mental health, and multi-dimensional mental wellbeing.

GHQ was the outcome in 8 studies, of which 6 were Good quality and 3 were confined to urban areas. All but one [45] found an inverse association between the amount of greenspace and GHQ score [5, 17, 2325, 47, 49, 50], implying reduced mental distress; again, linear regression coefficients varied considerably, from 0.003 to 0.431. The Fair quality study by Dzhambov et al., however, found no statistically significant association for objective greenspace quantities, but observed significantly lower GHQ scores for those with higher perceived greenness in their neighbourhood [45]. In a longitudinal study, Alcock et al. found that people moving to areas with higher greenspace proportions had significantly lower GHQ score after relocating, averaging 0.430 points lower 3 years post-move [23]. Therefore, there was adequate evidence for the inverse association between the amount of local-area greenspace and (lower) GHQ score.

Greenspace types

A total of 8 Good and 2 Fair quality studies classified greenspace according to greenspace types, using bespoke classification systems; no consensus was observed regarding greenspace typology. Four of these were longitudinal studies.

Only one Fair study measured hedonic and eudaimonic wellbeing, with WEMWBS, comparing linear associations between the amount of sport, recreational and ‘natural’ spaces within a 1.6km buffer of the individual [53]. The strongest associations were observed for sports (0.430 increase in WEMWBS for each additional space), followed by recreational and natural spaces (0.110 each).

One research group conducted four studies (3 longitudinal) using the longitudinal Swedish Health Survey (SHS), based in suburban and rural areas. They classified public greenspace within 300m of each residents’ home into 5 aspects: Serene (quiet, audible ‘nature’), Wild (undeveloped, no visible human impact), Lush (biodiversity), Spacious (large cohesive area) and Cultural (cultural heritage, old trees) [18, 54, 58]. Two studies measured GHQ: the first found associations between Serene or Spacious greenspace and slightly, but significantly, lower GHQ scores for physically active individuals; however, associations with Spacious greenspace held only for women [54]. In the second, only women moving to areas with Serene greenspace had significantly lowered GHQ scores, but with much higher odds than in Annerstedt et al.’s work [58]. In a cross-sectional analysis, these authors found that the total number of green aspects (Serene, Wild, Lush, Spacious, Cultural) was associated with slightly better SF-36 Vitality scores for women [18]. The third longitudinal study found marginally but significantly lower GHQ scores for greater numbers of different green aspects, including those moving between areas [60].

In a cross-sectional study, based on 12,697 observations from 2,020 residents of rural England, no association was found between LSOA land cover classes and GHQ scores. However, individuals who relocated to areas with more arable, grass, ‘natural’, mountainous and heath land had significantly lower GHQ scores post-move [38].

Among 3 cross-sectional studies, urban residents with higher amounts of local vegetative or ‘natural’ greenspaces reported better mental wellbeing: vegetation density and cover, from field surveys and satellite imagery in Australia, were strongly and significantly associated with life satisfaction [55]. The number of trees, or an indicator of how ‘green’ the neighbourhood is, were significantly associated with better mental health (SF-36 Mental Component) and life satisfaction [57, 59]. Residents’ ratings of the ‘quality of their local natural environment’, on a scale of 0–10 (very dissatisfied to very satisfied), was associated with higher SF-36 Mental Component Summary scores [59].

A large cross-sectional study in the UK used app data on users’ self-reported feelings, while their phones’ GPS linked their location to a land-cover database; this novel study therefore benefits from measuring happiness in situ. Being in mountainous, woodland or ‘semi-natural grassland’ areas, as opposed to urban, was associated with approximately 2-points higher happiness, on a scale of 0–10, although no additional factors were controlled for [56].

While most of these studies were Good quality, interpretation is difficult due to lack of consensus in greenspace classification; in addition, four reports were based on data from the same survey. All but one were restricted to either urban or rural areas, so comparisons between these environments is not possible; however, larger effect sizes were observed in rural studies. Two of the Swedish studies concluded that green aspects were associated with lower GHQ scores for women, while 6 others highlighted that Serene (quiet, ‘natural’) and ‘natural’ rural greenspaces were associated with improved life satisfaction, SF-36 and lower GHQ scores, although none defined the term ‘natural’. Additionally, two studies reported an association between subjective perceptions of local greenspace and mental wellbeing. Evidence is therefore limited.

Visits to greenspace

Seventeen papers reported studies of visits, either comparing mental wellbeing scores before and after an intervention (n = 7), or testing cross-sectional associations with greenspace visiting patterns (n = 10).

Fair quality studies compared happiness and positive affect for those walking in ‘natural’ versus indoor environments [69], and walks in urban versus green areas [66]. The former reported a statistically significant difference in favour of greenspace walking, the latter did not. In a further Fair quality cross-sectional study, Marselle et al. reported a positive association between perceived restorativeness of the walking environment and positive affect and happiness [67].

Duvall and Kaplan observed 73 individuals on a wilderness expedition; attention and affect were improved post-expedition, persisting for 3–4 weeks [62]. Although effects were quite large (score changes of 0.270 to 0.340), participants were not blind to the intervention. A Fair quality uncontrolled study encouraged individuals to engage with ‘nature’ for 30 days by noticing/protecting wildlife, sharing experiences, or connecting with ‘nature’. Participants reported greater happiness following the programme [71]. Similarly, Molsher and Townsend noted mental wellbeing improvements following engagement with environmental volunteering projects [68], although their study displayed high risk of bias. Jakubec, however, reported no association between visiting greenspaces and Quality of Life Inventory score, in a Poor quality study [64].

A further 10 cross-sectional studies of varying quality examined self-reported greenspace visit frequency. Three studies measured both hedonic and eudaimonic wellbeing, with mixed findings [61, 63, 72]. In the first Fair study, university students who claimed greater typical contact with nature reported better mental wellbeing using Ryff’s Scale of Psychological Wellbeing [63, 93]. These findings were not replicated in a Good study by Glichrist et al., who examined associations between mental wellbeing (SWEMWBS) and greenspaces surrounding workplaces in Scotland [61]. White et al.’s Good study, measuring ONS4, found that those spending time outdoors and in nature every day, compared to never, had strong odds (OR 1.960) of a high sense of worth, the effect size decreasing with visit frequency. No associations were detected for visit frequency and hedonic wellbeing, although those reporting visiting greenspace the previous day had higher happiness scores, with no associations for life satisfaction, anxiety or worth [72].

A further 5 studies, one of which was Poor, showed that quality of life and mental health were improved, and GHQ scores reduced, with the number of greenspace visits [19, 21, 50, 52, 65]; Triguero-Mas et al. also noted that associations with mental health were stronger for males than females [50] In a Good study, Mitchell found that those who more regularly visited a local park had lower GHQ scores [19]. However, although Panno et al. observed that greater frequency of greenspace visits was associated with higher hedonic wellbeing, these results were not statistically significant [70], and Dzhambov et al. found no association between time spent in greenspace and GHQ [45].

Due to the mixed quality and inconsistent results, evidence for an association between greenspace visit frequency and mental wellbeing is considered limited.

Views of greenspace

Association between views of greenspace and mental wellbeing was reported in 3 papers. Gilchrist et al.’s Good quality study found that workers’ satisfaction with their office views, particularly of trees, lawns and flowering plants, was associated with improved mental wellbeing (SWEMWBS) scores [61]. Similarly, urban residents reporting greater visibility of trees from their home had slightly better life satisfaction [59]. Pretty et al. observed increases in self-esteem for those viewing rural pleasant scenes, while both unpleasant urban and rural scenes could be detrimental; however, they did not control for potentially confounding factors [20]. The mixed quality and small study sample leads us to classify the evidence here as inadequate.

Greenspace accessibility

We identified 8 cross-sectional studies, mostly Good quality, which tested associations between greenspace accessibility and mental wellbeing. Two studies measured mental health using the SF-12 Mental Component, with significant positive findings [57, 74]. In the first, a weak association was found with Euclidean (direct) distance from homes to the nearest public greenspace [74]. In the second, Sugiyama et al. used the Neighbourhood Environment Walkability Scale, which measures self-reported greenspace access. Access to the highest of levels of greenspace (perceived neighbourhood greenness, terciles) was associated with strong odds (OR 1.270) of better mental health [57].

Only one, Fair study compared public greenspace within different Euclidean buffers around individuals’ postcodes [49]. Triguero-Mas et al. found greater amounts of greenspace within 300m were significantly associated with reduced risk of high GHQ scores (dichotomised around 3), with consistent results for control buffers of 100m, 500m, and 1km [49]. Bos et al. found that greenspace within 3km, but not 1km, of homes was significantly associated with greater quality of life [43], although this study was rated as Poor study because of limited statistical reporting. In a larger study, scores on the SF-36 Vitality scale were associated with rural greenspace, but this was only significant for women and within 300m (but not 100m), of their home [18]. Maas et al.’s large cross-sectional study showed that those with more greenspace within 1km, but not 3km, had slightly lower GHQ scores, contrary to findings by Bos et al. [43, 47]. Dadvand et al. also measured GHQ (dichotomised around 3), finding strong odds of low GHQ scores for the amount of greenspace within 100m of homes (OR 1.320), effect sizes reducing with distance (OR 1.250 for 250m, 1.170 for 500m); stronger associations were also noted for subjective, than objective, proximity to greenspace, measured as self-report and calculated presence of a greenspace within a 10-minute walk [73]. Dzhambov et al. also found a significant association between subjective accessibility (time to walk to nearest greenspace) and lower GHQ, although associations for objectively measured Euclidean distance were not statistically significant [45].

Although several of these studies reported an association between greenspace accessibility and aspects of mental wellbeing, different measures of both were used and findings were inconsistent, providing limited evidence of an association.

Subjective connectedness to nature

We identified 7 cross-sectional studies examining associations between subjective connection to nature and mental wellbeing. The Connectedness to Nature Scale measures the extent to which individuals ‘feel nature is part of their identity’, with particular emphasis on sense of care for nature; this has been linked to the theory of biophilia: that humans possess an innate desire to affiliate with other forms of life [3, 69]. Of these studies, 5 were of Poor quality, with no controls for potential confounding. Four studies demonstrated that self-reported ‘connection to nature’ was positively associated with mental wellbeing [7679]. Effect sizes were moderate and consistent across the studies, although lower methodological quality means their results have limited generalisability; only one was of Good quality, and adjusted for potentially confounding factors. Similarly, meaning in life, quality of life, happiness and affect were higher for those who reported greater connection to nature [65, 75, 76, 79]. Life satisfaction was also positively related to nature connectedness in two studies [79, 80], with moderate effect sizes, although Zhang et al. revealed that the association only held for those who actively engaged with nature [80]. While consistent in their findings, poor study quality means that the evidence is inadequate.

Discussion

Summary of findings

While both the World Health Organisation and United Nations agree that greenspace is vital for healthy, liveable environments [11, 12], it remains unclear which amounts, types and uses of greenspace are most beneficial to mental wellbeing. Previous reviews have focussed on associations between greenspace (or nature) and general health or mental distress [7, 8, 12, 31], but we are not aware of any previous systematic reviews of published evidence specifically for associations between greenspace and validated, positive measures of mental wellbeing in adults. Even after stratifying our review according to the six main ways in which greenspace was conceptualised and measured, methodological heterogeneity precluded meta-analysis. We therefore undertook a narrative synthesis.

The largest number of studies were concerned with the amount of local-area greenspace, although few used detailed hedonic and eudaimonic wellbeing measures. Consistent results revealed adequate evidence for an association between urban local-area greenspace and life satisfaction. This result did not hold for rural greenspace, however. There was also adequate evidence for an association between local-area greenspace and lower GHQ scores.

Inconsistencies in the categorisation of greenspace types, and dearth of definitions, made it difficult to synthesise results; limited evidence was therefore found for associations between mental wellbeing and variety and ‘nature’ in land cover. Evidence was similarly limited for greenspace accessibility, with results generally concluding that nearer greenspace has the strongest associations, but with results differing according to the mental wellbeing measure; limited evidence was also found for associations between greenspace visits and mental wellbeing.

However, while there was some evidence for an association between mental wellbeing and views of greenery and connectedness to nature, this was considered inadequate, due to the mixed quality and small sample sizes of studies. Table 4 provides full details of the evidence summary and implications for research and policy.

Table 4. Summary of findings and implications.

Greenspace Characteristic Summary of Evidence Strength of Evidence Implications
Amount of local area greenspace Positive association between urban greenspace and life satisfaction Adequate Research:
Studies are required to measure both hedonic and eudaimonic wellbeing
Associations may differ between urban and rural environments
National studies should stratify for urban/rural setting
Local-area statistics may be less effective at detecting associations than measures which consider greenspace relative to the individual. Greenspace within set distances of individuals should be further investigated.
Methods should consider the potential spatial nature of the data
More longitudinal analyses are required to establish causality
Greenspace measures should consider where people spend their time (ie while commuting, at work), not just relative to homes
Policy:
Increasing provision local-area greenspace in urban environments is recommended for potential benefits to life satisfaction
Inverse association between urban greenspace with GHQ Adequate Research:
Studies are required to measure positive mental wellbeing (both hedonic and eudaimonic dimensions)
Policy:
Increasing provision of urban local-area greenspace is recommended for potentially reducing symptoms of psychiatric distress
Greenspace types Some association between ‘nature’/variety in land cover and aspects of mental wellbeing Limited Research:
Studies are required to measure both hedonic and eudaimonic wellbeing
More consistency is needed in establishing a greenspace typology
Specific features of greenspace should be investigated
More consistency is needed in defining terms, particularly ‘nature’, which is often undefined
Measures of greenspace quality should also be included
Policy:
Variety and nature in greenspace types may be important, but currently more evidence is required to recommend this for mental wellbeing benefit
Visits to greenspace Frequency of visits to greenspace may be associated with aspects of mental wellbeing Limited Research:
Studies are required to measure both hedonic and eudaimonic wellbeing
More objective assessments of greenspace visiting patterns are required
Social context and individual experiences of greenspace patterns should be considered
Participants must be blind to interventions to ensure a fair sample
More controlled case studies, and longitudinal analyses may help in understanding the direction of associations
Policy:
Promoting visits to greenspace may improve aspects of mental wellbeing, though more evidence is required
Views of greenspace Views of greenspace/green features may be associated with some aspects of mental wellbeing Inadequate Research:
Studies are required to measure both hedonic and eudaimonic wellbeing
Much more research should examine associations between views of greenspace and mental wellbeing
With potential differences between views from homes and workplaces, greenspace measures should consider where people spend their time
Greenspace accessibility Greenspace closer to homes may be most strongly associated with aspects of mental wellbeing Limited Research:
More studies are required to measure both hedonic and eudaimonic wellbeing
Accessibility measures need greater consistency, including controlled sensitivity analyses
Network, rather than just Euclidean distances, should be applied
Social and physical barriers to access should be considered
Quality and facilities of greenspaces require further investigation and consistency, for example use of the Green Flag Award for parks
Spatial Methods
Thorough testing of Government guidelines is necessary to provide robust evidence of mental wellbeing benefit
Policy:
There is currently a lack of evidence recommend the guideline of providing greenspace within a 300m buffer specifically, for mental wellbeing in particular
Subjective connection to nature Personal connection to nature may be associated with mental wellbeing Inadequate Research:
Studies must control for potentially confounding factors
More objective assessments of connection to nature and mental wellbeing are required
More consistency is needed in defining terms, particularly ‘nature’, which is often undefined

Mental wellbeing measures

Only 14 of the 52 studies used a measure of mental wellbeing that captured both hedonic and eudaimonic dimensions, while others measured aspects such as life satisfaction, happiness and quality of life. GHQ, which was designed as a psychiatric screening tool, was included as a prevalent surrogate in the literature, which includes some positive items. Papers using other psychiatric screening tools were excluded if they covered only symptoms, ie mental distress.

Greenspace definitions and indicators

We identified 6 types of assessment in greenspace studies: amount of local-area greenspace, greenspace types, visits to greenspace, views of greenspace, greenspace accessibility and self-reported connection to nature.

The amount of local-area greenspace was most commonly measured as the proportion of greenspace in a resident’s local area, or more specifically within a set radius of participants’ homes. Most of these studies were restricted to urban areas. Most researchers quantified greenspace objectively, while a small number of studies reported associations with perceptions of the adequacy of the amount of local greenspace provision. All studies used either linear or logistic regression, which may overestimate associations in spatial data. Although a number of studies examined different types of greenspace, no consensus was observed for a typology, and as such conflicting results were observed.

One of the UN’s Sustainable Development Goals is to “provide universal access to…green and public spaces” [11]; most studies assessed accessibility by distance to greenspace. While the EU and UK recommend that individuals should have access to a greenspace within 300m of their home [14, 94], only one study conducted sensitivity analysis to test this guideline [49]; no difference in associations was observed for buffers of 100m, 300m, 500m and 1km. One study used buffer radii of 100m and 300m, reporting a significant association between the latter and mental wellbeing, while a second found that associations with GHQ decreased with distance, at 100m, 250m and 500m buffers. Others found contradictory results using radii of 1 and 3km. Another drawback was the use of Euclidean distance, which doesn’t account for access routes. Application of network distance and consideration of pedestrian routes may give a greater indication of accessibility on foot.

Greenspace visiting patterns were measured inconsistently, in small or cross-sectional studies. Individuals who visited greenspace more often reported greater mental wellbeing, though a second study found this held only for eudaimonic wellbeing; no associations were found in an analysis of greenspace adjacent to workplaces. This study did however report a positive association with views of greenspace from the workplace. This is in keeping with previous research showing that green views reduce the effects of stress [810, 95]. While two studies highlighted that the perceptions of greenspace quality were more strongly associated with mental wellbeing than quantity [52, 59], the size of this difference was not estimated.

Individual connection to nature, assessed in seven studies, relied on self-report for both the greenspace and wellbeing measures, thereby carrying a high risk of reporting bias, especially since few controlled for potentially confounding factors.

Strengths and limitations

We conducted a comprehensive database search, thorough screening of articles, risk of bias assessment, and detailed narrative synthesis of the 50 studies which met our inclusion criteria. We identified six different ways in which greenspace was conceptualised and measured, and by which we stratified our review. We believe this is the first review to systematically appraise the evidence for associations between greenspace and adult mental wellbeing, using only validated measures of positive mental health.

Selection criteria were designed to ensure results of sufficient quality and relevance, and we consulted an information specialist to maximise search efficiency. Screening was undertaken by two independent reviewers, to minimise potential bias. While our criteria were designed to be inclusive, an element of subjectivity means there was a possible risk of excluding potentially interesting studies; we attempted to minimise this by appraising each study with assessments recommended by the Cochrane Handbook, which provides guidance for internationally recognised highest-standard research [32, 3437].

We considered all greenspaces, not restricting our criteria to studies specifically in urban areas, although some studies were confined to urban or rural locations. Nationwide studies were likely to have included both, without stratifying for setting. It was difficult, therefore, to draw clear conclusions about interactions between urban and rural location and associations with mental wellbeing. Although there is interest in understanding how urban greenspaces should best be designed and constructed, it was not possible to draw conclusions specifically for those living in cities.

Only one-quarter of included studies measured both hedonic and eudaimonic mental wellbeing; the majority focused only on aspects such as life satisfaction, affect and vitality, while others used measures (such as the GHQ) which combined positive and negative (distress) items.

While several studies implied that ‘nature’ was associated with aspects of mental wellbeing, none provided a definition of this term. To further complicate matters ‘nature’ and ‘greenspace’ were sometimes used synonymously [1719, 21, 72]. Vegetative or ‘natural’ greenspaces, such as those described as ‘serene’ (quiet, ‘natural’), or with more trees, were most strongly associated with aspects of mental wellbeing, although one study found a stronger association for sports facilities. However, there were few direct comparisons between greenspace types. While Government Guidance provides a standardised greenspace typology for urban planning in the UK [15], no studies used this classification.

Studies that considered greenspace accessibility were limited to estimates of Euclidean distances from home rather than access routes [96]. These studies did not take account of participants’ routines, or where they spent their time. None of the included studies assessed greenspace quality (such as captured by the Green Flag Award [97], or the social contexts in which greenspaces are situated [7, 98].

Only 6 out of 50 papers reported longitudinal studies. Cross-sectional analyses cannot distinguish between reverse causality and associations which may be causal in nature, and, like all observational studies, are prone to confounding (especially by indication) and bias. Although 26 studies were deemed to be of Good quality, 12 were Fair, and the remaining 12 were Poor; this was mostly due to lack of control for potentially confounding, minimal statistical reporting, and, in 3 cases, lack of participant blinding to an intervention.

Conclusions

We sought to synthesis and appraise the evidence for associations between greenspace and mental wellbeing, but found few studies measuring both hedonic and eudaimonic wellbeing. Results suggest associations between greenspace and mental wellbeing, particularly hedonic wellbeing. We discovered adequate evidence for associations between urban greenspace and life satisfaction; however, the evidence for the remainder of the greenspace characteristics, including greenspace (land use) type, accessibility, viewing and visiting patterns, was limited or inadequate. Although not a true measure of mental wellbeing, studies using the GHQ were prevalent in the literature. This measure includes some positive items, and we further concluded that there was adequate evidence for associations between greenspace and lower GHQ scores. While our review was limited by the lack of available data to conduct a meta-analysis, we were able to highlight key areas for future research through our narrative synthesis.

Government guidelines for greenspace provision require robust evidence, but evidence is currently not sufficient for informed, specific planning recommendations. Further methodological work in this field is needed, including the development of operational definitions of ‘nature’ and ‘natural’, and agreement on a land use typology. Measures of greenspace quality are also needed. More studies are required to measure both hedonic and eudaimonic mental wellbeing. Greenspace accessibility should also be measured more specifically, using individual travel distances, using spatial methods of analysis, to better understand how greenspaces should be designed and incorporated into environments. Further research is needed that considers differences in associations between greenspace and mental wellbeing in urban versus rural settings.

Supporting information

S1 Table. Heatmap of risk of bias for studies evaluated using the Newcastle-Ottawa Scale adapted for cross-sectional studies.

(DOCX)

S2 Table. Heatmap of risk of bias for studies evaluated using the Cochrane RoB 2.0 tool.

(DOCX)

S3 Table. PRISMA 2009 checklist.

(DOC)

Acknowledgments

We thank Sam Johnson for her guidance on database search strategy.

Data Availability

All relevant data are within the paper and its Supporting Information files.

Funding Statement

This work was supported by the Engineering and Physical Sciences Research Council, Recipient: VH (through studentship at Warwick Institute for Science of Cities), Grant number: EP/L016400/1, URL: https://epsrc.ukri.org/. The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.

References

  • 1.United Nations. World Urbanization Prospects 2014: Highlights. United Nations Publications; 2014. [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Wilson EO. Biophilia: Harvard University Press; 1984. [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Wilson EO. Biophilia and the conservation ethic Evolutionary perspectives on environmental problems. 2007:249–57. [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Lachowycz K, Jones AP. Towards a better understanding of the relationship between greenspace and health: development of a theoretical framework. Landscape and Urban Planning. 2013;118:62–9. [Google Scholar]
  • 5.De Vries S, Verheij RA, Groenewegen PP, Spreeuwenberg P. Natural environments—healthy environments? An exploratory analysis of the relationship between greenspace and health. Environment and planning A. 2003;35(10):1717–31. [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Maas J, Verheij RA, Groenewegen PP, De Vries S, Spreeuwenberg P. Green space, urbanity, and health: how strong is the relation? Journal of epidemiology and community health. 2006;60(7):587–92. 10.1136/jech.2005.043125 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Douglas OL, Mike; Scott Mark;. Green space benefits for health and well-being: A life-sourse approach for urban planning, design and management. Cities. 2017;66:53–62. [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Hartig T, Mitchell R, De Vries S, Frumkin H. Nature and health. Annual review of public health. 2014;35:207–28. 10.1146/annurev-publhealth-032013-182443 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Kaplan R. Impact of urban nature: A theoretical analysis. Urban ecology. 1984;8(3):189–97. [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Ulrich RS, Simons RF, Losito BD, Fiorito E, Miles MA, Zelson M. Stress recovery during exposure to natural and urban environments. Journal of environmental psychology. 1991;11(3):201–30. [Google Scholar]
  • 11.United Nations General Assembly. New Urban Agenda2017. http://habitat3.org/the-new-urban-agenda.
  • 12.World Health Organisation. Urban green spaces and health—a review of evidence. 2016.
  • 13.Local Government Association. Being Mindful of Mental Health- The role of local government in mental health and wellbeing. Online: 2017 29 Jun 2017. Report No.
  • 14.Natural England. Nature nearby: accessible natural greenspace guidance. Natural England: Peterborough: 2010. [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Planning Policy Guidance. 17: Planning for open space, sport and recreation. Office of the Deputy Prime Minister, London[online] < www communities gov-uk/planningandbuilding/planningsystem/planningpolicy/planningpolicystatements/>[Accessed 1502 11]. 2002.
  • 16.HM Government. The Natural Choice: Securing the value of nature. Natural Environment White Paper; 2011.
  • 17.Astell-Burt T, Mitchell R, Hartig T. The association between green space and mental health varies across the lifecourse. A longitudinal study. Journal of epidemiology and community health. 2014;68(6):578–83. Epub 2014/03/08. 10.1136/jech-2013-203767 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Bjork J, Albin M, Grahn P, Jacobsson H, Ardo J, Wadbro J, et al. Recreational values of the natural environment in relation to neighbourhood satisfaction, physical activity, obesity and wellbeing. Journal of Epidemiology and Community Health. 2008;62(4). 10.1136/jech.2007.062414 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Mitchell R. Is physical activity in natural environments better for mental health than physical activity in other environments? Social Science & Medicine. 2013;91:130–4. 10.1016/j.socscimed.2012.04.012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Pretty J, Peacock J, Sellens M, Griffin M. The mental and physical health outcomes of green exercise. International journal of environmental health research. 2005;15(5):319–37. 10.1080/09603120500155963 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.van den Berg M, van Poppel M, van Kamp I, Andrusaityte S, Balseviciene B, Cirach M, et al. Visiting green space is associated with mental health and vitality: A cross-sectional study in four European cities. Health & Place. 2016;38:8–15. 10.1016/j.healthplace.2016.01.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Taylor L, Hochuli DF. Defining greenspace: Multiple uses across multiple disciplines. Landscape and Urban Planning. 2017;158:25–38. 10.1016/j.landurbplan.2016.09.024. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Alcock I, White MP, Wheeler BW, Fleming LE, Depledge MH. Longitudinal effects on mental health of moving to greener and less green urban areas. Environmental science & technology. 2014;48(2):1247–55. Epub 2013/12/11. 10.1021/es403688w . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.White MP, Alcock I, Wheeler BW, Depledge MH. Would you be happier living in a greener urban area? A fixed-effects analysis of panel data. Psychological Science. 2013;24(6):920–8. 10.1177/0956797612464659. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.White MP, Alcock I, Wheeler BW, Depledge MH. Coastal proximity, health and well-being: results from a longitudinal panel survey. Health & place. 2013;23:97–103. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Henderson LW, Knight T. Integrating the hedonic and eudaimonic perspectives to more comprehensively understand wellbeing and pathways to wellbeing. International Journal of Wellbeing. 2012;2(3). [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Ryan RM, Deci EL. On happiness and human potentials: A review of research on hedonic and eudaimonic well-being. Annual review of psychology. 2001;52(1):141–66. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Cassiers I. Beyond GDP, Measuring progress, true wealth, and the well-being of nations: Conference Proceedings. 2009:15, 97, 180, 1, 91, 93.
  • 29.De Neve J-E, Oswald AJ. Estimating the influence of life satisfaction and positive affect on later income using sibling fixed effects. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 2012;109(49):19953–8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Oswald AJ, Proto E, Sgroi D. Happiness and productivity. Journal of Labor Economics. 2015;33(4):789–822. [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Gascon M, Triguero-Mas M, Martínez D, Dadvand P, Forns J, Plasència A, et al. Mental health benefits of long-term exposure to residential green and blue spaces: a systematic review. International journal of environmental research and public health. 2015;12(4):4354–79. 10.3390/ijerph120404354 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Higgins JP, Green S. Cochrane handbook for systematic reviews of interventions: John Wiley & Sons; 2011. [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Khan KS, Ter Riet G, Glanville J, Sowden AJ, Kleijnen J. Undertaking systematic reviews of research on effectiveness: CRD’s guidance for carrying out or commissioning reviews: NHS Centre for Reviews and Dissemination; 2001.
  • 34.Higgins JP, Altman DG, Gøtzsche PC, Jüni P, Moher D, Oxman AD, et al. The Cochrane Collaboration’s tool for assessing risk of bias in randomised trials. Bmj. 2011;343:d5928 10.1136/bmj.d5928 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Wells G, Shea B, O’connell D, Peterson J, Welch V, Losos M, et al. The Newcastle-Ottawa Scale (NOS) for assessing the quality of nonrandomised studies in meta-analyses. Ottawa: Ottawa Hospital Research Institute; 2011. oxford. asp; 2011. [Google Scholar]
  • 36.McPheeters ML, Kripalani S, Peterson NB, Idowu RT, Jerome RN, Potter SA, et al. Closing the quality gap: revisiting the state of the science (vol. 3: quality improvement interventions to address health disparities). Evidence report/technology assessment. 2012;(2083):1. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Hawker S, Payne S, Kerr C, Hardey M, Powell J. Appraising the evidence: reviewing disparate data systematically. Qualitative Health Research. 2002;12(9):1284–99. 10.1177/1049732302238251 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Alcock I, White MP, Lovell R, Higgins SL, Osborne NJ, Husk K, et al. What accounts for ‘England’s green and pleasant land'? A panel data analysis of mental health and land cover types in rural England. Landscape and Urban Planning. 2015;142:38–46. 10.1016/j.landurbplan.2015.05.008 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Ambrey C, Fleming C. Public Greenspace and Life Satisfaction in Urban Australia. Urban Studies. 2014;51(6):1290–321. 10.1177/0042098013494417 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Ambrey CL. Urban greenspace, physical activity and wellbeing: The moderating role of perceptions of neighbourhood affability and incivility. Land Use Policy. 2016;57:638–44. 10.1016/j.landusepol.2016.06.034 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Ambrey CL. An investigation into the synergistic wellbeing benefits of greenspace and physical activity: Moving beyond the mean. Urban Forestry and Urban Greening. 2016;19:7–12. 10.1016/j.ufug.2016.06.020 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Ambrey C. Greenspace, physical activity and well-being in Australian capital cities: how does population size moderate the relationship? Public health. 2016;133:38–44. 10.1016/j.puhe.2015.11.013 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Bos EH, van der Meulen L, Wichers M, Jeronimus BF. A Primrose Path? Moderating Effects of Age and Gender in the Association between Green Space and Mental Health. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health. 2016;13(5). 10.3390/ijerph13050492 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.de Vries S, Van Dillen SM, Groenewegen PP, Spreeuwenberg P. Streetscape greenery and health: stress, social cohesion and physical activity as mediators. Social Science & Medicine. 2013;94:26–33. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Dzhambov A, Hartig T, Markevych I, Tilov B, Dimitrova D. Urban residential greenspace and mental health in youth: different approaches to testing multiple pathways yield different conclusions. Environmental research. 2018;160:47–59. 10.1016/j.envres.2017.09.015 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Houlden V, Weich S, Jarvis S. A cross-sectional analysis of green space prevalence and mental wellbeing in England. BMC public health. 2017;17(1):460 10.1186/s12889-017-4401-x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Maas J, Van Dillen SM, Verheij RA, Groenewegen PP. Social contacts as a possible mechanism behind the relation between green space and health. Health & place. 2009;15(2):586–95. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Taylor L, Hahs AK, Hochuli DF. Wellbeing and urban living: nurtured by nature. Urban Ecosystems. 2018;21(1):197–208. [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Triguero-Mas M, Dadvand P, Cirach M, Martínez D, Medina A, Mompart A, et al. Natural outdoor environments and mental and physical health: relationships and mechanisms. Environment international. 2015;77:35–41. 10.1016/j.envint.2015.01.012 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Triguero-Mas M, Donaire-Gonzalez D, Seto E, Valentín A, Martínez D, Smith G, et al. Natural outdoor environments and mental health: Stress as a possible mechanism. Environmental research. 2017;159:629–38. 10.1016/j.envres.2017.08.048 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Vemuri AW, Costanza R. The role of human, social, built, and natural capital in explaining life satisfaction at the country level: Toward a National Well-Being Index (NWI). Ecological Economics. 2006;58(1):119–33. 10.1016/j.ecolecon.2005.02.008 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Ward Thompson C, Aspinall P, Roe J. Access to Green Space in Disadvantaged Urban Communities: Evidence of salutogenic effects based on biomarker and self-report measures of wellbeing. In: Abbas MY, editor. Amer International Conference on Quality of Life, Aicqol2014. Procedia Social and Behavioral Sciences. 1532014. p. 10–22.
  • 53.Wood L, Hooper P, Foster S, Bull F. Public green spaces and positive mental health–investigating the relationship between access, quantity and types of parks and mental wellbeing. Health & place. 2017;48:63–71. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Annerstedt M, Ostergren PO, Bjork J, Grahn P, Skarback E, Wahrborg P. Green qualities in the neighbourhood and mental health-results from a longitudinal cohort study in Southern Sweden. European Journal of Public Health. 2012;22:275–6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Luck GW, Davidson P, Boxall D, Smallbone L. Relations between urban bird and plant communities and human well-being and connection to nature. Conservation biology: the journal of the Society for Conservation Biology. 2011;25(4):816–26. Epub 2011/05/04. 10.1111/j.1523-1739.2011.01685.x . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56.MacKerron G, Mourato S. Happiness is greater in natural environments. Global Environmental Change. 2013;23(5):992–1000. 10.1016/j.gloenvcha.2013.03.010 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Sugiyama T, Leslie E, Giles-Corti B, Owen N. Associations of neighbourhood greenness with physical and mental health: do walking, social coherence and local social interaction explain the relationships? Journal of epidemiology and community health. 2008;62(5):e9 Epub 2008/04/24. . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 58.van den Bosch MA, Ostergren PO, Grahn P, Skarback E, Wahrborg P. Moving to Serene Nature May Prevent Poor Mental Health—Results from a Swedish Longitudinal Cohort Study. International journal of environmental research and public health. 2015;12(7):7974–89. Epub 2015/07/18. 10.3390/ijerph120707974 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 59.Vemuri AW, Grove JM, Wilson MA, Burch WR, Jr. A Tale of Two Scales: Evaluating the Relationship among Life Satisfaction, Social Capital, Income, and the Natural Environment at Individual and Neighborhood Levels in Metropolitan Baltimore. Environment & Behavior. 2011;43(1):3–25. 10.1177/0013916509338551. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 60.Weimann H, Rylander L, Albin M, Skärbäck E, Grahn P, Östergren P-O, et al. Effects of changing exposure to neighbourhood greenness on general and mental health: A longitudinal study. Health & Place. 2015;33:48–56. 10.1016/j.healthplace.2015.02.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 61.Gilchrist K, Brown C, Montarzino A. Workplace settings and wellbeing: Greenspace use and views contribute to employee wellbeing at pen-urban business sites. Landscape and Urban Planning. 2015;138:32–40. 10.1016/j.landurbplan.2015.02.004 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 62.Duvall J, Kaplan R. Enhancing the well-being of veterans using extended group-based nature recreation experiences. Journal of rehabilitation research and development. 2014;51(5):685–96. Epub 2014/12/17. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 63.Herzog TR, Strevey SJ. Contact with nature, sense of humor, and psychological well-being. Environment and Behavior. 2008;40(6):747–76. [Google Scholar]
  • 64.Jakubec SL, Carruthers Den Hoed D, Ray HK, Ashok. Mental well-being and quality-of-life benefits of inclusion in nature for adults with disabilities and their caregivers. Landscape Research. 2016:1–12. [Google Scholar]
  • 65.Kamitsis I, Francis AJ. Spirituality mediates the relationship between engagement with nature and psychological wellbeing. Journal of Environmental Psychology. 2013;36:136–43. [Google Scholar]
  • 66.Marselle MR, Irvine KN, Warber SL. Walking for well-being: are group walks in certain types of natural environments better for well-being than group walks in urban environments? International journal of environmental research and public health. 2013;10(11):5603–28. Epub 2013/11/01. 10.3390/ijerph10115603 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 67.Marselle MR, Irvine KN, Lorenzo-Arribas A, Warber SL. Moving beyond green: exploring the relationship of environment type and indicators of perceived environmental quality on emotional well-being following group walks. International journal of environmental research and public health. 2015;12(1):106–30. Epub 2014/12/30. 10.3390/ijerph120100106 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 68.Molsher R, Townsend M. Improving Wellbeing and Environmental Stewardship Through Volunteering in Nature. Ecohealth. 2016;13(1):151–5. 10.1007/s10393-015-1089-1 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 69.Nisbet EK, Zelenski JM. Underestimating nearby nature: Affective forecasting errors obscure the happy path to sustainability. Psychological Science. 2011;22(9):1101–6. 10.1177/0956797611418527. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 70.Panno A, Carrus G, Lafortezza R, Mariani L, Sanesi G. Nature-based solutions to promote human resilience and wellbeing in cities during increasingly hot summers. Environmental research. 2017;159:249–56. 10.1016/j.envres.2017.08.016 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 71.Richardson M, Cormack A, McRobert L, Underhill R. 30 Days Wild: Development and Evaluation of a Large-Scale Nature Engagement Campaign to Improve Well-Being. PloS one. 2016;11(2):e0149777 10.1371/journal.pone.0149777 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 72.White MP, Pahl S, Wheeler BW, Depledge MH, Fleming LE. Natural environments and subjective wellbeing: Different types of exposure are associated with different aspects of wellbeing. Health & place. 2017;45:77–84. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 73.Dadvand P, Bartoll X, Basagaña X, Dalmau-Bueno A, Martinez D, Ambros A, et al. Green spaces and general health: roles of mental health status, social support, and physical activity. Environment international. 2016;91:161–7. 10.1016/j.envint.2016.02.029 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 74.Krekel C, Kolbe J, Wüstemann H. The Greener, the Happier? The Effects of Urban Green and Abandoned Areas on Residential Well-Being. The Effects of Urban Green and Abandoned Areas on Residential Well-Being (January 2015) SOEPpaper. 2015;(728).
  • 75.Cervinka R, Röderer K, Hefler E. Are nature lovers happy? On various indicators of well-being and connectedness with nature. Journal of Health Psychology. 2012;17(3):379–88. 10.1177/1359105311416873. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 76.Howell AJ, Dopko RL, Passmore H-A, Buro K. Nature connectedness: Associations with well-being and mindfulness. Personality and Individual Differences. 2011;51(2):166–71. [Google Scholar]
  • 77.Howell AJ, Passmore H-A, Buro K. Meaning in nature: meaning in life as a mediator of the relationship between nature connectedness and well-being. Journal of Happiness Studies. 2013;14(6):1681–96. [Google Scholar]
  • 78.Nisbet EK, Zelenski JM, Murphy SA. Happiness is in our nature: Exploring nature relatedness as a contributor to subjective well-being. Journal of Happiness Studies. 2011;12(2):303–22. 10.1007/s10902-010-9197-7. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 79.Zelenski JM, Nisbet EK. Happiness and Feeling Connected: The Distinct Role of Nature Relatedness. Environment and Behavior. 2014;46(1):3–23. 10.1177/0013916512451901. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 80.Zhang JW, Howell RT, Iyer R. Engagement with natural beauty moderates the positive relation between connectedness with nature and psychological well-being. Journal of Environmental Psychology. 2014;38:55–63. [Google Scholar]
  • 81.Tennant R, Hiller L, Fishwick R, Platt S, Joseph S, Weich S, et al. The Warwick-Edinburgh mental well-being scale (WEMWBS): development and UK validation. Health and Quality of life Outcomes. 2007;5(1):1. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 82.Guðmundsdóttir D, editor The short Warwick-Edinburgh mental well-being scale (SWEMWBS): Internal validity and the relationship between SWEMWBS, age, gender and other health and well-being measures. 2nd Applied Positive Psychology Conference, Warwick; 2009.
  • 83.Office for National Statistics. Personal Wellbeing User Guidance Online2016 [cited 2018 21st January]. https://www.ons.gov.uk/peoplepopulationandcommunity/wellbeing/methodologies/personalwellbeingsurveyuserguide.
  • 84.Psychiatric Research Unit. WHO (Five) Well-Being Index (1998 version). WHO Collaborating Centre in Mental Health; 1998. [Google Scholar]
  • 85.World Health Organization. WHOQOL-BREF: introduction, administration, scoring and generic version of the assessment: field trial version, December 1996. 1996. [Google Scholar]
  • 86.Ware JE, Kosinski M, Dewey JE, Gandek B. SF-36 health survey: manual and interpretation guide: Quality Metric Inc.; 2000. [Google Scholar]
  • 87.Self A, Thomas J, Randall C. Measuring national well-being: life in the UK, 2012. Newport: UK Office for National Statistics; 2012. [Google Scholar]
  • 88.Organisation for Economic Co-operation and Development. OECD guidelines on measuring subjective well-being 2013. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 89.Cimprich B. Development of an intervention to restore attention in cancer patients. Cancer nursing. 1993;16(2):83–92. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 90.Watson D, Clark LA, Tellegen A. Development and validation of brief measures of positive and negative affect: the PANAS scales. Journal of personality and social psychology. 1988;54(6):1063 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 91.McNair DM, Lorr M, Droppleman LF. Profile of mood states: Educational and Industrial Testing Service San Diego, CA; 1981. [Google Scholar]
  • 92.Hu Y, Stewart-Brown S, Twigg L, Weich S. Can the 12-item General Health Questionnaire be used to measure positive mental health? Psychological medicine. 2007;37(07):1005–13. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 93.Ryff CD, Keyes CLM. The structure of psychological well-being revisited. Journal of personality and social psychology. 1995;69(4):719 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 94.Tarzia V. European Common Indicators: towards a local sustainability profile. Ambiente Italia Research Institute, Milano, Italy: 2003. [Google Scholar]
  • 95.Ulrich RS. Human responses to vegetation and landscapes. Landscape and urban planning. 1986;13:29–44. [Google Scholar]
  • 96.Coombes E, Jones AP, Hillsdon M. The relationship of physical activity and overweight to objectively measured green space accessibility and use. Social Science & Medicine. 2010;70(6):816–22. 10.1016/j.socscimed.2009.11.020. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 97.Green Flag Award. What is the Green Flag Award Online: Department for Communities and Local Government; 2017 [cited 2017 18th December]. http://www.greenflagaward.org.uk/about-us/what-is-the-green-flag-award/.
  • 98.Fone DL, Dunstan F. Mental health, places and people: a multilevel analysis of economic inactivity and social deprivation. Health & place. 2006;12(3):332–44. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

S1 Table. Heatmap of risk of bias for studies evaluated using the Newcastle-Ottawa Scale adapted for cross-sectional studies.

(DOCX)

S2 Table. Heatmap of risk of bias for studies evaluated using the Cochrane RoB 2.0 tool.

(DOCX)

S3 Table. PRISMA 2009 checklist.

(DOC)

Data Availability Statement

All relevant data are within the paper and its Supporting Information files.


Articles from PLoS ONE are provided here courtesy of PLOS

RESOURCES