Skip to main content
eLife logoLink to eLife
. 2018 Sep 17;7:e37683. doi: 10.7554/eLife.37683

Conditioning sharpens the spatial representation of rewarded stimuli in mouse primary visual cortex

Pieter M Goltstein 1,2,†,, Guido T Meijer 1,2, Cyriel MA Pennartz 1,2,
Editors: Daeyeol Lee3, Andrew J King4
PMCID: PMC6141231  PMID: 30222107

Abstract

Reward is often employed as reinforcement in behavioral paradigms but it is unclear how the visuospatial aspect of a stimulus-reward association affects the cortical representation of visual space. Using a head-fixed paradigm, we conditioned mice to associate the same visual pattern in adjacent retinotopic regions with availability and absence of reward. Time-lapse intrinsic optical signal imaging under anesthesia showed that conditioning increased the spatial separation of mesoscale cortical representations of reward predicting- and non-reward predicting stimuli. Subsequent in vivo two-photon calcium imaging revealed that this improved separation correlated with enhanced population coding for retinotopic location, specifically for the trained orientation and spatially confined to the V1 region where rewarded and non-rewarded stimulus representations bordered. These results are corroborated by conditioning-induced differences in the correlation structure of population activity. Thus, the cortical representation of visual space is sharpened as consequence of associative stimulus-reward learning while the overall retinotopic map remains unaltered.

Research organism: Mouse

Introduction

Involvement of sensory processing in perceptual and stimulus-outcome learning can alter stimulus selectivity of neurons in the sensory cortex (Bao et al., 2001; Schoups et al., 2001; Ghose et al., 2002; Fritz et al., 2003; Yang and Maunsell, 2004; Blake et al., 2006; David et al., 2012; Xu et al., 2012; Goltstein et al., 2013; Jeanne et al., 2013; Poort et al., 2015). Even in the absence of an explicit behavioral paradigm, repeated reward pairing can by itself already lead to an improvement of stimulus processing in sensory cortex (Seitz et al., 2009). However, it is unclear how visuospatial aspects of stimulus-outcome learning translate to changes in neuronal representations. The ecological relevance of this question is underscored by adaptive behaviors of rodents that differentiate between segments of visual space, such as when coping with potential predators moving overhead (e.g. birds of prey; Zhang et al., 2012b; Shang et al., 2015; Wei et al., 2015), or visual stimulus patterns at ground level, possibly associated with food and water (Furtak et al., 2012). Specifically, it is unknown how neural coding in the retinotopically organized visual cortex is affected by learning when adjacent locations in the visual field are differentially predictive of reward or other outcomes.

Located early in the visual stream, the primary visual cortex (V1) has neurons that primarily seem to act as low-level feature detectors, responding selectively to properties such as orientation, contrast, temporal and spatial frequency of bars and gratings, eye-specific inputs and retinotopic location (Hubel and Wiesel, 1959; Hubel and Wiesel, 1962; Mrsic-Flogel et al., 2007; Niell and Stryker, 2008; Smith and Häusser, 2010; Freeman et al., 2013). However, recent studies have shown that activity in mouse V1 is strongly modulated by non-visual factors such as other sensory modalities, internal state, locomotion and anticipation of reward (Shuler and Bear, 2006; Niell and Stryker, 2010; Bennett et al., 2013; Lee et al., 2014; Keller et al., 2012; Saleem et al., 2013; Meijer et al., 2017). Widespread presence of feedback and neuromodulatory influences provide a means for learning experiences to drive plasticity and alter V1 local circuitry (Gilbert, 1996; Chubykin et al., 2013; Zhang et al., 2014; Ji et al., 2015).

Learning can affect neocortical function via a number of mechanisms. Neural responses in primary visual cortex may change to increase bottom-up saliency of a stimulus (Zhang et al., 2012a), for instance by a selective increase in amplitude of the response to conditioned stimuli (Fritz et al., 2003; Blake et al., 2006; Andermann et al., 2010; Goltstein et al., 2013; Poort et al., 2015). Alternatively, neurons can be recruited to become tuned to a conditioned stimulus (Weinberger et al., 1993), which in case of the tonotopic map of auditory cortex leads to an enlarged cortical region dedicated to this stimulus (Bao et al., 2001). Furthermore, the selectivity of neuronal tuning curves may be altered (David et al., 2012; Goltstein et al., 2013), for example amplifying small but relevant changes in stimulus properties to large changes in neuronal activity (Schoups et al., 2001). Finally, some changes can only be observed in simultaneously recorded neuronal populations, like changes in sparseness of population responses (Ghose et al., 2002; Gdalyahu et al., 2012) or the correlation structure of ensemble activity (Averbeck et al., 2006; Poort and Roelfsema, 2009; Jeanne et al., 2013; Montijn et al., 2015). While the effect of perceptual and reward learning in V1 is often restricted to smaller parts of the visual field (Gilbert et al., 2009; Seitz et al., 2009), less is known about how such plasticity unfolds with respect to the neuronal micro-architecture of V1’s retinotopic organization.

Here, we probe how visuospatial stimulus-reward learning affects the spatial cortical organization of the conditioned stimulus representation. First, we chronically tracked the mesoscale neural representation of the reward-predictive field location within the retinotopic map. Next, we used two-photon calcium imaging to test whether visuospatial information was processed more reliably and efficiently by neurons located at the border between the cortical representations of the rewarded and non-rewarded stimulus.

Results

Head-restrained classical conditioning using spatially confined stimuli

Adult male C57Bl/6 mice were conditioned to associate moving gratings of identical orientation, but presented at different locations in the visual field, with upcoming reward or absence of reward (Figure 1a,b,d and Figure 1—figure supplement 1a–e). Animals were exposed to the conditioning paradigm for 10 to 17 daily sessions of 40 to 60 trials each (Figure 1e). In a conditioning paradigm such as presented here (comparable to trace conditioning), anticipatory nasal and oral movements (unconditioned response, for example licks and sniffs) can emerge in direct relation to the unconditioned stimulus (approach of the reward arm; DeBold et al., 1965; Balsam, 1984; Huerta et al., 2000; Bensafi et al., 2003; Kehoe and Joscelyne, 2005; Drew et al., 2005; Joscelyne and Kehoe, 2007; Arzi et al., 2012; Raybuck and Lattal, 2014). Therefore, we detected and quantified such movements offline (Online Materials and methods; Figure 1c and Figure 1—figure supplement 2a) and plotted them as a function of time relative to arrival of the reward arm in the final position.

Figure 1. Classical conditioning in head-restrained mice.

(a) Schematic depiction of the setup used for classical conditioning. Head-restrained mice faced a computer screen at a 45° angle. Reward (Vanilla dessert) was delivered through a tube on a movable arm (Reward arm). Cameras recorded putative sniffs and licks (Behavior tracking) and eye movements (Eye-tracking). Right: Example of compound stimuli used for conditioning; location (‘top’ or ‘bottom’) predicted reward, orientation of drifting gratings was identical in both locations during conditioning. (b) Outline of the mouse head (arrow, top panel). Reward-delivery (arrow, bottom panel). (c) Example of putative sniff, video-tracked in a small area surrounding the tip of the nose (red box). (d) Schematic showing the sequence of events in a single conditioning trial. Upper line shows stimulus onset and offset. Middle line represents reward arm position for rewarded (red) and non-rewarded (blue) trials. Lower line indicates time. Horizontal arrows indicate jitter in timing. (e) Timeline of the full conditioning experiment in days (‘#d’ indicates number of days; IOS1, IOS2 and IOS3 indicate intrinsic optical imaging time points; Red indicates day of head-bar implantation; Green indicates day of calcium imaging). (f) Peri-reward nasal movements in the first (left panel) and last (right panel) conditioning session. A large peak in nasal movements can be seen during reward delivery (t = 0) and in response to stimulus offset, 1.5 s before (non) reward. Reward-arm movement started at −0.4 s in both rewarded and non-rewarded trials. Insets: Nasal movements in the anticipatory period, when the arm was en-route, but did not exceed the non-reward position (gray box, −0.4 to −0.16 s before reward time). (g) Mean nasal movement in the anticipatory period in the first (top panel) and last (bottom panel) conditioning session. Single lines represent individual mice. (h) Mean anticipatory nasal movements on different days of the conditioning experiment. All panels: Data represent mean ± SEM across mice. Red lines: rewarded trials. Blue lines: non-rewarded trials. *p<0.05, **p<0.01, WMPSR test.

Figure 1.

Figure 1—figure supplement 1. Operational procedures of the experimental apparatus for head-restrained conditioning.

Figure 1—figure supplement 1.

(a) Top panel: Pre-training and handling of a mouse that was implanted with a chronic head bar. Syringe contained liquid reward (Vanilla desert, see Materials and methods). Bottom panel: Zoom in on stainless steel head fixation plate. (b–e) Video monitoring of task-related mouse behavior in rewarded (b–c) and non-rewarded (d–e) trials. (1) Trial onset light (only visible to experimenter); (2) Rewarded or Non-rewarded stimulus; (3) Reward tube in reward or non-reward position; (4) Silhouette of mouse head. (f) Timing of arm movement in rewarded and non-rewarded trials with 40 ms (single video frame) resolution. 'Rew' indicates reward position endpoint, 'nRw' indicates non-reward endpoint of the arm. Left image: Neither position is reached. Middle image: Non-reward position reached. Right image: Reward position reached.
Figure 1—figure supplement 2. Peri-reward oral-movement during visual conditioning.

Figure 1—figure supplement 2.

(a) Example of a putative lick. Licks were detected in the area, delineated by the position of the lower and upper jaw during visually identified licks (red box; manually outlined per video). (b) Peri-reward oral movement (putative licks) in the first conditioning session. A large peak in oral movement indicates reward consumption at the time of reward delivery (t = 0). The inset displays putative licks from −0.5 to 0 s before reward, with the gray region indicating the anticipatory period from −0.4 to −0.16 s before the arm reached the reward position. Red: Rewarded trials; Blue: non-rewarded trials. (c) Same as b) but for the last conditioning session of each mouse. Pre-reward oral-movement was stronger in the anticipatory period of rewarded trials compared to non-rewarded trials (**p<0.01, WMPSR test). (d) Mean oral movement in the anticipatory period of the first training session. Single lines represent individual mice. (e) Mean oral-movement in the anticipatory period of the last conditioning session. All but one animal showed more intense anticipatory oral-movement in rewarded trials compared to non-rewarded trials (**p<0.01, WMPSR test). (f) Difference in oral-movement during the first three and last four conditioning sessions (*p<0.05, **p<0.01; WMPSR test). All data are presented as mean ± SEM across mice.
Figure 1—figure supplement 3. Tracking of pupil diameter and movement during head-restrained conditioning of awake mice.

Figure 1—figure supplement 3.

(a) Example of pupil detection in a single video frame (acquired using a NIR camera) of a mouse eye with an average sized (1) and large sized (4) pupil. The center of the pupil was approximated using a radial symmetry transform (see Materials and methods). From this point outwards, the edge of the pupil was estimated towards all directions (white circles in 2 and 5), except for the direction of the reflection spot (similar to the Starburst algorithm; see Materials and methods). A circular fit (straight lines in 2 and 5) provided the center and diameter of the pupil (3 and 6). (b) Tracking of vertical pupil center in visual conditioning trials of the first training session. Units are in pixels, which correspond roughly to ~2 retinal degrees (see scale bar). Green traces show data from trials with a stimulus in the upper part of the visual field ('Stim. Top') and purple traces are for stimuli in the lower part of the visual field ('Stim. Bottom'; differences were n.s.). Black/white bars from 0 s to 6 s mark visual stimulation. Yellow drops plotted from 8 s to 12 s mark reward period. (c) Idem, but for the last training session. (d) Average vertical pupil movement during the 6 s stimulus period in the first three and last four conditioning sessions (**p<0.01, Mann-Whitney U test across four time points). (e,f,g) Same as in (b), (c) and (d), but for horizontal pupil movement. (h) Pupil diameter in the first training session for trials in which the rewarded stimulus (red) was shown, versus trials in which the non-rewarded stimulus (blue) was shown. (i) Same as h), but for the last day of conditioning. (j) Average pupil dilation during the last 2 s of reward consumption in the first three and last four conditioning sessions (**p<0.01, Mann-Whitney U test across four time points). All data in b) to j) are relative to baseline and presented as average ± SEM across six mice.

From the first conditioning session onwards, putative licks and sniffs emerged aligned to stimulus onset (−6 s), offset (−2 s), reward arm movement onset (−0.5 s) and reward delivery (0 s; Figure 1f, left panel and Figure 1—figure supplement 2b). Initially there were no differences in behavioral responses between rewarded trials and non-rewarded trials, up to the time of reward consumption (Wilcoxon Matched-Pairs Signed-Rank (WMPSR) test; Nasal movement: p=0.32; Oral movement: p=0.90, n = 10 mice; Figure 1f–g and Figure 1—figure supplement 2d). From the second conditioning session onwards, however, a difference in anticipatory oral and nasal movements began to emerge between rewarded and non-rewarded trials in the period of reward-arm movement, defined from −0.4 s to −0.16 s before (non-) reward time (Figure 1f and Figure 1—figure supplement 2c). During this brief time window before reward delivery, no distinction between rewarded and non-rewarded trials could yet be made by anything other than the conditioned stimulus alone, as reward-arm movement trajectory and speed were identical in rewarded and non-rewarded trials up to 55 ms before the time point at which reward was delivered or not (Online Materials and methods; Figure 1—figure supplement 1f). On the last day, all but one mouse showed increased nasal and oral movements during this reward-anticipation period (WMPSR test; Last day; Oral movements: p=0.004; Nasal movements: p=0.004, n = 10 mice; Figure 1g,h and Figure 1—figure supplement 2e,f). No differences in oral and nasal behaviors were found during the period of visual stimulation, which is not unexpected because reward was never available before the reward arm moved to the reward position. Therefore, the conditioning related increases in anticipatory nasal and oral movements during reward arm movement most likely reflect learned expectation of reward delivery.

Eye movements during head-restrained conditioning

Awake behaving rodents display eye movements despite the absence of foveal vision (Sakatani and Isa, 2004; Sakatani and Isa, 2007; Zoccolan et al., 2010; Adesnik et al., 2012; Keller et al., 2012; Wallace et al., 2013; Reimer et al., 2014; Vinck et al., 2015). Although mice track the movement of bars at low temporal frequencies (<1 Hz; van Alphen et al., 2010), it is not known whether they preferentially orient their eyes towards behaviorally relevant stimuli. Large-amplitude eye movements could, in our experiments, deteriorate retinotopic selectivity of visual stimuli. Therefore, pupil location and pupil diameter were tracked in six animals over the entire course of the behavioral experiment (Figure 1—figure supplement 3a). This revealed a slight but systematic bias of pupil orientation towards the location of a presented stimulus along the vertical axis (Data combined across last 4 days, WMPSR test, Vertical: p=0.00091, Horizontal: p=0.39, n = 6 mice; Figure 1—figure supplement 3b–g). Nonetheless, the amplitude of the shift in vertical eye position did not differ by more than 1.0 retinal degrees (Online Materials and methods; Figure 1—figure supplement 3c,d). Given that V1 receptive fields in the mouse cover much larger regions (i.e. diameter ≧10 retinal degrees; Smith and Häusser, 2010; Bonin et al., 2011) and that the conditioned stimulus size was 30 retinal degrees (Azimuth and Elevation), this subtle variation in eye position did not strongly reduce the retinotopic specificity of the visual stimulus during conditioning.

During the reward delivery period, eye movements and pupil dilations were more pronounced. The eye position showed a downward gaze-shift, potentially to the incoming reward spout (Figure 1—figure supplement 3c). Dilation of the pupil may have indicated arousal as a consequence of reward consumption (Data combined across last 4 days: WMPSR test, p=7.1⋅10−5, n = 6 mice; Figure 1—figure supplement 3i,j; Bradley et al., 2008). Thus, despite the lack of foveal vision, head restrained mice make eye-movement responses and show pupillary reactions to behaviorally relevant stimuli, like rewards. Differences in these behaviors between rewarded and non-rewarded trials were large during the reward delivery phase, while during stimulus presentation differences between the upper and lower visual field were negligible.

Mesoscopic shifts in cortical representations of trained stimuli

To follow the cortical retinotopic organization throughout learning, we used repeated transcranial imaging of intrinsic optical signals in area V1 of anesthetized mice. The retinotopic pattern of the mesoscale cortical response was quantified by reducing the spatial dimensionality of the intrinsic response maps to the axis that maximally separated the two stimulus locations (Online Materials and methods; Figure 2a–b and Figure 2—figure supplement 1a). To account for variation between sessions, the response to trained stimuli was referenced to the response to control stimuli that were presented in the same retinotopic location (and had an orientation that was not presented in the conditioning sessions; for example gray curves in Figure 2d and Figure 2—figure supplement 1b). Using this method, we observed that the response amplitude to the non-rewarded, trained stimulus was decreased after conditioning when compared to before conditioning (amplitude of non-rewarded minus orthogonal, across time points: Kruskal-Wallis, normalized per mouse, H(2, 24)=13.15, p=0.0014, post hoc WMPSR test, Before vs. After, p=0.0039; n = 9 mice; Figure 2d,g and Figure 2—figure supplement 1b). The response amplitude to the rewarded conditioned stimulus was not significantly reduced at the time point after conditioning (amplitude of rewarded minus orthogonal, mouse-normalized, across time points: Kruskal-Wallis, normalized per mouse, H(2, 24)=6.39, p=0.041, post hoc WMPSR test, Before vs. After, p=0.57, n = 9 mice).

Figure 2. Repeated imaging of intrinsic optical signals in V1 of mice subjected to conditioning.

(a) Visual stimuli (left panels) and average stimulus-induced intrinsic optical signal (IOS; right panels), showing strong retinotopically selective responses to the rewarded and non-rewarded location in area V1 (within red boundary) and weaker responses in the lateral supplementary visual areas LM and AL (to left of red box). Orth, orientation orthogonal to trained orientation (Cond). (b) Left panels: Example of 11-pixel wide cutout from the red box in a, that was automatically rotated so as to include the cortical area of maximal activation for both the top and bottom stimulus location. Right panels: The IOS response to each stimulus as a function of cortical space (maximally separating the top and bottom stimulus) and time (derived from the 11-pixel wide cutouts by collapsing the ‘x’ dimension per imaging frame and concatenating time points). Upper panels: Response to rewarded location and orientation. Lower panels: Response to non-rewarded location and orientation. (c) Schematic showing quantification of the response amplitude and overlap of cortical responses to visual stimuli in adjacent retinotopic locations. (d) Mean ± SEM z-scored amplitude of the IOS response (across the period of maximum activation, 7 to 11 s) as a function of cortical space along the axis that maximally separates the rewarded and non-rewarded stimulus. Red: Rewarded stimulus; Blue: Non-rewarded, trained stimulus; Grey: Orthogonal orientations at the rewarded or non-rewarded location respectively. Left panel: Data acquired before the first conditioning session (day 0; IOS1, see also Figure 1e). Middle panel: After five conditioning sessions (IOS2). Right panel: Data acquired after the last conditioning session on the same day as, but before, calcium imaging (IOS3). (e) Mean overlap (across all mice) between the response to the orthogonal or trained stimuli in the rewarded and non-rewarded locations as a function of cortical space (y-axis) and time (x-axis). Scale bar above applies for both panels. (f) As in d), but for the overlap between the response to the rewarded and non-rewarded trained stimulus (plotted in Violet). (g) ΔResponse amplitude (difference between trained and control orientations) in the cortical region that responded to the rewarded location (red) and non-rewarded location (blue) separately, across imaging time points (**p=0.0039, Kruskal-Wallis, post hoc WPMSR test). (h) As in g), but for ΔOverlap per region and across imaging time points (*p=0.039, WMPSR test; **p=0.011, Kruskal-Wallis test, post hoc WMPSR test). (i) As in g), but for ΔSpatial selectivity (Rewarded vs. Non-rewarded: *p=0.039, WMPSR test; Before vs. After: *p=0.046, Kruskal-Wallis test). (j) Distance between the peak responses to the spatially segregated stimuli (difference between trained and orthogonal control stimuli, positive values indicate larger distance between trained stimulus representation peaks).

Figure 2.

Figure 2—figure supplement 1. Repeated intrinsic optical signal imaging in V1 of a mouse subjected to conditioning.

Figure 2—figure supplement 1.

(a) Example data of mouse #15. Left panels: Visual stimuli used for this mouse. Middle panels: Average stimulus-induced intrinsic optical signal (IOS), showing retinotopically selective responses to the rewarded (Rew) and non-rewarded (nRw) location in area V1 (within red boundary), but also in the lateral supplementary visual area LM (situated left of the red box). Right panels: Single image frames (2 Hz sampling rate) from a single trial for each stimulus. On top, black bars and arrow above indicate time of stimulus presentation. (b) Intrinsic signal imaging data across nine mice. Mean ± SEM amplitude of the inverted z-scored IOS response to each of the stimuli, plotted against time. Stimulus onset is at t = 4 s. Red: Rewarded stimulus; Blue: Non-rewarded stimulus; Grey: Orthogonal orientations at the rewarded or non-rewarded location respectively. Left column: Data acquired before the first conditioning session (day 0; IOS1). Middle column: After five conditioning sessions (IOS2). Right column: Data acquired after the last conditioning session on the same day as, but before, calcium imaging (IOS3).

The change in response amplitude was associated with an altered spatial distribution of the intrinsic signal response to the trained stimuli (see Figure 2d). In order to further quantify this, we calculated how much the intrinsic response to visual stimulation in each of the two adjacent locations of the trained stimuli overlapped with the other on the cortical surface (Figure 2c,e) and compared this with how much the response to the orthogonal control stimuli overlapped (difference between gray and colored lines in Figure 2f). This difference between spatial overlap of trained and spatial overlap of control stimuli (IOS ΔOverlap) inversely reflects how well the intrinsic responses to trained stimuli are segregated in cortical space, as compared to the control stimuli. After conditioning (i.e., on day 11–18), the IOS ΔOverlap was significantly reduced in the cortical region that responded to the rewarded stimulus (Kruskal-Wallis test, normalized per mouse, H(2,24) = 10.00, p=0.0067, post hoc WMPSR test, Before vs. After, p=0.020, n = 9 mice; Figure 2f,h). In the region responding to the non-rewarded stimulus, the IOS ΔOverlap was not significantly reduced as a function of training (Kruskal-Wallis test, normalized per mouse, H(2,24) = 0.98, p=0.61, n = 9 mice; Figure 2f,h). After training, the IOS ΔOverlap in the cortical region representing the rewarded stimulus was significantly reduced compared to the region mostly responsive to the non-rewarded stimulus (WSRMP test, p=0.039, n = 9 mice; Figure 2f,h).

Because the measure of overlap is sensitive to the absolute response amplitude, we also quantified the mesoscopic segregation of intrinsic cortical responses using a ratiometric index, the spatial selectivity index. This showed a similar effect of sharpened spatial selectivity for the rewarded condition and location (Kruskal-Wallis test, mouse-normalized, H(2,24) = 6.16, p=0.046, WMPSR test, rewarded vs. non-rewarded, p=0.039, n = 9 mice; Figure 2i). The magnitude of these above-mentioned effects (‘IOS-ΔOverlap’, ‘selectivity for stimulus location’ and ‘response amplitude to the non-rewarded trained stimulus’) correlated strongly within individual mice (IOS-ΔOverlap × Selectivity: r = −0.94, p=0.0002, n = 9 mice; Amplitude × Selectivity: r = 0.81, p=0.008, n = 9 mice; Amplitude × ΔOverlap: r = −0.65, p=0.058, n = 9 mice), which suggests that they resulted from closely related mechanisms that operated on the rewarded as well as the non-rewarded side of the stimulus representation.

Finally, to test whether the representation of the rewarded and non-rewarded trained stimuli drifted apart in cortical space, we quantified the distance between the peaks in the intrinsic response profiles. Despite a trend, this measure of spatial distance between trained stimulus representations did not significantly increase (Kruskal-Wallis test, normalized per mouse, H(2,24) = 0.57, p=0.75, n = 9 mice; Figure 2j). Hence, classical conditioning, using rewarded and non-rewarded stimuli that occupy neighboring regions of visual space, selectively reshaped the retinotopic organization for the trained stimuli compared to control stimuli, while the overall representations of the trained stimuli did not significantly drift apart.

Spatial organization of neuronal population activity for trained stimuli

To understand how formation of a visuospatial stimulus-reward association affects neuronal response properties within the retinotopic representation, we performed OGB1-calcium imaging (Stosiek et al., 2003) after the final conditioning session (Figure 3a). Recordings were made in multiple V1-subregions per animal, positioned along the rewarded/non-rewarded axis within V1 (Figure 3b). Every field-of-view was assigned to a cortical location group; Full non-rewarded, Border non-rewarded, Border rewarded and Full rewarded, based on the population response amplitude to visual stimulation in each of the retinotopic locations using moving gratings of non-trained orientations (Online Materials and methods; Figure 3b and Figure 3—figure supplement 1).

Figure 3. Two-photon imaging of orientation-tuned responses in different retinotopic regions of V1.

(a) Example of calcium imaging in a field of view that was located in the Border non-rewarded region. Left: Overview image and inset showing neurons loaded with the calcium indicator OGB1-AM (green) and double-labeled astrocytes (yellow). Right panels 1–4 correspond to ROI’s 1–4 in inset a. Left columns of all 4 ROI response panels: Fitted tuning curves for movement orientation/direction (Red: visual stimulus in rewarded visual field; Blue: visual stimulus in non-rewarded visual field) and mean (±SEM) response to each of the eight directions (black vertical bars). Traces on right: Average ∆F/F time courses for each of the eight movement directions separately (Grey shaded columns: trained orientation). (b) Top-view scheme of the imaging locations on the visual cortex. The dashed purple line represents the ‘rewarded/non-rewarded axis’. The boundary between the rewarded and non-rewarded stimulus representations is at location index value ‘0’ and marked with a dashed purple arrow (see Online Materials and methods). ‘A’ indicates the approximate location of the field of view in a. (c) Calcium-imaging derived ∆Overlap in each of the location bins. Overlaid gray line represents mean (±SEM, shaded region) of the overlap measured after conditioning using intrinsic imaging (*p<0.05, **p<0.01, Kruskal-Wallis, post-hoc Mann-Whitney U test). This overlap was computed as in Figure 2. (d) Fraction of orientation-tuned neurons preferring the rewarded (red), non-rewarded (blue) or orthogonal (lighter shaded colors) stimuli (*p<0.05, Anova, post-hoc t-test). (e) Average response amplitude in ∆F/F (%) of significantly orientation-tuned neurons to their respective preferred orientations, per location bin. Red: Cells preferring the rewarded orientation. Light red: Cells preferring the orthogonal stimulus in the rewarded location. Blue and light blue: The same, but for the non-rewarded stimulus location. Crossed arrow indicates Anova interaction effect,+P < 0.05. Inset on the right shows the mean (±SEM) response amplitude as measured in the last intrinsic imaging session (see Figure 2d).

Figure 3.

Figure 3—figure supplement 1. Binning of recording location into discrete groups.

Figure 3—figure supplement 1.

(a) Histogram of the number of recording sessions with an imaging location index (see Online Materials and methods for details) ranging from −1 (Non-rewarded side, nRw) to +1 (Rewarded side, Rew), classified as ‘Full non-rewarded’, ‘Border non-rewarded’, ‘Border rewarded', ‘Full rewarded’. The width of the bar is indicative of the range of included session location indices. (b) Mean imaging location index in each of the location bins showing a linear increase of location selectivity.

Using this location on the rewarded/non-rewarded axis, we compared findings obtained using intrinsic signal imaging with calcium imaging data. First, we calculated the measure Ca2+ ΔOverlap, which was defined as the overlap between the rewarded- and non-rewarded stimulus-evoked calcium response, using mean cellular calcium responses to the trained orientations for each imaging field. These overlap values were averaged across imaging fields per cortical location bin and subtracted by the (identically calculated) overlap of the orthogonal control stimuli. The Ca2+ ΔOverlap was significantly lower for imaging fields in the full rewarded region and at the rewarded/non-rewarded location border, compared to field-of-views that were located fully in the non-rewarded stimulus region (Kruskal-Wallis test, H(3,45) = 11.6, p=0.0088, n = 49 imaging fields; Figure 3c). The pattern of overlap in location selective responses based on calcium imaging data resembled the results obtained using intrinsic signal imaging (Gray line in Figure 3c indicates IOS ΔOverlap as in Figure 2d), showing the strongest reduction in Overlap (as compared to control) in the Border rewarded and Border non-rewarded regions. Also selectivity for retinotopic location, now calculated using calcium imaging data, was larger in the Full rewarded and Border rewarded regions, as compared to the Full non-rewarded region (Kruskal-Wallis test, H(3,45) = 19.3, p=0.0024, n = 49 imaging fields). The measured ΔOverlap in the rewarded stimulus location correlated strongly and significantly across methods (IOS vs. Ca2+, r = 0.81, p=0.016, n = 8 mice) and a weaker, but similarly positive correlation was found for retinotopic location selectivity (r = 0.69, p=0.058, n = 8 mice).

If the spatial segregation of the cortical population responses was driven by local map expansion, this would predict a larger fraction of neurons being tuned to rewarded or non-rewarded stimulus orientations compared to orthogonal orientations. The fraction of responsive neurons per stimulus orientation and location can be estimated only from the calcium imaging data acquired at single cell resolution and cannot be directly compared to intrinsic imaging data. Interestingly, the fraction of responsive cells per stimulus orientation was significantly reduced in the Full rewarded location and did not differ significantly between the Border rewarded and Border non-rewarded locations (2-way Anova, interaction effect of Location bin versus Preferred orientation, F(3,90) = 3.99, p=0.010; n = 49 recordings; Post hoc, Trained versus Orthogonal orientation for full-rewarded location: p<0.05; Figure 3d). Instead, we observed that the population response amplitude to rewarded stimuli increased more steeply from Border non-rewarded locations to Border rewarded locations compared to the increase of the population response to non-rewarded stimuli from Border rewarded to Border non-rewarded locations (2-way Anova, interaction effect of location vs. population response difference, Trained stimuli: F(3,85) = 3.59, p=0.017, Untrained stimuli: F(3,85) = 1.48, p=0.23; n = 49 recordings; Figure 3e). While the response amplitude to the non-rewarded trained orientation appeared also smaller compared to the non-rewarded control orientation, similar to what we observed in the intrinsic imaging data (see Figure 3e, inset right), this difference was not significant. Nonetheless, the reduction in response amplitude (trained orientation minus orthogonal control), as obtained using calcium imaging, did covary with the reduction in response amplitude that was observed in the intrinsic imaging data (IOS vs. Ca2+, r = 0.92, p=0.028, n = 5 mice for which we had both intrinsic imaging data and calcium imaging recordings from the non-rewarded location).

These results suggest that the observed mesoscopic disambiguation of the representation of the rewarded and non-rewarded stimulus (as observed using intrinsic imaging) is expressed at the single neuron level (as observed using calcium imaging) and is potentially mediated by a spatially restricted change in response patterns of a select population of neurons, rather than an overall expansion of the cortical map for rewarded or non-rewarded stimuli.

The rewarded conditioned stimulus drives a smaller population of orientation-tuned neurons with larger response amplitudes

To investigate how response patterns of orientation-tuned neurons were altered after conditioning, we quantified tuning curve parameters for individual neurons as a function of their imaging location (Full vs Border, with rewarded/non-rewarded pooled), whether the cell responded to the rewarded or non-rewarded stimulus location (rewarded vs non-rewarded, with Full/Border pooled), and the similarity of the neuron’s preferred orientation to the trained orientation. While the populations of orientation-tuned cells located in Border- and Full imaging regions did not show significant differences in fraction of neurons being tuned to trained and other orientations, response amplitude and tuning curve bandwidth (Kruskal-Wallis test, p>0.05; n = 11 mice; Figure 4—figure supplement 1), we observed significant differences between these parameters for cells that responded to the rewarded and non-rewarded stimulus location.

On average, the percentage of cells that preferred the rewarded and non-rewarded stimulus location was similar, although not identical (total: 32.2%, see Table 1 for fraction per mouse; rewarded: 17.8%, non-rewarded: 14.5%; WMPSR-test, p=0.58, n = 11 mice; Figure 4a). However, the relative fractions of cells preferring trained and other orientations differed significantly between groups that were tuned to rewarded and non-rewarded locations (these fractions were normalized to the overall fraction of responsive neurons per mouse, across all preferred orientations and locations and adjusted for relative bin width so that the expected fraction given a flat distribution of preferred orientations was 1.0; Figure 4b,c). Specifically, for cells tuned to the rewarded location there was a relatively smaller fraction of cells preferring the trained orientation, compared to the set of cells that responded to the non-rewarded (i.e. orthogonal) orientation (Kruskal-Wallis test, ‘Rewarded−Non-rewarded’ vs. ‘ΔOri. from trained’, H(4,50) = 9.76, p=0.045; post hoc WMPSR-test, ‘0°–10°’ vs. ‘50°–70°’, p=0.042, n = 11 mice; Figure 4d). This effect corresponds to what is shown in Figure 3d, in which we observed fewer neurons being responsive to the trained stimulus in the Full rewarded cortical region compared to the orthogonal control orientation.

Table 1. Overview of mice included in behavioral and imaging analyses.

Each row lists the following information for the mouse that can be identified by the number in the column Mouse. Rew: Location of the rewarded stimulus. nRw: Location of the non-rewarded stimulus. Dir: Direction of the moving grating used in the conditioning paradigm. Beh-track: Data for which behavior was video-tracked and included in behavioral analysis (Figure 1 and Figure 1—figure supplement 2). # trials: Total number of trials that a mouse performed across all conditioning sessions. Eye: Data included in analysis of eye-movements (Figure 1—figure supplement 3). IOS: Data included in analysis of intrinsic optical signals (Figure 2 and Figure 2—figure supplement 1). Ca2+ Full: Data included in calcium imaging analysis, 'Full imaging locations'. Ca2+ Border: Data included in calcium imaging analysis, 'Border imaging locations'. (Figures 36, Figure 3—figure supplement 1, Figure 4—figure supplement 1). % tuned: Overall fraction of orientation tuned neurons per mouse.

Mouse Rew nRw Dir Beh- track # trials Eye IOS Ca2+
Full
Ca2+
Border
% tuned
#02 Bottom Top 180° 30.9
#03 Top Bottom 10.9
#05 Bottom Top 68.8
#07 Top Bottom 90° 613 20.2
#08 Top Bottom 270° 378
#09 Bottom Top 180° 640 36.9
#10 Bottom Top 180° 575 40.6
#11 Top Bottom 180° 436 32.8
#12 Top Bottom 520 36.9
#13 Bottom Top 90° 595
#14 Bottom Top 270° 640 40.7
#15 Top Bottom 520 40.5
#16 Bottom Top 480 7.2
Total 10 6 9 11 9

Figure 4. Parameters describing orientation-tuned neurons preferring the rewarded or non-rewarded stimulus location.

(a) Left panel: Mean ± SEM percentage of significantly orientation-tuned cells per mouse. ‘A’: All locations. ‘R’: Rewarded location. ‘N’: Non-rewarded location. (b) Absolute fraction of orientation-tuned neurons responding to the rewarded (red) and non-rewarded (blue) location separately, and relative to the trained orientation (e.g. neurons with a preferred orientation nearly identical to the trained orientation were in bin 0°−10°). Bars above data points indicate relative bin-widths. Note that because the left-most bin (0–10 degrees) is only half the width of the other bins, it is expected to contain half of the fraction of the other bins. (c) Distribution (normalized per mouse and bin-width) of preferred orientations relative to the trained orientation (given a flat distribution of preferred orientations, the expected fraction of each bin would be 1.0). (d) Difference in fraction of orientation tuned neurons to the rewarded minus the non-rewarded location (positive values indicate a larger fraction of neurons tuned to the rewarded stimulus location; *p=0.042, Kruskal-Wallis test, post hoc WMPSR test). (e) The mean (±SEM) ∆F/F response to the preferred direction as a function of the preferred orientation relative to the trained orientation for rewarded and non-rewarded location-tuned neurons separately (*p<0.05, WMPSR test). The lines that fall between 0% to 0.5% ∆F/F show the response amplitude of the same neurons, but to the orientation that was orthogonal to the preferred orientation. We note that the sequence of bins is thus not representative of the entire tuning curve but shows the response amplitude to a specific (preferred or orthogonal) direction. (f) Same as e), but for bandwidth (*p<0.05, WMPSR test). (g) As in e), but for sparseness of the tuning curve response calculated across the population all neurons. For all panels: Red colors indicate groups of cells preferring ‘rewarded’ conditioned orientations, while blue colors indicate groups of cells preferring ‘non-rewarded’ trained orientations. Both colors fade to gray, indicating that the preferred orientation of the cells becomes more dissimilar from the trained orientation.

Figure 4.

Figure 4—figure supplement 1. Comparison of orientation tuning parameters between cells located close and far away from the rewarded/non-rewarded border.

Figure 4—figure supplement 1.

(a) Distribution of preferred orientation of orientation-tuned neurons relative to the trained orientation (mean per bin, averaged ± SEM across mice). 'A': All locations. 'F': Full imaging regions. 'B': Border imaging regions. (b) The mean (±SEM, averaged across mice) ∆F/F response to the preferred direction as a function of the preferred orientation relative to the trained orientation. (c) Idem, but for the tuning curve bandwidth. For all panels, solid lines: Data from the Full rewarded and Full non-rewarded regions of the visual cortex. Dashed lines: Data from the Border rewarded and Border non-rewarded regions of the visual cortex. All performed tests compared data from Full and Border regions across mice, no significant differences were observed.

Although the fraction of cells tuned to the conditioned orientation at the rewarded stimulus location was reduced, their response amplitude to the trained orientation was increased compared to cells responding to the non-rewarded location (Kruskal-Wallis test, ‘Rewarded−Non-rewarded’ vs. ‘ΔOri. from trained’, H(4,48) = 11.6, p=0.020; n = 11 mice; Figure 4e). Moreover, neurons with tuning curves that flanked the preferred orientation (ΔOri. from trained between 10° and 30°) had broader tuning curves (Kruskal-Wallis test, ‘ΔOri. from trained’, H(4,48) = 14.1, p=0.0068; Post hoc WMPSR test, ‘0°–10°’ vs. ‘10°–30°’, p=0.0039; n = 11 mice; Figure 4f). The latter effect indicates that neurons with preferred orientations that slightly differ from the trained orientation increased their response to the trained orientation (regardless of the reward-location association), rendering their orientation tuning curves broader (Goltstein et al., 2013). The orientation selectivity index did not significantly differ between rewarded and non-rewarded tuned neurons, or as function of angle between preferred orientation and the trained orientation (Kruskal-Wallis test, p>0.16; n = 11 mice). When we performed these analyses on tuning curves of the neurons that were not significantly tuned, we did not observe any of the above-described differences.

In order to test whether the differences described above reflected an overall sparser population response to rewarded conditioned stimuli, we quantified population sparseness (ap, lower values indicate a sparser population response; Rolls and Treves, 2011) for the tuning curve response of all neurons. The population response to trained stimuli in the rewarded location did not show a significantly lower sparseness compared to the population response to visual stimulation in the non-rewarded location (Kruskal-Wallis test, ‘ΔOri. from trained’, H(4,50) = 0.23, p=0.99; n = 11 mice; Figure 4g).

These differences in response properties of orientation-tuned neurons indicate that, although fewer neurons were significantly responsive to the conditioned stimulus orientation, the individual neurons that responded to the rewarded stimulus did so more strongly.

Improved neuronal population coding for trained stimulus location

Thus far our data obtained using intrinsic signal imaging as well as calcium imaging indicate that, after training, the cortical map for trained stimulus location and orientation became more selective for retinotopic position. We therefore hypothesized that populations of simultaneously recorded single neurons would show an improved ability to discriminate the location of trained stimuli as compared to the (same) locations of untrained control stimuli. To test this, we used a Bayesian algorithm with cross-validation to decode stimulus location from single trial responses to the very stimuli that were used in the conditioning paradigm. Performance of the decoding algorithm for those stimuli was compared with that for stimuli having orientations differing 45 or 90 degrees from the trained orientation (untrained stimuli).

First, we decoded calcium responses recorded in field-of-views located around the rewarded/non-rewarded border on the cortical surface (see Figures 3b and 5a, inset left). In this population of significantly tuned Border neurons, decoding of stimulus location was significantly better for the trained orientation as compared to the orthogonal orientation (WMPSR test, difference between ‘trained’ and ‘orthogonal orientation’ averaged across all sample sizes per mouse, p=0.0078; n = 9 mice; Figure 5a, left panel). Decoding of activity patterns from cells recorded in regions further away from the border (see Figure 5a, right panel) did not differ between trained and untrained orientations (WMPSR test, difference between ‘trained’ and ‘orthogonal orientation’ averaged across all sample sizes per mouse, p=0.46; n = 11 mice; Figure 5a, right panel).

Figure 5. Population coding of visual field location by visual cortex neurons.

Figure 5.

(a) Left panel: Performance of decoding retinotopic location using activity of simultaneously recorded neurons in regions close to the rewarded/non-rewarded location border as a function of increasing population size. Chance level is 50%. Inset illustrates which field-of-view locations were included in these data. Dark purple curves are data from the population with a preferred orientation similar to the trained orientation. Light purple curves are data from neurons preferring orientations differing 45° from the trained orientation. Gray curves are for the data from cells preferring orientations orthogonal to the trained orientation. Data are shown as mean ± SEM averaged across 9 mice. **p<0.01, WMPSR test. Right panel: Same, but for neurons recorded in regions further away from the rewarded/non-rewarded location border. (b) Per mouse, the mean conditioning related-improvement in coding for visual field location was plotted against the behavioral difference in nasal (upper panel) and oral movement (lower panel) to the rewarded (Rew) and non-rewarded (nRw) stimulus location.

Data from six separate animals contributed both behavioral measurements and Border-region imaging fields of view. Of these, five mice showed better decoding of trained stimulus location compared to untrained orientations, while a single animal did not show a difference. Moreover, the magnitude of the difference in decoding performance between trained and other (45° and 90°) orientations correlated significantly with the magnitude of the difference in anticipatory nasal and oral movements in the behavioral paradigm (Nasal movements: r = 0.83, p=0.041; Oral movements: r = 0.96, p=0.003; n = 6 mice; Figure 5b). Decoding performance did not correlate significantly with the total number of conditioning trials that the mice were exposed to (Total number of trials: r = −0.18, p=0.73; n = 6 mice). The single mouse that did not show a significant improvement in decoding performance also failed to show an increase of both anticipatory nasal- and oral-movements in the behavioral paradigm, while it was still exposed to 640 conditioning trials (see Table 1, mouse #09).

Conditioning-related suppression of noise correlations in high signal correlation pairs

Effects of conditioning were observed in a subset of similarly tuned neurons within the overall heterogeneously tuned group of V1 neurons. We assessed the degree to which cells in these subgroups can be seen as functional units by comparing similarity of tuning curves (signal correlation) and stimulus-independent covariations (noise correlation; see for example Averbeck et al., 2006; Montijn et al., 2016). Signal and noise correlations were calculated on tuning curves of all simultaneously recorded pairs of cells that were either responsive to the rewarded or non-rewarded location and grouped according to the similarity of their preferred orientations relative to the trained orientation, and whether they were located in the Full or Border stimulus region. However, there were no significant differences in average signal correlations and noise correlations between the trained and control orientations (Bootstrap confidence intervals, p>0.05; Figure 6a).

Figure 6. Conditioning related interactions of signal and noise correlations.

Figure 6.

(a) Signal- and noise correlations for pairs of simultaneously recorded neurons as a function of the difference between the preferred orientation of the neurons and the trained orientation. Left panel: Data from pairs that were located further away from the region where the rewarded and non-rewarded stimuli bordered. Right panel: As left, but from pairs located in the cortical region where the representations of the rewarded and non-rewarded stimuli bordered. Solid lines: Signal correlations. Dashed lines: Noise correlations. Data are shown as mean ± error bars that indicate 95% confidence intervals. (b) Noise correlations for the selection of simultaneously recorded pairs that exhibited the 10% highest signal correlations (solid lines) and the pairs having the 10% lowest signal correlations (dashed lines). Left panel: data from the Full rewarded and Full non-rewarded regions. Right panel: data from Border rewarded and non-rewarded regions. Data are shown as mean ± error bars that indicate 95% confidence intervals (*p<0.05, **p<0.01 multiple comparison corrected bootstrap confidence intervals).

Interactions between signal and noise correlations can hamper or facilitate efficient population coding. For instance, population coding benefits when cells with high signal correlations have low noise correlations and vice versa (Oram et al., 1998; Averbeck et al., 2006; Poort and Roelfsema, 2009; Jeanne et al., 2013). To investigate whether such an effect occurred in our data, we selected pairs of cells that displayed the 10% highest and 10% lowest signal correlations and calculated noise correlations for these pairs separately (Jeanne et al., 2013). In the border imaging regions, high signal correlation pairs that were tuned to the trained orientations exhibited significantly lower noise correlations compared to pairs preferring other orientations (Bootstrap confidence intervals, p<0.01; nborder = 20 imaging fields of view; Figure 6b, right panel, solid lines). In addition, in the Full imaging regions, we observed that low signal correlation pairs had stronger noise correlations when they were tuned to the trained orientations as compared to the control orientations (Bootstrap confidence intervals, p<0.05; nfull = 24 imaging fields of view; Figure 6b, left panel, dotted lines). Thus, the effect of classical conditioning on noise correlations depends on the signal correlation of the pair of neurons, which can have the overall effect of enhancing the discriminability of trained stimuli in the population.

Discussion

Associative learning is known to affect neural responses across a range of spatial scales (Jehee et al., 2012; Bao et al., 2001; Goltstein et al., 2013), but how learning affects the spatial dimension of sensory cortical maps has remained elusive. Our results indicate how associative learning (by coupling a visual stimulus to reward) affects orientation-specific coding of single neurons in a retinotopically restricted fashion but fails to lead to an overall expansion of the cortical region representing the trained visual space, thus leaving the retinotopic map intact. These data demonstrate that learning-induced changes in single neurons emerge in concert with enhanced stimulus representations at the population level increasing location discriminability within the V1 cortical map.

Considerations regarding imaging methodology

The central finding in this study is improved coding for stimulus location by a population of neurons that preferred the trained orientation and was located within the cortical retinotopic region where the trained stimuli bordered. This enhanced selectivity for retinotopic location was observed using both a mesoscale (intrinsic imaging) and a microscale (calcium imaging) method. The data acquired using intrinsic imaging suggested that this effect was mediated by a reduction in response amplitude to the non-rewarded trained stimulus (Figure 2d,g). While visibly present, this reduction was less pronounced when observed at the single cell resolution using calcium imaging (Figure 3e). The difference in results may be inherent to the difference in methods, with intrinsic signal imaging picking up a widespread mixed, metabolic signal across cortical neurons (including dendrites, axons and synapses) and layers, and with calcium imaging focusing on somatic spiking activity in layer 2/3 neurons selectively. That the magnitude of reduction in trained stimulus-representation Overlap, as measured with each method (Figure 2f and Figure 3c), was correlated across mice suggests that the effects are related. What was observed at the mesoscale as an overall change in response amplitude, may in part reflect a subtler change in stimulus selectivity or jointly orchestrated activity patterns at the single neuron, somatic level within cortical layer 2/3.

It is of note that the results obtained with calcium imaging, such as differences in fraction and amplitude of responsive neurons and changes in population decoding for retinotopic location, were observed in comparison to untrained control orientations or control regions, and not compared to a hypothetical time point before learning. Therefore, it cannot be fully excluded that these effects were mediated, in part, by changes of the opposite sign in neurons that were tuned to untrained control orientations. However, some effects of conditioning were quite selective for the trained orientation as compared to different untrained control (i.e. oblique and orthogonal) orientations (see e.g. Figures 4e and 5). While this does not fully confirm that learning effects were only present in neurons tuned to the trained orientation, it indicates that, at least, the trained orientation stood out from the way the broader population of untrained stimuli was processed.

Sharper spatial tuning within the V1 retinotopic map

Cortical representations, as also found in the auditory cortical tonotopic map or the somatosensory homunculus, have been known to exhibit plasticity (Bakin and Weinberger, 1996; Buonomano and Merzenich, 1998; Bao et al., 2001). An example of map plasticity like observed here, however, has not been found before in visual cortex, where shifts in retinotopic organization were, until now, described as a consequence of selectively depriving cortical input only (Heinen and Skavenski, 1991; Gilbert and Wiesel, 1992; Keck et al., 2008). The kind of refinement of the retinotopic map as shown in our experiments—a sharper retinotopic gradient (Figure 3e) in calcium response patterns coupled to a reduced overlap in intrinsic optical responses (Figure 2e,f,h) and better location-decoding performance (Figure 5a)—is somewhat comparable to emerging discontinuities in the cortical digit representation after separation of webbed fingers (Mogilner et al., 1993) or fading discontinuities in the somatosensory map after surgical syndactyly (Allard et al., 1991). However, map-plasticity in these studies emerged by self-organizing plasticity, while in our study, appetitive conditioning directed the alterations in the retinotopic map. Not only is the treatment (learning paradigm) more subtle than the interventions in the studies mentioned above, but the observed map-plasticity underscores that also under naturalistic conditions stimulus-reward learning can direct functional changes in cortical maps, expressed in the way the cells in these maps encode sensory information.

The changes in neuronal tuning observed in our study can also be considered in the context of perceptual learning. Training to discriminate small differences in stimulus parameters can result in sharpening of tuning curves in the primary visual cortex (Schoups et al., 2001), an effect that has been suggested to help disambiguate fine differences in stimulus orientation. The presently reported learning-induced reduction in overlap of the population response to trained stimuli resembles such a mechanism of tuning curve sharpening, but now pertains to the cortical mapping of visual space. While our study did not directly compare the conditioned group with a sham-trained (randomly rewarded) group, we did observe a correlation between the amount of anticipatory behavior and changes in neuronal population coding (Figure 5), but no correlation to the number of trials. This argues that the observed effects are not merely the result of stimulus exposure but are associated with behavioral learning in the conditioning paradigm.

Mechanisms underlying learning-induced map plasticity in V1

Neuronal learning mechanisms underlying classic forms of map plasticity are generally thought to be Hebbian in nature (Buonomano and Merzenich, 1998), functioning through rapid induction of LTP and LTD for spared and deprived inputs respectively or via competition between inputs (Feldman and Brecht, 2005). Additionally, local secretion of neurotransmitters like acetylcholine and noradrenaline plays a role (Bear and Singer, 1986). In auditory cortex for instance, large shifts in tonotopic representations have been observed after pairing a tone with basal forebrain stimulation (Bakin and Weinberger, 1996; Kilgard and Merzenich, 1998). In the current study, we did not observe such a large-scale expansion of the conditioned stimulus representation, but rather an increase in selectivity of the cortical map. Recent work in the primary visual cortex of the mouse has established that cholinergic neuromodulation can act to improve neuronal selectivity and perception in a rapid and task specific way (Pinto et al., 2013). Moreover, during an appetitive conditioning paradigm, cholinergic input from basal forebrain to V1 induced strong local activity patterns leading up to the time of reward delivery (Liu et al., 2015), which may drive stimulus-selective plasticity (Chubykin et al., 2013). The plasticity in V1’s retinotopic map, which we induced here using appetitive conditioning, may therefore have been mediated by local release of acetylcholine. However, the strengthening of feedback connections from higher visual or other cortices (Pennartz, 1997; Makino and Komiyama, 2015) could provide an alternative or additional mechanism by which learning leads to changes in cortical maps. In addition, the observed reduction in response amplitude to the non-rewarded trained stimulus could indicate a role for an LTD type mechanism reducing excitatory drive of non-rewarded stimulus representation (Nabavi et al., 2014). Alternatively, it could reflect strengthening of long-range lateral connections from for example the rewarded retinotopic location, or feed-back from higher visual areas, onto inhibitory neurons in the non-rewarded retinotopic location to increase local inhibitory drive of the non-rewarded stimulus representation (Makino and Komiyama, 2015).

While neuromodulation and long-range connectivity are strong candidates to mediate learning in our behavioral paradigm, the conditioning-related improvement in retinotopic selectivity was observed under anesthesia, which makes it likely that these results depend to some degree on changes in feedforward or local recurrent synaptic connectivity. We hypothesize that these cells increase their local connectivity with neurons that are tuned to the same retinotopic location, thereby strengthening their spatial selectivity (see Figures 2f,h and 3c–e) but reducing precise selectivity for stimulus orientation (see Figure 4f). From this point of view, it will be interesting to perform an experiment studying the exact receptive field position and structure for each neuron along with its precise local synaptic connectivity. Such an experiment could also reveal whether the receptive field centers of certain subsets of neurons (e.g. cells tuned to the trained orientation) drift away from the border region, or whether the spatial extent of receptive fields selectively shrinks so that it is less likely to cross the trained stimulus border. That receptive fields may have shifted should not be considered a potential confounder, but rather as a part of the underlying mechanism improving discriminability of spatially adjacent cortical representations.

Learning-related interactions between signal and noise correlations

Several theoretical studies have shown that reduction of noise correlations between cells exhibiting high signal correlations can improve population coding (Zohary et al., 1994; Oram et al., 1998; Shadlen and Newsome, 1998; Abbott and Dayan, 1999; Averbeck et al., 2006; but see Montijn et al., 2016), but experimental evidence in support of these mechanisms being employed during learning is only sparsely available (Jeanne et al., 2013). Here, we show a decrease of noise correlations within high signal correlation pairs that was strongest at the V1 location where the trained stimuli bordered (Figure 6b). This provides support for the theoretical notion of how interactions between signal- and noise correlations can facilitate coding of stimulus information, and also shows that these effects can arise in the primary visual cortex as a result of associative learning. In addition, groups of cells exhibiting low (negative) signal correlations may improve population coding by increasing noise correlations, even without transmitting information in the structure of the noise correlation itself (Oram et al., 1998; Abbott and Dayan, 1999; Averbeck et al., 2006). Additionally, in imaging regions further away from the border of the trained stimuli, we observed stronger noise correlations in groups of trained orientation tuned cells with relatively low signal correlations. Possibly this effect depends on how heterogeneously a population of neurons is tuned to the behaviorally relevant stimulus and therefore only manifested in the Full imaging regions where the majority neurons might have been synchronously driven by a single (rewarded or non-rewarded) stimulus.

A less dense, but more efficient cortical representation for reward-associated stimuli

With or without stimulus awareness, stimulus-reward pairing can enhance feature detection in primary visual cortex (Shuler and Bear, 2006; Seitz et al., 2009; Frankó et al., 2010; Goltstein et al., 2013). While we observed that visual conditioning resulted in a reduced fraction of neurons being tuned to the trained orientation in the rewarded location (Figures 3d and 4d), those neurons that remained to prefer the conditioned or similar orientations responded with larger ΔF/F amplitudes to the reward associated stimulus (Figure 4e). This indicates that population coding for the conditioned orientation in the rewarded location, contained fewer neurons compared to their untrained orientation tuned counterparts, while maintaining the average population response amplitude, thus potentially rendering their response more efficient by representing the same information using fewer cells (see also Gdalyahu et al., 2012).

In summary, this study highlights a selective role for reward-dependent learning in refining the primary cortical retinotopic map. It demonstrates how coordinated changes in neuronal tuning and inter-neuronal correlations can result in a more optimal population code for trained stimuli and improve the local representation of visual space while leaving the overall retinotopic map intact.

Materials and methods

Animals

Experiments were conducted with approval of the ethical committee for animal experiments of the University of Amsterdam. Male C57BL/6 mice (for n, see Table 1; Harlan, The Netherlands) ranging in age from 70 to 116 days at the start of the experiment were housed pair-wise in large cages (40 cm length ×25 cm width ×25 cm height) on a reversed day night cycle (9 AM light-off; 9 PM light-on). Water and food were available ad libitum, except for a period of six to eight hours prior to behavioral training, when food was not available. Non head-fixed habituation of the mice to the conditioning environment and the frame for head-restraint spanned four to five days and included handling in the behavioral lab and free access to the reward substance, a semifluid vanilla-flavored dairy product (Figure 1—figure supplement 1a).

Visual conditioning of head-restrained mice

Mice were fitted with a stainless-steel cranial plate that allowed repeated head-restraint (Figure 1—figure supplement 1a; Dombeck et al., 2007; Niell and Stryker, 2010). In brief, under surgical anesthesia (Isoflurane inhalation, induction: 3% in O2; maintenance: 1.5% in O2) and analgesia (Buprenorphine, 0.05 mg kg−1 bodyweight, injected subcutaneously 30 min before surgery), three skull screws were placed, two in the frontal skull plates and one lateral-right in the occipital skull plate. The head bar was positioned with the central opening over cortical area V1 and attached with cyanoacrylate glue and black-pigmented dental cement to the skull and screws. The central opening was closed with a transparent silicone elastomer (Quick-sil, World Precision Instruments, Germany) and a cover glass. Mice were allowed to recover for four days, during which non head-fixed habituation was continued. In the next four to five days, animals were head-fixed in the behavioral apparatus while given free access to the vanilla-flavored reward through the feeding tube for 15 to 20 min per day.

Appetitive conditioning with retinotopically selective visual stimuli was done in 8 to 17 sessions. The number of conditioning sessions (8 to 17) before two-photon imaging depended on the order in which animals were used for calcium imaging, which was determined by chance. The number of trials ranged from 8 to 12 trials in the first training session to 20–35 in subsequent training sessions. The amount of rewarded and non-rewarded trials in each training session was nearly equal; on a single day, the number of rewarded and non-rewarded trials never differed by more than three trials and the total number of rewarded and non-rewarded trials differed by only 0.5% (3.1 out of 539.7 trials). The total number of conditioning trials that each mouse was exposed to is reported in Table 1. Conditioning sessions lasted 20–30 min, during which the animal was head-restrained facing a 21’ computer screen (Dell) at a distance of 20 cm (Figure 1a,b). The center of the screen was positioned 45° lateral (Azimuth) at the height of the eye of the mouse (0° elevation). Stimuli were oriented gratings (100% contrast square wave; spatial frequency: 0.05 cycles degree−1; temporal frequency: two cycles second−1) moving in a single direction, presented either in an area of 30 by 30 retinal degrees above, or below the horizontal meridian (Figure 1a, right panels). For each mouse, the rewarded stimulus was randomly assigned to one visual field location and the non-rewarded to the other (see Table 1). This assignment was varied across mice, with the rewarded stimulus being as often above the meridian as below it. Stimulus orientation and direction of the rewarded and non-rewarded stimuli were the same and all parameters remained unchanged during the entire behavioral experiment. Each rewarded or non-rewarded trial started with a click, followed by 4 to 6 s of stimulus presentation against a background with equal luminescence. Next followed a pre-reward delay of 1.5 to 2 s. In the last 0.4 s of the pre-reward delay a servo-controlled arm moved a small plastic tube to a position within licking distance of the mouse in rewarded trials, while the arm stopped just out of range of the mouse in non-rewarded trials (Figure 1b,d and Figure 1—figure supplement 1b–e). By applying back-pressure on the tube using a syringe pump (RE-S, RAZEL, United Kingdom), an amount of 20–40 µl reward substance was delivered in each rewarded trial. After a reward consumption period of 4 s, the arm moved back and an inter-trial interval of 20 to 30 s was inserted between subsequent trials. The temporal offset between reward delivery and visual stimulation was implemented to prevent consumption-related eye-movements or other motion artifacts during visual stimulus presentation.

Behavioral parameters were monitored using a standard visible light CCD camera fitted with a wide-field low-magnification lens at a frame rate of 25 Hz (Figure 1, ‘Behavior tracking’). This camera was positioned on the left side of the mouse to record its silhouette and that of the reward arm against the grey background of the screen, as well as to record the rewarded and non-rewarded stimulus presentation (Figure 1b and Figure 1—figure supplement 1b,d). Behavioral parameters were aligned to stimulus onset and (non-) reward delivery time, which was determined by the arm arriving at the endpoint (Figure 1b and Figure 1—figure supplement 1c,e). The trajectory and speed of the arm was identical in rewarded and non-rewarded trials up to the point where, in rewarded trials, the arm exceeded the non-reward endpoint. The traversal from the non-reward endpoint to the reward endpoint happened within a single 40 ms image frame (Figure 1—figure supplement 1f). The measured time-difference between detection of the arm arriving at the non-reward endpoint and detection at the reward endpoint across all trials was on average 55 ms, implying a movement speed between 0.5 and 1.0 m s−1. We added an extra margin of 100 ms to the 55 ms difference in end point arrival time to buffer possible variations in movement speed. As a result, we excluded behavioral data in the period from 160 ms before the reward or non-reward endpoint in the comparison of learning effects.

Nasal movements (putative sniffs) and oral movements (putative licks) were quantified by applying a threshold to each video frame at the mean intensity, resulting in a binary silhouette of the mouse, and counting the number of pixels that changed intensity compared to the previous video frame in the nasal or oral region (Figure 1c and Figure 1—figure supplement 2a, red box). Licks are rapid, sparsely occurring events; therefore a threshold was applied to the number of pixels changed in each frame, which resulted in ‘all or none’ detection of oral movement. Sniffs are slower and more continuous events that are better reflected by a continuous measure. We therefore used the mean number of pixels that changed intensity as a consequence of nasal-movement directly as a measure for sniffing.

Eye-tracking

Position and diameter of the pupil was monitored using a near infrared CCD camera (Jai, United States) through a high magnification long working distance lens (Thorlabs, Germany) with a frame rate of 25 Hz (see Figure 1 for camera position at ‘Eye tracking’). Eye-tracking was done offline using a custom written algorithm (Zoccolan et al., 2010). For each video frame, location of the center of the pupil was estimated by applying a radial symmetry transform (Loy and Zelinsky, 2003). The edge of the pupil was detected from this point of origin into all directions with 10° angular resolution. Outlier edge detection points (beyond mean ±3 SD) were removed and the remaining data points were fitted with a circle (Figure 1—figure supplement 3a). This procedure provides estimates for the X and Y position and diameter of the pupil in pixels. Using the estimated average diameter of the mouse eye (3.3 mm; Jeon et al., 1998) and the field of view of our camera, a 1-pixel displacement/enlargement in pupil-tracking data was estimated to correspond to approximately 2–2.5 retinal degrees of visual field.

Transcranial optical imaging of intrinsic signals

Intrinsic optical imaging (Bonhoeffer et al., 1996) was performed using a commercially available setup (Optical Imaging Ltd, Israel). The mouse was head-restrained while being under anesthesia (isoflurane inhalation, induction: 3% in O2; maintenance: 1–1.5% in O2). This procedure kept the animal immobile, whereas the eye-blink reflex was just noticeable. The skull was illuminated through the cover glass with 630 nm (bandpass filtered) light. Images were acquired at an effective sampling rate of 2 Hz. A screen was placed at the exact same position as in the behavioral setup (see Figure 1a). Five stimuli (the rewarded stimulus, the non-rewarded stimulus, stimuli at the same location but with orthogonal orientations, and an isoluminant grey screen, the null stimulus; Figure 2a, left panels) were presented in 20 trials each. During the entire imaging experiment, the screen displayed the isoluminant null stimulus, except when a grating stimulus was presented. Each trial started with the recording of a four-second baseline period, followed by four seconds of continuous stimulus presentation and seven seconds of post-stimulus response time. The intertrial interval lasted an additional five seconds.

Analysis of intrinsic optical signals

Intrinsic signal images were referenced per trial against the mean of the first 8 (baseline) frames, resulting in a percentage signal increase per pixel. A region in the primary visual cortex, large enough to contain the responses to both rewarded and non-rewarded stimuli, was selected manually and within-trial signal drift was corrected for by subtracting the value of each pixel in this region by the average intensity of a non-responsive image-region a few millimeters anterior. For each recording, a single mean and standard deviation was calculated per pixel across all stimuli and repetitions and each pixel was subsequently z-scored by subtracting the mean and dividing over the standard deviation.

Within the region of V1, the location of maximum activation for the rewarded and non-rewarded area was automatically detected by locating the largest activation in smoothed average response images for visual stimulation by the orthogonal stimuli in the rewarded area and non-rewarded area. Using this (untrained) template response map, we calculated the angle that aligned an imaginary line, connecting peaks of maximal visual activity evoked by stimulation in the different visual fields, parallel to the vertical axis of the template image (Figure 2a, red windows). In most mice this rotation was around 25° clockwise. All further data from each recording was automatically rotated to the calculated rotation angle. Next, cutouts of this 2-dimensional rotated image, from −5 to +5 pixels centered on the vertical transection of the average peak responses in the (untrained) template, were averaged to a 1-dimensional spatial response profile for each time point separately that represented response strength as a function of cortical distance aligned to the Rewarded/Non-rewarded axis (Figure 2b, left panels, cutout before averaging the x-dimension). Response profiles from different time points (imaging frames) of the same trial were concatenated to cortical distance ×time matrices (Figure 2b, right panels). On a total of 10 mice, one mouse was removed from these analyses because the peak location of the intrinsic response could not be (automatically) detected in both the rewarded and the non-rewarded location due to blood vessel response artifacts in the imaging data.

Amplitude time courses (Figure 2—figure supplement 1b) of the intrinsic optical signal response were calculated by averaging values of pixels in the spatial dimension over a range of −5 to +5 pixels around the maximum pixel in the cortical distance ×time matrices. Amplitude-cortical space profiles (Figure 2d) were constructed similarly, by averaging intensity values in the period where the response peaked (i.e. 3 to 7 s after stimulus onset) over the time dimension. Overlap of cortical space profiles was calculated by, for each spatial point on the profile, subtracting the sum of the individual profile data points from their maximum, resulting in the part of the response that was shared (see Figure 2c and Figure 2f). ΔOverlap (Figure 2h) indicated the difference between overlap calculated for trained and for orthogonal stimulus orientations presented at the same location. Spatial selectivity was calculated similarly by, for each spatial point on the profile, dividing the difference of the responses to the rewarded (Rrewarded) and non-rewarded (Rnon-rewarded) locations by their sum (Rrewarded - Rnon-rewarded) / (Rrewarded + Rnon-rewarded).

Calcium imaging

Under surgical anesthesia (Isoflurane inhalation, induction: 3% in O2; maintenance: 1.5% in O2) and analgesia (Buprenorphine, 0.05 mg kg−1 bodyweight, injected subcutaneously 30 min before surgery), a craniotomy was performed over the area of V1 that was identified to respond to the trained stimuli using intrinsic optical imaging. In these regions, cells were loaded with the fluorescent calcium indicator Oregon Green BAPTA1-AM (OGB) and Sulforhodamine 101 (SR101; for staining astrocytes/glial cells) using a protocol for multi-cell bolus loading (Stosiek et al., 2003; Nimmerjahn et al., 2004; Goltstein et al., 2013; Goltstein et al., 2015). After surgical procedures were completed, the Isoflurane concentration was carefully lowered and maintained at 0.8% for the entire course of the experiment (such that the mouse remained under anesthesia).

Two-photon laser scanning microscopy was performed with a Leica SP5 resonant scanner and a Spectra-Physics Mai Tai High Performance Mode Locked Ti:Sapphire laser (wavelength 810 nm). Fluorescence was collected in non-descanned photo-multiplier tubes, filtered at 525 nm (maximum range 500–550 nm) for OGB and 585 nm (maximum range 565–605 nm) for SR101. Frame-averaged images (eight frames, 512 × 512 pixels) from a square region of approximately 330 × 330 μm were acquired at a scan speed of approximately 2.8 Hz. Visual stimulation was done with moving gratings identical to those used for behavioral conditioning, but now 8 directions of movement (four orientations) were shown in either visual field (10 repetitions with a duration of 8 scan frames per stimulus).

Analysis of calcium imaging data

Images acquired with two-photon microscopy were realigned to compensate for small movement artifacts in the x–y plane using an algorithm that relies on the cross-correlation between the two-dimensional Fourier transforms of the to-be-aligned image and a reference image (Guizar-Sicairos et al., 2008). Neurons were manually identified and outlined and the mean fluorescence was calculated for each image frame. The fluorescence time series of each neuron was corrected for possible contamination by non cell-specific fluorescence signals that originate from the neuropil using previously described methods (Kerlin et al., 2010; Goltstein et al., 2015). Visual responses were quantified per trial as ∆F/F, or in more detail: (F–F0)/F0. F was determined from the average fluorescence in the 8-frame (~2.8 s) period during visual stimulation. F0 was calculated from the average fluorescence in the 8-frame period preceding visual stimulation, when a gray screen with equal luminance was presented.

For each field of view, an imaging location index was calculated from the mean response of all neurons to the orthogonal (untrained) orientations (Equation 1). This index approaches the value of −1 for fields of view that have primarily cells tuned to the non-rewarded location and +1 when most cells are tuned to the rewarded location. For fields of view where the fraction of cells responding to the rewarded and non-rewarded location was similar the index has a value around 0:

Imaging location index=(1N1NROrth(RewLoc)n)(1N1NROrth(nRwLoc)n)(1N1NROrth(RewLoc)n)+(1N1NROrth(nRwLoc)n) (1)

Where N is the total number of neurons in a recording session and ROrth(RewLoc)n is the mean response of the nth neuron to the orientation orthogonal to the trained orientation, presented in the reward-predictive visual field.

Based on the imaging location index, the dataset was divided in four groups along the rewarded/non-rewarded axis (Figure 3b). Two groups consisted of recordings that were made in regions that were mostly driven by one retinotopic location; the ‘Full rewarded’ and ‘Full non-rewarded’ groups (Imaging location index <−0.66 for Full-nRw and >0.66 for Full-Rew). The two other groups consisted of recordings obtained from the region of cortex where the rewarded and non-rewarded retinotopic representations bordered; the ‘Border rewarded’ and ‘Border non-rewarded’ groups (Imaging location index >−0.66 but <0 for Border-nRw and >0 but<0.66 for Border-Rew). The cut-off of 0.66 was chosen such that only cortical regions that were strongly driven by one stimulus location compared to the other stimulus location (~5 times stronger) would be classified as Full rewarded or Full non-rewarded.

The number of recordings in each location group ranged from 8 to 14 (Figure 3—figure supplement 1a) and the average within-group imaging-location index showed a linear increase across location groups along the rewarded/non-rewarded axis (Figure 3—figure supplement 1b). The latter indicates that each location-group represented a part of the visual space that was approximately equally distant from the neighboring location groups in cortical space and thus validates the approach. An important aspect here is that, as in the analysis of intrinsic imaging data, the imaging location index was calculated using responses to untrained stimuli, which facilitates the comparison between results obtained using these two imaging techniques.

Neurons were classified as being orientation-tuned or non-tuned by testing for a difference in their responses across eight directions in the upper or lower visual field separately using a 1-way Anova against p<0.05. Neurons that displayed significant orientation tuning, but for which the ∆F/F response to the preferred direction did not exceed 2%, were considered to be false positives, or to have a low signal to noise ratio and were therefore discarded from the analysis. Tuning curves for movement direction were constructed by fitting the mean responses across all directions in each area separately by a two-peaked Gaussian function (Li et al., 2008). Tuning curve bandwidth was calculated as the half-width of the fitted tuning curve at 1/√2 height (Ringach et al., 2002). Orientation selectivity (OSI) was calculated by dividing the difference between response to the preferred orientation and the orthogonal orientation by the sum of these: (Rpref-ori - Rortho-ori)/(Rpref-ori + Rortho-ori) (Niell and Stryker, 2008).

Population sparseness ap (Equation 2) was quantified as described in Rolls and Treves, 2011. The vector y contains mean ∆F/F responses of all N neurons to a single stimulus and yn is the response of a single neuron n to a single stimulus. Higher values of ap indicate that many neurons in the population were responsive to the stimulus. Lower values of ap indicate a sparser population response, with only few strongly responding neurons resulting in a more skewed distribution of ∆F/F amplitudes:

ap=(n=1Nyn/N)2(n=1Nyn2)/N (2)

Population decoding of the stimulated visual field (rewarded or non-rewarded location), was done using an algorithm based on Bayes theorem (Equation 3; Oram et al., 1998; Dayan and Abbott, 2001). Single trial responses were decoded using a ‘leave one trial-block out’ (cross-validated) principle. One trial from each stimulus category (rewarded or non-rewarded location) was removed from the dataset and each of the removed trials was decoded separately. The probability for a single-trial calcium response r of an individual neuron to be observed in a certain stimulus condition s, p(r|s), was calculated from a Gaussian probability distribution, estimated by the mean and SD of all remaining responses of the decoded neuron in that stimulus condition. When multiple neurons were used for decoding, probabilities of the individual neurons’ responses were multiplied. The prior, p(s), was identical across stimuli and the overall probability of observing the decoded response, p(r), was estimated from all remaining responses (Equation 3; Oram et al., 1998). This procedure gave two probabilities per decoded trial; one for the stimulation delivered at the rewarded location and one for the non-rewarded location. The highest probability was chosen as decoder output. Performance for decoding single neuron or population (multiple simultaneously recorded neurons) calcium responses was quantified by comparing the decoder output with actual stimulus position and resulted in a fraction correct (the theoretical chance level of 50% correct was experimentally confirmed).

psr=prsp(s)p(r) (3)

Signal correlations between pairs of cells were quantified by calculating the mean Pearson correlation coefficient of the average responses of those cells to all 16 stimulus types (eight directions × 2 locations). Noise correlations were quantified by subtracting the average response to the preferred orientation from the individual trial responses to that orientation (because of 8 repeats per stimulus for the rewarded and non-rewarded location this resulted in a vector of 16 mean-subtracted single trial responses), and subsequently calculating the Pearson correlation coefficient between the mean-subtracted single trial responses of the pair of cells, but only for the preferred stimulus in the rewarded and non-rewarded location (Hofer et al., 2011; Jeanne et al., 2013).

Statistics

All data are presented and tested as mean ± SEM across mice unless otherwise noted. Most parameters that were reported in this manuscript were likely not-normally distributed and were therefore tested with a Mann-Whitney U test or a Wilcoxon matched-pairs signed-rank test (WMPSR). When multiple groups were involved, we applied a Kruskal-Wallis test, with posthoc Mann-Whitney U tests or WMPSR tests; or an Anova with posthoc t-tests if the data followed a normal distribution (Figure 3d,e). When iterating comparisons between multiple groups without first using a test for multiple groups (Anova/Kruskal-Wallis test), we corrected the P-value using Bonferroni’s method.

Signal- and noise correlations (Figure 6) were calculated per imaging field of view, on all simultaneously recorded pairs of neurons. Next, 95% and 99% confidence intervals were calculated using bootstrap resampling on the resulting dataset with signal- or noise correlations from all fields of view (resampling with replacement; Sample size of 250 pairs; 10000 resamples; Efron, 1979).

Acknowledgements

The authors thank Laura van Mourik-Donga for technical assistance and Lior Cohen, Jorrit Montijn, Mark Hübener and Tobias Rose for helpful comments on the manuscript. This work was supported by the NWO Program for Excellence, Brain and Cognition, 433-09-208, NWO VICI Grant 918.46.609 and the European Union’s Horizon 2020 Research and Innovation Program under Grant Agreement No. 785907 (Human Brain Project SGA2).

Funding Statement

The funders had no role in study design, data collection and interpretation, or the decision to submit the work for publication.

Contributor Information

Pieter M Goltstein, Email: goltstein@neuro.mpg.de.

Cyriel MA Pennartz, Email: C.M.A.Pennartz@uva.nl.

Daeyeol Lee, Yale School of Medicine, United States.

Andrew J King, University of Oxford, United Kingdom.

Funding Information

This paper was supported by the following grants:

  • Nederlandse Organisatie voor Wetenschappelijk Onderzoek 433-09-208 to Cyriel MA Pennartz.

  • Horizon 2020 Framework Programme Grant Agreement No. 785907 (Human Brain Project SGA2) to Cyriel MA Pennartz.

  • Nederlandse Organisatie voor Wetenschappelijk Onderzoek 918.46.609 to Cyriel MA Pennartz.

Additional information

Competing interests

No competing interests declared.

Author contributions

Conceptualization, Data curation, Software, Formal analysis, Investigation, Visualization, Methodology, Writing—original draft, Writing—review and editing.

Data curation, Software, Formal analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Writing—review and editing.

Conceptualization, Supervision, Funding acquisition, Writing—original draft, Project administration, Writing—review and editing.

Ethics

Animal experimentation: Experiments were conducted with approval of the ethical committee for animal experiments of the University of Amsterdam (Protocol number DED162).

Additional files

Transparent reporting form
DOI: 10.7554/eLife.37683.015

Data availability

The full dataset of this study is available on gin.g-node, url: https://web.gin.g-node.org/pgoltstein/goltstein_meijer_pennartz_2018

The following dataset was generated:

Pieter M Goltstein, author; Guido T Meijer, author; Cyriel MA Pennartz, author. Behavioral-, intrinsic imaging-, calcium imaging- and eye-tracking-data. 2010 https://web.gin.g-node.org/pgoltstein/goltstein_meijer_pennartz_2018 Publicly available at the Gin: Modern Research Data Management for Neuroscience website.

References

  1. Abbott LF, Dayan P. The effect of correlated variability on the accuracy of a population code. Neural Computation. 1999;11:91–101. doi: 10.1162/089976699300016827. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Adesnik H, Bruns W, Taniguchi H, Huang ZJ, Scanziani M. A neural circuit for spatial summation in visual cortex. Nature. 2012;490:226–231. doi: 10.1038/nature11526. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Allard T, Clark SA, Jenkins WM, Merzenich MM. Reorganization of somatosensory area 3b representations in adult owl monkeys after digital syndactyly. Journal of Neurophysiology. 1991;66:1048–1058. doi: 10.1152/jn.1991.66.3.1048. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Andermann ML, Kerlin AM, Reid RC. Chronic cellular imaging of mouse visual cortex during operant behavior and passive viewing. Frontiers in Cellullar Neuroscience. 2010;4:3. doi: 10.3389/fncel.2010.00003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Arzi A, Shedlesky L, Ben-Shaul M, Nasser K, Oksenberg A, Hairston IS, Sobel N. Humans can learn new information during sleep. Nature Neuroscience. 2012;15:1460–1465. doi: 10.1038/nn.3193. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Averbeck BB, Latham PE, Pouget A. Neural correlations, population coding and computation. Nature Reviews Neuroscience. 2006;7:358–366. doi: 10.1038/nrn1888. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Bakin JS, Weinberger NM. Induction of a physiological memory in the cerebral cortex by stimulation of the nucleus basalis. PNAS. 1996;93:11219–11224. doi: 10.1073/pnas.93.20.11219. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Balsam P. Relative time in trace conditioning. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences. 1984;423:211–227. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.1984.tb23432.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Bao S, Chan VT, Merzenich MM. Cortical remodelling induced by activity of ventral tegmental dopamine neurons. Nature. 2001;412:79–83. doi: 10.1038/35083586. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Bear MF, Singer W. Modulation of visual cortical plasticity by acetylcholine and noradrenaline. Nature. 1986;320:172–176. doi: 10.1038/320172a0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Bennett C, Arroyo S, Hestrin S. Subthreshold mechanisms underlying state-dependent modulation of visual responses. Neuron. 2013;80:350–357. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2013.08.007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Bensafi M, Porter J, Pouliot S, Mainland J, Johnson B, Zelano C, Young N, Bremner E, Aframian D, Khan R, Sobel N. Olfactomotor activity during imagery mimics that during perception. Nature Neuroscience. 2003;6:1142–1144. doi: 10.1038/nn1145. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Blake DT, Heiser MA, Caywood M, Merzenich MM. Experience-dependent adult cortical plasticity requires cognitive association between sensation and reward. Neuron. 2006;52:371–381. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2006.08.009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Bonhoeffer T, Grinvald A. Optical Imaging based on intrinsic signals: the methodology. In: Mazziotta J. C, Toga A. W, editors. Brain Mapping; the Methods. Academic Press; 1996. pp. 55–97. [Google Scholar]
  15. Bonin V, Histed MH, Yurgenson S, Reid RC. Local diversity and fine-scale organization of receptive fields in mouse visual cortex. Journal of Neuroscience. 2011;31:18506–18521. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2974-11.2011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Bradley MM, Miccoli L, Escrig MA, Lang PJ. The pupil as a measure of emotional arousal and autonomic activation. Psychophysiology. 2008;45:602–607. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2008.00654.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Buonomano DV, Merzenich MM. Cortical plasticity: from synapses to maps. Annual Review of Neuroscience. 1998;21:149–186. doi: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.21.1.149. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Chubykin AA, Roach EB, Bear MF, Shuler MG. A cholinergic mechanism for reward timing within primary visual cortex. Neuron. 2013;77:723–735. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2012.12.039. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. David SV, Fritz JB, Shamma SA. Task reward structure shapes rapid receptive field plasticity in auditory cortex. PNAS. 2012;109:2144–2149. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1117717109. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Dayan P, Abbott LF. Theoretical Neuroscience. Cambridge, MA: MIT press; 2001. [Google Scholar]
  21. DeBold RC, Miller NE, Jensen DD. Effect of strength of drive determined by a new technique for appetitive classical conditioning of rats. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology. 1965;59:102–108. doi: 10.1037/h0021632. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Dombeck DA, Khabbaz AN, Collman F, Adelman TL, Tank DW. Imaging large-scale neural activity with cellular resolution in awake, mobile mice. Neuron. 2007;56:43–57. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2007.08.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Drew MR, Zupan B, Cooke A, Couvillon PA, Balsam PD. Temporal control of conditioned responding in goldfish. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes. 2005;31:31–39. doi: 10.1037/0097-7403.31.1.31. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Efron B. Bootstrap methods: another look at the jackknife. The Annals of Statistics. 1979;7:1–26. doi: 10.1214/aos/1176344552. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  25. Feldman DE, Brecht M. Map plasticity in somatosensory cortex. Science. 2005;310:810–815. doi: 10.1126/science.1115807. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Frankó E, Seitz AR, Vogels R. Dissociable neural effects of long-term stimulus-reward pairing in macaque visual cortex. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 2010;22:1425–1439. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2009.21288. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Freeman J, Ziemba CM, Heeger DJ, Simoncelli EP, Movshon JA. A functional and perceptual signature of the second visual area in primates. Nature Neuroscience. 2013;16:974–981. doi: 10.1038/nn.3402. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Fritz J, Shamma S, Elhilali M, Klein D. Rapid task-related plasticity of spectrotemporal receptive fields in primary auditory cortex. Nature Neuroscience. 2003;6:1216–1223. doi: 10.1038/nn1141. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Furtak SC, Ahmed OJ, Burwell RD. Single neuron activity and theta modulation in postrhinal cortex during visual object discrimination. Neuron. 2012;76:976–988. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2012.10.039. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Gdalyahu A, Tring E, Polack PO, Gruver R, Golshani P, Fanselow MS, Silva AJ, Trachtenberg JT. Associative fear learning enhances sparse network coding in primary sensory cortex. Neuron. 2012;75:121–132. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2012.04.035. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Ghose GM, Yang T, Maunsell JH. Physiological correlates of perceptual learning in monkey V1 and V2. Journal of Neurophysiology. 2002;87:1867–1888. doi: 10.1152/jn.00690.2001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Gilbert CD, Li W, Piech V. Perceptual learning and adult cortical plasticity. The Journal of Physiology. 2009;587:2743–2751. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.2009.171488. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Gilbert CD, Wiesel TN. Receptive field dynamics in adult primary visual cortex. Nature. 1992;356:150–152. doi: 10.1038/356150a0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Gilbert CD. Plasticity in visual perception and physiology. Current Opinion in Neurobiology. 1996;6:269–274. doi: 10.1016/S0959-4388(96)80083-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Goltstein PM, Coffey EB, Roelfsema PR, Pennartz CM. In vivo two-photon Ca2+ imaging reveals selective reward effects on stimulus-specific assemblies in mouse visual cortex. Journal of Neuroscience. 2013;33:11540–11555. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1341-12.2013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Goltstein PM, Montijn JS, Pennartz CM. Effects of isoflurane anesthesia on ensemble patterns of Ca2+ activity in mouse v1: reduced direction selectivity independent of increased correlations in cellular activity. PloS One. 2015;10:e0118277. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0118277. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Guizar-Sicairos M, Thurman ST, Fienup JR. Efficient subpixel image registration algorithms. Optics Letters. 2008;33:156–158. doi: 10.1364/OL.33.000156. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Heinen SJ, Skavenski AA. Recovery of visual responses in foveal V1 neurons following bilateral foveal lesions in adult monkey. Experimental Brain Research. 1991;83:670–674. doi: 10.1007/BF00229845. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Hofer SB, Ko H, Pichler B, Vogelstein J, Ros H, Zeng H, Lein E, Lesica NA, Mrsic-Flogel TD. Differential connectivity and response dynamics of excitatory and inhibitory neurons in visual cortex. Nature Neuroscience. 2011;14:1045–1052. doi: 10.1038/nn.2876. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Hubel DH, Wiesel TN. Receptive fields of single neurones in the cat's striate cortex. The Journal of Physiology. 1959;148:574–591. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.1959.sp006308. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Hubel DH, Wiesel TN. Receptive fields, binocular interaction and functional architecture in the cat's visual cortex. The Journal of Physiology. 1962;160:106–154. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.1962.sp006837. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Huerta PT, Sun LD, Wilson MA, Tonegawa S. Formation of temporal memory requires NMDA receptors within CA1 pyramidal neurons. Neuron. 2000;25:473–480. doi: 10.1016/S0896-6273(00)80909-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Jeanne JM, Sharpee TO, Gentner TQ. Associative learning enhances population coding by inverting interneuronal correlation patterns. Neuron. 2013;78:352–363. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2013.02.023. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Jehee JF, Ling S, Swisher JD, van Bergen RS, Tong F. Perceptual learning selectively refines orientation representations in early visual cortex. Journal of Neuroscience. 2012;32:16747–16753. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.6112-11.2012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Jeon CJ, Strettoi E, Masland RH. The Major cell populations of the mouse retina. The Journal of Neuroscience. 1998;18:8936–8946. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.18-21-08936.1998. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Ji W, Gămănuţ R, Bista P, D'Souza RD, Wang Q, Burkhalter A. Modularity in the organization of mouse primary visual cortex. Neuron. 2015;87:632–643. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2015.07.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Joscelyne A, Kehoe EJ. Time and stimulus specificity in extinction of the conditioned nictitating membrane response in the rabbit (Oryctolagus cuniculus) Behavioral Neuroscience. 2007;121:50–62. doi: 10.1037/0735-7044.121.1.50. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Keck T, Mrsic-Flogel TD, Vaz Afonso M, Eysel UT, Bonhoeffer T, Hübener M. Massive restructuring of neuronal circuits during functional reorganization of adult visual cortex. Nature Neuroscience. 2008;11:1162–1167. doi: 10.1038/nn.2181. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Kehoe EJ, Joscelyne A. Temporally specific extinction of conditioned responses in the rabbit (Oryctolagus cuniculus) nictitating membrane preparation. Behavioral Neuroscience. 2005;119:1011–1022. doi: 10.1037/0735-7044.119.4.1011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Keller GB, Bonhoeffer T, Hübener M. Sensorimotor mismatch signals in primary visual cortex of the behaving mouse. Neuron. 2012;74:809–815. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2012.03.040. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Kerlin AM, Andermann ML, Berezovskii VK, Reid RC. Broadly tuned response properties of diverse inhibitory neuron subtypes in mouse visual cortex. Neuron. 2010;67:858–871. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2010.08.002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Kilgard MP, Merzenich MM. Cortical map reorganization enabled by nucleus basalis activity. Science. 1998;279:1714–1718. doi: 10.1126/science.279.5357.1714. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Ko H, Hofer SB, Pichler B, Buchanan KA, Sjöström PJ, Mrsic-Flogel TD. Functional specificity of local synaptic connections in neocortical networks. Nature. 2011;473:87–91. doi: 10.1038/nature09880. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Lee AM, Hoy JL, Bonci A, Wilbrecht L, Stryker MP, Niell CM. Identification of a brainstem circuit regulating visual cortical state in parallel with locomotion. Neuron. 2014;83:455–466. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2014.06.031. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Li Y, Van Hooser SD, Mazurek M, White LE, Fitzpatrick D. Experience with moving visual stimuli drives the early development of cortical direction selectivity. Nature. 2008;456:952–956. doi: 10.1038/nature07417. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Liu CH, Coleman JE, Davoudi H, Zhang K, Hussain Shuler MG. Selective activation of a putative reinforcement signal conditions cued interval timing in primary visual cortex. Current Biology. 2015;25:1551–1561. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2015.04.028. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Loy G, Zelinsky A. Fast radial symmetry for detecting points of interest. IEEE Transactions on Pattern Analysis and Machine Intelligence. 2003;25:959–973. doi: 10.1109/TPAMI.2003.1217601. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  58. Makino H, Komiyama T. Learning enhances the relative impact of top-down processing in the visual cortex. Nature Neuroscience. 2015;18:1116–1122. doi: 10.1038/nn.4061. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Meijer GT, Montijn JS, Pennartz CMA, Lansink CS. Audiovisual modulation in mouse primary visual cortex depends on Cross-Modal stimulus configuration and congruency. The Journal of Neuroscience. 2017;37:8783–8796. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0468-17.2017. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Mogilner A, Grossman JA, Ribary U, Joliot M, Volkmann J, Rapaport D, Beasley RW, Llinás RR. Somatosensory cortical plasticity in adult humans revealed by magnetoencephalography. PNAS. 1993;90:3593–3597. doi: 10.1073/pnas.90.8.3593. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Montijn JS, Goltstein PM, Pennartz CM. Mouse V1 population correlates of visual detection rely on heterogeneity within neuronal response patterns. eLife. 2015;4:e10163. doi: 10.7554/eLife.10163. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Montijn JS, Meijer GT, Lansink CS, Pennartz CM. Population-Level neural codes are robust to Single-Neuron variability from a multidimensional coding perspective. Cell Reports. 2016;16:2486–2498. doi: 10.1016/j.celrep.2016.07.065. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  63. Mrsic-Flogel TD, Hofer SB, Ohki K, Reid RC, Bonhoeffer T, Hübener M. Homeostatic regulation of eye-specific responses in visual cortex during ocular dominance plasticity. Neuron. 2007;54:961–972. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2007.05.028. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  64. Nabavi S, Fox R, Proulx CD, Lin JY, Tsien RY, Malinow R. Engineering a memory with LTD and LTP. Nature. 2014;511:348–352. doi: 10.1038/nature13294. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  65. Niell CM, Stryker MP. Highly selective receptive fields in mouse visual cortex. Journal of Neuroscience. 2008;28:7520–7536. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0623-08.2008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  66. Niell CM, Stryker MP. Modulation of visual responses by behavioral state in mouse visual cortex. Neuron. 2010;65:472–479. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2010.01.033. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  67. Nimmerjahn A, Kirchhoff F, Kerr JN, Helmchen F. Sulforhodamine 101 as a specific marker of astroglia in the neocortex in vivo. Nature Methods. 2004;1:31–37. doi: 10.1038/nmeth706. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  68. Okun M, Steinmetz N, Cossell L, Iacaruso MF, Ko H, Barthó P, Moore T, Hofer SB, Mrsic-Flogel TD, Carandini M, Harris KD. Diverse coupling of neurons to populations in sensory cortex. Nature. 2015;521:511–515. doi: 10.1038/nature14273. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  69. Oram MW, Földiák P, Perrett DI, Sengpiel F. The 'Ideal Homunculus': decoding neural population signals. Trends in Neurosciences. 1998;21:259–265. doi: 10.1016/S0166-2236(97)01216-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  70. Pennartz CM. Reinforcement learning by hebbian synapses with adaptive thresholds. Neuroscience. 1997;81:303–319. doi: 10.1016/S0306-4522(97)00118-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  71. Pinto L, Goard MJ, Estandian D, Xu M, Kwan AC, Lee SH, Harrison TC, Feng G, Dan Y. Fast modulation of visual perception by basal forebrain cholinergic neurons. Nature Neuroscience. 2013;16:1857–1863. doi: 10.1038/nn.3552. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  72. Poort J, Khan AG, Pachitariu M, Nemri A, Orsolic I, Krupic J, Bauza M, Sahani M, Keller GB, Mrsic-Flogel TD, Hofer SB. Learning enhances sensory and multiple Non-sensory representations in primary visual cortex. Neuron. 2015;86:1478–1490. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2015.05.037. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  73. Poort J, Roelfsema PR. Noise correlations have little influence on the coding of selective attention in area V1. Cerebral Cortex. 2009;19:543–553. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhn103. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  74. Raybuck JD, Lattal KM. Bridging the interval: theory and neurobiology of trace conditioning. Behavioural Processes. 2014;101:103–111. doi: 10.1016/j.beproc.2013.08.016. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  75. Reimer J, Froudarakis E, Cadwell CR, Yatsenko D, Denfield GH, Tolias AS. Pupil fluctuations track fast switching of cortical states during quiet wakefulness. Neuron. 2014;84:355–362. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2014.09.033. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  76. Ringach DL, Shapley RM, Hawken MJ. Orientation selectivity in macaque V1: diversity and laminar dependence. The Journal of Neuroscience. 2002;22:5639–5651. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.22-13-05639.2002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  77. Rolls ET, Treves A. The neuronal encoding of information in the brain. Progress in Neurobiology. 2011;95:448–490. doi: 10.1016/j.pneurobio.2011.08.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  78. Sakatani T, Isa T. PC-based high-speed video-oculography for measuring rapid eye movements in mice. Neuroscience Research. 2004;49:123–131. doi: 10.1016/j.neures.2004.02.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  79. Sakatani T, Isa T. Quantitative analysis of spontaneous saccade-like rapid eye movements in C57BL/6 mice. Neuroscience Research. 2007;58:324–331. doi: 10.1016/j.neures.2007.04.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  80. Saleem AB, Ayaz A, Jeffery KJ, Harris KD, Carandini M. Integration of visual motion and locomotion in mouse visual cortex. Nature Neuroscience. 2013;16:1864–1869. doi: 10.1038/nn.3567. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  81. Schoups A, Vogels R, Qian N, Orban G. Practising orientation identification improves orientation coding in V1 neurons. Nature. 2001;412:549–553. doi: 10.1038/35087601. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  82. Seitz AR, Kim D, Watanabe T. Rewards evoke learning of unconsciously processed visual stimuli in adult humans. Neuron. 2009;61:700–707. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2009.01.016. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  83. Shadlen MN, Newsome WT. The variable discharge of cortical neurons: implications for connectivity, computation, and information coding. The Journal of Neuroscience. 1998;18:3870–3896. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.18-10-03870.1998. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  84. Shang C, Liu Z, Chen Z, Shi Y, Wang Q, Liu S, Li D, Cao P. BRAIN CIRCUITS. A parvalbumin-positive excitatory visual pathway to trigger fear responses in mice. Science. 2015;348:1472–1477. doi: 10.1126/science.aaa8694. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  85. Shuler MG, Bear MF. Reward timing in the primary visual cortex. Science. 2006;311:1606–1609. doi: 10.1126/science.1123513. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  86. Smith SL, Häusser M. Parallel processing of visual space by neighboring neurons in mouse visual cortex. Nature Neuroscience. 2010;13:1144–1149. doi: 10.1038/nn.2620. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  87. Stosiek C, Garaschuk O, Holthoff K, Konnerth A. In vivo two-photon calcium imaging of neuronal networks. PNAS. 2003;100:7319–7324. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1232232100. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  88. van Alphen B, Winkelman BH, Frens MA. Three-dimensional optokinetic eye movements in the C57BL/6J mouse. Investigative Opthalmology & Visual Science. 2010;51:623–630. doi: 10.1167/iovs.09-4072. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  89. Vinck M, Batista-Brito R, Knoblich U, Cardin JA. Arousal and locomotion make distinct contributions to cortical activity patterns and visual encoding. Neuron. 2015;86:740–754. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2015.03.028. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  90. Wallace DJ, Greenberg DS, Sawinski J, Rulla S, Notaro G, Kerr JN. Rats maintain an overhead binocular field at the expense of constant fusion. Nature. 2013;498:65–69. doi: 10.1038/nature12153. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  91. Wei P, Liu N, Zhang Z, Liu X, Tang Y, He X, Wu B, Zhou Z, Liu Y, Li J, Zhang Y, Zhou X, Xu L, Chen L, Bi G, Hu X, Xu F, Wang L. Processing of visually evoked innate fear by a non-canonical thalamic pathway. Nature Communications. 2015;6:6756. doi: 10.1038/ncomms7756. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  92. Weinberger NM, Javid R, Lepan B. Long-term retention of learning-induced receptive-field plasticity in the auditory cortex. PNAS. 1993;90:2394–2398. doi: 10.1073/pnas.90.6.2394. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  93. Xu S, Jiang W, Poo MM, Dan Y. Activity recall in a visual cortical ensemble. Nature Neuroscience. 2012;15:449–455. doi: 10.1038/nn.3036. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  94. Yang T, Maunsell JH. The effect of perceptual learning on neuronal responses in monkey visual area V4. Journal of Neuroscience. 2004;24:1617–1626. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4442-03.2004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  95. Zhang S, Xu M, Kamigaki T, Hoang Do JP, Chang WC, Jenvay S, Miyamichi K, Luo L, Dan Y. Selective attention. Long-range and local circuits for top-down modulation of visual cortex processing. Science. 2014;345:660–665. doi: 10.1126/science.1254126. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  96. Zhang X, Zhaoping L, Zhou T, Fang F. Neural activities in v1 create a bottom-up saliency map. Neuron. 2012a;73:183–192. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2011.10.035. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  97. Zhang Y, Kim IJ, Sanes JR, Meister M. The most numerous ganglion cell type of the mouse retina is a selective feature detector. PNAS. 2012b;109:E2391–E2398. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1211547109. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  98. Zoccolan D, Graham BJ, Cox DD. A self-calibrating, camera-based eye tracker for the recording of rodent eye movements. Frontiers in Neuroscience. 2010;4:193. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2010.00193. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  99. Zohary E, Celebrini S, Britten KH, Newsome WT. Neuronal plasticity that underlies improvement in perceptual performance. Science. 1994;263:1289–1292. doi: 10.1126/science.8122114. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Decision letter

Editor: Daeyeol Lee1
Reviewed by: Daeyeol Lee2

In the interests of transparency, eLife includes the editorial decision letter and accompanying author responses. A lightly edited version of the letter sent to the authors after peer review is shown, indicating the most substantive concerns; minor comments are not usually included.

Thank you for submitting your article "Conditioning sharpens the spatial representation of rewarded stimuli in mouse primary visual cortex" for consideration by eLife. Your article has been reviewed by three peer reviewers, including Daeyeol Lee as the Reviewing Editor and Reviewer #1, and the evaluation has been overseen by David Van Essen as the Senior Editor.

The reviewers have discussed the reviews with one another and the Reviewing Editor has drafted this decision to help you prepare a revised submission.

Summary:

Goldstein et al., examined how the neural representation of visual stimuli in the mouse primary visual cortex (V1) might be altered during classical conditioning, using both intrinsic optimal imaging and calcium imaging methods. For the intrinsic optical imaging, they found that the overlap of the activity induced by visual stimulus in the same orientation used during conditioning across the cortical surface was reduced compared to the overlap for the stimulus in the orthogonal orientation. This might suggest that the conditioning improved the quality of location information specifically for the experienced orientation. They obtained somewhat similar results from the calcium imaging of individual V1 neurons. First, they found that the overlap was smaller in all the locations other than the "full-non-rewarded" location (however, see below). Second, they found that the fraction of neurons tuned to the tested orientation was reduced, but their response strength increased, although this effect looked relatively small (Figure 4B). Finally, they showed that the encoding of spatial information was enhanced more for the tested orientation than for untested orientations.

Overall, this study addresses an important question regarding how the cortical representation of sensory stimuli might be affected by their behavioral relevance. The experiments and analyses were carried out rigorously, and multiple analytical approaches were applied thoroughly. Nevertheless, there are some apparent inconsistencies in the main findings, which should be addressed or discussed better by the authors.

Essential revisions:

1) The most significant weakness of this manuscript is its heavy reliance on the measure related to the changes in the overlap in the neural activity induced by different visual stimuli across the cortical surface.

For the intrinsic imaging data, the authors focus on the changes in the overlap in the neural activity across the cortical surface. Thanks to the data presented in Figure 2figure supplement 1, however, it can be seen that in reality the data are a bit more complicated. In particular, the overlap was reduced mainly because the activity for the stimulus in the non-rewarded location was reduced in the cortical area corresponding to the rewarded location. By contrast, the activity related to the conditioned stimulus in the rewarded location did not change. If one assumes that learning is driven by stimulus-reward association during conditioning, this result is counter-intuitive. The authors should discuss this finding more directly, rather than focusing on the change in the overlap instead.

The results from the intrinsic and calcium imaging experiments are also only superficially similar. The reduction in the overlap for tested orientation is driven mostly change in the "full" non-rewarded location (Figure 3C and 3D). Again, this is counter-intuitive and problematic, since it would be expected the results for un-rewarded stimulus would provide the baseline. In addition, the fact the significant change is now in the cortical area corresponding to the non-rewarded location is opposite to the results from the intrinsic optical imaging, where the change was seen in the cortical area corresponding to the rewarded location. Therefore, contrary to the author's assertion, the results from these two imaging modalities are not entirely consistent. This should be discussed more directly.

IOS ΔOverlap is a key measure put forth by the authors in advancing the narrative that conditioning results in a greater separation between the nR and R location stimuli-evoked responses in comparison to that of the non-conditioned (orthogonal) stimuli. They imply with this measure that post conditioning, NR and R stimuli decrease in their overlap; but, as the measure is a difference of differences, I think it can be misleading to refer to the effect observed as such. For instance, let us suppose that conditioning was to elevate (across its spatial extent) the IOS response to the R stimulus and decrease the response to the nR. The resulting "mountain" of overlap for the conditioned case would be shifted toward the nR location. By subtracting it from the mountain of overlap for the ortho case (which is used as a proxy for what might be expected were the conditioned orientation to be pseudo-conditioned), a profile of ΔOverlap like that shown in 2D bottom would result. So, the observed profile can arise not from increasing the spatial segregation of the nR and R stimuli in the conditioned case, but rather from an increase in the R stimulus evoked response across its spatial extent and a decrease in the NR response across its spatial extent. So, what then is the reported cause underlying the ΔOverlap? The authors use of the spatial selectivity index indicates that a feature of conditioning was that the amplitude of the nR was decreased. So, while I find the ΔOverlap a somewhat obtuse manner of assessing the data, and how it is presented and discussed to be somewhat confusing, these findings do support the conclusion that "conditioning using rewarded and non-rewarded stimuli occupying neighboring regions of visual space selectively affect the retinotopic organization for the conditioned stimuli compared to the control stimuli". It appears from these data that the main driver of the effect was to diminish the evoked response to the nR stim across its spatial extent in the conditioned case (and from Figure 2—figure supplement 2C bottom left, from elevating the response evoked by the R stim at the NR location).

2) The analysis on the fraction of tuned neurons might be flawed (Figure 4). In particular, the left panel of Figure 4A shows that the fraction of orientation neurons was higher in the cortical region for the rewarded location, and the results in the right panel of Figure 4B are normalized, presumably according to the overall proportion for each cortical area (since their averages look comparable for both regions). This implies that the main change might be for the untested orientations in non-rewarded region. Again, this is counter-intuitive and would be inconsistent with the author's conclusion that the representation of the rewarded stimulus became sparser.

3) The analysis presented in the manuscript does not convincingly demonstrate that the representation of conditioned stimulus became sparser and stronger at the same time. In order to show this, it would be better to examine the entire distribution of tuning strengths across the entire population, rather than calculating the sparsity and strength using an arbitrary criterion.

eLife. 2018 Sep 17;7:e37683. doi: 10.7554/eLife.37683.020

Author response


[…] Essential revisions:

1) The most significant weakness of this manuscript is its heavy reliance on the measure related to the changes in the overlap in the neural activity induced by different visual stimuli across the cortical surface.

For the intrinsic imaging data, the authors focus on the changes in the overlap in the neural activity across the cortical surface. Thanks to the data presented in Figure 2—figure supplement 1, however, it can be seen that in reality the data are a bit more complicated. […] The authors should discuss this finding more directly, rather than focusing on the change in the overlap instead.

The results from the intrinsic and calcium imaging experiments are also only superficially similar. The reduction in the overlap for tested orientation is driven mostly change in the "full" non-rewarded location (Figure 3C and 3D). Again, this is counter-intuitive and problematic, since it would be expected the results for un-rewarded stimulus would provide the baseline. In addition, the fact the significant change is now in the cortical area corresponding to the non-rewarded location is opposite to the results from the intrinsic optical imaging, where the change was seen in the cortical area corresponding to the rewarded location. Therefore, contrary to the author's assertion, the results from these two imaging modalities are not entirely consistent. This should be discussed more directly.

IOS ΔOverlap is a key measure put forth by the authors in advancing the narrative that conditioning results in a greater separation between the nR and R location stimuli-evoked responses in comparison to that of the non-conditioned (orthogonal) stimuli. They imply with this measure that post conditioning, NR and R stimuli decrease in their overlap; but, as the measure is a difference of differences, I think it can be misleading to refer to the effect observed as such. […] It appears from these data that the main driver of the effect was to diminish the evoked response to the nR stim across its spatial extent in the conditioned case (and from Figure 2—figure supplement 2C bottom left, from elevating the response evoked by the R stim at the NR location).

From this comment we understand that the reviewers have concerns regarding (1) the interpretation of the intrinsic imaging data (in particular the measure of Overlap) which we address first, and (2) with the link between the intrinsic imaging data and the calcium imaging data as presented in Figure 3, which we will discuss further below.

Regarding the intrinsic imaging data:

The reviewers suggested that, conceptually, the change in response amplitude may be the most basic parameter describing the learning-related changes in intrinsic signal responses, as opposed to the derived parameter of Overlap. We agree with the reviewers that the reduction in response amplitude to the non-rewarded trained stimulus is an important aspect of the change after conditioning and have therefore adjusted the order in which we present the data. We now clearly and directly indicate in the first paragraph of the subsection “Mesoscopic shifts in cortical representations of conditioned stimuli” that we observe a reduction in response amplitude to the trained non-rewarded stimulus (as compared to the orthogonal). In order to facilitate interpretation of the effect by the reader, we have added the visualization of the average real data response profiles (originally presented in Figure 2—figure supplement 2C) in the main Figure 2D and we have added a Figure panel (2G) showing the average response amplitude across the different imaging time points (controlled for the amplitude to the orthogonal control stimuli).

In the next paragraph of the manuscript (subsection “Mesoscopic shifts in cortical representations of conditioned stimuli”), we describe how the change in response amplitude can lead to a reduction in overlap of the cortical representations and again facilitate this by showing now the overlap-profiles of all time points (originally presented in Figure 2—figure supplement 2D) in the main Figure (2F). In addition, Figure 2H shows the average difference in overlap between conditioned and control stimuli across the consecutive imaging time points. We believe that by presenting the data more extensively and in this particular order, the reader should be able to better judge how the intrinsic signal response changes as result of conditioning and better understand how overlap is computed from the ‘raw’ response amplitude.

Furthermore, as the reviewers mentioned, the measure of overlap is indeed sensitive to the ‘raw’ response amplitude. We still show the measure of Overlap because we think that it captures best how the two cortico-topically adjacent representations segregate. But by now displaying the curves of Figure 2D and 2F directly below each other in the main figure, the reader can see better how the overlap depends on the amplitude. Moreover, we have (1) acknowledged the potential confounder of response amplitude directly in the manuscript in subsection and (2) presented the results obtained using the ratiometric spatial selectivity index now also visually in the main Figure 2I.

In order to more explicitly address the question of whether the representation of the Rewarded and Non-rewarded stimulus drifted apart in cortical space (i.e. by a translation on the cortical plane, not an amplitude modulation), we have added an analysis of the distance between the peaks of the conditioned stimulus representations (subsection “Spatial organization of neuronal population activity for conditioned stimuli”). While after conditioning they appeared to be slightly further apart compared to the control orientations, this was not significantly different from chance.

In addition, we would like to point out that all three measures (response amplitude to the non-rewarded conditioned stimulus, overlap and spatial selectivity index) are strongly correlated with each other and therefore most likely reflect closely related mechanisms operating on both the rewarded and non-rewarded side of the conditioned stimulus representation (subsection “Spatial organization of neuronal population activity for conditioned stimuli”).

Thus, we observed closely related changes in overlap, spatial selectivity index and the response amplitude to the non-rewarded trained stimulus. That the response amplitude to the non-rewarded stimulus was reduced after conditioning is our opinion not necessarily counter-intuitive. A reduction in response amplitude might occur if an LTD type mechanism engages during conditioning at times when the non-rewarded stimulus presentation is not followed by reward, thus reducing the overall excitatory drive onto the non-rewarded stimulus representation (see e.g. Navabi et al., 2014). Alternatively, it could reflect a strengthening of feed-back connections from e.g. higher visual areas onto local inhibitory neurons increasing inhibitory drive of the non-rewarded stimulus representation (Makino and Komiyama 2015). These considerations have been added to the Discussion section.

Regarding the link between intrinsic imaging and calcium imaging data:

The reviewers stated that the results obtained using calcium imaging are not consistent with those obtained using intrinsic imaging. Specifically, the reviewers note that the change in Ca-Overlap is on the Full Non-rewarded side. We address this issue by providing better explanation and discussion of the similarities as well as the differences between the two methods:

1) Figure 3C shows the reduction in Ca-Overlap, which is greatest for the Border Non-rewarded and Border Rewarded locations but is also present for the Full Rewarded region. In the intrinsic imaging data, the reduction is indeed more strongly biased to the Rewarded side. In order to prevent further confusion on these results, we have added an extra sentence stating this similarity as well as this difference in the results (subsection “Spatial organization of neuronal population activity for conditioned stimuli”) and added a paragraph on the comparison of intrinsic imaging data in the Discussion section. We would like to note here that both the measures of Overlap and of Spatial selectivity correlated positively between methods (calcium imaging and intrinsic imaging; see subsection “Spatial organization of neuronal population activity for conditioned stimuli”).

2) Figure 3D shows the fraction of tuned neurons, which is a measure of how many neurons are significantly tuned to the orientation of the grating stimuli. This measure is in our opinion not directly comparable with intrinsic imaging data, as it does not take into account the actual response amplitude of the neurons. Therefore, this does not argue in favor, nor against, similarity with the intrinsic imaging data. We adjusted the text on this point in subsection “Orientation-tuned neurons respond more sparsely and strongly to rewarded conditioned stimuli”).

3) Finally, although the calcium imaging response amplitude to the trained non-rewarded stimulus appeared lower compared to the response amplitude of the control orientation in the non-rewarded stimulus location (similar effect as in the intrinsic imaging data, see Figure 3E and inset for comparison), this difference was not significantly different and therefore not entirely consistent. In order to be fully transparent, we specifically point this out in the Results section. We think that this difference between calcium imaging and intrinsic imaging data might, in part, be due to the different nature of the two methods (with intrinsic imaging measuring a mixed signal across layers, reflecting metabolism and oxygen consumption, depending on activity of somata, dendrites, synapses, glia and axons versus calcium activity faithfully reflecting layer 2/3 somatic spiking activity). We have added this point to the Discussion section.

We would like to add that the response amplitudes observed using calcium imaging and intrinsic imaging show differences in the same direction (comparison of main panel with inset in Figure 3E), and that the reduction in response amplitude to the Non-rewarded stimulus (compared to control) is positively correlated in the (few) mice where we have data using both techniques (subsection “Orientation-tuned neurons respond more sparsely and strongly to rewarded conditioned stimuli”).

In conclusion, we acknowledge that the data obtained using intrinsic imaging and calcium imaging are not an exact mirror image of each other, but also argue that the results are comparable to a reasonable degree, especially considering the quite different nature of the two methods. We believe that the data obtained using each method provide evidence suggesting (1) that the spatial selectivity of cortical neuronal activity patterns improves compared to the control stimuli, and (2) that the changes following conditioning are expressed in only a select group of neurons defined by the neurons’ preferred orientation and spatial location on the cortical retinotopic map (this is also stated in subsection “Orientation-tuned neurons respond more sparsely and strongly to rewarded conditioned stimuli”).

2) The analysis on the fraction of tuned neurons might be flawed (Figure 4). In particular, the left panel of Figure 4A shows that the fraction of orientation neurons was higher in the cortical region for the rewarded location, and the results in the right panel of Figure 4B are normalized, presumably according to the overall proportion for each cortical area (since their averages look comparable for both regions). This implies that the main change might be for the untested orientations in non-rewarded region. Again, this is counter-intuitive and would be inconsistent with the author's conclusion that the representation of the rewarded stimulus became sparser.

Figure 4A, left panel, indeed shows that the fraction of neurons that responded to the Rewarded region (17.8%) was slightly (but non-significantly, Wilcoxon Matched-pairs signed-rank test, p=0.69) larger than the fraction of neurons that responded to the non-rewarded region (14.5%). However, the normalization of the fraction of responsive neurons per orientation and rewarded/non-rewarded region (for original Figure 4A) was done using the summed total of all responsive neurons across both rewarded and non-rewarded regions, and all preferred orientations per mouse. The normalization was done such that if the preferred orientations of a mouse would have a flat distribution, each bin would have the value of 1.0 (irrespective of the absolute fraction of responsive cells per mouse, and irrespective of the bin width). We realize that the figure legend described the normalization procedure rather briefly, and wrongly suggested that we normalized data by the mean of the individual orientation bins. The specific reason to perform the normalization per mouse was to ensure that the analysis of the fraction of tuned neurons per orientation/region was not biased by variation in the absolute number of responsive neurons across individual mice and variation in recording regions across mice. Therefore, we would like to emphasize that the actual analysis was not flawed (only the description in the figure legend was possibly unclear). We have added a statement on the overall difference in numbers of responsive neurons in subsection “Improved neuronal population coding for conditioned stimulus location”, expanded Figure 4 to better reflect the absolute and relative fractions of tuned neurons, added explanation to the legend of Figure 4, and added a statement explaining how the normalization was done in subsection “Improved neuronal population coding for conditioned stimulus location”.

As the reviewer indicates, it remains possible that conditioning affected the neuronal representation of the non-trained orientation. The most straightforward way to address this question is to compare timepoints before and after conditioning, but the calcium imaging data have no such control as they were obtained in an acute experiment. The intrinsic imaging experiment was repeated across timepoints, but the response amplitude of the intrinsic signal hardly compares to the fraction of responsive neurons obtained using calcium imaging. However, some effects of conditioning were very selective for cells exactly tuned to the conditioned orientation (e.g. the increased response amplitude to rewarded stimulus presentation in Figure 4E and the improved decoding performance in figure 5A). This argues that (at least) in these cases the rewarded conditioned stimulus representation stood out from the oblique and orthogonal control orientations, increasing the likelihood that changes were expressed in the rewarded stimulus representation selectively, rather than in the general population of neurons excluding the rewarded representation. We have addressed this potential caveat in a paragraph in the Discussion section.

3) The analysis presented in the manuscript does not convincingly demonstrate that the representation of conditioned stimulus became sparser and stronger at the same time. In order to show this, it would be better to examine the entire distribution of tuning strengths across the entire population, rather than calculating the sparsity and strength using an arbitrary criterion.

As the reviewer requested, we have quantified the sparseness of the population tuning curve responses across all cells using the measure described in Rolls and Treves (2011). Although the overall population tuning curve response was slightly sparser for orientations that were more similar to the trained orientation presented in the rewarded region, this difference was not significant. We have added this result to the manuscript in subsection “Effects of conditioning on signal and noise correlations” and as a new panel in Figure 4G.

In order to better describe these data, we have adapted the manuscript to remove the conclusion that the population response was “sparser but stronger”. Instead the manuscript now reports that there were fewer neurons significantly responding to the rewarded orientation in the rewarded location (as is most significantly evident from Figure 3D, but from Figure 4D as well) and that the neurons that were still significantly responding, did so with a larger amplitude (as shown in Figure 4E). This has been updated in the Results section and the Discussion section.

Associated Data

    This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

    Supplementary Materials

    Transparent reporting form
    DOI: 10.7554/eLife.37683.015

    Data Availability Statement

    The full dataset of this study is available on gin.g-node, url: https://web.gin.g-node.org/pgoltstein/goltstein_meijer_pennartz_2018

    The following dataset was generated:

    Pieter M Goltstein, author; Guido T Meijer, author; Cyriel MA Pennartz, author. Behavioral-, intrinsic imaging-, calcium imaging- and eye-tracking-data. 2010 https://web.gin.g-node.org/pgoltstein/goltstein_meijer_pennartz_2018 Publicly available at the Gin: Modern Research Data Management for Neuroscience website.


    Articles from eLife are provided here courtesy of eLife Sciences Publications, Ltd

    RESOURCES