Abstract
Importance
Presently, 81 countries mandate the fortification of grain products with folic acid to lessen the risk of neural tube defects in the developing fetus. Epidemiologic data on severe mental illness suggest potentially broader effects of prenatal folate exposure on postnatal brain development, but this link remains unsubstantiated by biological evidence.
Objective
To evaluate associations among fetal folic acid exposure, cortical maturation, and psychiatric risk in youths.
Design, Setting, and Participants
A retrospective, observational clinical cohort study was conducted at Massachusetts General Hospital (MGH) among 292 youths 8 to 18 years of age born between January 1993 and December 2001 (inclusive of folic acid fortification rollout ±3.5 years) with normative results of clinical magnetic resonance imaging, divided into 3 age-matched groups based on birthdate and related level of prenatal folic acid fortification exposure (none, partial, or full). Magnetic resonance imaging was performed between January 2005 and March 2015. Two independent, observational, community-based cohorts (Philadelphia Neurodevelopmental Cohort [PNC] and National Institutes of Health Magnetic Resonance Imaging Study of Normal Brain Development [NIH]) comprising 1078 youths 8 to 18 years of age born throughout (PNC, 1992-2003) or before (NIH, 1983-1995) the rollout of folic acid fortification were studied for replication, clinical extension, and specificity. Statistical analysis was conducted from 2015 to 2018.
Exposures
United States–mandated grain product fortification with folic acid, introduced in late 1996 and fully in effect by mid-1997.
Main Outcomes and Measures
Differences in cortical thickness among nonexposed, partially exposed, and fully exposed youths (MGH) and underlying associations between age and cortical thickness (all cohorts). Analysis of the PNC cohort also examined the association of age–cortical thickness slopes with the odds of psychotic symptoms.
Results
The MGH cohort (139 girls and 153 boys; mean [SD] age, 13.3 [2.3] years) demonstrated exposure-associated cortical thickness increases in bilateral frontal and temporal regions (9.9% to 11.6%; corrected P < .001 to P = .03) and emergence of quadratic (delayed) age-associated thinning in temporal and parietal regions (β = –11.1 to –13.9; corrected P = .002). The contemporaneous PNC cohort (417 girls and 444 boys; mean [SD] age, 13.5 [2.7] years) also exhibited exposure-associated delays of cortical thinning (β = –1.59 to –1.73; corrected P < .001 to P = .02), located in similar regions and with similar durations of delay as in the MGH cohort. Flatter thinning profiles in frontal, temporal, and parietal regions were associated with lower odds of psychosis spectrum symptoms in the PNC cohort (odds ratio, 0.37-0.59; corrected P < .05). All identified regions displayed earlier thinning in the nonexposed NIH cohort (118 girls and 99 boys; mean [SD] age, 13.3 [2.6] years).
Conclusions and Relevance
The results of this study suggest an association between gestational exposure to fortification of grain products with folic acid and altered cortical development and, in turn, with reduction in the risk of psychosis in youths.
This cohort study evaluates associations among fetal folic acid exposure through fortification of grain products, cortical maturation, and psychiatric risk in youths.
Key Points
Question
Is increased fetal exposure to folic acid, implemented through population-wide fortification of grain products, associated with clinically meaningful changes in postnatal brain development?
Findings
In a cohort of 292 youths 8 to 18 years of age with normative results of clinical magnetic resonance imaging, delayed age-associated thinning of the cerebral cortex, a pattern suggesting reduced risk for severe mental illness, emerged among individuals who gestated during and after the fortification rollout in the United States (1996-1997). Studies of 2 additional independent US cohorts (N = 1078) confirmed the reliability and temporal specificity of fortification-associated delays of cortical thinning and demonstrated an associated reduction in psychosis risk.
Meaning
Beyond its known association with the prevention of neural tube defects, increased gestational exposure to folic acid through food fortification may protect against psychosis through altered postnatal cortical development.
Introduction
In March 1996, the US government mandated that all food manufacturers fortify enriched grain products with 140 μg of folic acid per 100 g of food by January 1, 1998.1 This intervention was implemented to provide increased fetal exposure to folic acid (a synthetic and more highly bioavailable form of naturally occurring folate) in the first month of gestation, a time critical to neural tube development but before many pregnancies are recognized. The fortification rollout rapidly doubled blood folate levels in women of childbearing age2 and substantially diminished neural tube defects in newborns.3,4 At present, 81 countries require folic acid fortification of grain products (eFigure 1 in the Supplement).
Folate may play other important roles in the development of the fetal central nervous system, given its contributions to DNA synthesis, DNA and histone methylation, and gene expression. The hypothesis that prenatal exposure to folate may also influence postnatal brain development arises in part from epidemiologic studies that linked starvation during early fetal life with both neural tube defects and schizophrenia,5 and is further supported by studies that linked periconceptional folic acid supplements to lower risk of language delay and autism6,7,8,9; however, 1 study failed to find such an association.10 A critical unanswered question is whether variation in fetal exposure to folate subsequently influences brain development during the formative years preceding late adolescence and early adulthood, a period associated with heightened risk for psychiatric disorders.
The present study used the US rollout of folic acid fortification of grain products to examine the association between increased fetal exposure to folic acid and subsequent cortical development. Our primary measurement was cortical thickness obtained from magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) scans because it provides a clinically relevant developmental marker. Studies of healthy pediatric samples reveal a steady age-associated decrease in thickness across most of the cortical mantle,11 a pattern thought to reflect synaptic pruning12 and cortical myelination.13,14 Whereas the trajectory of thinning is typically linear,11 departures from this pattern can have functional consequences; delayed onset of thinning has been associated with higher intelligence,15 but accelerated loss of gray matter has been described in patients with schizophrenia16 and their unaffected relatives,17 as well as in school-aged children with autism.18
Using data from normative clinical brain MRI scans accessed from the Massachusetts General Hospital (MGH),19 we compared cortical thickness indices within a large cohort of youths born just before, during, or just after the rollout of folic acid fortification and who, therefore, would have received progressively greater exposure to folic acid during gestation. Although little, if any, fortification was in place by September 1996, its rapid deployment ensured that the transition was nearly complete within New England by July 1997.20 Therefore, comparison groups were predefined based on date of birth, so that no individuals in the pre-rollout group (born prior to July 1, 1996) were exposed to fortification during any part of gestation, every individual in the post-rollout group (born after June 30, 1998) was exposed during the entire pregnancy, and individuals in the rollout group (born between these dates) were intermediately exposed. We then turned to 2 additional large, US-based pediatric MRI repositories, the Philadelphia Neurodevelopmental Cohort (PNC) and the National Institutes of Health MRI Study of Normal Brain Development (NIH), to test the reliability and specificity of fortification-related associations with cortical development, and the relevance of fortification-associated MRI changes to psychopathologic characteristics.
Methods
MGH Cohort
Patients with brain MRI scans were identified through purposeful sampling of the MGH medical record (eFigure 2A in the Supplement). The search returned 3311 radiology reports, based on both general inclusion criteria (8.0-18.0 years of age at time of scan, date of birth between January 1993 and December 2001, and MRI scans occurring between January 2005 and March 2015) and a predetermined algorithm to optimize age matching of groups. After excluding MRI scans with abnormalities that were identified in the corresponding radiology reports (eTable 1 in the Supplement), and then subjecting the remaining scans to stringent quality control procedures blinded to birthdate (eFigure 2B in the Supplement), we arrived at 292 usable, clinically normative scans, comprising 97 pre-rollout (nonexposed), 96 rollout (partially exposed), and 99 post-rollout (fully exposed) unique individuals (Table 1 and the eAppendix in the Supplement). Study procedures were approved by Partners Human Research Committee, which granted a waiver of informed consent, since this retrospective study of the medical record involved only deidentified data.
Table 1. Characteristics of Massachusetts General Hospital Cohort Participants.
Characteristic | Participants, No. (%) | Statistics | P Value | ||
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Nonexposed (n = 97) | Partially Exposed (n = 96) | Fully Exposed (n = 99) | |||
Participant-Level Data (RPDR) | |||||
Age, mean (SD), y | 13.3 (2.1) | 13.5 (2.8) | 12.9 (2.0) | F = 1.72 | .18 |
Female sex | 49 (50.5) | 43 (44.8) | 47 (47.5) | χ2 = 0.67 | .72 |
Race/ethnicity | |||||
African American | 6 (6.2) | 3 (3.1) | 6 (6.1) | FE = 6.21 | .81 |
Asian | 5 (5.2) | 2 (2.1) | 3 (3.0) | ||
White | 71 (73.2) | 69 (71.9) | 73 (73.7) | ||
Hispanic | 9 (9.3) | 11 (11.5) | 7 (7.1) | ||
Not recorded | 2 (2.1) | 6 (6.3) | 6 (6.1) | ||
Other | 4 (4.1) | 5 (5.2) | 4 (4.0) | ||
Insurance | |||||
Private | 58 (59.8) | 48 (50.0) | 67 (67.6) | FE = 6.41 | .13 |
Public | 38 (39.2) | 46 (47.9) | 31 (31.3) | ||
Other | 1 (1.0) | 2 (2.1) | 1 (1.0) | ||
Scanner | |||||
1.5-T General Electric | 66 (68.0) | 41 (42.7) | 25 (25.3) | FE = 45.5 | <.001 |
1.5-T Siemens Avanto | 6 (6.2) | 2 (2.1) | 3 (3.0) | ||
1.5-T Siemens Aera | 0 | 0 | 1 (1.0) | ||
3.0-T Siemens Trio | 24 (24.7) | 50 (52.1) | 67 (67.7) | ||
3.0-T Siemens Skyra | 1 (1.0) | 3 (3.1) | 3 (3.0) | ||
Neighborhood Block-Level Data (ACS)a | |||||
Household income, median, $ | 81 252 | 79 368 | 85 361 | F = 0.59 | .59 |
Unemployment | 8.6 | 9.7 | 8.8 | F = 1.28 | .28 |
Highest educational level | |||||
No high school | 5.4 | 5.3 | 4.1 | Group: F = 1.02; group × level: F = 0.53 | .36; .89 |
Some high school | 6.0 | 6.0 | 5.5 | ||
High school graduate | 26.3 | 26.9 | 26.2 | ||
Some college | 15.2 | 14.4 | 14.2 | ||
Associates degree | 7.2 | 7.7 | 7.3 | ||
Bachelor’s degree | 22.2 | 21.0 | 23.6 | ||
Graduate degree | 17.8 | 17.4 | 19.0 | ||
Vitamin consumption | |||||
Households reporting use (index) | 105.6 | 104.6 | 107.3 | F = 0.76 | .47 |
Spending per household, $ | 76.6 | 76.1 | 82.3 | F = 0.81 | .45 |
Reason for MRI Scan | |||||
Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder | 1 (1.0) | 1 (1.0) | 0 | FE = 1.28 | .55 |
Altered mental status | 9 (9.3) | 4 (4.2) | 9 (9.1) | FE = 2.44 | .34 |
Ataxia | 4 (4.1) | 5 (5.2) | 2 (2.0) | FE = 1.46 | .44 |
Autism | 3 (3.1) | 9 (9.4) | 7 (7.1) | FE = 3.28 | .18 |
Cognitive delay or learning disability | 2 (2.1) | 5 (5.2) | 8 (8.1) | FE = 3.60 | .16 |
Family history of neurologic disorder | 0 | 2 (2.1) | 1 (1.0) | FE = 1.86 | .44 |
Focal neurologic finding | 8 (8.2) | 13 (13.5) | 11 (11.1) | χ2 = 1.39 | .50 |
Head injury | 8 (8.2) | 7 (7.3) | 9 (9.1) | χ2 = 0.21 | .96 |
Non-CNS tumor or surgery | 3 (3.1) | 3 (3.1) | 3 (3.0) | FE = 0.14 | >.99 |
Not given | 1 (1.0) | 1 (1.0) | 0 | FE = 1.28 | .55 |
Pituitary or endocrine | 1 (1.0) | 3 (3.1) | 9 (9.1) | FE = 7.48 | .02 |
Psychosis | 0 | 2 (2.1) | 0 | FE = 2.71 | .11 |
Seizures or epilepsy | 21 (21.6) | 29 (30.2) | 27 (27.3) | χ2 = 1.88 | .40 |
Somatic symptomsb | 48 (49.5) | 37 (38.5) | 45 (45.5) | χ2 = 2.39 | .31 |
Syncope | 2 (2.1) | 4 (4.2) | 2 (2.0) | FE = 1.05 | .61 |
Previous Medication Use | |||||
Psychotropic medications | |||||
Anticonvulsants | 15 (15.5) | 19 (19.8) | 19 (19.4) | χ2 = 0.74 | .70 |
Antidepressants | 11 (11.3) | 12 (12.5) | 17 (17.2) | χ2 = 1.58 | .46 |
Antipsychotics | 8 (8.2) | 7 (7.3) | 7 (7.1) | χ2 = 0.11 | .96 |
Benzodiazepines | 11 (11.3) | 14 (14.6) | 20 (20.2) | χ2 = 3.03 | .23 |
Stimulants | 3 (3.1) | 6 (6.3) | 11 (11.1) | FE = 4.81 | .08 |
Folic acid or multivitamin | 2 (2.1) | 2 (2.1) | 2 (2.0) | FE = 0.22 | >.99 |
Abbreviations: ACS, 2010 American Community Survey; CNS, central nervous system; FE, Fisher exact test (used in lieu of the χ2 test when <5 patients appeared in at least 1 cell); MRI, magnetic resonance imaging; RPDR, Research Patient Data Registry.
Only percentage data, not numbers of individuals, were available from the ACS.
Somatic symptoms: nausea, headache, and/or dizziness.
Although rapid implementation of the fortification rollout and use of age-matched comparison groups diminished the risk for temporal confounding, we assessed numerous factors that could potentially influence any between-group differences in cortical thickness. To account for socioeconomic and biological diversity in the sample, we extracted from the electronic medical record demographic and socioeconomic information (age at scan, sex, race/ethnicity, and public vs private insurance); reason for MRI scan; and previous use of psychotropic medications, folic acid, or multivitamins. To provide additional measures of socioeconomic status and vitamin use, we performed geospatial analysis to tag patients’ last known addresses to block-level data from the 2010 American Community Survey.21 These data included median household income, household educational attainment, unemployment rate, vitamin intake, and vitamin-related spending (eAppendix in the Supplement).
PNC Cohort
The PNC participants have been described elsewhere.22 In brief, participants included here comprised a subset of 861 individuals, 8.0 to 18.0 years of age, recruited from community health settings in Philadelphia, Pennsylvania. Participants underwent standardized clinical and MRI assessment using a single 3-T magnet. Clinical assessment23 characterized participants as either typically developing or exhibiting psychiatric symptoms, categorized as psychosis spectrum, attenuated psychosis, or other types of psychopathologic conditions (eAppendix in the Supplement). All included MRI scans passed stringent quality control as previously described.24 The distribution of birthdates among 8- to 18-year-olds in the PNC sample was such that nonexposed (n = 322), partially exposed (n = 189), and fully exposed (n = 350) individuals were well represented (eFigure 3 in the Supplement). Study procedures were approved by the institutional review boards of the Children’s Hospital of Philadelphia and the University of Pennsylvania. Adult participants provided written informed consent. Minors provided assent, and their parent or guardian provided written informed consent.
NIH Cohort
The NIH participants have been characterized elsewhere.25 In brief, healthy youths were recruited across 6 sites nationwide and underwent MRI scans on 1.5-T magnets up to 3 times at various ages. Here, we selected a subsample from this cohort whose MRI scans previously passed stringent image quality control,11 and we constrained it to our age interval of interest (8.0-18.0 years). We also excluded participants who might have been exposed to folic acid fortification based on their age at first enrollment. The final sample included 217 individual participants and 383 MRI scans. All procedures were approved by the relevant institutional review board at each of the 6 pediatric study centers, where the MRI scans took place, at a clinical coordinating center at Washington University in St Louis, and at a data coordinating center at the Montreal Neurological Institute, McGill University. Written informed consent was obtained from parents and adult participants, and minors provided assent.
Statistical Analysis
Primary analyses used general linear models in FreeSurfer, version 5.0 (Martinos Center for Biomedical Imaging). Main analyses in the MGH cohort contrasted mean cortical thickness and age-associated change in thickness (linear and quadratic models) in the fully exposed vs nonexposed groups. Main analyses in the contemporaneous PNC cohort and comparison NIH cohort assessed for significant quadratic associations of age with cortical thickness across each cohort. Nuisance variables, including age, sex, total brain volume (all cohorts), scanner field strength (MGH), and site (NIH), were entered as covariates of no interest. To ensure coverage of the entire cortex, we did not limit the analysis to a priori anatomical regions of interest. Rather, we used 10 000 Monte Carlo simulations to determine whether identified clusters bounded by a vertexwise threshold of P < .05 were sufficiently large to survive control for multiple comparisons across the entire surface (clusterwise P < .05). For clusters demonstrating significant quadratic associations of age with thickness, the delay in cortical thinning (ie, time until thinning onset) was estimated using least squares analyses (MATLAB, version R2015b; Mathworks Inc). To assess the associations of cortical thinning delay with clinical outcomes in the PNC cohort, multinomial logistic regression examined the association of the local age-thickness slope with adjusted odds for diagnosis of psychosis spectrum, attenuated psychosis, and other types of psychopathologic conditions compared with typically developing participants (SPSS, version 25 [SPSS Inc]; eAppendix in the Supplement).
Results
MGH Cohort
Included and excluded patients were comparable across demographic measures (eTable 2 in the Supplement). Among included individuals, exposure groups did not differ significantly by age at MRI scan, sex, scan indication, or insurance status; a slight but nonsignificant increase in use of psychotropic medications was noted over time, consistent with previous epidemiologic studies.26 The distribution of scanner field strengths differed among groups owing to a shift from 1.5- to 3-T clinical magnets in the late 2000s, a factor taken into account in the main analyses. Tagging patients’ last known addresses to block-level data obtained through the 2010 American Community Survey, we observed no differences among fortification groups in per capita use or spending on nonprescription vitamins, or in other measures that could affect folate intake (eg, median income, household educational level, or unemployment rate) (Table 1).
Group differences in cortical thickness were observed in bilateral frontal and inferior temporal regions. In each significant cluster, cortical thickness was higher in the fully exposed group compared with the nonexposed group, with intermediate effects observed in the partially exposed group (Figure 1A and B; eFigure 4 and eTable 3 in the Supplement). Sensitivity analyses revealed significant associations of scanner field strength and manufacturer with cortical thickness, but the direction of these associations varied by region, consistent with prior studies.27,28 Between-group differences remained significant after adjustment for these variables, and no significant group × field strength or group × manufacturer interactions were observed (eTable 4 in the Supplement).
To understand this pattern in the context of age-associated change in cortical thickness, we next assessed for group differences in intercept (ie, thickness means centered at 8 years of age) and slope (linear age effects), as well as for any differences in nonlinear (quadratic) age-associated change. Intercept in the bilateral frontal cortex (pars orbitalis and precentral) and the right inferior temporal gyrus was higher in the fully exposed group than in the nonexposed group; these differences diminished with age (Figure 1C; eFigure 5 and eTable 3 in the Supplement; and Video). There were no significant differences in the cortical thickness–age slope that contributed to the increased cortical thickness in the exposed groups. However, in the left inferior temporal cortex, as well as in the left inferior parietal cortex, we observed significant differences in age-squared effects, where cortical thinning in fully exposed participants was delayed compared with cortical thinning in nonexposed participants (Figure 2A and B; and eTable 5 in the Supplement). Additional modeling using least squares regression localized the onset of cortical thinning in fully exposed participants, defined by the optimal break point between flat and sloped lines, to 13.0 years of age (left inferior temporal gyrus) and 13.8 years of age (left inferior parietal lobule; eFigure 6A in the Supplement).
PNC and NIH Cohorts
The MGH data set relied on clinical MRI scans that were acquired through nonuniform clinical protocols, using different magnets, and within a single US city. To verify and generalize the findings from the MGH cohort, we next turned to 2 additional large US cohorts that were studied prospectively in standardized research settings (Table 2): 1 cohort with birthdates centered around the folic acid fortification rollout and MRI scans performed with a single 3-T magnet (PNC; eFigure 3 in the Supplement), and 1 cohort that included only youths who were born prior to folic acid fortification (NIH).
Table 2. Characteristics of PNC and NIH Cohort Participants.
Characteristic | PNC Cohort (N = 861) | NIH Cohort (N = 217) | ||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Nonexposed (n = 322) | Partially Exposed (n = 189) | Fully Exposed (n = 350) | Statistics | P Value | ||
Age, mean (SD), y | 16.3 (1.0) | 13.9 (0.8) | 10.7 (1.4) | F = 2032 | <.001 | 13.3 (2.6)a |
Female sex, No. (%) | 183 (56.8) | 95 (50.3) | 166 (47.4) | χ2 = 6.10 | .05 | 118 (54.4) |
Race, No. (%) | ||||||
African American | 132 (41.0) | 89 (47.1) | 151 (43.1) | χ2 = 5.35 | .72 | 18 (8.3) |
American Indian or Alaskan | 0 | 1 (0.5) | 1 (0.3) | 0 | ||
Asian | 4 (1.2) | 1 (0.5) | 6 (1.7) | 1 (0.5) | ||
White | 152 (47.2) | 77 (40.7) | 151 (43.1) | 171 (78.8) | ||
Hawaiian or Pacific Islander | 0 | 0 | 0 | 0 | ||
>1 Race | 34 (10.6) | 21 (11.1) | 41 (11.7) | 27 (12.4) | ||
Ethnicity, No. (%) Hispanic | 15 (4.7) | 10 (5.3) | 36 (10.3) | χ2 = 9.25 | .01 | 29 (13.4) |
Maternal educational level, No. (%) | ||||||
<High school graduate | 13 (4.0) | 8 (4.2) | 18 (5.1) | χ2 = 15.1 | .23 | 1 (0.5) |
High school graduate | 109 (33.9) | 63 (33.3) | 103 (29.4) | 33 (15.2) | ||
Some college or Associates degree | 60 (18.6) | 56 (29.6) | 87 (24.9) | 61 (28.1) | ||
Bachelor’s degree | 80 (24.8) | 37 (19.6) | 91 (26.0) | 73 (33.6) | ||
Some graduate education or Master’s degree | 35 (10.9) | 11 (5.8) | 30 (8.6) | 6 (2.8) | ||
Graduate degree | 13 (4.0) | 8 (4.2) | 13 (3.7) | 41 (18.9) | ||
Data not available | 12 (3.7) | 6 (3.2) | 8 (2.3) | 2 (0.9) | ||
Paternal educational level, No. (%) | ||||||
<High school graduate | 14 (4.3) | 11 (5.8) | 23 (6.6) | χ2 = 17.5 | .13 | 5 (2.3) |
High school graduate | 117 (36.3) | 74 (39.2) | 136 (38.9) | 42 (19.4) | ||
Some college or Associates degree | 45 (14.0) | 36 (19.0) | 58 (16.6) | 52 (24.0) | ||
Bachelor’s degree | 67 (20.8) | 23 (12.2) | 59 (16.9) | 62 (28.6) | ||
Some graduate education or Master’s degree | 25 (7.8) | 15 (7.9) | 34 (9.7) | 7 (3.2) | ||
Graduate degree | 19 (5.9) | 5 (2.6) | 14 (4.0) | 47 (21.7) | ||
Data not available | 35 (10.9) | 25 (13.2) | 26 (7.4) | 2 (0.9) |
Abbreviations: NIH, National Institutes of Health Magnetic Resonance Imaging Study of Normal Brain Development; PNC, Philadelphia Neurodevelopmental Cohort.
Mean age of participant across all scans, including up to 3 scans per participant.
Because MRI scans in the PNC data set were collected during a relatively brief time, nonexposed participants were significantly older than partially exposed or fully exposed participants. Whereas between-group cortical thickness differences would be strongly confounded by age differences, examination of age-associated thinning contours provided an opportunity to replicate findings from the MGH cohort. Specifically, younger PNC participants (who were exposed to folic acid fortification) should demonstrate delayed cortical thinning, whereas younger NIH participants (who were not exposed to folic acid fortification) should not demonstrate delayed cortical thinning.
Within the PNC cohort, quadratic (delayed) age-related thinning was observed in 4 clusters that overlapped with those identified in the MGH analysis: left frontal, right inferior temporal, left inferior parietal, and right inferior parietal (Figure 2C and D; and eFigure 7A and B and eTable 6 in the Supplement). Least squares regression localized the onsets of cortical thinning to between 13.0 and 14.3 years of age (eFigure 6B in the Supplement). Conversely, bilateral lingual gyrus (which was not implicated in the MGH cohort) exhibited significantly accelerated cortical thinning.
To confirm that quadratic thinning effects emerged largely after folic acid fortification, we turned to the NIH cohort, in which all included participants were born prior to the rollout. Age-related associations with cortical thickness were assessed within the same 6 clusters that showed delayed thinning in the MGH (left inferior temporal gyrus and left inferior parietal lobule) or PNC (right inferior temporal gyrus, left frontal cortex, left inferior parietal lobule, and right inferior parietal lobule) cohorts. Consistent with a previous analysis11 that included a more extended age range and that was not limited to nonexposed participants, nonlinear cortical thinning in the NIH cohort was sparse, with only 1 cluster demonstrating significant quadratic thinning (left frontal cortex; Figure 2E and F; and eFigure 7C and eTable 7 in the Supplement). Even so, within this cluster, the break point for thinning occurred at a significantly younger age when compared with the PNC break point (χ2 = 11.87; P < .001; eFigure 6C in the Supplement).
Risk of Psychosis
To gauge the association of altered cortical thinning with clinically relevant phenotypes, we again turned to the PNC cohort, which included a detailed, standardized clinical characterization of all participants. Of the 861 youths included in the MRI analysis, clinical evaluations determined that 248 were typically developing, 199 had a diagnosis of psychosis spectrum, 105 had attenuated psychotic symptoms, and the remaining 309 had various other psychopathologic conditions, as previously described (eAppendix in the Supplement).23,24,29 For each of the 4 PNC regions that demonstrated postfortification quadratic thinning, best-fit local thinning slopes were calculated for each participant, based on linear change in cortical thickness across an optimized age range (1 year) centered around that participant (Figure 3A). These local slopes were then evaluated as factors associated with participant-level diagnosis of psychosis spectrum, attenuated psychosis, or other types of psychopathologic conditions vs typical development using multinomial logistic regression, controlling for nuisance covariates. Across 3 of 4 regions, flatter (ie, less negative) local slopes were associated with significantly reduced adjusted odds of psychosis spectrum diagnosis (odds ratio, 0.37-0.59; P < .001 to P = .02, Figure 3B and C). Local slopes were not associated with risk of other types of psychopathologic conditions in any region. For participants with attenuated psychosis, local slope associations were stronger than for other types of psychopathologic conditions, but nonsignificant.
Finally, to confirm that the associations of fortification exposure with cortical thinning were not themselves confounded by the inclusion of individuals with psychosis spectrum symptoms in the PNC cohort, we repeated the original analysis of quadratic thinning using only typically developing individuals or participants with other types of psychopathologic conditions (n = 541). Significant quadratic age-associated thinning persisted in all regions after exclusion of individuals with psychosis spectrum symptoms and youths with attenuated psychosis (eTable 8 in the Supplement).
Discussion
Evaluating 3 independent MRI cohorts of 8- to 18-year-old youths, we investigated the association of prenatal exposure to folic acid fortification with subsequent cortical development through adolescence. Within a large clinical cohort (MGH), we observed widespread increases in frontal and temporal cortical thickness between comparable groups of youths who gestated just after, compared with just before, the rollout of folic acid fortification. Youths who gestated during the rollout, and who therefore had partial exposure, demonstrated intermediate increases, consistent with a dose association. Exposure-associated differences were most pronounced in younger individuals, and in 2 regions (left inferior temporal and inferior parietal), we observed a delay in the onset of age-associated cortical thinning. An analogous pattern was evident in the contemporaneous PNC cohort: individuals who were exposed to folic acid fortification exhibited delays of cortical thinning of similar duration, which occurred in similar frontal, temporal, and parietal regions as those identified in the MGH cohort. Flatter age-related thinning profiles were associated with reduced risk of psychosis spectrum symptoms in this cohort. In contrast, the NIH cohort, comprising only individuals who were not exposed to fortification, exhibited earlier cortical thinning in the same regions. Collectively, these data suggest an association of prenatal exposure to folic acid fortification with increased cortical thickness through early adolescence, accompanied by delayed onset of cortical thinning and reduced risk of psychosis.
Adolescence directly precedes the period of greatest risk for psychiatric disorders, some of which are characterized by reductions in cortical thickness present at the onset of illness.30 Furthermore, some of the most severe child-onset psychiatric disorders, including autism and early-onset schizophrenia, are associated with marked accelerations in loss of gray matter during the age range that we studied.16,18 Within the PNC cohort, reductions in gray matter in multiple brain regions were associated with psychosis spectrum status in a previous analysis conducted without regard to exposure to folic acid fortification24; however, these regions differed from those demonstrating fortification-associated thinning delays herein. Rather, within these regions, shallower thinning slopes were associated with reduced risk for psychosis spectrum symptoms, suggesting a possible protective effect of fortification-associated delays in cortical thinning. This association was relatively specific for psychosis because local slopes were not associated with other psychopathologic conditions, and attenuated associations were seen in individuals with milder psychotic spectrum symptoms. The present findings are consistent with recent reports of salutary behavioral outcomes after periconceptional intake of folic acid6,7,8,9,31 and with a recent study linking maternal folate deficiency with reduced brain volume in a cohort of young European children.32
Limitations
Although relatively large imaging cohorts, replication and temporal specificity analyses using independent samples, and linkage of imaging and clinical findings represent the strengths of our study, a number of potential limitations warrant consideration. Unrecognized temporal confounders are of particular concern in natural experimental designs. Critically, the rapid deployment of folic acid fortification in the United States allowed us to study a relatively narrow and continuous range of birthdates, which diminished the risk of potential temporal confounders, including group differences in postnatal exposure to folic acid fortification. We also sought to address a number of other potential clinical, demographic, and socioeconomic confounders through extended use of electronic medical records and block-level American Community Survey data, but we saw no substantial differences among exposure groups. Although, on the population level, the rollout of folic acid fortification rapidly doubled blood folate levels without changing folic acid supplement (vitamin) use,2,33 the lack of individual-level data on maternal folate intake represents an inevitable limitation of this experiment’s design. However, exposure groups were comparable on block-level vitamin spending and consumption.
Although exposure groups in the MGH cohort were generally well matched, we cannot rule out the potential role of magnetic field strength differences within that cohort. Field strength effects would not be expected to bias results consistently because previous work indicates that the associations of field strength with cortical thickness measurements vary in direction and magnitude across the cortex27; recognizing this heterogeneity, field strength was entered as a covariate in the surface-wide analysis. Furthermore, within regions demonstrating between-group differences, these differences remained significant in sensitivity analyses that controlled for scanner field strength and manufacturer. Perhaps more important, that similar exposure-related associations with cortical thinning were observed in the PNC cohort, which used a single 3-T magnet, suggests that scanner differences did not substantially influence the MGH cohort results. More broadly, similar findings across 2 cohorts that differed substantially in terms of population sampling strategy (retrospective and clinical vs prospective and community-based), imaging and clinical assessment (medical record–based vs standardized), and geographic location—but that were comparable in terms of exposure, sex, age, race/ethnicity, and urbanicity—suggest reliability of the findings.
The present findings also raise questions about the long-lasting effects of variation in the fetal methylome because folate supplies 1-carbon moieties that regulate gene expression. One possibility suggested by the present results is that programming of cortical maturation in youths is sensitive to fetal folate levels, potentially via epigenetic modification of genes that regulate cortical development,34 repair of de novo mutations,35 or mitigation of toxic exposures.36,37,38,39 Although, to our knowledge, these results are the first to link prenatal exposure to folic acid fortification to changes in subsequent cortical development, the specific mechanisms underlying these effects have yet to be elucidated.
Conclusions
In replicated cohorts, fetal exposure to population-wide folic acid fortification was associated with subsequent alterations in cortical development among school-aged youths. In turn, these cortical changes were associated with reduced risk of psychosis. Our findings suggest that protective effects of prenatal folic acid exposure may extend beyond prevention of neural tube defects and span neurodevelopment during childhood and adolescence.
References
- 1.US Food and Drug Administration Food standards: amendment of standards of identity for enriched grain products to require addition of folic acid. Fed Regist 1996;61:8781-8797. https://www.federalregister.gov/documents/1996/03/05/96-5014/food-standards-amendment-of-standards-of-identity-for-enriched-grain-products-to-require-addition-of. Published March 5, 1996. Accessed November 21, 2016.
- 2.Pfeiffer CM, Hughes JP, Lacher DA, et al. . Estimation of trends in serum and RBC folate in the US population from pre- to postfortification using assay-adjusted data from the NHANES 1988-2010. J Nutr. 2012;142(5):886-893. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Czeizel AE. Folic acid in the prevention of neural tube defects. J Pediatr Gastroenterol Nutr. 1995;20(1):4-16. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Canfield MA, Collins JS, Botto LD, et al. ; National Birth Defects Prevention Network . Changes in the birth prevalence of selected birth defects after grain fortification with folic acid in the United States: findings from a multi-state population-based study. Birth Defects Res A Clin Mol Teratol. 2005;73(10):679-689. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Susser E, St Clair D. Prenatal famine and adult mental illness: interpreting concordant and discordant results from the Dutch and Chinese Famines. Soc Sci Med. 2013;97:325-330. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Surén P, Roth C, Bresnahan M, et al. . Association between maternal use of folic acid supplements and risk of autism spectrum disorders in children. JAMA. 2013;309(6):570-577. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Roth C, Magnus P, Schjølberg S, et al. . Folic acid supplements in pregnancy and severe language delay in children. JAMA. 2011;306(14):1566-1573. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Schmidt RJ, Tancredi DJ, Ozonoff S, et al. . Maternal periconceptional folic acid intake and risk of autism spectrum disorders and developmental delay in the CHARGE (CHildhood Autism Risks from Genetics and Environment) case-control study. Am J Clin Nutr. 2012;96(1):80-89. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Levine SZ, Kodesh A, Viktorin A, et al. . Association of maternal use of folic acid and multivitamin supplements in the periods before and during pregnancy with the risk of autism spectrum disorder in offspring. JAMA Psychiatry. 2018;75(2):176-184. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Virk J, Liew Z, Olsen J, Nohr EA, Catov JM, Ritz B. Preconceptional and prenatal supplementary folic acid and multivitamin intake and autism spectrum disorders. Autism. 2016;20(6):710-718. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Ducharme S, Albaugh MD, Nguyen TV, et al. ; Brain Development Cooperative Group . Trajectories of cortical thickness maturation in normal brain development—the importance of quality control procedures. Neuroimage. 2016;125:267-279. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Sowell ER, Peterson BS, Thompson PM, Welcome SE, Henkenius AL, Toga AW. Mapping cortical change across the human life span. Nat Neurosci. 2003;6(3):309-315. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Sowell ER, Thompson PM, Leonard CM, Welcome SE, Kan E, Toga AW. Longitudinal mapping of cortical thickness and brain growth in normal children. J Neurosci. 2004;24(38):8223-8231. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Yakovlev P, Lecours A. The myelinogenetic cycles of regional maturation of the brain In: Minokowski A, ed. Regional Development of the Brain Early in Life. Oxford, England: Blackwell Scientific Publications; 1967:3-70. [Google Scholar]
- 15.Shaw P, Greenstein D, Lerch J, et al. . Intellectual ability and cortical development in children and adolescents. Nature. 2006;440(7084):676-679. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Thompson PM, Vidal C, Giedd JN, et al. . Mapping adolescent brain change reveals dynamic wave of accelerated gray matter loss in very early-onset schizophrenia. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2001;98(20):11650-11655. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Mattai AA, Weisinger B, Greenstein D, et al. . Normalization of cortical gray matter deficits in nonpsychotic siblings of patients with childhood-onset schizophrenia. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2011;50(7):697-704. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Zielinski BA, Prigge MB, Nielsen JA, et al. . Longitudinal changes in cortical thickness in autism and typical development. Brain. 2014;137(pt 6):1799-1812. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Murphy SN, Herrick C, Wang Y, et al. . High throughput tools to access images from clinical archives for research. J Digit Imaging. 2015;28(2):194-204. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Jacques PF, Selhub J, Bostom AG, Wilson PW, Rosenberg IH. The effect of folic acid fortification on plasma folate and total homocysteine concentrations. N Engl J Med. 1999;340(19):1449-1454. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.US Census Bureau. Decennial census of population and housing. https://www.census.gov/programs-surveys/decennial-census/data/datasets.2010.html. Accessed May 8, 2018.
- 22.Satterthwaite TD, Elliott MA, Ruparel K, et al. . Neuroimaging of the Philadelphia Neurodevelopmental Cohort. Neuroimage. 2014;86:544-553. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Calkins ME, Moore TM, Merikangas KR, et al. . The psychosis spectrum in a young US community sample: findings from the Philadelphia Neurodevelopmental Cohort. World Psychiatry. 2014;13(3):296-305. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Satterthwaite TD, Wolf DH, Calkins ME, et al. . Structural brain abnormalities in youth with psychosis spectrum symptoms. JAMA Psychiatry. 2016;73(5):515-524. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Evans AC; Brain Development Cooperative Group . The NIH MRI study of normal brain development. Neuroimage. 2006;30(1):184-202. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Wong IC, Murray ML, Camilleri-Novak D, Stephens P. Increased prescribing trends of paediatric psychotropic medications. Arch Dis Child. 2004;89(12):1131-1132. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Govindarajan KA, Freeman L, Cai C, Rahbar MH, Narayana PA. Effect of intrinsic and extrinsic factors on global and regional cortical thickness. PLoS One. 2014;9(5):e96429. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Han X, Jovicich J, Salat D, et al. . Reliability of MRI-derived measurements of human cerebral cortical thickness: the effects of field strength, scanner upgrade and manufacturer. Neuroimage. 2006;32(1):180-194. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Gur RC, Calkins ME, Satterthwaite TD, et al. . Neurocognitive growth charting in psychosis spectrum youths. JAMA Psychiatry. 2014;71(4):366-374. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Narr KL, Bilder RM, Toga AW, et al. . Mapping cortical thickness and gray matter concentration in first episode schizophrenia. Cereb Cortex. 2005;15(6):708-719. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Roza SJ, van Batenburg-Eddes T, Steegers EA, et al. . Maternal folic acid supplement use in early pregnancy and child behavioural problems: the Generation R Study. Br J Nutr. 2010;103(3):445-452. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Ars CL, Nijs IM, Marroun HE, et al. . Prenatal folate, homocysteine and vitamin B12 levels and child brain volumes, cognitive development and psychological functioning: the Generation R Study [published online January 22, 2016]. Br J Nutr. doi: 10.1017/S0007114515002081 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Centers for Disease Control and Prevention (CDC) Knowledge and use of folic acid by women of childbearing age—United States, 1995 and 1998. MMWR Morb Mortal Wkly Rep. 1999;48(16):325-327. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Kirkbride JB, Susser E, Kundakovic M, Kresovich JK, Davey Smith G, Relton CL. Prenatal nutrition, epigenetics and schizophrenia risk: can we test causal effects? Epigenomics. 2012;4(3):303-315. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.McClellan JM, Susser E, King MC. Maternal famine, de novo mutations, and schizophrenia. JAMA. 2006;296(5):582-584. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Dolinoy DC, Huang D, Jirtle RL. Maternal nutrient supplementation counteracts bisphenol A-induced hypomethylation in early development. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2007;104(32):13056-13061. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Brown AS, Bottiglieri T, Schaefer CA, et al. . Elevated prenatal homocysteine levels as a risk factor for schizophrenia. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2007;64(1):31-39. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Schmidt RJ, Kogan V, Shelton JF, et al. . Combined prenatal pesticide exposure and folic acid intake in relation to autism spectrum disorder. Environ Health Perspect. 2017;125(9):097007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Bjørk M, Riedel B, Spigset O, et al. . Association of folic acid supplementation during pregnancy with the risk of autistic traits in children exposed to antiepileptic drugs in utero. JAMA Neurol. 2018;75(2):160-168. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.