Abstract
Learning and memory formation involve long-term potentiation (LTP) of synaptic strength. A fundamental feature of LTP induction in the brain is the need for coincident pre- and postsynaptic activity. This restricts LTP expression to activated synapses only (homosynaptic LTP) and leads to its input specificity. In the spinal cord, we discovered a fundamentally different form of LTP that is induced by glial cell activation and mediated by diffusible, extracellular messengers, including d-serine and tumor necrosis factor (TNF), and that travel long distances via the cerebrospinal fluid, thereby affecting susceptible synapses at remote sites. The properties of this gliogenic LTP resolve unexplained findings of memory traces in nociceptive pathways and may underlie forms of widespread pain hypersensitivity.
Activity-dependent, homosynaptic long-term potentiation (LTP) (1) at synapses in nociceptive pathways contributes to pain amplification (hyperalgesia) at the site of an injury or inflammation (2–5). Homosynaptic LTP can, however, not account for pain amplification at areas surrounding (secondary hyperalgesia) or remote from (widespread hyperalgesia) an injury. It also fails to explain hyperalgesia that is induced independently of neuronal activity in primary afferents—e.g., by the application of or the withdrawal from opioids (opioid-induced hyperalgesia) (6). Glial cells are believed to contribute to these forms of hyperalgesia and to LTP in nociceptive pathways (7–10). Induction of homosynaptic LTP can be accompanied by LTP in adjacent, inactive synapses converging onto the same neuron, especially early in development. The respective molecular signals for this heterosynaptic form of LTP are thought to be confined within the cytoplasm of the activated neuron, spreading tens of micrometers only (11). We have now tested the hypothesis that, in contrast to current beliefs, activation of glial cells is causative for the induction of LTP at spinal C-fiber synapses and that this gliogenic LTP constitutes a common denominator of homo- and heterosynaptic LTP in the spinal cord.
Our previous study revealed that selective activation of spinal microglia by fractalkine induces transient facilitation, but no LTP, at C-fiber synapses (12). Here, we recorded monosynaptic C-fiber–evoked excitatory postsynaptic currents (EPSCs) from lamina I neurons in rat lumbar spinal cord slices. To investigate whether selective activation of spinal astrocytes is sufficient for the induction of synaptic plasticity in the absence of any other conditioning stimulus, we used ultraviolet (UV)–flash photolysis of caged inositol 1,4,5-trisphosphate (IP3) in astrocytic networks (fig. S1 and movie S1). This induced a robust long-term depression at C-fiber synapses (gliogenic LTD; to 69 ± 9%, n = 7, P < 0.001) (fig. S1C) but no LTP. UV flashes were without any effect on synaptic strength when applied in the absence of caged IP3 (fig. S1D) or in the presence of the glial cell toxin fluoroacetate (fig. S1E). To coactivate microglia and astrocytes, we next applied the purinergic P2X7 receptor (P2X7R) agonist benzoyl-benzoyl adenosine triphosphate (BzATP). This never affected holding currents or membrane potentials in any of the spinal neurons tested (fig. S2), supporting the observation that, in the spinal dorsal horn, and unlike other P2X receptors (13), P2X7Rs are expressed exclusively on glial cells (14–18). ATP is finally hydrolyzed to adenosine. We therefore applied the adenosine 1 receptor antagonist DPCPX to block adenosine-mediated presynaptic inhibition (fig. S3). Combined activation of microglia and astrocytes by BzATP induced LTP in 13 out of 22 C-fiber inputs (to 156 ± 13%, P < 0.001) (Fig. 1A). BzATP-induced LTP was abolished by the selective P2X7R antagonist A-438079 (Fig. 1B) and by fluoroacetate (Fig. 1C). This demonstrates that selective activation of P2X7R on spinal glial cells caused gliogenic LTP at synapses between C fibers and lamina I neurons.
High-frequency stimulation (HFS) of primary afferent C fibers triggers the release of ATP from primary afferent neurons (19, 20), activates glial cells (21, 22), and induces LTP (2, 3), leading to the intriguing hypothesis that HFS-induced LTP at spinal C-fiber synapses might be a variety of gliogenic LTP. If true, one would predict that HFS induces LTP not only at conditioned but also at unconditioned C-fiber synapses and that, in striking contrast to current beliefs, homo- and heterosynaptic LTP could be expressed independently of each other. To directly test these predictions, we used transverse lumbar spinal cord slices with long dorsal roots attached that were separated into halves. We recorded from 22 dorsal horn lamina I neurons that received independent monosynaptic C-fiber input from each dorsal root half. HFS applied to one dorsal root half induced LTP in the conditioned pathway in 12 of these neurons (“homosynaptic LTP”; to 134 ± 9%, P < 0.001) (Fig. 2Aa, red filled circles). Out of these 12 neurons, where homosynaptic LTP was induced, 6 also showed LTP at the unconditioned pathway (“heterosynaptic LTP”). In total, heterosynaptic LTP was induced in 11 out of 22 neurons (to 174 ± 19%, P < 0.001) (Fig. 2Ba, blue filled circles) because, importantly, in 5 of these neurons, heterosynaptic LTP was induced in the absence of homosynaptic LTP (to 161 ± 9%, P < 0.005) (Fig. 2C), a finding that cannot be explained by current models of synaptic plasticity.
We investigated whether HFS-induced homo- and heterosynaptic LTP require activation of glial cells via P2X7R. Blockade of glial P2X7R by A-438079 fully blocked LTP induction at the conditioned and at the unconditioned sites (Fig. 2, Ab and Bb). This was also achieved by blocking glial cell metabolism with fluoroacetate [Fig. 2, Ac and Bc and (21)]. Both homo- and heterosynaptic LTP were abolished by blocking postsynaptic N-methyl-d-aspartate receptors (NMDARs) (Fig. 2, Ad and Bd). d-serine is a coagonist at NMDARs that is released from astrocytes (23). Here, preincubation of slices with the d-serine–degrading enzyme d-amino acid oxidase (DAAO) abolished both homo- and heterosynaptic LTP (Fig. 2, Ae and Be). We then investigated whether d-serine alone is sufficient to enhance synaptic strength at C-fiber synapses. Bath application of d-serine facilitated synaptic strength at C-fiber synapses (to 120 ± 2% in 13 out of 32 cells; P < 0.001) (fig. S4A). This amplification was abolished by blockade of NMDARs (in 12 out of 13 cells; P = 0.094) (fig. S4B). Taken together, our data demonstrate that the combined activation of microglia and astrocytes, either via P2X7R or by HFS, was sufficient to induce gliogenic LTP. When gliogenic LTP is induced by conditioning HFS, it may appear as homo- and/or heterosynaptic LTP that can be elicited independently of each other.
We next asked whether gliogenic LTP also exists in vivo. HFS applied to the sciatic nerve induced LTP of spinal C-fiber–evoked field potentials in deeply anesthetized rats (to 211 ± 16% at 220 to 240 min; n = 49, P < 0.001) (Fig. 3A). HFS-induced LTP was blocked by spinal application of either fluoroacetate (Fig. 3B) or DAAO (Fig. 3C), indicating that it required the activation of spinal glial cells and d-serine signaling. Application of fluoroacetate or DAAO after the induction of LTP had no effects on LTP maintenance (to 192 ± 23% and to 181 ± 30%, respectively, at 220 to 240 min; n = 6; P = 0.433 and 0.546, respectively) (fig. S5), indicating that once LTP was induced, glial cells were no longer required. Thus, the gliogenic nature refers to the induction but not to the maintenance phase of LTP.
We then investigated whether HFS leads to the release of diffusible mediators that spread over long distances to trigger LTP. We induced LTP by HFS, collected the spinal superfusate from the respective lumbar segments, and transferred it to the spinal cord dorsum of naïve animals. The maintenance of LTP in the donor animals was not affected by exchanging the superfusate (Fig. 3A). The superfusate induced, however, a robust LTP in the recipient animals (to 173 ± 32% of control at 160 to 180 min; n = 10, P = 0.009) (Fig. 4A), demonstrating that LTP could be transferred between individuals. The superfusate collected from naïve donor animals had, in contrast, no effect on synaptic transmission in any of the recipient animals (Fig. 4B). When glial cells were blocked in the recipient animals, “transferable LTP” was still induced (to 160 ± 20%; n = 9, P < 0.001) (Fig. 4C). Blockade of interleukin-1β (IL-1β) signaling in the recipient animals also had no effect on the induction of transferable LTP (to 133 ± 12% at 180 to 240 min; n = 10, P = 0.001) (Fig. 4D). However, LTP induction was prevented by blocking TNF (Fig. 4E), d-serine signaling (Fig. 4F), or spinal NMDARs (Fig. 4G) in the recipient animals. Application of d-serine to the spinal cord dose-dependently induced a reversible synaptic facilitation (to 152 ± 9% at 220 to 240 min; n = 10, P < 0.001) (fig. S6), whereas TNF application triggers robust LTP at C-fiber synapses (21). These data indicate that transferable LTP required activation of glial cells in the donor but not in the recipient animals and that the combined actions of the gliotransmitters d-serine and TNF were required for its induction.
Collectively, our data indicate that the combined activation of microglia and astrocytes, either by P2X7R agonists or by HFS of primary afferents, triggered gliogenic LTP at C-fiber synapses with spinal lamina I neurons through the release of d-serine and cytokines such as TNF. Crucially, glial cell–derived signaling molecules accumulated in the extracellular space, including the cerebrospinal fluid, at biologically active but presently unknown concentrations, and induced LTP at C-fiber synapses, constituting the phenomenon of gliogenic LTP.
Gliogenic LTP is a new form of paracrine synaptic plasticity in the central nervous system and may lead to pain amplification close to and remote from an injury or an inflammation. This is in line with the concept of chronic pain as a gliopathy involving neurogenic neuroinflammation (7, 24). These new insights may pave the way for novel pain therapies (25, 26). P2X7Rs play a key role in chronic inflammatory and neuropathic pain (27) and in other neurodegenerative and neuropsychiatric disorders (28). Glial cells display considerable diversity between and within distinct regions of the central nervous system (29). If the presently identified gliogenic LTP also existed at some brain areas, it could be of relevance not only for pain but also for other disorders, such as cognitive deficits, fear and stress disorders, and chronic immune-mediated diseases (24, 29, 30).
Supplementary Material
Acknowledgments
This work was supported by grants P 29206-B27 and W1205 from the Austrian Science Fund (FWF) to J.S. We thank L. Czarnecki for laboratory support and B. Heinke and G. Janeselli for technical support. All of the data are archived on servers of the Center for Brain Research, Medical University of Vienna.
Footnotes
The authors declare no conflicts of interest. M.T.K., R.D.-S., and J.S. designed the research. M.T.K., R.D.-S., M.G., S.D.H., and H.L.T. generated and analyzed the data. M.T.K., R.D.-S., and J.S. wrote the paper, with input from the other authors.
References and notes
- 1.Bliss TVP, Collingridge GL. Nature. 1993;361:31–39. doi: 10.1038/361031a0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Ikeda H, Heinke B, Ruscheweyh R, Sandkühler J. Science. 2003;299:1237–1240. doi: 10.1126/science.1080659. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Ikeda H, et al. Science. 2006;312:1659–1662. doi: 10.1126/science.1127233. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Kuner R. Nat Med. 2010;16:1258–1266. doi: 10.1038/nm.2231. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Li X-Y, et al. Science. 2010;330:1400–1404. doi: 10.1126/science.1191792. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Drdla R, Gassner M, Gingl E, Sandkühler J. Science. 2009;325:207–210. doi: 10.1126/science.1171759. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Ji R-R, Berta T, Nedergaard M. Pain. 2013;154(Suppl 1):S10–S28. doi: 10.1016/j.pain.2013.06.022. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.McMahon SB, Malcangio M. Neuron. 2009;64:46–54. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2009.09.033. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Grace PM, Hutchinson MR, Maier SF, Watkins LR. Nat Rev Immunol. 2014;14:217–231. doi: 10.1038/nri3621. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Gong Q-J, et al. Glia. 2009;57:583–591. doi: 10.1002/glia.20786. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Tao HW, Zhang LI, Engert F, Poo M. Neuron. 2001;31:569–580. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(01)00393-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Clark AK, et al. J Neurosci. 2015;35:4552–4570. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2061-14.2015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Gu JG, MacDermott AB. Nature. 1997;389:749–753. doi: 10.1038/39639. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Chu Y-X, Zhang Y, Zhang Y-Q, Zhao Z-Q. Brain Behav Immun. 2010;24:1176–1189. doi: 10.1016/j.bbi.2010.06.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Kobayashi K, Takahashi E, Miyagawa Y, Yamanaka H, Noguchi K. Neurosci Lett. 2011;504:57–61. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2011.08.058. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Aoyama R, et al. Pain. 2011;152:2085–2097. doi: 10.1016/j.pain.2011.05.014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.He W-J, et al. Behav Brain Res. 2012;226:163–170. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2011.09.015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Ficker C, et al. Glia. 2014;62:1671–1686. doi: 10.1002/glia.22707. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Jung J, Shin YH, Konishi H, Lee SJ, Kiyama H. Biochem Biophys Res Commun. 2013;430:488–493. doi: 10.1016/j.bbrc.2012.12.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Fields RD, Ni Y. Sci Signal. 2010;3:ra73. doi: 10.1126/scisignal.2001128. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Gruber-Schoffnegger D, et al. J Neurosci. 2013;33:6540–6551. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5087-12.2013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Sekiguchi KJ, et al. Nat Commun. 2016;7 doi: 10.1038/ncomms11450. 11450. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Martineau M, et al. J Neurosci. 2013;33:3413–3423. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3497-12.2013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Xanthos DN, Sandkühler J. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2014;15:43–53. doi: 10.1038/nrn3617. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Ji R-R, Xu ZZ, Strichartz G, Serhan CN. Trends Neurosci. 2011;34:599–609. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2011.08.005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Wei X-H, et al. J Neurosci. 2013;33:1540–1551. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0324-12.2013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Chessell IP, et al. Pain. 2005;114:386–396. doi: 10.1016/j.pain.2005.01.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Basso AM, et al. Behav Brain Res. 2009;198:83–90. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2008.10.018. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Khakh BS, Sofroniew MV. Nat Neurosci. 2015;18:942–952. doi: 10.1038/nn.4043. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Aguzzi A, Barres BA, Bennett ML. Science. 2013;339:156–161. doi: 10.1126/science.1227901. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.