Abstract
The number of lesbian, gay, and bisexual (LGB) adults aged 50 and older is projected to reach 5 million in the USA by 2030 (Fredriksen-Goldsen, Kim, Shiu, Goldsen, & Emlet, 2015). Older bisexuals experience more negative mental and physical health outcomes when compared to both heterosexuals and other sexual minorities (Fredriksen-Goldsen, Shiu, Bryan, Goldsen, & Kim, 2017). As bisexuals are the numeric majority of sexual minorities in the USA (Herbenick et al., 2010), bisexual aging processes are critical to understand whether researchers wish to reduce sexual minority health disparities and promote healthy aging. In the current study, we use a national probability sample of adults from the Midlife in the United States (MIDUS) study to assess life satisfaction across an 18-year period. We aimed to identify whether life satisfaction—an indicator of psychological health and well-being—is similar for same-age bisexual, lesbian and gay, and heterosexual midlife individuals, and whether sexual orientation predicts change in life satisfaction across adulthood. Further, we tested whether life satisfaction among bisexuals changes at the same rate and in the same pattern as for lesbian, gay, and heterosexual individuals. Overall, we found a linear pattern of increase in life satisfaction across adulthood. However, when we accounted for sexual orientation, a different pattern emerged for bisexuals. Whereas heterosexuals and lesbian and gay individuals experienced increases in life satisfaction across adulthood, bisexuals’ life satisfaction did not increase over this period. Implications for bisexual health and well-being are discussed.
Keywords: Bisexual, Life satisfaction, Older adults, Midlife, LGB aging
Introduction
Research on sexual minority health during middle and later adulthood is scant, yet the number of lesbian, gay, and bisexual (LGB) adults aged 50 and older is projected to reach 5 million in the USA by 2030 (Fredriksen-Goldsen et al., 2015). LGB adults share similar aging concerns as their heterosexual counterparts and also experience unique challenges related to stigma and discrimination that may contribute to the aging process (Hatzenbuehler, 2009; Kimmel, 2014; Meyer, 2003; Orel, 2014). Compared to heterosexual peers, LGB older adults report higher rates of loneliness and depression, and they also evidence greater rates of cardiovascular disease and weakened immune systems, among other health disparities (for a review, see Choi & Meyer, 2016; Emlet, 2016). Further, there is increasing evidence that older bisexuals experience significantly poorer health when compared not only to heterosexuals, but also to other sexual minorities (Fredriksen-Goldsen et al., 2017). Many of the health disparities experienced by LGB individuals are preventable or modifiable. If overlooked, however, they will contribute to a substantial rise in healthcare costs as LGB individuals age (Institute of Medicine, 2011; U.S. Department of Health and Human Services, 2012).
In this paper, we investigate patterns of change across 18 years in self-reported life satisfaction by sexual orientation. Specifically, we examine how bisexual individuals compare to their lesbian, gay, and heterosexual counterparts in terms of life satisfaction across the adult life span. Life satisfaction is a key indicator of an individual’s overall mental and physical health and is typically defined as a global evaluation of one’s past, present, or future well-being (Diener, 1984; Diener et al., 2017; Shin & Johnson, 1978). People’s assessments of life satisfaction draw on, for example, past experiences, personal values, and comparisons with others (Diener, Suh, Lucas, & Smith, 1999). Recent studies have linked life satisfaction to several specific measures of health; for example, cross-sectional studies have found that greater life satisfaction is associated with better health behaviors (e.g., more physical exercise; Grant, Wardle, & Steptoe, 2009) and better physical health (Pressman & Cohen, 2005; Salovey, Rothman, Detweiler, & Steward, 2000). Longitudinal studies similarly demonstrate that greater life satisfaction predicts better long-term physical health (Rauma et al., 2014; Siahpush, Spittal, & Singh, 2008), fewer disabilities (Collins, Goldman, & Rodriguez, 2008; Koivumaa-Honkanen et al., 2004), and reduced risk of mortality (Chida & Steptoe, 2008; Xu & Roberts, 2010). Particularly relevant to examining health and well-being across the life span, there is evidence to suggest that individual’s perceived life satisfaction may be tied to their age.
In general, older adults in the USA have greater life satisfaction than younger adults, and some research shows that life satisfaction remains stable or unchanged throughout middle adulthood (from 30’s through 40’s) and then increases thereafter (from 40’s through 60’s; Lachman, Teshale, & Agrigoroaei, 2015). It is unclear, however, how life satisfaction unfolds with age among sexual minorities—a group of individuals who experience high levels of stress and maltreatment throughout their life span. The primary goals of the current research were to identify whether life satisfaction in adulthood is similar for same-age bisexual, lesbian and gay, and heterosexual individuals, and whether sexual orientation predicts change in life satisfaction across adulthood. Further, we tested whether life satisfaction among bisexuals changes at the same rate and in the same pattern as it does for lesbian, gay, and heterosexual individuals across adulthood.
Life Satisfaction and Subjective-Well-Being Across Adulthood
Life satisfaction is a central component of subjective well-being and an indicator of people’s overall evaluation of the conditions of their lives (Diener et al., 2017). When asked to evaluate their satisfaction with life, people tend to draw on the quality of immediate factors in their lives such as their health, financial security, close relationships, and work conditions (DeNeve, Diener, Tay, & Xuereb, 2013; Kahneman & Deaton, 2010; Schimmack & Oishi, 2005). Unsurprisingly, there are many benefits associated with greater life satisfaction and subjective well-being (for reviews, see DeNeve et al., 2013; Diener, Kanazawa, Suh, & Oishi, 2015). People with greater subjective well-being compared to those with lower subjective well-being tend to have more supportive relationships, to live longer, and to have evidence of a stronger immune system and cardiovascular function (Diener & Seligman, 2002; Lyubomirsky, King, & Diener, 2005; Pressman & Cohen, 2005). In fact, life satisfaction may even relate to health at the day-to-day level. Smyth, Zawadzki, Juth, and Sciamanna (2017) found that individuals with higher (vs lower) levels of life satisfaction experienced better daily indicators of well-being, such as less stress and better momentary affect; this same study controlled for physical limitations, suggesting that life satisfaction has important implications for well-being in everyday life above and beyond physical problems. Put simply, life satisfaction and well-being are tightly intertwined.
Recent research suggests that life satisfaction changes in significant ways across the adult life span. Cross-sectional studies have found mean differences in subjective well-being between age-groups. Compared to younger adults, older adults in the USA report significantly greater life satisfaction (Hamarat et al., 2001; Prenda & Lachman, 2001), happiness (Thomas et al., 2016), meaning in life (Steger, Oishi, & Kashdan, 2009), and lower negative affect (Carstensen, Pasupathi, Mayr, & Nesselroade, 2000; Mroczek & Almeida, 2004). Despite declines in physical health, the idea that we become happier as we age has been dubbed the “paradox of aging” (Thomas et al., 2016). A number of explanations have been given for this observation. For example, older adults are more aware of their mortality; thus, they may invest more time in meaningful goals and activities (Charles & Carstensen, 2010). Older adults are also often better equipped to regulate emotional material after a lifetime of learning effective strategies. Finally, it is possible that those who live longer may tend to be healthier and therefore happier (see Charles & Carstensen, 2009, 2010). Each of these reasons could play a role in eventual increases in psychological well-being as people age.
There is also growing evidence that subjective well-being follows a curvilinear shape across the adult life span. Specifically, both women and men in the USA report somewhat stable levels of life satisfaction and happiness throughout middle adulthood, with increases in life satisfaction from middle to older adulthood (Lachman et al., 2015). The stability of life satisfaction in midlife has been attributed to midlife being a time replete with positive and negative experiences. Midlife is filled with the demands of balancing work, family, and children; but also, it is a time of peak-earning potential, self-confidence, and community involvement (Aldwin & Levenson, 2001; Finke, Huston, & Sharpe, 2006; Mroczek & Almeida, 2004). Despite some inconsistences, research overall suggests that life gets better emotionally with time. However, less is known about these psychological processes of aging among subpopulations and minority groups who may have less fortunate and privileged life circumstances. We were particularly interested in whether sexual minorities’ life satisfaction increased with age as has been documented in previous aging literature (e.g., Lachman et al., 2015).
Bisexual Older Adults’ Health and Well-Being
The minority stress model theorizes that adverse health outcomes in sexual minorities are predicted by increased exposure to environmental and lifetime stressors, discrimination, and victimization (Meyer, 2003). These experiences accrue over time and have been tied to negative health outcomes, especially among older LGB adults (Fredriksen-Goldsen et al., 2015; Grossman, D’Augelli, & O’Connell, 2001). Stressors include both major and minor events, such as being denied a job or promotion, managing disclosure of a stigmatized identity, and expectations of social rejection. Disparities in health between older sexual minorities and heterosexual peers are considerable: Sexual minorities report higher rates of physical limitations, immune dysfunction, and mental distress compared to their heterosexual counterparts (Cochran, Sullivan, & Mays, 2003; Fredriksen-Goldsen, Kim, Barkan, Muraco, & Hoy-Ellis, 2013; Mays & Cochran, 2001). Reporting an LGB identity has also been associated with lower eudaimonic well-being scores, including purpose in life, self-acceptance, and environmental mastery (Riggle, Rostosky, & Danner, 2009). Moreover, a higher proportion of older LGB adults tend to be single, to live alone, and to be childless compared to older heterosexuals (de Vries, 2009; Wallace, Cochran, Durazo, & Ford, 2011). In one study, more than half of the LGB elders reported feelings of loneliness and isolation from others (Fredriksen-Goldsen et al., 2011). In this same study, bisexual women and men reported more loneliness on average than lesbian and gay older adults.
Along these lines, bisexuals appear to face the greatest mental and physical health disparities within the LGB community as a whole, and this pattern replicates in older bisexual adults (Bostwick, Hughes, & Everett, 2015; Fredriksen-Goldsen, Kim, Barkan, Balsam, & Mincer, 2010; Fredriksen-Goldsen et al., 2017; Gonzales, Przedworski, & Henning-Smith, 2016). For instance, in one study, bisexual adults reported a higher propensity for smoking and alcohol use, compared to straight and gay counterparts (Gorman, Denney, Dowdy, & Medeiros, 2015). Using data from a national population-based study in the USA, Bostwick et al. (2010) found that men and women who identified as bisexual or engaged in bisexual behavior were at heightened risk compared with gay and straight peers for mood and anxiety disorders both over the past year and over the course of a lifetime. By midlife, bisexual adults also report a decrease in contact with the bisexual community and subculture (Weinberg, Williams, & Pryor, 2001). Thus, bisexuals may find themselves in a particularly vulnerable position for health as they not only are faced with the psychological stress of being a sexual minority, but also likely encounter greater health issues.
The Present Study
The findings reviewed above highlight the need for researchers to identify bisexuals as a distinct group with their own health issues and concerns within sexual minority samples rather than collapsing all sexual minority individuals into a single and homogenous entity. Despite cross-sectional designs that have provided valuable information on the lived experiences and challenges of bisexuals, it remains unclear how life satisfaction changes in the same individuals across time and by sexual orientation. Age-related changes can be described as changes that happen to everyone as they age (Baltes, Reese, & Nesselroade, 1977; Forbes, Eaton, & Krueger, 2017). In the current study, we hypothesize age-related changes in life satisfaction over time but also that bisexuals will experience lower life satisfaction on average—and less rapid inclines in life satisfaction across adulthood—than their lesbian, gay, and heterosexual peers. We examined these questions in the Midlife in the United States (MIDUS) study. MIDUS is a national probability sample and includes three waves of longitudinal data collected over a span of 18 years (1995–2014), which afforded us with the ability to examine bisexuals’ change in life satisfaction over time and in comparison with heterosexuals and other sexual minorities.
Using MIDUS data, we tested linear and curvilinear trends in life satisfaction that have been documented in other studies of adult samples in the USA (e.g., Lachman et al., 2015). We examined whether sexual orientation predicted variability in age-related change in life satisfaction when accounting for covariates (e.g., gender, education). To test whether bisexual individuals experienced similar trends in life satisfaction as their heterosexual, lesbian, and gay counterparts in adulthood, we performed four steps of analyses. In steps 1 and 2, we examined age-related changes in life satisfaction over time and we accounted for sociodemographic characteristics (i.e., gender, race, education, and marital status) that have been shown to influence age-satisfaction trajectories. Next, we tested whether sexual orientation predicted reports of average life satisfaction and, finally, we investigated whether changes in life satisfaction over 18 years varied by sexual orientation.
Method
Sample and Procedures
Data are from the Midlife in the United States (MIDUS) study (Brim, Ryff, & Kessler, 2004). MIDUS was first sponsored by the MacArthur Midlife Research Network and later funded by the National Institute on Aging. Details important to the interpretation of the present study are below, and additional information about MIDUS is provided in other documents (see Brim et al., 2007; Radler, 2014; Ryff et al., 2012, 2017). MIDUS is a national probability sample of noninstitutionalized, English-speaking adults recruited through nationwide random digit dialing. The MIDUS project was designed to investigate factors associated with age-related differences and changes in health and well-being across the adult life span. There are three waves of data in MIDUS (MIDUS-I, MIDUS-II, and MIDUS-III), and each wave is separated by approximately 9 years.
A total of 7108 participants were interviewed first in 1995–1996 (MIDUS-I: age range 20–75 years, M = 46.40, SD = 13.00; 51.1% female). Of the initial participants, 4963 were followed up a second time in 2004–2006 (MIDUS-II: retention rate=69.8%; age range 28–84 years, M = 55.40, SD = 12.45; 53.3% female), and 3294 for a third time in 2013–2014 (MIDUS-III: retention rate = 46.3%; age range 39–93 years, M = 63.60, SD = 11.35; 54.9%female). Data were collected at each wave via a computer- assisted personal interview protocol and subsequent self-administered questionnaires sent through the mail. These self-administered, mail-back questionnaires were the focus of the present study. Data collection for the MIDUS studies was approved by Institutional Review Boards at each participating site, and all participants provided informed consent. Monetary incentives were used to maximize participation in all three phases of data collection.
Analytic Sample
Participants were part of the present study if they reported both their sexual orientation and life satisfaction at least once across the three waves of data. These criteria were met by a final sample of 6304 participants (88.7% of the full MIDUS sample). People who did not provide data on their sexual orientation and/or life satisfaction (n=804) tended to be slightly younger at the start of the study (MMIDUS-I[SD]=43.75[13.96]), t(7047)=−5.85, p <.001, and included a larger percentage of men, χ2(1, N=7058)=8.96, p=.003, compared to those who reported on these primary variables of interest. All 6304 participants at MIDUS-I reported their age and gender, and 96.6% had no missing data that was relevant to the primary analyses. Of these 6304 participants, 4742 (75.2%) were recontacted and subsequently took part in the second wave of MIDUS (MIDUS-II), with 84.6% reporting all primary variables of interest. Of these 4742 participants, 3230 (68.1%) people completed the third wave (MIDUS-III) and 84.3% had no missing data. Rates of attrition and missing data in the present sample are comparable to (and even lower than) the full MIDUS sample in which 7108, 4963 (69.8%), and 3294 (66.4%) people completed MIDUS- I, II, and III, respectively (see Radler & Ryff, 2010 for information on MIDUS retention). Participants’ sexual orientation was identified and coded as heterosexual (n=6108), lesbian or gay (n= 112), and bisexual (n=84), and ages ranged from 20 to 75 (MIDUS- I), 30 to 84 (MIDUS-II), and 39 to 93 (MIDUS-III). Descriptive statistics for participants in the current study are presented in Table 1.
Table 1.
Sample descriptive statistics (by sexual orientation) presented by MIDUS wave: mean (standard deviation) or N (valid %): coding scheme A
MIDUS-I (1995–1996) (n=6304) | MIDUS-II (2004–2006) (n=4742) | MIDUS-III (2013–2014) (n=3230) | ||||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
|
|
|
||||||||||
Heterosexual | Lesbian/gay | Bisexual | Total | Heterosexual | Lesbian/gay | Bisexual | Total | Heterosexual | Lesbian/gay | Bisexual | Total | |
ns | 6108(96.9%) | 112 (1.8%) | 84 (1.3%) | 6304 (100%) | 4595(96.9%) | 79 (1.7%) | 68 (1.4%) | 4742 (100%) | 3132(97.0%) | 59 (1.8%) | 39 (1.2%) | 3230 (100%) |
Age (in years) | 46.77 (12.85) | 42.49(12.12) | 47.08 (13.50) | 46.70 (12.85) | 55.59 (12.37) | 52.62 (12.11) | 56.87 (13.16) | 55.56 (12.38) | 63.74 (11.31) | 59.86(10.68) | 63.74 (11.78) | 63.67 (11.32) |
Gender | ||||||||||||
Female | 3206(52.5%) | 49 (43.8%) | 43 (51.2%) | 3298(52.3%) | 2471(53.8%) | 32 (40.5%) | 38 (55.9%) | 2541(53.6%) | 1742(55.6%) | 21 (35.6%) | 20 (51.3%) | 1783(55.2%) |
Male | 2902(47.5%) | 63 (56.3%) | 41 (48.8%) | 3006(47.7%) | 2124(46.2%) | 47 (59.5%) | 30 (44.1%) | 2201(46.4%) | 1390(44.4%) | 38 (64.4%) | 19 (48.7%) | 1447(44.8%) |
Race/ethnicity | ||||||||||||
White | 5352(90.7%) | 104 (93.7%) | 74 (90.2%) | 5530(90.8%) | 4106(93.0%) | 75 (96.2%) | 61 (92.4%) | 4242(93.0%) | 2833(93.7%) | 55 (94.8%) | 35 (92.1%) | 2923(93.7%) |
non-White | 546 (9.3%) | 7 (6.3%) | 8 (9.8%) | 561 (9.2%) | 309 (7.0%) | 3 (3.8%) | 5 (7.6%) | 317 (7.0%) | 191 (6.3%) | 3 (5.2%) | 3 (7.9%) | 197 (6.3%) |
Education | ||||||||||||
Some college or more | 3826(62.8%) | 76 (67.9%) | 54 (64.3%) | 3956(62.9%) | 2999(65.4%) | 59 (74.7%) | 45 (66.2%) | 3103(65.5%) | 2163(69.1%) | 45 (76.3%) | 29 (74.4%) | 2237(69.3%) |
High school or less | 2270(37.2%) | 36 (32.1%) | 30 (35.7%) | 2336(37.1%) | 1588(34.6%) | 20 (25.3%) | 23 (33.8%) | 1631(34.5%) | 965 (30.9%) | 14 (23.7%) | 10 (25.6%) | 989 (30.7%) |
Marital Status | ||||||||||||
Married | 4209(68.9%) | 24 (21.4%) | 31 (36.9%) | 4264(67.7%) | 3330(72.5%) | 16 (20.8%) | 24 (36.4%) | 3370(71.2%) | 2146(68.6%) | 12 (20.3%) | 17 (43.6%) | 2175(67.4%) |
Not married | 1897(31.1%) | 88 (78.6%) | 53 (63.1%) | 2038(32.3%) | 1262(27.5%) | 61 (79.2%) | 42 (63.6%) | 1365(28.8%) | 982 (31.4%) | 47 (79.7%) | 22 (56.4%) | 1051(32.6%) |
Life Satisfaction (0–10) | 7.72 (1.29)a | 7.30 (1.37)b | 7.33 (1.65)b | 7.71 (1.29) | 7.77 (1.23)a | 7.53 (1.47)a, b | 7.35 (1.54)b | 7.76 (1.24) | 7.83 (1.28)a | 7.34 (1.80)b | 6.71 (1.75)b | 7.80 (1.31) |
Table reflects coding scheme in which participants are coded as lesbian, gay, or bisexual if they identified as lesbian, gay, or bisexual at the last wave that participants reported their sexual orientation. Subscripts indicate instances in which a differs from b within each MIDUS wave at p<.01 with a Bonferroni correction in post hoc analyses. Greater scores indicate greater levels of life satisfaction
Measures
Sexual Orientation
A single item measured sexual orientation at each wave: “How would you describe your sexual orientation? Would you say you are heterosexual (sexually attracted only to the opposite sex), homosexual (sexually attracted only to your own sex), or bisexual (sexually attracted to both men and women)?” Although a handful of studies have explored sexual orientation in the MIDUS sample (i.e., Cochran et al., 2003; Corliss, Cochran, & Mays, 2002; Mays & Cochran, 2001; Riggle et al., 2009) and results from these studies have provided compelling evidence for relationships between LGB status and health within the MIDUS dataset, most were published prior to the collection of MIDUS-II and III, and therefore these studies included only a single wave of participants’ sexual orientation (reports were from MIDUS-I in 1995). More than one report of sexual identity paints a more comprehensive picture of sexual orientation across the adult life span. Mock and Eibach (2012) found that approximately 2% of men and women identified their sexual identity differently 9 years later, from MIDUS-I to MIDUS-II (see also Diamond & Rosky, 2016, for sexual orientation stability and change in MIDUS). We created two different coding schemes (described below) to examine whether sexual orientation predicted life satisfaction; although both coding procedures yielded a similar pattern and direction of results, both are presented given that this study is the first to examine the role of sexual orientation across three MIDUS waves of data collection.
First, we coded participants’ sexual orientation using the last instance they reported their identity (Coding Scheme A). For example, if a woman identified as heterosexual at MIDUS-I and then as lesbian at MIDUS-II and III, she was coded as lesbian or gay via this coding method. Using this method, participants were coded as heterosexual (n=6108), lesbian or gay (n=112), and bisexual (n=84; see Table 1 for details on this classification). Using a second method, we categorized participants as a sexual minority if they endorsed a lesbian, gay, or bisexual identity at least once across the three waves (Coding Scheme B). For example, if a woman identified as lesbian at MIDUS-I, lesbian at MIDUS-II, and heterosexual at MIDUS-III, we categorized her as lesbian or gay in the current analyses. This categorization resulted in a larger number of sexual minorities: heterosexual (n=6067), lesbian or gay (n=129), and bisexual (n=108; see Supplementary Table 1 for details on this classification). Primary analyses use the first coding system, but we report the analyses of the second coding system in supplementary materials. We coded sexual orientation as [2] heterosexual, [1] lesbian or gay, and [0] bisexual because SPSS identifies the highest coded value as the reference group, and we intended for heterosexuals to be our reference category.
Age
Participants reported their age at each wave. We specified age as a continuous, time-varying predictor to estimate the functional relationship of age with life satisfaction. Age was centered at 46.70 years, the mean of all 6304 participants in the first wave of data collection (MIDUS-I). Centering age in this manner allows the intercept to be interpreted as the estimated initial status (i.e., the expected value of life satisfaction when age in years=46.70). Each regression coefficient can be interpreted as the effect of a 1-year age difference. Wave was coded as [2] (MIDUS-I: 1995– 1996), [1] (MIDUS-II: 2004–2006), and [0] (MIDUS-III: 2013 –2014), and these codes were used to designate each wave as a repeated measurement.
Covariates
Sociodemographic factors were selected as covariates based on their potential for confounding the association between our primary variables of interest. Gender, race/ethnicity, education, and marital status represent important individual differences to consider with respect to aging and sexual orientation (e.g., Knies, Nandi, & Platt, 2016; Stutzer & Frey, 2006). Participants reported their gender at each wave (coded as [1] female, [0] male), and we treated gender as time-invariant (constant) because no participants reported a change in gender across the three waves. Race/ethnicity was also treated as constant and coded as [1] White or [0] not White (reports were from MIDUS-I). Education and marital status were analyzed as time-dependent covariates because some participants bettered their education and/or had a change in marital status over the 18-year study period. Education was coded as [1] graduated high school or less or [0] attended some college or more, and marital status as [1] married or [0] not married to designate high school or less and married as reference groups, respectively. These variables have been dichotomized similarly in previous studies that have analyzed MIDUS data (e.g., Forbes et al., 2017; Riggle et al., 2009).
Life Satisfaction
Life satisfaction was assessed using a five-item measure in which participants were asked to rate five dimensions of their lives on a scale from 0 (the worst possible) to 10 (the best possible): life overall, work, health, relationship with spouse/partner, and relationship with children (Prenda & Lachman, 2001). For example, participants responded to the following item for the health dimension, “Using a scale from 0 to 10 where 0 means ‘the worst possible health’ and 10 means ‘the best possible health,’ how would you rate your health these days?” Scores for relationship with spouse/partner and relationship with children are averaged to create one item. This score is used with the other three items to calculate an overall mean score of life satisfaction. Higher scores indicate greater overall life satisfaction. Life satisfaction was assessed identically at MIDUS-I, MIDUS-II, and MIDUS-III, and internal consistency across each wave has been documented as .67, .65, and.63, respectively (see Brim et al., 2007; Fleeson, 2004).
Analysis Plan
The linear mixed models (LMMs) procedure in SPSS (version 24) was used to conduct all analyses (see Hoffman, 2015; Shek & Ma, 2011; West, 2009 for more on the linear mixed models technique in SPSS). We computed growth models for change using multilevel modeling (MLM) procedures established for data analysis with repeated measures, with life satisfaction as the dependent variable (assessed identically at three MIDUS waves). MLM procedures can accommodate missing data at the within-person level and observations that are unevenly spaced; thus, this technique provides powerful and reliable estimates of changes over time (Bryk & Raudenbush, 1987; Rogosa & Willett, 1985). Using MLM procedures, we simultaneously modeled how each person’s life satisfaction changed with age (within-person change) and how trends in life satisfaction differed across bisexual, lesbian or gay, and heterosexual people (between-person differences in change).
To explore the optimal predictor(s)of change in life satisfaction over time, we assessed model fit with the Bayesian information criterion (BIC): The smaller the information criterion value is, the better the model fit (as suggested by Forbes et al., 2017; Yang & Land, 2013). The unstructured variance–covariance structure, which estimates unique correlations for each pair of time points, fit to our data best and allowed our models to converge. We additionally fit our multilevel model for change with maximum likelihood (ML) estimation to calculate missing responses based on available data, rather than through imputation (see Raudenbush & Bryk, 2002). All analyses built on the first step, and we used F-tests and/or t-tests as appropriate to evaluate the significance of the fixed effects in our models.
To control for dependency in the observations, we specified in the repeated command of each model that measurements over the three waves of data were from the same individual. We included a random intercept (i.e., our heterosexual, lesbian/gay, and bisexual groups might each have different baselines defining their life satisfaction trajectories) and a random effect for age, which allowed each groups’ life satisfactions lopes to vary by age. Life satisfaction was the dependent variable for all steps of analyses. Sexual orientation, race, and gender were held constant (fixed), and education and marital status were analyzed as time-dependent. We also tested whether patterns of life satisfaction were linear or curvilinear over time.
Results
To test whether bisexual individuals experienced similar age-related trends in life satisfaction as their heterosexual, lesbian, and gay counterparts during adulthood, we performed four steps of analyses.
Age-Related Predictors of Change in Life Satisfaction Across Adulthood
First, we examined whether age accounted for change in people’s life satisfaction across adulthood (see Table 2, step 1). Age was positively related to life satisfaction, such that life satisfaction increases throughout adulthood, F(1,8925.98)=98.80, p<.001. We also tested a quadratic age variable to assess whether life satisfaction changed in a curvilinear fashion overtime, but this effect was not significant, F(1,7435.71)=1.12, p=.290. Additional computations supported linear changes in life satisfaction as the model that best described our data, as opposed to curvilinear. The BICs for the linear age model versus the quadratic age model were 40,497.17 and 40,505.57, respectively; thus, we computed whether the decrease in −2 Log Likelihood (−2LL) between these two models was significant ( ,393.04–40,391.97=1.07; dfchange =12– 11=1). The critical value for the Chi-square statistic for df=1 is 6.63 (p<.01); therefore, as 1.07 is less than 6.63, the addition of the quadratic term did not significantly improve the model. Thus, the quadratic term was not included in any of our models.
Table 2.
Estimates of fixed effects (95% confidence intervals) for the full models predicting change in life satisfaction: coding scheme A
Parameter (reference category) | Step 1 (n=6304) | Step 2 (n=6085) | Step 3 (n=6085) | Step 4 (n=6085) |
---|---|---|---|---|
Intercept | 7.66 (7.63, 7.69) | 7.80 (7.75, 7.86) | 7.81 (7.76, 7.87) | 7.81 (7.75, 7.86) |
Age (mean centered at 46.70 years) | .008 (.007, .01) | .009 (.007, .01) | .008 (.007, .01) | .009 (.007, .01) |
Gender (ref: female) | – | −.12 (−.18, −.06) | −.12 (−.17, −.06) | −.12 (−.17, −.06) |
Race/ethnicity (ref: white) | – | −.04 (−.14, .06) | −.05 (−.15, .05) | −.05 (−.15, .05) |
Education (ref: graduated high school or less) | – | .08 (.03, .14) | .09 (.03, .14) | .09 (.03, .14) |
Marital status (ref: married) | – | −.41 (−.46, −.36) | −.40 (−.45, −.35) | −.40 (−.45, −.35) |
Sexual orientation (ref: heterosexual) | ||||
Lesbian/gay | – | – | −.15 (−.36, .06) | −.15 (−.36, .06) |
Bisexual | – | – | −.41 (−.66, −.17) | −.34 (−.59,−.09) |
Age × sexual orientation (ref: heterosexual) | ||||
Lesbian/gay | – | – | – | −.003 (−.02, .01) |
Bisexual | – | – | – | −.01 (−.03, −.0001) |
Table reflects coding scheme in which participants are coded as lesbian, gay, or bisexual if they identified as lesbian, gay, or bisexual at the last wave that participants reported their sexual orientation. As participants endorsed life satisfaction on a 10-point scale, age estimates can be multiplied by 10 to get the percentage of change in life satisfaction for every 1 year of age. Estimates are in bold when the global F-test for the effect or the t test for the different levels of categorical effects is significant (p<.05). Greater scores indicate greater levels of life satisfaction
Sociodemographic Covariates of Age and Sexual Orientation
Second, we accounted for additional sociodemographic characteristics (i.e., gender, race, education, and marital status) to examine whether our linear age effects held above and beyond these individual differences (see Table 2, step2). Consistent with extant research, gender, education, and marital status each separately predicted life satisfaction, such that women had significantly greater life satisfaction than men on average, t(5674.45)=−4.15, p< .001, more educated people had greater life satisfaction on average than people with a high school diploma or less, t(9539.44)=3.14, p=.002, and married people were more satisfied with life than nonmarried people on average, t(12,115.73)=−16.21, p<.001. Race/ethnicity was not a significant predictor of life satisfaction in the current study, t(5914.81)= −0.87, p=.383. The parameters in our first model remained mostly unchanged with the addition of these covariates, again suggesting that linear age was a robust predictor of change in life satisfaction in the present sample.
Life Satisfaction as a Function of Sexual Orientation
Third, we investigated whether overall reports of life satisfaction varied by sexual orientation. Sexual orientation was a significant predictor of life satisfaction, F(2,5312.57)=6.61, p<.001. As shown in Table 2 (step 3), bisexuals reported significantly lower life satisfaction on average compared to their heterosexual counterparts, t(5499.87)= −3.83, p=.001; however, lesbian and gay individuals did not differ from heterosexuals in their overall reports of life satisfaction, t(5131.74)= −1.42, p=.157.1
Age-Related Change in Life Satisfaction as a Function of Sexual Orientation
In the final step of our analyses, we added to our model the interaction between age and sexual orientation to test whether bisexuals, lesbian and gay individuals, and heterosexuals had different trajectories of change in life satisfaction across the adult life span. As expected, heterosexuals showed a linear increase in life satisfaction with age, b=.009, t(8721.77)=10.63, p<.001 (see Fig. 1). Lesbian and gay individuals had a similar pattern in life satisfaction over time compared to heterosexuals, such that their trajectories did not significantly differ from those of heterosexuals, b=−.003, t(8143.45)= −.437, p=.662. However, bisexuals’ patterns in life satisfaction across age differed from the pattern experienced by heterosexuals, b = −.01, t(9452.97) = − 1.97, p = .049. These results suggest that bisexuals do not experience an increase in life satisfaction with age, as compared to their gay/lesbian and heterosexual counterparts.
Fig. 1.
Growth plot of fitted data from the final model (step 4). Plot illustrates the interaction between sexual orientation and age in predicting life satisfaction
General Discussion
The current study used a national probability sample to investigate how bisexual people compare to their lesbian, gay, and heterosexual counterparts in terms of life satisfaction across the adult life span. We predicted age-related changes in life satisfaction over time, and that bisexuals would experience lower life satisfaction compared to their lesbian, gay, and heterosexual peers. In the overall sample, we found age-related linear patterns of change in life satisfaction across adulthood, such that life satisfaction increases with every year of age. However, as expected, compared with heterosexuals, bisexual adults experienced significantly lower life satisfaction and differed in change trajectories of life satisfaction. Although bisexuals’ life satisfaction trajectory differed from that of heterosexuals, we did not find a significant difference between bisexuals and lesbian and gay people (nor did lesbian and gay people’s trajectory for life satisfaction significantly differ from that of heterosexuals), suggesting that lesbian and gay individuals’ average life satisfaction and changes in life satisfaction fall somewhere in between those of heterosexual and bisexual individuals, but do not significantly differ from either group. Yet, the stark contrast in average levels and changes in life satisfaction between bisexuals and heterosexuals is alarming and deserves future attention.
Why might bisexuals have lower life satisfaction and different trajectories than heterosexual peers? The minority stress model provides clear evidence that negative experiences endured by sexual minorities contribute to health disparities between heterosexuals and bisexuals (Meyer, 2003). However, despite the large body of research that tests various aspects of the minority stress model, we know relatively less about how the minority stress model accounts for experiences and health in older adult samples. We demonstrate that older bisexual adults may be particularly vulnerable to disparities in health and well-being, perhaps because of the accumulation of stress and psychosocial disadvantages over a lifetime. Notably, the minority stress model highlights certain buffers to the disruptive ways negative life events contribute to minority-related stress and negative health outcomes. Channels of social and community support, for example, may buffer minority stress and, in turn, attenuate health disparities (Meyer, 2003). There is emerging evidence that bisexuals may be less likely to have access to such buffers and, in particular, feelings of isolation and loneliness may be relevant to older age (Fredriksen-Goldsen et al., 2017).
Although bisexuals are the largest subgroup of sexual and gender minorities (Herbenick et al., 2010), they have been termed “a minority within a minority” and report receiving “double stigma” from both lesbian/gay and heterosexual communities (McLean, 2008; Pistella, Salvati, Ioverno, Laghi, & Baiocco, 2016). This “double stigma” can begin at a very young age and persist over the life span (Hatzenbuehler & Pachankis, 2016). For example, bisexual women and men report feeling marginalized, excluded, and stigmatized both within and outside of sexual minority communities (Callis, 2013; Hartman, 2006; McLean, 2008) and older bisexuals report less social support and community belonging compared with older lesbian and gay adults (Fredriksen-Goldsen et al., 2017). Thus, though lesbian, gay, and bisexual people did not significantly differ from one another in this research, it seems reasonable that bisexuals may be less satisfied with life than their lesbian and gay counterparts given that their experiences of stigma are different, they may lack access to community-level social support relative to lesbian and gay individuals, and they may experience more health problems. Given the relatively small sample sizes of bisexuals and lesbian and gay people, it is possible that differences between bisexuals and lesbian and gay people may be uncovered with more statistical power, which would be consistent with recent research that finds psychological health disparities across LGB people broadly (Bostwick et al., 2015; Fredriksen-Goldsen et al., 2010, 2017; Gonzales et al., 2016). Moreover, we were unable to explore gender as an additional layer of sexual minority status, despite having relatively equal numbers of bisexual women and men, because our sample size was too small for testing a three-way interaction between age, LGB status, and gender.
Another worthy future direction is to evaluate the roles of social and community support networks for bisexuals in later adulthood (e.g., what kind of support do bisexuals perceive or expect to receive in older age?). Increasing channels of social support may be a significant pathway to healthy and successful aging for bisexuals by increasing life satisfaction. The minority stress model predicts that social and community-level support help to counter the negative health impacts of stress on sexual minorities (Meyer, 2003). Many lesbian and gay individuals find support and care through “families of choice” or people they choose to play a significant role in their lives even though they are not legally or biologically related (Barker, Herdt, & de Vries, 2006; Brennan-Ing, Seidel, Larson, & Karpiak, 2014; Croghan, Moone, & Olson, 2014; Grossman, Daugelli, & Hershberger, 2000). However, some bisexuals report a lack of social and community-level support from other sexual minorities and report feeling stigmatized from both the heterosexual and lesbian/gay communities (e.g., Callis, 2013; Dworkin, 2006; McLean, 2008). This may be particularly true of older bisexuals. For example, in one study, lesbian and gay elders were more inclined to care for other elder lesbian and gay adults than they were to care for bisexual and heterosexual elders (Grossman, D’Augelli, & Dragowski, 2007). To improve life satisfaction among bisexuals, future research must consider how to improve social dynamics within broader LGB communities.
Implications and Limitations
Fortunately, subjective well-being is malleable and thus presents a possible target for intervention. Future research should consider strategies to increase life satisfaction for at-risk adults. Interventions do not need to occur solely in early life. Midlife can be an advantageous time for people to gain control of their health and set themselves on a healthier path to aging. As life satisfaction may decrease from major life events such as bereavement and unemployment (Anusic, Yap, & Lucas, 2014; Clark, Diener, Georgellis, & Lucas, 2008), life satisfaction is also susceptible to factors that may increase well-being.
Future research could additionally examine how the three facets of subjective well-being (i.e., life satisfaction, positive affect, and negative affect; Diener, 1984) vary by LGB status. In the current research, we chose to attend to patterns of life satisfaction given the little attention paid to how life satisfaction varies by sexual orientation across age, and the many implications that life satisfaction has for various aspects of well-being, such as health, financial security, close relationships, and work conditions. However, positive affect and negative affect should be explored as additional indicators of whether bisexuals depart from heterosexuals in terms of their psychological health. Further, it may be more feasible to intervene on positive affect and negative affect (e.g., interventions to improve gratitude and pride to boost positive affect; Lyubomirsky, Dickerhoof, Boehm, & Sheldon, 2011) than on life satisfaction, which may function as a broader concept that relates to past treatment as well as current status. Intervention on life satisfaction may require a more long-term process, such as through improving LGB community relations or reducing stigma toward bisexuality.
We encourage researchers to consider bisexuals as their own group with their own health concerns, stressors, and challenges. Bisexuals are frequently collapsed within the “LGB” category in research. Only about one quarter of studies that include bisexuals consider bisexual women as a distinct category separate from lesbian women (Carr, 2011). Our research adds to a growing body of literature that supports disaggregating these groups to fully understand their experiences, strengths, and risk factors. For example, if bisexuals were included in the lesbian and gay category in the present study, we may have concluded that LGB people do not differ from heterosexuals as a whole. This conclusion would have been inaccurate, as it would neglect the nuance we found in life satisfaction: Bisexuals are not experiencing an increase in life satisfaction despite what theories of life satisfaction and general aging processes might predict.
Along these lines, people’s sexual orientation can and does change over time. In the current study, we were unable to treat sexual orientation as a predictor that varied with time because we did not have enough variability in within-person reports of sexual orientation across the three waves (i.e., most people did not change their reports of sexual orientation in the 18-year time frame). Moreover, we had a narrow measure of sexual orientation—one that assesses attraction, as opposed to treating sexual orientation as a complex and multifaceted construct that could include identity, desire, and behavior. A measure that was more focused on identity, for example, would likely have excluded people who experience same-gender attraction but do not identify with a bisexual, lesbian, or gay label. On the other hand, a measure that is linked to attraction (as used in the current study) may have captured people who experience attraction, but do not identify themselves as bisexual, lesbian, or gay. It is an empirical and fascinating question to consider how the properties of sexual orientation measurement may yield slightly different results given that slightly different people and experiences are represented in the samples.
Conclusion
Although the causal directionality of the life satisfaction and health connection is not well-established, it is clear that life satisfaction is a telling indicator of well-being (see Diener & Chan, 2011). Ultimately, the negative outcomes associated with lower life satisfaction are concerning for bisexuals, as life satisfaction is related to a host of negative health outcomes that we observe in bisexual people’s adulthood (e.g., depression and social isolation). Health and policy advocates benefit from understanding how to best target interventions toward different older adult age-groups. The current study represents one step toward understanding disparities in well-being among those who identify as bisexual. Using MIDUS and other midlife samples of LGB people, researchers can identify specific age brackets that are most vulnerable to certain health outcomes and, likewise, most susceptible to change. The current research highlights how life satisfaction does not follow a universal upward trajectory across the life span, and when used as a multifaceted proxy for psychological health, can identify disparities in people’s quality of life. Again, we found that aging bisexuals in the current study did not report increases in life satisfaction that were experienced by their lesbian/gay and heterosexual counterparts. We hope that the findings of this research motivate researchers to consider sexual orientation as an integral part of the aging process.
Supplementary Material
Acknowledgments
Britney M. Wardecker is partially supported by National Institute on Aging Grant T32 AG049676 to the Pennsylvania State University. Since 1995, the MIDUS study has been funded by the John D. and Catherine T. MacArthur Foundation Research Network and by the National Institute on Aging Grants P01-AG020166 and U19-AG051426.
Footnotes
We adjusted our reference group to bisexuals (by recoding our sexual orientation variable as [2] bisexual, [1] lesbian or gay, and [0] heterosexual) so that we could examine whether life satisfaction differed between bisexuals and lesbian and gay individuals. Contrary to our hypothesis, lesbian and gay individuals did not differ from bisexuals in life satisfaction, t(5488.86)=1.64, p=.101; lesbian or gay individuals’ reports of life satisfaction were somewhere in between those of heterosexuals and bisexuals, but did not significantly differ from either group.
Electronic supplementary material The online version of this article (https://doi.org/10.1007/s10508-018-1151-5) contains supplementary material, which is available to authorized users.
References
- Aldwin CM, Levenson MR. Stress, coping, and health at midlife: A developmental perspective. In: Lachman ME, editor. Handbook of Midlife Development. New York, NY: Wiley; 2001. [Google Scholar]
- Anusic I, Yap SC, Lucas RE. Testing set-point theory in a swiss national sample: Reaction and adaptation to major life events. Social Indicators Research. 2014;119(3):1265–1288. doi: 10.1007/s11205-013-0541-2. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Baltes PB, Reese HW, Nesselroade JR. Life-Span Developmental Psychology: Introduction to Research Methods. Monterey, CA: Brooks-Cole; 1977. [Google Scholar]
- Barker JC, Herdt G, de Vries B. Social support in the lives of lesbians and gay men at midlife and later. Sexuality Research & Social Policy. 2006;3(2):1–23. doi: 10.1525/srsp.2006.3.2.1. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Bostwick WB, Boyd CJ, Hughes TL, McCabe SE. Dimensions of sexual orientation and the prevalence of mood and anxiety disorders in the United States. American Journal of Public Health. 2010;100(3):468–475. doi: 10.2105/ajph.2008.152942. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bostwick WB, Hughes TL, Everett B. Health behavior, status, and outcomes among a community-based sample of lesbian and bisexual women. LGBT Health. 2015;2(2):121–126. doi: 10.1089/lgbt.2014.0074. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brennan-Ing M, Seidel L, Larson B, Karpiak SE. Social care networks and older LGBT adults: Challenges for the future. Journal of Homosexuality. 2014;61(1):21–52. doi: 10.1080/00918369.2013.835235. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brim OG, Baltes PB, Bumpass LL, Cleary PD, Featherman DL, Hazzard WR, … Shweder RA. National Survey of Midlife Development in the United States (MIDUS 1), 1995–1996. Ann Arbor, MI: Inter-university Consortium for Political and Social Research (ICPSR) [distributor]; 2007. Retrieved from . [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Brim OG, Ryff CD, Kessler RC. How Healthy Are We? A National Study of Well-Being at Midlife. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press; 2004. [Google Scholar]
- Bryk AS, Raudenbush SW. Application of hierarchical linear models to assessing change. Psychological Bulletin. 1987;101(1):147–158. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.101.1.147. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Callis AS. The black sheep of the pink flock: Labels, stigma, and bisexual identity. Journal of Bisexuality. 2013;13(1):82–105. doi: 10.1080/15299716.2013.755730. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Carr CL. Women’s bisexuality as a category in social research, revisited. Journal of Bisexuality. 2011;11(4):550–559. doi: 10.1080/15299716.2011.620868. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Carstensen LL, Pasupathi M, Mayr U, Nesselroade JR. Emotional experience in everyday life across the adult lifespan. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 2000;79(4):644–655. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.79.4.644. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Charles ST, Carstensen LL. Socioemotional selectivity theory. In: Reis HT, Sprecher S, editors. Encyclopedia of Human Relationships. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications; 2009. pp. 1578–1581. [Google Scholar]
- Charles ST, Carstensen LL. Social and emotional aging. Annual Review of Psychology. 2010;61:383–409. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.093008.100448. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chida Y, Steptoe A. Positive psychological well-being and mortality: A quantitative review of prospective observational studies. Psychosomatic Medicine. 2008;70(7):741–756. doi: 10.1097/PSY.0b013e31818105ba. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Choi SK, Meyer IH. LGBT Aging: A Review of Research Findings, Needs, and Policy Implications. Los Angeles: The Williams Institute; 2016. [Google Scholar]
- Clark AE, Diener E, Georgellis Y, Lucas RE. Lags and leads in life satisfaction: A test of the baseline hypothesis. The Economic Journal. 2008;118(529):F222–F243. doi: 10.1111/j.1468-0297.2008.02150.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Cochran SD, Sullivan JG, Mays VM. Prevalence of mental disorders, psychological distress, and mental health services use among lesbian, gay, and bisexual adults in the United States. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 2003;71(1):53–61. doi: 10.1037/0022-006X.71.1.53. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Collins AL, Goldman N, Rodriguez G. Is positive well-being protective of mobility limitations among older adults? The Journals of Gerontology: Series B, Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences. 2008;63(6):P321–P327. doi: 10.1093/geronb/63.6.p321. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Corliss HL, Cochran SD, Mays VM. Reports of parental maltreatment during childhood in a United States population-based survey of homosexual, bisexual, and heterosexual adults. Child Abuse and Neglect. 2002;26(11):1165–1178. doi: 10.1016/S0145-2134(02)00385-x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Croghan CF, Moone RP, Olson AM. Friends, family, and caregiving among midlife and older lesbian, gay, bisexual, and transgender adults. Journal of Homosexuality. 2014;61(1):79–102. doi: 10.1080/00918369.2013.835238. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- DeNeve JE, Diener E, Tay L, Xuereb C. The objective benefits of subjective well-being. In: Helliwell J, Layard R, Sachs J, editors. World Happiness Report 2013. New York, NY: UN Sustainable Development Solutions Network; 2013. pp. 54–79. [Google Scholar]
- de Vries B. Aspects of life and death, grief and loss in lesbian, gay, bisexual and transgender communities. In: Doka KJ, Tucci AS, editors. Living with grief: Diversity in end-of-life care. 2009. [Google Scholar]
- Diamond LM, Rosky CJ. Scrutinizing immutability: Research on sexual orientation and U.S. legal advocacy for sexual minorities. Journal of Sex Research. 2016;53(4–5):363–391. doi: 10.1080/00224499.2016.1139665. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Diener E. Subjective well-being. Psychological Bulletin. 1984;95(3):542–575. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.95.3.542. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Diener E, Chan MY. Happy people live longer: Subjective well-being contributes to health and longevity. Applied Psychology: Health and Well-Being. 2011;3(1):1–43. doi: 10.1111/j.1758-0854.2010.01045.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Diener E, Heintzelman SJ, Kushlev K, Tay L, Wirtz D, Lutes LD, Oishi S. Findings all psychologists should know from the new science on subjective well-being. Canadian Psychology. 2017;58(2):87–104. [Google Scholar]
- Diener E, Kanazawa S, Suh EM, Oishi S. Why people are in a generally good mood. Personality and Social Psychology Review. 2015;19(3):235–256. doi: 10.1177/1088868314544467. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Diener E, Seligman MEP. Very happy people. Psychological Science. 2002;13(1):81–84. doi: 10.1111/1467-9280.00415. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Diener E, Suh EM, Lucas RE, Smith H. Subjective well-being: Three decades of progress. Psychological Bulletin. 1999;125(2):276–302. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.125.2.276. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Dworkin SH. The aging bisexual: The invisible of the invisible minority. In: Kimmel DC, Rose T, David S, editors. Lesbian, Gay, Bisexual and Transgender Aging: Research and Clinical Perspectives. New York, NY: Columbia University Press; 2006. pp. 36–52. [Google Scholar]
- Emlet CA. Social, economic, and health disparities among LGBT older adults. Generations. 2016;40(2):16–22. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Finke MS, Huston SJ, Sharpe DL. Balance sheets of early boomers: Are they different from pre-boomers? Journal of Family and Economic Issues. 2006;27(3):542–561. doi: 10.1007/s10834-006-9026-7. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Fleeson W. The quality of american life at the end of the century. In: Brim OG, Ryff CD, Kessler RC, editors. How Healthy Are We? A National Study of Well-Being at Midlife. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press; 2004. pp. 252–272. [Google Scholar]
- Forbes MK, Eaton NR, Krueger RF. Sexual quality of life and aging: A prospective study of a nationally representative sample. The Journal of Sex Research. 2017;54(2):137–148. doi: 10.1080/00224499.2016.1233315. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fredriksen-Goldsen KI, Kim HJ, Barkan SE, Balsam KF, Mincer SL. Disparities in health-related quality of life: A comparison of lesbians and bisexual women. American Journal of Public Health. 2010;100(11):2255–2261. doi: 10.2105/ajph.2009.177329. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fredriksen-Goldsen KI, Kim HJ, Barkan SE, Muraco A, Hoy-Ellis CP. Health disparities among lesbian, gay, and bisexual older adults: Results from a population-based study. American Journal of Public Health. 2013;103(10):1802–1809. doi: 10.2105/AJPH.2012.301110. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fredriksen-Goldsen KI, Kim HJ, Emlet CA, Muraco A, Erosheva EA, Hoy-Ellis CP, … Petry H. The Aging and Health Report: Disparities and Resilience Among Lesbian, Gay, Bisexual, and Transgender Older Adults. Seattle: Institute for Multigenerational Health; 2011. [Google Scholar]
- Fredriksen-Goldsen KI, Kim HJ, Shiu C, Goldsen J, Emlet CA. Successful aging among LGBT older adults: Physical and mental health-related quality of life by age group. The Gerontologist. 2015;55(1):154–168. doi: 10.1093/geront/gnu081. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fredriksen-Goldsen KI, Shiu C, Bryan AE, Goldsen J, Kim HJ. Health equity and aging of bisexual older adults: Pathways of risk and resilience. The Journals of Gerontology: Series B, Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences. 2017;72(3):468–478. doi: 10.1093/geronb/gbw120. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gonzales G, Przedworski J, Henning-Smith C. Comparison of health and health risk factors between lesbian, gay, and bisexual adults and heterosexual adults in the United States: Results from the national health interview survey. JAMA Internal Medicine. 2016;176(9):1344–1351. doi: 10.1001/jamainternmed.2016.3432. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gorman BK, Denney JT, Dowdy H, Medeiros RA. A new piece of the puzzle: Sexual orientation, gender, and physical health status. Demography. 2015;52(4):1357–1382. doi: 10.1007/s13524-015-0406-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Grant N, Wardle J, Steptoe A. The relationship between life satisfaction and health behavior: A cross-cultural analysis of young adults. International Journal of Behavioral Medicine. 2009;16(3):259–268. doi: 10.1007/s12529-009-9032-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Grossman AH, D’Augelli AR, Dragowski EA. Caregiving and care receiving among older lesbian, gay, and bisexual adults. Journal of Gay & Lesbian Social Services. 2007;18(3–4):15–38. doi: 10.1300/J041v18n03_02. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Grossman AH, Daugelli AR, Hershberger SL. Social support networks of lesbian, gay, and bisexual adults 60 years of age and older. The Journals of Gerontology: Series B, Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences. 2000;55(3):171–179. doi: 10.1093/geronb/55.3.P171. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Grossman AH, D’Augelli AR, O’Connell TS. Being lesbian, gay, bisexual, and 60 or older in North America. Journal of Gay & Lesbian Social Services. 2001;13(4):23–40. doi: 10.1300/J041v13n04_05. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Hamarat E, Thompson D, Zabrucky KM, Steele D, Matheny KB, Aysan F. Perceived stress and coping resource availability as predictors of life satisfaction in young, middle-aged, and older adults. Experimental Aging Research. 2001;27(2):181–196. doi: 10.1080/036107301750074051. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hartman JE. Another kind of “chilly climate”. Journal of Bisexuality. 2006;5(4):61–76. doi: 10.1300/J159v05n04_06. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Hatzenbuehler ML. How does sexual minority stigma “get under the skin”?A psychological mediation framework. Psychological Bulletin. 2009;135(5):707–730. doi: 10.1037/a0016441. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hatzenbuehler ML, Pachankis JE. Stigma and minority stress as social determinants of health among lesbian, gay, bisexual, and transgender youth. Pediatric Clinics. 2016;63(6):985–997. doi: 10.1016/j.pcl.2016.07.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Herbenick D, Reece M, Schick V, Sanders SA, Dodge B, Fortenberry JD. Sexual behavior in the United States: Results from a national probability sample of men and women ages 14–94. The Journal of Sexual Medicine. 2010;7(5):255–265. doi: 10.1111/j.1743-6109.2010.02012.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hoffman L. Longitudinal Analysis: Modeling Within-Person Fluctuation and Change. New York, NY: Routledge; 2015. [Google Scholar]
- Institute of Medicine. The Health of Lesbian, Gay, Bisexual, and Transgender People: Building a Foundation for Better Understanding. Washington, DC: The National Academies Press; 2011. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kahneman D, Deaton A. High income improves evaluation of life but not emotional well-being. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 2010;107(38):16489–16493. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1011492107. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kimmel DC. Lesbian, gay, bisexual, and transgender aging concerns. Clinical Gerontologist. 2014;37(1):49–63. doi: 10.1080/07317115.2014.847310. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Knies G, Nandi A, Platt L. Life satisfaction, ethnicity and neighbourhoods: Is there an effect of neighbourhood ethnic composition on life satisfaction? Social Science Research. 2016;60:110–124. doi: 10.1016/j.ssresearch.2016.01.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Koivumaa-Honkanen H, Koskenvuo M, Honkanen RJ, Viinamaki H, Heikkila K, Kaprio J. Life dissatisfaction and subsequent work disability in an 11-year follow-up. Psychological Medicine. 2004;34(2):221–228. doi: 10.1017/s0033291703001089. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lachman ME, Teshale S, Agrigoroaei S. Midlife as a pivotal period in the life course: Balancing growth and decline at the crossroads of youth and old age. International Journal of Behavioral Development. 2015;39(1):20–31. doi: 10.1177/0165025414533223. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lyubomirsky S, Dickerhoof R, Boehm JK, Sheldon KM. Becoming happier takes both a will and a proper way: An experimental longitudinal intervention to boost well-being. Emotion. 2011;11(2):391–402. doi: 10.1037/a0022575. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lyubomirsky S, King L, Diener E. The benefits of frequent positive affect: Does happiness lead to success? Psychological Bulletin. 2005;131(6):803–855. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.131.6.803. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mays VM, Cochran SD. Mental health correlates of perceived discrimination among lesbian, gay, and bisexual adults in the united states. American Journal of Public Health. 2001;91(11):1869–1876. doi: 10.2105/ajph.91.11.1869. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McLean K. Inside, outside, nowhere: Bisexual men and women in the gay and lesbian community. Journal of Bisexuality. 2008;8(1–2):63–80. doi: 10.1080/15299710802143174. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Meyer IH. Prejudice, social stress, and mental health in lesbian, gay, and bisexual populations: Conceptual issues and research evidence. Psychological Bulletin. 2003;129(5):674–697. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.129.5.674. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mock SE, Eibach RP. Stability and change in sexual orientation identity over a 10-year period in adulthood. Archives of Sexual Behavior. 2012;41(3):641–648. doi: 10.1007/s10508-011-9761-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mroczek DK, Almeida DM. The effect of daily stress, personality, and age on daily negative affect. Journal of Personality. 2004;72(2):355–378. doi: 10.1111/j.0022-3506.2004.00265.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Orel NA. Investigating the needs and concerns of lesbian, gay, bisexual, and transgender older adults: The use of qualitative and quantitative methodology. Journal of Homosexuality. 2014;61(1):53–78. doi: 10.1080/00918369.2013.835236. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pistella J, Salvati M, Ioverno S, Laghi F, Baiocco R. Coming-out to family members and internalized sexual stigma in bisexual, lesbian and gay people. Journal of Child and Family Studies. 2016;25(12):3694–3701. doi: 10.1007/s10826-016-0528-0. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Prenda KM, Lachman ME. Planning for the future: A life management strategy for increasing control and life satisfaction in adulthood. Psychology and Aging. 2001;16(2):206–216. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pressman SD, Cohen S. Does positive affect influence health? Psychological Bulletin. 2005;131(6):925–971. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.131.6.925. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Radler BT. The Midlife in the United States (MIDUS) Series: A national longitudinal study of health and well-being. Open Health Data. 2014;2(1):e3. doi: 10.5334/ohd.ai. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Radler BT, Ryff CD. Who participates? Longitudinal retention in the MIDUS national study of health and well-being. Journal of Aging and Health. 2010;22(3):307–331. doi: 10.1177/0898264309358617. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Raudenbush SW, Bryk AS. Hierarchical Linear Models: Applications and Data Analysis Methods. 2. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications; 2002. [Google Scholar]
- Rauma PH, Koivumaa-Honkanen H, Williams LJ, Tuppurainen MT, Kroger HP, Honkanen RJ. Life satisfaction and bone mineral density among postmenopausal women: Cross-sectional and longitudinal associations. Psychosomatic Medicine. 2014;76(9):709–715. doi: 10.1097/psy.0000000000000114. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Riggle ED, Rostosky SS, Danner F. LGB identity and eudaimonic well-being in midlife. Journal of Homosexuality. 2009;56(6):786–798. doi: 10.1080/00918360903054277. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rogosa DR, Willett JB. Understanding correlates of change by modeling individual differences in growth. Psychometrika. 1985;50(2):203–228. doi: 10.1007/bf02294247. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Ryff CD, Almeida DM, Ayanian J, Binkley N, Carr DS, Coe C, … Williams D. National Survey of Midlife Development in the United States (MIDUS 3), 2013–2014. Ann Arbor, MI: Inter-university Consortium for Political and Social Research (ICPSR) [distributor]; 2017. Retrieved from . [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Ryff CD, Almeida DM, Ayanian J, Carr DS, Cleary PD, Coe C, … Williams D. National Survey of Midlife Development in the United States (MIDUS 2), 2004–2006. Ann Arbor, MI: Inter-university Consortium for Political and Social Research (ICPSR) [distributor]; 2012. Retrieved from . [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Salovey P, Rothman AJ, Detweiler JB, Steward WT. Emotional states and physical health. American Psychologist. 2000;55(1):110–121. doi: 10.1037//0003-066x.55.1.110. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schimmack U, Oishi S. The influence of chronically and temporarily accessible information on life satisfaction judgments. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 2005;89(3):395–406. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.89.3.395. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shek DT, Ma CM. Longitudinal data analyses using linear mixed models in SPSS: Concepts, procedures and illustrations. Scientific World Journal. 2011;11:42–76. doi: 10.1100/tsw.2011.2. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shin DC, Johnson DM. Avowed happiness as an overall assessment of the quality of life. Social Indicators Research. 1978;5(1):475–492. doi: 10.1007/bf00352944. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Siahpush M, Spittal M, Singh GK. Happiness and life satisfaction prospectively predict self-rated health, physical health, and the presence of limiting, long-term health conditions. American Journal of Health Promotion. 2008;23(1):18–26. doi: 10.4278/ajhp.061023137. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Smyth JM, Zawadzki MJ, Juth V, Sciamanna CN. Global life satisfaction predicts ambulatory affect, stress, and cortisol in daily life in working adults. Journal of Behavioral Medicine. 2017;40(2):320–331. doi: 10.1007/s10865-016-9790-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Steger MF, Oishi S, Kashdan TB. Meaning in life across the life span: Levels and correlates of meaning in life from emerging adulthood to older adulthood. The Journal of Positive Psychology. 2009;4(1):43–52. doi: 10.1080/17439760802303127. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Stutzer A, Frey BS. Does marriage make people happy, or do happy people get married? The Journal of Socio-Economics. 2006;35(2):326–347. doi: 10.1016/j.socec.2005.11.043. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Thomas ML, Kaufmann CN, Palmer BW, Depp CA, Martin AS, Glorioso DK, … Jeste DV. Paradoxical trend for improvement in mental health with aging: A community-based study of 1546 adults aged 21–100 years. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry. 2016;77(8):e1019–e1025. doi: 10.4088/JCP.16m10671. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- U.S. Department of Health and Human Services. Healthy people 2020 objectives: Lesbian, gay, bisexual, and transgender health. 2012 Retrieved from https://www.healthypeople.gov/2020/topics-objectives/topic/lesbian-gay-bisexual-and-transgender-health.
- Wallace SP, Cochran SD, Durazo EM, Ford CL. Policy Brief (UCLA Center for Health Policy Research) 2011. The health of aging lesbian, gay and bisexual adults in California; pp. 1–8. (Pb2011–2012) [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Weinberg MS, Williams CJ, Pryor DW. Bisexuals at midlife: Commitment, salience, and identity. Journal of Contemporary Ethnography. 2001;30(2):180–208. doi: 10.1177/089124101030002002. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- West BT. Analyzing longitudinal data with the linear mixed models procedure in SPSS. Evaluation and the Health Professions. 2009;32(3):207–228. doi: 10.1177/0163278709338554. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Xu J, Roberts RE. The power of positive emotions: It’s a matter of life or death–subjective well-being and longevity over 28 years in a general population. Health Psychology. 2010;29(1):9–19. doi: 10.1037/a0016767. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yang Y, Land KC. Age-Period-Cohort Analysis: New Models, Methods, and Empirical Applications. Boca Raton, FL: CRC Press; 2013. [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.