Skip to main content
Eurosurveillance logoLink to Eurosurveillance
. 2018 Jun 21;23(25):1700424. doi: 10.2807/1560-7917.ES.2018.23.25.1700424

Reducing demand for antibiotic prescriptions: evidence from an online survey of the general public on the interaction between preferences, beliefs and information, United Kingdom, 2015

Laurence S J Roope 1,2, Sarah Tonkin-Crine 2,3, Christopher C Butler 2,3, Derrick Crook 2,4,5,6, Tim Peto 2,4,5,6, Michele Peters 7, A Sarah Walker 2,4,5, Sarah Wordsworth 1,2,4
PMCID: PMC6152238  PMID: 29945699

Abstract

Background: Antimicrobial resistance (AMR), a major public health threat, is strongly associated with human antibiotic consumption. Influenza-like illnesses (ILI) account for substantial inappropriate antibiotic use; patient understanding and expectations probably play an important role. Aim: This study aimed to investigate what drives patient expectations of antibiotics for ILI and particularly whether AMR awareness, risk preferences (attitudes to taking risks with health) or time preferences (the extent to which people prioritise good health today over good health in the future) play a role. Methods: In 2015, a representative online panel survey of 2,064 adults in the United Kingdom was asked about antibiotic use and effectiveness for ILI. Explanatory variables in multivariable regression included AMR awareness, risk and time preferences and covariates. Results: The tendency not to prioritise immediate gain over later reward was independently strongly associated with greater awareness that antibiotics are inappropriate for ILI. Independently, believing antibiotics were effective for ILI and low AMR awareness significantly predicted reported antibiotic use. However, 272 (39%) of those with low AMR awareness said that the AMR information we provided would lead them to ask a doctor for antibiotics more often, significantly more than would do so less often, and in contrast to those with high AMR awareness (p < 0.0001). Conclusion: Information campaigns to reduce AMR may risk a paradoxical consequence of actually increasing public demand for antibiotics. Public antibiotic stewardship campaigns should be tested on a small scale before wider adoption.

Keywords: antibiotic use, antimicrobial resistance, Time preference, Risk-aversion, Information, Survey

Introduction

Antimicrobial resistance (AMR) is a growing threat to global health and the economy. A recent report commissioned by the United Kingdom (UK) government estimated that AMR could reduce global gross domestic product by 2–3.5%, costing up to USD 1014 (EUR 1.39 × 1014) from 2014 to 2050 [1]. Many pathogens are becoming resistant faster than alternative antibiotics are being developed [2]. There are strong associations between the volume of human antibiotic consumption and AMR [3]. Reducing AMR requires better antibiotic stewardship [4,5].

Respiratory tract infections (RTIs) account for most human antibiotic use [6,7]. However, RTIs are often viral, and more than half of the antibiotics taken for influenza-like-illnesses (ILI) are unnecessary. Even in healthcare systems such as the UK’s National Health Service, where general practitioners (GPs) act as gatekeepers to prescriptions, patient expectations probably play an important role in unnecessary antibiotic consumption [8,9]. Several multi-faceted public campaigns in high-income countries have successfully reduced antibiotic use, but isolating the impact of campaign elements intended to improve AMR awareness is difficult [10].

A large proportion of the public in the UK believe antibiotics are effective for ILI [11]. However, little is known about the links between AMR awareness and expectations of antibiotics, or of the influence of risk preferences (attitudes to taking risks with health) or time preferences (the extent to which people prioritise good health today over good health in the future). These latter characteristics predict other health-related behaviours [12-14]; we hypothesised that they might be associated with inappropriate antibiotic use. Moreover, the probable impact of public information campaigns about AMR on antibiotic demand is uncertain. Therefore, we surveyed a representative sample of the general adult population in the UK to assess AMR awareness, the role that this and other factors (particularly risk and time preferences) play in unnecessary antibiotic consumption for ILI, and the impact of information provision (awareness campaigns) on future unnecessary antibiotic use. The study’s key outcomes were individuals’ beliefs about effectiveness and reported use of antibiotics for ILI.

Methods

Survey design

We developed a web-based survey to ask the general public questions about antibiotics and AMR. Survey development was informed by discussions with three clinical experts (a GP and two junior doctors) and a patient and public involvement group (n = 7).

The survey (Supplement 1) asked respondents to consider Health State A: You have a fever, aching muscles, a headache, a dry chesty cough, a sore throat, and tiredness. This description was intended to convey ILI, for which antibiotics are often expected but not generally necessary or useful. Health State A was described symptomatically, rather than specifying a ‘flu’ or ‘virus’, for two reasons: Firstly, individuals may interpret such terms differently. Secondly, people may know that antibiotics are inappropriate for viral conditions, yet not recognise Health State A symptoms as more consistent with a viral than a bacterial infection.

Respondents were asked how they would respond in the hypothetical situation of having experienced Health State A for 5 days – in particular whether they would go to a doctor and request antibiotics. They were also asked whether they thought antibiotics would be likely to help this condition, and about their antibiotic use in the previous 12 months for similar conditions. Participants with dependent children then answered analogous questions about the hypothetical situation of their youngest child being ill.

The survey captured information on a wide range of factors that might be associated with patient expectations for, and use of, antibiotics, including sociodemographic factors such as age, sex, ethnicity and health. A short-form questionnaire [15] captured personality traits – ‘extraversion’, ‘agreeableness’, ‘conscientiousness’, ‘neuroticism’ and ‘openness.’ The survey also elicited respondents’ attitudes to taking risks with health (risk preferences), and their prioritisation of good health today over good health in the future (time preferences).

The respondents were then given information (Box) that paraphrased text on the websites of four institutions aiming to improve awareness of AMR [16-19]. Participants were asked how surprising they found the information, and how it would influence (i) their future attendance at a doctor’s surgery for conditions like Health State A; (ii) their likelihood of asking a doctor for antibiotics in future. Participants with children were asked analogous questions regarding how they would act for their youngest child.

Box. Information about antimicrobial resistance given to questionnaire respondents, United Kingdom, 2015.

Antibiotic resistance occurs when an antibiotic loses its ability to effectively control or kill growing bacteria. It is an increasingly serious threat to public health. Without effective antibiotics, many routine treatments will become increasingly dangerous. Setting broken bones, and even basic operations, rely on access to antibiotics that work. Antibiotic resistance is believed to be caused by unnecessary use of antibiotics, and inappropriate use, such as not taking them as prescribed, skipping doses, or saving them for later use.

Risk and time preferences

Risk preferences are typically elicited in surveys via a series of questions involving the choice between a lottery and a monetary outcome, e.g. “Which would you prefer to receive today: EUR 50, or a 50% chance of receiving nothing and a 50% chance of receiving EUR 100.” However, recent evidence suggests that risk (and time) preferences are domain-specific [20,21]. We therefore framed our elicitation method in a health context.

Alongside Health State A respondents were asked to consider Health State B: “You have a fever, chest pain, night sweats, a cough that brings up phlegm, loss of appetite, extreme tiredness, and some weight loss.” Health State B was intended to represent a substantially more serious condition than A, such as tuberculosis or lung cancer. Respondents were asked to assign each health state a value from 0 (worst imaginable health state) to 10. For the method to work, we needed respondents to recognise that Health State B was worse than A. However, it was important that it did not sound so severe that few respondents would ever take a gamble in which Health State B was a possible outcome. Following an internal pilot (n = 152 participants), the words ‘…and some weight loss’ were added to the original Health State B description to encourage more respondents to evaluate Health State B as more severe than A.

Attitudes towards risk were elicited via a set of standard gamble questions with two options. The options varied in terms of the fictitious health state lasting 2 weeks that would be assigned to the respondent. In each question, Option 1 was described as being like a lottery: a respondent might be in Full Health, but could instead be in Health State B. In Option 2, the respondent would be guaranteed Health State A. Throughout the questions, the probability of Health State B in Option 1 was varied, to elicit the probability required for a respondent to be indifferent between choosing Option 1’s gamble and Option 2’s guaranteed health state. Together with the subjective health state evaluations, this enabled construction of a risk aversion variable.

Time preferences were elicited via a similar series of questions, each containing two options. In Option 1, respondents would immediately face 2 weeks of Health State A, followed by Full Health for the next 18 years. In Option 2, respondents would instead face 2 weeks of Health State B, at some specified time in the next 18 years, and Full Health at all other times. Throughout the questions, the timing of Health State B in Option 2 was varied, to elicit how far into the future it should occur for a respondent to be indifferent between Health State A now, and Health State B then. This enabled construction of variables indicating the extent to which respondents discount the future, including those who were never happy to accept Health State B, no matter how far into the future, but preferred Health State A today. The latter individuals are referred to hereafter as ‘very low discounters’. This indicates an extreme time preference where the personal discount rate is close to zero.

Survey participants

The survey was conducted online using a panel of respondents provided by Survey Sampling International (SSI), a data collection and market research company. SSI was commissioned to obtain a sample of at least 2,000 completed responses, representative of adult members of the general public in terms of sex, age, ethnicity and geographic region. The sample size was powered to quantify the proportion of individuals who believe antibiotics are effective for ILI, at a 95% confidence level, with 2.5% precision and assuming 25% missing responses. Survey invitations were emailed to 6,280 SSI panel members resident in the UK.

A small incentive, worth approximately EUR 0.83, was offered for survey completion in the form of ‘Nectar points’ (a UK loyalty card scheme via which customers accrue discounts redeemable at various outlets). The invitations were sent over 3 weeks in May and June 2015. The first 152 responses were collected in an internal pilot, to confirm that there were no unexpected technical problems. As noted above, following the internal pilot, an amendment was made to the description of Health State B.

The SSI survey was undertaken outside the NHS setting and therefore did not need NHS ethical approval. Completion of the questionnaire was considered as indicating consent. Respondents were able to refuse to participate in the questionnaire at any stage in the process. All data were processed in accordance with the UK Data Protection Act 1998.

Statistical analysis

Four dependent variables were initially modelled using probit regressions, each with several sets of independent variables; ‘Believe antibiotics would help Health State A’, ‘Find AMR information surprising’, ‘Would ask doctor for antibiotics if I went’, and ‘Have taken antibiotics for a condition similar to Health State A in last 12 months’. The same models were then fitted to the dependent variables ‘definitely/probably visit a doctor more often’ and ‘definitely/probably ask a doctor for antibiotics more often’ in response to the provided AMR information. There was a ‘prefer not to answer’ option for questions on income, sex, ethnicity, religion and education, leading to slightly lower sample sizes for models including these independent variables. All models used complete cases (i.e. no missing variables) for specific groups of independent variables. Model 1 included sociodemographic data. Model 2 additionally adjusted for discounting and risk aversion. Model 3 also adjusted for personality. Model 4 also adjusted for awareness of AMR and that antibiotics are unlikely to benefit ILI. Coefficients for sociodemographic and respondent characteristics in Models 2, 3 and 4 therefore correspond to residual effects not mediated through their impact on discounting, risk aversion, personality or awareness of AMR and the ineffectiveness of antibiotics for ILI. The 152 pilot participants were excluded from all models including risk aversion and time preferences. The relative quality of Models 1–4 was assessed by both the Akaike information criterion (AIC) and Bayesian information criterion (BIC). These standard tests assign values to each model and deem the model with the minimum value to have the highest quality. Subgroups of respondents sharing similar characteristics were identified using hierarchical clustering of the sociodemographic, health and personality variables. Income is reported in this paper in EUR, using the conversion rate from GBP applicable on 31 May 2015.

Results

Respondent characteristics

2,064 (33%) of the 6,280 individuals contacted completed the survey. An additional 223 people began the questionnaire but did not complete it. A further 102 individuals clicked on the link in the invitation email but did not begin the questionnaire.

Table 1 provides some key characteristics of the respondents, including personality traits and risk and time preference indicators. The latter variables could only be estimated in 1,214 and 1,192 respondents, respectively, who evaluated Health State B as worse than A. Elicitation of time preference variables additionally required respondents to assign Health State A a score under 10 (Full Health). Those with and without risk/time indicators were broadly similar in terms of age, income, ethnicity, employment, country of origin and region of residence. However, fewer respondents with preference indicators were male (p = 0.0006), Christian (p = 0.007) or married/partnered (p = 0.002), and more had a higher education (p = 0.003). Those with preference indicators had lower ‘extraversion,’ (p = 0.001) and more ‘neuroticism’ (p = 0.03) and ‘openness’ (p = 0.04).

Table 1. Respondent characteristics, behavioural survey on antibiotic prescriptions, United Kingdom, 2015 (n = 2,064).

Variable Full sample Restricted sample with risk and time preferences p valuea
N with data Mean SD N with data Mean SD
Age 2,064 44 15.7 1,117 44.0 15.9 0.55
Household equivalent income (EUR) 1,886 31,066 25,487 1,027 31,672 25,373 0.26
Own self-rated health (0–10) 2,064 7.3 2.0 1,117 7.5 1.8 0.0001
N with data n % N with data n %
Male 2,061 994 48.2 1,117 500 44.8 0.0006
White 2,042 1,821 89.2 1,107 1,000 90.3 0.07
Christian 2,007 1,010 50.8 1,097 527 48.0 0.007
Higher-education 2,047 954 46.6 1,112 552 49.6 0.003
Unemployed 2,064 105 5.1 1,117 56 5.0 0.87
Sick/disabled 2,064 82 4.0 1,117 40 3.6 0.32
Married/civil partnership/live with partner 2,064 1,351 65.5 1,117 697 62.4 0.002
United Kingdom-born 2,064 1,856 89.9 1,117 1,006 90.1 0.82
Geographic region
East Anglia 2,064 171 8.3 1,117 107 9.6 0.02
East Midlands 2,064 129 6.3 1,117 77 6.9 0.19
West Midlands 2,064 181 8.8 1,117 95 8.5 0.65
London 2,064 297 14.4 1,117 160 14.3 0.93
North East 2,064 80 3.9 1,117 36 3.2 0.10
North West 2,064 239 11.6 1,117 132 11.8 0.71
South East 2,064 337 16.3 1,117 191 17.1 0.30
South West 2,064 180 8.7 1,117 92 8.2 0.40
Yorkshire and Humberside 2,064 166 8.0 1,117 76 6.8 0.02
Wales 2,064 91 4.4 1,117 49 4.4 0.96
Scotland 2,064 161 7.8 1,117 87 7.8 0.98
Northern Ireland 2,064 32 1.6 1,117 15 1.3 0.41
Personality and behaviourb
N with data Mean SD N with data Mean SD
Extraversion 2,064 5.9 1.9 1,117 5.8 2.0 0.001
Agreeableness 2,064 7.0 1.6 1,117 6.9 1.6 0.20
Conscientiousness 2,064 7.7 1.7 1,117 7.7 1.7 0.94
Neuroticism 2,064 5.8 2.1 1,117 5.9 2.2 0.03
Openness 2,064 6.8 1.6 1,117 6.9 1.7 0.04
Risk-aversec NR NR NR 1,117 2.389 2.386 NR
N with data n % N with data n %
Very low discounter NR NR NR 1,117 337 30.2 NR

NR: not reported; SD: standard deviation.

a p values from t-tests for continuous and chi-squared test for categorical factors compare those with and without risk and time preference indicators.

b Extraversion, agreeableness, conscientiousness, neuroticism, and openness were measured on a scale from 2 to 10.

c The risk-averse variable was measured on a scale from −9 to 9.

Beliefs about antibiotic use

A total of 988 (48%) respondents said they would ‘definitely/probably’ visit the GP if they experienced Health State A for 5 days. In this situation, 706 of 1,816 (39%) respondents would ask a GP for antibiotics. (The denominator here is less than 2,064 because 248 respondents said they would ‘definitely not’ visit the GP if they experienced Health State A for 5 days. In the survey (Supplement 1), these respondents were not asked the subsequent question “If you went to see a GP about this condition, do you think you would request antibiotics?”) Some 762 (37%) believed that antibiotics would ‘definitely/probably’ help and 426 (21%) respondents reported taking antibiotics for ILI in the last 12 months. Some 430 (53%) respondents believed that antibiotics would help their child if they had ILI, while 200 (25%) said that their child had taken antibiotics for ILI during the last 12 months. A total of 705 (34%) respondents were ‘very/somewhat’ surprised by the provided AMR information.

Models of ‘Believe antibiotics would help Health State A’, ‘Find AMR information surprising’, ‘Would ask doctor for antibiotics if I went’ and ‘Have taken antibiotics for a condition similar to Health State A in last 12 months’ indicated associations with several sociodemographic characteristics (Supplement 2, Tables 2.1–2.4). Being more extraverted was significantly positively associated with these outcomes, while being more conscientious, more open and a very low discounter were negatively associated. Adjusting for these traits, effect size and significance of many demographic characteristics declined or disappeared, and generally declined further after also adjusting for AMR awareness and for knowledge that antibiotics are unlikely to help ILI.

Reported response to AMR information

Of the 705 who found the AMR information ‘very/somewhat’ surprising, 283 (40%) said it would lead them to ‘definitely/probably’ visit a doctor more often, while 272 (39%) said it would lead them to ‘definitely/probably’ ask for antibiotics more often. These proportions were significantly higher than the respective percentages for ‘definitely/probably’ visiting a doctor, and asking for antibiotics less often (92 (13%) and 112 (16%), respectively; Wilcoxon single-sample sign-rank p < 0.0001) (Table 2). This distribution of responses differed significantly from those who did not find information on AMR surprising (both Wilcoxon rank-sum p < 0.0001). This adverse reaction to the information, among those surprised by it, was even stronger in the context of taking one’s child to the doctor and requesting antibiotics for them.

Table 2. How survey respondents said information about AMR would affect the number of times that they and their children visit a doctor and request antibiotics for conditions like Health State A, United Kingdom, 2015 (n = 2,064).

For oneself (adult) For one’s child
Find information on AMR surprising (n=705) Don’t find information on AMR surprising (n=1,359) Find information on AMR surprising (n=370) Don’t find information on AMR surprising (n=446)
n % n % n % n %
Visits to GP
Definitely visit less 29 4.1 131 9.6 10 2.7 16 3.6
Probably visit less 63 8.9 142 10.5 31 8.4 48 10.8
No change 309 43.8 999 73.5 158 42.7 340 76.2
Probably visit more 194 27.5 29 2.1 100 27.0 16 3.6
Definitely visit more 89 12.6 16 1.2 63 17.0 7 1.6
Don’t know 21 3.0 42 3.1 8 2.2 19 4.3
p (change within group)a < 0.0001 < 0.0001 < 0.0001 < 0.0001
p (difference between groups)b < 0.0001 < 0.0001
Ask for antibiotics
Definitely ask less 40 5.7 194 14.3 18 4.9 31 7.0
Probably ask less 72 10.2 182 13.4 48 13.0 63 14.1
No change 303 43.0 897 66.0 132 35.7 312 70.0
Probably ask more 179 25.4 23 1.7 105 28.4 13 2.9
Definitely ask more 93 13.2 8 0.6 54 14.6 6 1.4
Don’t know 18 2.6 55 4.1 13 3.5 21 4.7
p (change within group)a < 0.0001 < 0.0001 < 0.0001 < 0.0001
p (difference between groups)b < 0.0001 < 0.0001

AMR: antimicrobial resistance; GP: general practitioner; ILI: influenza-like-illness

a Wilcoxon single-sample signed-rank test that the median value within each group is no change.

b Wilcoxon rank-sum test comparing the distribution of responses in those who do and do not find the AMR information surprising.

p values calculated excluding ‘Don’t know’ category and coding a 5-point Likert scale for each variable.

Of the 1,359 who reported finding the AMR-information ‘not very/not at all’ surprising, 273 (20%) said it would lead them to ‘definitely/probably’ visit a doctor less often, while 376 (28%) said it would lead them to ‘definitely/probably’ ask for antibiotics less often, significantly higher than the respective percentages for ‘definitely/probably’ visiting a doctor, and asking for antibiotics, more often (Wilcoxon single-sample sign-rank p < 0.0001). The majority of those who found the AMR information ‘not very/not at all’ surprising said it would lead to ‘no change’ in how often they would visit a doctor (n = 999; 74%) or ask for antibiotics (n = 897; 66%).

Several factors were univariably associated with ‘definitely/probably’ asking for antibiotics more often (Table 3).

Table 3. Differences in characteristics of those who would and would not ‘definitely/probably’ ask a doctor for antibiotics more often after receiving information about AMR, United Kingdom, 2015 (n = 1,991).

Would ‘definitely/probably’ ask doctor for antibiotics more often ‘No change’ or would ‘definitely/probably’ ask doctor for antibiotics less often Univariablea Multivariableb
Sociodemographic data
N with data n % N with data n % p value p value
Male 302 174 57.6 1,687 781 46.3 0.0003 0.04
White 300 233 77.7 1,674 1532 91.5 < 0.0001 0.60
Christian 293 143 48.8 1,645 848 51.6 0.39 0.50
Higher education 301 156 51.8 1,676 769 45.9 0.06 0.74
Unemployed 303 13 4.3 1,688 84 5.0 0.61 0.74
Sick/disabled 303 11 3.6 1,688 69 4.1 0.71 0.11
Married/partnered 303 221 72.9 1,688 1089 64.5 0.004 0.03
United Kingdom-born 303 262 86.5 1,688 1,535 90.9 0.02 0.96
N with data Mean SD N with data Mean SD p value p value
Age (years) 303 35 11.6 1,688 46 15.8 < 0.0001 0.47
Household income 287 EUR 30,793 EUR 26,049 1,539 EUR 31,226 EUR 25,492 0.80 0.30
Own self-rated health (0–10) 303 7.0 2.0 1,688 7.4 2.0 0.0009 0.08
Personality
Extraversion 303 6.0 1.4 1,688 5.9 2.0 0.23 0.26
Agreeableness 303 6.9 1.5 1,688 7.0 1.6 0.53 0.82
Conscientiousness 303 7.1 1.7 1,688 7.8 1.7 < 0.0001 0.27
Neuroticism 303 5.8 1.7 1,688 5.8 2.2 0.92 0.85
Openness 303 6.4 1.4 1,688 6.9 1.7 < 0.0001 0.002
Risk and time preferences
Risk aversion 130 1.6 2.5 1,027 2.5 2.4 0.0003 0.11
N with data n % N with data n % p value p value
Very low discounter 139 7 5.0 1,038 342 32.9 < 0.0001 < 0.0001
Attitudes to antibiotics and ILI
Surprised by AMR information 303 272 89.8 1,688 415 24.6 < 0.0001 < 0.0001
Believe antibiotics would help Health State A 303 232 76.6 1,688 514 30.5 < 0.0001 < 0.0001
Taken antibiotics for Health State A in last 12 months 303 169 55.8 1,688 245 14.5 < 0.0001 ND
N with data Mean SD N with data Mean SD p value p value
Health State A rating 303 6.3 2.3 1,688 5.5 2.1 < 0.0001 ND

AMR: antimicrobial resistance; ILI: influenza-like illness; ND: not done; SD: standard deviation.

a t-test for continuous and chi-squared test for categorical factors.

b From a multivariable probit model including all factors. Model coefficients and further details are available in Supplement 2, Table 2.5, final column.

In all specifications of the multivariable models (Figure), those who would ‘definitely/probably’ visit and ‘definitely/probably’ ask a doctor for antibiotics more often were significantly more likely to be male and married/partnered. They were independently significantly more likely to believe antibiotics are likely to help ILI, and more likely to be surprised by our AMR information, but less likely to be a very low discounter, and less open. Of the models including risk and time preferences, Model 4, which also adjusted for personality, AMR awareness and knowledge that antibiotics are unlikely to help ILI, was preferred to Models 2 and 3 according to both the AIC and BIC model selection tests.

Figure.

Probit regression models for the survey responses ‘visiting doctor more’ and ‘asking for more antibiotics’ in response to AMR information, United Kingdom, 2015 (n = 1,769)

AMR: antimicrobial resistance; SE: standard error; UK: United Kingdom.

Solid symbols: p < 0.05; empty symbols: p > 0.05. See Supplement 2, Table 2.5 for full model details.

Figure

Cluster analysis

Cluster Group 1 contained most (176/276; 64%) respondents who would ‘definitely/probably’ ask for antibiotics more often, while Group 5 contained most of the remainder (66/276; 24%) (Table 4). Group 5 contained the highest percentage of people who would ‘definitely/probably’ ask for antibiotics more often (66/264; 25%), and Group 1 the second highest (176/1,055; 17%). Group 1 and Group 5 had more income and were younger than the survey sample averages. Group 1 was 99% (1,046/1,055) white, almost 100% (1,050/1,055) UK-born and 48% (511/1,055) male, while 48% (511/1,055) had a higher-education. Group 5 was 33% white (88/264), 38% (101/264) UK-born and 35% (92/264) male and 72% (191/264) with a higher education.

Table 4. Percentage of different profiled groups ‘definitely/probably’ asking a doctor for antibiotics more often after receiving information about AMR, United Kingdom, 2015 (n = 1,816).

Response to AMR information Cluster groups Total
Group 1 Group 2 Group 3 Group 4 Group 5
Total in cluster group 1,055 77 73 347 264 1,816
‘Definitely/probably ask less often’ or ‘no change’ 853 63 64 320 187 1,487
‘Definitely/probably ask more often’ 176 8 9 17 66 276
‘Don’t know’ 26 6 0 10 11 53
Percentage who would ‘definitely/probably ask more often’ 16.7% 10.4% 12.3% 4.9% 25.0% 15.2%
Mean age (years) 39.6 37.1 47.8 64.0 36.6 44.0
Percentage male 48.4% 55.8% 43.8% 60.8% 34.9% 48.2%
Percentage white 99.1% 96.1% 97.3% 99.4% 33.3% 89.2%
Percentage United Kingdom-born 99.5% 96.1% 100.0% 100.0% 38.3% 89.9%
Percentage with higher education 48.4% 29.9% 21.9% 32.3% 72.4% 46.6%
Percentage sick/disabled 0% 0% 100% 0% 0% 4.0%
Mean household income (EUR) 33,947 12,872 15,891 29,478 31,804 31,066

AMR: antimicrobial resistance.

The Table shows the subset of characteristics which varied most across the different groups (key differences in italics). All characteristics are listed in Supplement 3, Table 3.1

Discussion

In this study, lack of AMR-awareness and the belief that antibiotics would help ILI were strongly associated with self-reported likelihood of consulting and requesting antibiotics for ILI, and with antibiotic consumption. This lack of knowledge was a mediating variable in the association between consulting/requesting antibiotics for ILI and sociodemographic characteristics. This may indicate a role for public information campaigns, perhaps targeted at specific population groups. It is encouraging that respondents who were unsurprised by the AMR information provided said it would make them less likely to consult a GP or request antibiotics. It is therefore possible that over a longer timeframe, repeated exposure to AMR information could reduce antibiotic requests. However, many with poor AMR awareness said they would react to such information by consulting doctors and asking for antibiotics more, not less often. This identifies a potential paradox where the overall impact of a public campaign could be increased demand for antibiotics. Such phenomena have been found in public health campaigns outside the field of AMR [22,23]. A meta-analysis of fear-appeal health campaigns found that fear-appeal messages, while often effective, are likely to backfire if people do not believe they are able to adequately protect themselves from the threat, a possibility which seems plausible here [24].

Being a very low discounter was strongly associated with awareness of AMR and the likely minimal benefit from antibiotics for ILI, and with not consulting a doctor or requesting antibiotics for ILI. Like personality, it was a mediating variable in the association between consulting/requesting antibiotics for ILI and sociodemographic characteristics. Attaching substantial importance to health in the very distant future may encourage both the acquisition of health literacy, and avoiding choices that could damage long-term health prospects for minor short-term benefits. We found no evidence of a significant association of risk-aversion with attitudes towards antibiotic use or resistance. It cannot safely be concluded, however, that risk preferences do not play a role in the demand for antibiotics. The lack of significance could stem from risk aversion leading some people to take antibiotics, e.g. to avoid uncertainty over complications, but others to avoid antibiotics, for fear of possible AMR-related problems.

Strengths and limitations

An important limitation is that respondents’ reported future behaviour in response to the AMR information may differ from what they would actually do in real life. The reported paradoxical behaviour, where some respondents said they would ask for antibiotics more, not less often in response to the AMR information, may not reflect their actual behaviour.

Another limitation is that only members of the online survey panel participated. Thus the sample was limited to those with internet-access, basic computer literacy and an interest in completing surveys. However, use of this panel also meant that age, sex, ethnicity, geographic region and proportion unemployed were broadly representative of the general population in the UK, although the percentage with higher education (47%) was higher than the population average (27%) and mean income was lower.

A major strength was our inclusion of variables not generally used in epidemiological studies, relating to personality and, particularly, risk and time preferences. Knowledge of how these characteristics are associated with AMR awareness and appropriate antibiotic use could help inform public campaigns. For example, advertising budgets could be used more efficiently by concentrating advertisements in specific media (television channels/programmes, websites and social media) that are typically consumed by audiences with these characteristics. Unfortunately, the risk and time variables could only be estimated in, respectively, 58% and 59% of the sample.

Comparison with existing literature

There have been several surveys of public perceptions of AMR and appropriate use of antibiotics in primary care and a systematic review of the public’s knowledge and beliefs about AMR [11,25]. However, none of these studies have explicitly evaluated responses to information about AMR or considered a role for risk or time preferences. Our descriptive results broadly concur with previous studies in confirming wide-ranging misunderstanding regarding antibiotic efficacy for ILI and lack of knowledge about AMR.

Several public campaigns in high-income countries have successfully reduced antibiotic use in primary care [10,26]. In France, following a multi-faceted ‘antibiotics are not automatic’ campaign, there was a substantial decrease in antibiotic prescriptions between 2002 and 2007 compared with the pre-intervention period 2000 to 2002 [27]. A review by Huttner et al. concluded that because nearly all successful campaigns were multi-faceted, it was difficult to unpick whether improvements were due to changes in the behaviour of patients, physicians or both, or whether the observed improvements were related to confounding factors which would have happened in the absence of the campaigns [10]. All campaigns reviewed tried to convey the seriousness of AMR, sometimes using a fear message; most campaigns also tried to educate the public that antibiotics are ineffective for respiratory infections. Some campaigns encouraged people to complete antibiotic courses, an area of controversy [28]. A 1999 campaign in the UK to educate the public about appropriate antibiotic use and to raise AMR awareness was associated with improved knowledge about antibiotics but there was no evidence of reduced antibiotic use [29]. Campaigns in Greece and Spain also failed to show such an effect [10].

Conclusion

Although based on hypothetical reported future behaviour, the results of this study suggest that public information campaigns to reduce unnecessary antibiotic use may risk a paradoxical consequence of increased, rather than decreased, public demand for antibiotics. This does not negate the potentially important role such campaigns may have; several public information campaigns have proven effective as part of multi-faceted interventions to reduce unnecessary prescribing. However, it underscores the importance of testing public antibiotic stewardship information campaigns on a small scale before rolling them out widely. Choosing the right words may be critical to success, and different strategies may be needed for different population subgroups [30].

Most effective campaigns have tried to convey that AMR is a serious problem, while also explaining that antibiotics are ineffective for respiratory infections [10]. The information tested in this study contained only the former element. Information containing both elements might have performed better. One of the strongest predictors of self-reported inappropriate antibiotic use, and of negative response to the AMR information, was belief in the efficacy of antibiotics for ILI. Information explicitly challenging this belief, perhaps coupled with reassurance that cold and influenza symptoms are easily treated with rest, fluids and paracetamol, could potentially form the basis for a successful public campaign. More research is urgently needed on finding an effective way to communicate this important message.

Acknowledgements

Firstly we wish to thank all the people who completed our survey. We also thank Joanna Coast, Richard Smith, Helen Barker, Jane Simms, Ali Vaughan, Charles Crichton, Timothy Walker, Amy Mason, Claire Scarborough, Nicola Fawcett, Teresa Street, Oluwafemi Akinremi, Apostolos Tsiachristas, James Buchanan, Jilles Fermont, Rachael Morton, Elizabeth Stokes, Filipa Landeiro, Jacqueline Murphy, Barbara Kitchener, Yaling Yang, Richéal Burns, Peter Eibich, Ines Rombach, Anees Pari, Thomas Rouyard, Oliver Rivero-Arias, Christopher Rooney, Amar Marthi, Sam Williams, and various members of our Public and Patient Involvement (PPI) group – for a wide variety of useful comments and suggestions on preliminary versions of our survey instrument.

This work was supported by the National Institute for Health Research Health Protection Research Unit (NIHR HPRU) in Healthcare Associated Infections and Antimicrobial Resistance at the University of Oxford and in partnership with Public Health England (PHE) [grant number HPRU-2012-10041]. Michele Peters is a Senior Research Scientist supported by the Department of Health funded Policy Research Unit on Quality and Outcomes of Person Centred Care (QORU), a collaboration between the London School of Economics and Political Science (LSE) and the Universities of Kent and Oxford. Tim Peto and Derrick Crook are National Institute for Health Research (NIHR) Senior Investigators. The funders had no involvement in the study design, collection, analysis or interpretation of data, or in writing the article and deciding to submit it for publication.

Conflict of interest: None declared.

Authors’ contributions: LR led the design of the questionnaire, with significant contributions from ST-C, TP, MP, ASW, and SW. LR led the data analysis, with contributions from ST-C, MP, CCB, ASW, and SW. All authors helped to interpret the results of the study. LR led the drafting of the paper, with contributions from all authors. All authors have seen and approved the final article.

References

  • 1.The Review on Antimicrobial Resistance. Antimicrobial resistance: tackling a crisis for the health and wealth of nations. London: UK Government; 2014. Available from: https://amr-review.org/sites/default/files/AMR%20Review%20Paper%20-%20Tackling%20a%20crisis%20for%20the%20health%20and%20wealth%20of%20nations_1.pdf
  • 2.Leeb M. Antibiotics: a shot in the arm. Nature. 2004;431(7011):892-3. 10.1038/431892a [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Goossens H, Ferech M, Vander Stichele R, Elseviers M, ESAC Project Group Outpatient antibiotic use in Europe and association with resistance: a cross-national database study. Lancet. 2005;365(9459):579-87. 10.1016/S0140-6736(05)70799-6 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Bartlett JG. A call to arms: the imperative for antimicrobial stewardship. Clin Infect Dis. 2011;53(Suppl 1):S4-7. 10.1093/cid/cir362 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Laxminarayan R. Antibiotic effectiveness: balancing conservation against innovation. Science. 2014;345(6202):1299-301. 10.1126/science.1254163 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Low D. Reducing antibiotic use in influenza: challenges and rewards. Clin Microbiol Infect. 2008;14(4):298-306. 10.1111/j.1469-0691.2007.01910.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Petersen I, Johnson AM, Islam A, Duckworth G, Livermore DM, Hayward AC. Protective effect of antibiotics against serious complications of common respiratory tract infections: retrospective cohort study with the UK General Practice Research Database. BMJ. 2007;335(7627):982. 10.1136/bmj.39345.405243.BE [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Cockburn J, Pit S. Prescribing behaviour in clinical practice: patients’ expectations and doctors’ perceptions of patients’ expectations--a questionnaire study. BMJ. 1997;315(7107):520-3. 10.1136/bmj.315.7107.520 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Coenen S, Michiels B, Renard D, Denekens J, Van Royen P. Antibiotic prescribing for acute cough: the effect of perceived patient demand. Br J Gen Pract. 2006;56(524):183-90. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Huttner B, Goossens H, Verheij T, Harbarth S, CHAMP consortium Characteristics and outcomes of public campaigns aimed at improving the use of antibiotics in outpatients in high-income countries. Lancet Infect Dis. 2010;10(1):17-31. 10.1016/S1473-3099(09)70305-6 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.McNulty CAM, Nichols T, French DP, Joshi P, Butler CC. Expectations for consultations and antibiotics for respiratory tract infection in primary care: the RTI clinical iceberg. Br J Gen Pract. 2013;63(612):e429-36. 10.3399/bjgp13X669149 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Kang MI, Ikeda S. Time discounting and smoking behavior: evidence from a panel survey(*). Health Econ. 2014;23(12):1443-64. 10.1002/hec.2998 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Picone G, Sloan F, Taylor D., Jr Effects of risk and time preference and expected longevity on demand for medical tests. J Risk Uncertain. 2004;28(1):39-53. 10.1023/B:RISK.0000009435.11390.23 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Anderson LR, Mellor JM. Predicting health behaviors with an experimental measure of risk preference. J Health Econ. 2008;27(5):1260-74. 10.1016/j.jhealeco.2008.05.011 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Rammstedt B, John OP. Measuring personality in one minute or less: A 10-item short version of the Big Five Inventory in English and German. J Res Pers. 2007;41(1):203-12. 10.1016/j.jrp.2006.02.001 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.What is Antibiotic Resistance and Why is it a problem? Boston: Alliance for the Prudent Use of Antibiotics (APUA). [Accessed Nov 2015]. Available from: http://www.tufts.edu/med/apua/about_issue/antibiotic_res.shtml
  • 17.World Health Organization (WHO). Antimicrobial resistance. Geneva; WHO; 2018. Available from: http://www.who.int/mediacentre/factsheets/fs194/en/
  • 18.Become an antibiotic guardian. London: Public Health England; [Accessed Nov 2015]. Available from: www.antibioticguardian.com
  • 19.The Antibiotic Awareness Campaign. London: NHS; 2015, http://www.nhs.uk/NHSEngland/ARC/Pages/AboutARC.aspx
  • 20.Dohmen T, Falk A, Huffman D, Sunde U, Schupp J, Wagner GG. Individual risk attitudes: Measurement, determinants, and behavioral consequences. J Eur Econ Assoc. 2011;9(3):522-50. 10.1111/j.1542-4774.2011.01015.x [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Jimura K, Myerson J, Hilgard J, Keighley J, Braver TS, Green L. Domain independence and stability in young and older adults’ discounting of delayed rewards. Behav Processes. 2011;87(3):253-9. 10.1016/j.beproc.2011.04.006 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Nyhan B, Reifler J, Richey S, Freed GL. Effective messages in vaccine promotion: a randomized trial. Pediatrics. 2014;133(4):e835-42. 10.1542/peds.2013-2365 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Cho H, Salmon CT. Unintended effects of health communication campaigns. J Commun. 2007;57(2):293-317. 10.1111/j.1460-2466.2007.00344.x [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Peters GJ, Ruiter RA, Kok G. Threatening communication: a critical re-analysis and a revised meta-analytic test of fear appeal theory. Health Psychol Rev. 2013;7(sup1) Suppl 1;S8-31. 10.1080/17437199.2012.703527 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.McCullough AR, Parekh S, Rathbone J, Del Mar CB, Hoffmann TC. A systematic review of the public’s knowledge and beliefs about antibiotic resistance. J Antimicrob Chemother. 2016;71(1):27-33. 10.1093/jac/dkv310 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Goossens H, Guillemot D, Ferech M, Schlemmer B, Costers M, van Breda M, et al. National campaigns to improve antibiotic use. Eur J Clin Pharmacol. 2006;62(5):373-9. 10.1007/s00228-005-0094-7 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Sabuncu E, David J, Bernède-Bauduin C, Pépin S, Leroy M, Boëlle PY, et al. Significant reduction of antibiotic use in the community after a nationwide campaign in France, 2002-2007. PLoS Med. 2009;6(6):e1000084. 10.1371/journal.pmed.1000084 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Llewelyn MJ, Fitzpatrick JM, Darwin E, SarahTonkin-Crine, Gorton C, Paul J, et al. The antibiotic course has had its day. BMJ. 2017;358:j3418. 10.1136/bmj.j3418 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.McNulty CA, Boyle P, Nichols T, Clappison P, Davey P. Don’t wear me out--the public’s knowledge of and attitudes to antibiotic use. J Antimicrob Chemother. 2007;59(4):727-38. 10.1093/jac/dkl558 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Smith RA, MacGeorge EL, Hackman NM, M’ikanatha NM. Campaign preparation for complex initiatives: a person-centred approach to audience segmentation of parents’ antibiotic stewardship. Health Commun. 2017;1-10.; Epub ahead of print 10.1080/10410236.2017.1384345 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Articles from Eurosurveillance are provided here courtesy of European Centre for Disease Prevention and Control

RESOURCES