Skip to main content
Springer logoLink to Springer
. 2017 Dec 26;33(10):989–1001. doi: 10.1007/s10654-017-0347-7

Effect of retirement on cognitive function: the Whitehall II cohort study

Baowen Xue 1,, Dorina Cadar 3, Maria Fleischmann 1, Stephen Stansfeld 4, Ewan Carr 1,2, Mika Kivimäki 1, Anne McMunn 1, Jenny Head 1
PMCID: PMC6153553  PMID: 29280030

Abstract

According to the ‘use it or lose it’ hypothesis, a lack of mentally challenging activities might exacerbate the loss of cognitive function. On this basis, retirement has been suggested to increase the risk of cognitive decline, but evidence from studies with long follow-up is lacking. We tested this hypothesis in a cohort of 3433 civil servants who participated in the Whitehall II Study, including repeated measurements of cognitive functioning up to 14 years before and 14 years after retirement. Piecewise models, centred at the year of retirement, were used to compare trajectories of verbal memory, abstract reasoning, phonemic verbal fluency, and semantic verbal fluency before and after retirement. We found that all domains of cognition declined over time. Declines in verbal memory were 38% faster after retirement compared to before, after taking account of age-related decline. In analyses stratified by employment grade, higher employment grade was protective against verbal memory decline while people were still working, but this ‘protective effect’ was lost when individuals retired, resulting in a similar rate of decline post-retirement across employment grades. We did not find a significant impact of retirement on the other cognitive domains. In conclusion, these findings are consistent with the hypothesis that retirement accelerates the decline in verbal memory function. This study points to the benefits of cognitively stimulating activities associated with employment that could benefit older people’s memory.

Electronic supplementary material

The online version of this article (10.1007/s10654-017-0347-7) contains supplementary material, which is available to authorized users.

Keywords: Cognition, Retirement, Longitudinal study, Piecewise regression, Employment grade

Introduction

Good cognitive functioning represents an essential element of healthy ageing and independent living [1]. There is some evidence that ageing affects cognitive functions that are primarily associated with executive processing and other functions of the frontal lobe [2, 3]. Thus, fluid abilities, such as memory, processing speed, and spatial ability tend to decline faster with age than crystallised functions, including vocabulary, information and comprehension [46]. However, the decline in these abilities is not necessarily homogenous across the population, as some people maintain cognitive vitality even into extreme old age [79]. On the one hand, there is evidence that the adult brain shows neuroplasticity and neurogenesis, representing the brain’s ability to generate new neurons and rewire itself [1012]. On the other hand, accelerated deterioration or impairment in one or more cognitive functions beyond the ‘normal’ age-related decline could be predictive of the onset of dementia, a major cause of disability and dependency among older people worldwide [13, 14]. Therefore, it is important to identify and understand the predictors of interindividual differences in cognitive decline.

The theory of cognitive reserve proposes that some individuals have a larger cognitive reserve than others. It has been postulated that innate cognitive resources (such as childhood IQ), cognitive stimulation during brain maturation in childhood (such as education), and cognitively engaged lifestyle during adulthood (such as cognitively demanding occupation) can increase cognitive reserve, thus building up a buffer against cognitive decline in old age [15, 16]. The ‘use it or lose it’ hypothesis similarly suggests that a person can maintain cognitive function by engaging in cognitively demanding activities, whereas failing to keep mentally active will detrimentally affect cognitive function and could accelerate cognitive decline or even the onset of dementia [17]. Accordingly, retirement may be a potential trigger for cognitive decline, assuming that retirees leave paid work that is cognitively demanding. Many studies have supported this assumption showing that retirement is associated with lower cognitive functioning [1823], and later retirement is associated with better cognition and lower risk of dementia [2427], although some studies have found no association [28, 29] or even a positive effect [30] of retirement on levels of cognition.

When studying the effects of retirement on cognition, it is important to consider reverse causality. Declines in cognitive function may negatively affect the management of work tasks and thus could be a determinant of the decision to retire [31, 32]. For example, chronic diseases, such as stroke, might affect both cognitive function and retirement decisions [33, 34]. The vast majority of studies have compared retirees with working people to assess the potential effect of retirement on cognition, and it is possible that their results are biased due to a ‘healthy worker’ effect (i.e. people who remain in work are likely to be healthier than those who have stopped working). Some studies have relied on the use of instrumental variables, such as state pension age or early retirement windows, to eliminate bias due to unobserved heterogeneity and endogeneity [1821, 28, 29]. The validity of the instrumental variables method relies on choosing an instrument that is not correlated with other factors that influence health and retirement. For example, in cross-national studies that have used state pension age as the instrument, it is possible that differences in state pension age may have been correlated with other national differences that affect health. A further limitation of these studies is their short follow-ups and use of only one measure of cognitive function. A few studies have examined change in cognitive function before and after retirement [22, 35], but most have relatively short follow-up period [36], and thus cannot estimate the long-term effects of retirement on cognitive function.

In this report from the Whitehall II cohort study, we compared trajectories of cognitive function before and after retirement within same persons up to 14 years before (mean 7.1 years) and 14 years after (mean 7.0 years) retirement, which included up to four repeated measures of cognitive function per person. This long period of follow-up helps reduce the possibility of reverse causation from health-related selection out of employment. To examine whether the ‘use it or lose it’ hypothesis applies to specific cognitive domains, we included several measures of verbal memory, abstract reasoning, phonemic verbal fluency, and semantic verbal fluency as cognitive outcomes. We further hypothesised that the influence of retirement on cognition may vary by socioeconomic status such that cognitive decline is more marked in individuals retiring from cognitively demanding higher employment grade jobs than in those who retire from less cognitively demanding low employment grade jobs [37]. Sex differences were also tested, because men and women may show advantages in different cognitive domains [38], and previous research has highlighted the gendered nature of employment trajectories and retirement [39], although men’s and women’s employment trajectories are becoming increasingly similar [40].

Methods

Study population and study design

This study used data from the Whitehall II prospective occupational cohort study. All civil servants aged 35–55 working in the London offices of 20 Whitehall departments in 1985–1988 were invited to participate. The response rate was 73% and a sample of 6895 men and 3413 women was recruited (phase 1). These civil servants were employed in a wide variety of roles from clerical grades, through to senior administrative grades, reflecting different employment grades and salaries. Follow-up surveys were conducted every 2–3 years. All participants provided written consent and the University College London ethics committee approved this study.

The data for the present analyses were drawn from phases 5 (1997–1999), 7 (2002–2004), 9 (2007–2009), and 11 (2012–2013) of the Whitehall II Study when cognitive tests were administered during the clinical examinations. Phase 3 (1991–1994) was not used because cognitive testing was introduced midway through phase 3 and consequently only half of respondents completed the cognitive test at that phase. For the current study, participants were eligible for inclusion if they had data on cognitive function at least once before and once after retirement. We excluded participants who were already not working at phase 5 and those who did not retire during follow-up or returned to work after retirement. There were 3691 eligible participants who moved from work to retirement, but 258 of these were excluded due to missing cognition outcome (i.e. without cognition measures at least once prior and at least once after retirement.) The final sample comprised 3433 participants (11,858 observations). The process of sample selection is shown in Fig. 1. Participants’ average age when taking the cognitive tests was 54.0 years (range 45–68) at phase 5, 59.5 years (range 51–74) at phase 7, 64.3 years (range 56–79) at phase 9, and 68.2 years (range 60–83) at phase 11.

Fig. 1.

Fig. 1

Flowchart of sample selection process

Measures

Cognitive function

The cognitive test battery, including verbal memory, abstract reasoning, phonemic verbal fluency, and semantic verbal fluency, was introduced to the Whitehall II cohort study in phase 5 and was repeated using the same tests at all subsequent assessments (phases 7, 9, and 11). The tests have good test–retest reliability (range 0.6–0.9), assessed in 556 participants who were invited back to the clinic within 3 months of having taken the test in phase 5 [41]. Verbal memory was assessed with a 20-word free recall test. Participants were presented with a list of 20 one- or two- syllable words at two-second intervals and then had 2 min to recall in writing as many words as possible (maximum possible score = 20) [42]. Abstract reasoning was assessed by the Alice Heim 4 Part 1 test (AH4). This test measures the ability to identify patterns and to infer principles and rules, which is composed of a series of 65 questions (32 verbal and 33 mathematical) of increasing difficulty (maximum possible score = 65). Participants had 10 min to complete this section [43]. Phonemic verbal fluency was assessed by asking participants to write as many words beginning with the letter ‘S’ as they could (maximum score = 35), and semantic verbal fluency was assessed by recalling as many animal names as possible (maximum score = 35). One minute was allowed for each verbal fluency test [44].

Retirement and year of retirement

Respondents’ employment status was measured by self-reports at each phase. Participants were considered to be in employment if they were still working in the civil service or were in paid employment elsewhere (full or part time). Participants were classified as retired if they moved from work to retirement directly or moved from work to unemployed/other, and then to retirement.

All respondents who retired from the civil service provided their exact year of exit from the civil service, but those who retired from employment outside the civil service were not asked the exact year of exit. For these 1632 individuals (46% of selected sample) whose exact exit year was unknown, we used the mid-point between the last phase still in paid work and the subsequent phase no longer working. We used the year of retirement as the centre point to calculate the cognitive trajectories before and after retirement.

Health-related retirement

At each phase, participants who were not working could indicate whether this was because of long-term sickness. Participants who retired from the civil service answered whether this was on health grounds. We considered participants who were ‘long-term sick’ or who indicated that the route of leaving the civil service was ‘retirement on health grounds’ as health-related retirement.

Covariates

We included retirement age as a covariate. Because all the analyses in this paper were centred at the year of retirement (see statistical method section), including retirement age as a covariate can effectively adjust for age effects. We adjusted for birth year to take account of the possibility of period effects. Gender and self-reported highest educational qualification were also included as covariates. Educational qualification was grouped into: O-level or lower (‘low’), A-level or equivalent (‘middle’), and degree level or higher (‘high’). To account for practice effects (i.e. gains in scores on cognitive tests when a person was retested on the same or similar instruments), we controlled for the number of cognitive tests a participant had completed in previous phases. Although cognitive test scores in phase 3 were not used in the analysis, the practice effect at this phase was counted.

Time-fixed covariates based on the last interview before retirement were employment grade, still working in the civil service, psychosocial job demands, job decision latitude, and spouse’s or partner’s employment status. Employment grade was measured, in order of increasing salary, as clerical/support (‘low’), professional/executive (‘middle’), or administrative (‘high’) [37]. For those who had left the civil service, the last employment grade before leaving was used. Job demands were measured by four items such as ‘Do you have to work very fast?’ Decision latitude was measured by nine items such as ‘Do you have a choice in deciding how to do your work?’ [45]. Respondents rated each question item whether it was ‘often’, ‘sometimes’, ‘seldom’ or ‘never/almost never’ the case. Each answer was scored from 0 to 3 and was added up so that a higher score reflected greater job demands or higher job decision latitude. Continuous scores were divided into tertiles [46]. Spouse’s employment status was measured by asking whether a spouse is currently doing any paid work. Those reporting not being married/cohabiting were coded as ‘no spouse’.

Time-varying covariates (phases 5, 7, 9, and 11) included smoking status, alcohol consumption, depressive symptoms, systolic blood pressure (SBP), diastolic blood pressure (DBP), body mass index (BMI), total blood cholesterol, coronary heart disease (CHD), stroke, all malignant cancers, and diabetes/intermediate hyperglycaemia. By treating these variables as time-varying, we account for reported changes in health conditions and health behaviours over time. Smoking status (current, never, ex-smoker) and alcohol consumption in the past week (0, 1–10, more than 10 units) were based on self-reports. Symptoms of depression were measured by the depression subscale of the General Health Questionnaire (GHQ), and cut-off points of four out of 12 were used to identify depression cases [47]. Blood pressure (mm Hg), BMI (kg/m2), and total blood cholesterol (mmol/l) were objectively measured during the clinical examinations and were included as continuous covariates in the model. CHD (yes/no) includes diagnosed non-fatal myocardial infarction (MI) and ‘definite’ angina. Non-fatal MI was defined following MONICA criteria [48] based on study electrocardiograms, hospital acute ECGs, and cardiac enzymes and validated using discharge diagnoses from National Health Service (NHS) Hospital Episode Statistics (HES) data or General Practitioner (GP) confirmation up to the end of phase 11. Self-reports of non-fatal MI were not used [49]. ‘Definite’ angina included self-reported cases of angina only if they were subsequently validated by these other sources. Self-reported stroke events (yes/no) were collected throughout follow-up, and were validated by HES data linkage, GP’s confirmation, or retrieval of hospital medical records up to phase 9 [49, 50]. Cancer incidence data (yes/no) for the period 1971–2015 were obtained from the NHS Central Register for nearly all participants. Diabetes/intermediate hyperglycaemia (yes/no) was defined by the WHO criteria of oral glucose tolerance test and by a self-reported diagnosis of diabetes [51].

Statistical methods

To test a change of the response function (Y) of a varying independent variable (X), we used piecewise linear regression with two segments separated by a ‘knot’ [52, 53]. We used year of retirement as the knot (i.e. year 0), and thus, generated two independent variables reflecting ‘years before retirement’ (− 14 to − 1) and ‘years after retirement’ (1–14). Retired less than a year was counted as 1 year. Linear mixed models were fitted for each cognition outcome, in turn, and these two variables were entered into the model. The coefficients for the variable ‘years before retirement’ (i.e. slope before) represented the average change in cognition per year before retirement. Coefficients for the variable ‘years after retirement’ (i.e. slope after) represented the average change in cognition for each additional year after retirement. If retirement did not affect cognition, we would expect the trajectories of cognitive function to be similar before and after retirement. Therefore, to test whether retirement influenced cognitive decline, independent of age-related change, we examined differences in the slope for cognition before and after retirement. The ‘slope change’ was defined as the ‘slope after retirement’ minus the ‘slope before retirement’ (this was also expressed as percentage change, calculated as ‘slope change’ divided by slope before retirement multiplied by 100). All analyses were carried out in Stata 14. We also examined whether a nonlinear piecewise model was better than the linear model by adding quadratic terms of ‘years before retirement’ and ‘years after retirement’ into each model. To take account of the clustering of the data, mixed models with repeated measures and individuals as the two random-effects levels were conducted. The models allowed for both random intercepts (for each individual) and random coefficients (for the terms ‘years before’ and ‘years after’ retirement).

To assess whether the effect of retirement differed by cognition domains, we conducted a test of heterogeneity on the effect of retirement using multivariate multilevel models with all cognition outcomes included in one model.

To visualise the results from these regressions, we show predicted trajectories of each cognitive function outcome, both before and after retirement. These predicted trajectories from adjusted models were calculated at the sample mean of each covariate. In addition to piecewise linear trajectories (where ‘years before retirement’ and ‘years after retirement’ were treated as continuous), predicted adjusted means at each time point (where ‘years before retirement’ and ‘years after retirement’ were treated as categorical) are shown as dots in the figures.

We tested for potential moderators, including employment grade (based on last response before retirement) and sex in the association between retirement and cognition outcomes, by adding interaction terms (‘years before retirement × employment grade’ and ‘years after retirement × employment grade’; ‘years before retirement × sex’ and ‘years after retirement × sex’) in the model for each cognition outcome.

Missing data

For time-fixed covariates (employment grade, still working in the civil service, job demands, job decision latitude, and partner’s employment status), missing data in the last interview before retirement was first replaced by prior responses. The remaining missing data of time-fixed covariates and missing data of other covariates for the eligible participants were imputed in Stata, using multivariate imputation by chained equations, and 30 datasets were imputed. We included all variables from the analyses (i.e. independent variables, outcome variables, covariates, and moderators) in the imputation model. After running the imputation, we deleted imputed outcome values in the regression. Percentage of missing data was shown in Table 1.

Table 1.

Descriptive statistics for the study sample (n = 3433)a

Observed N (% missing) Observed % Imputed % Observed mean (SD) Imputed mean (SD)
Retirement age 3433 (0%) 61.2 (4.6) 61.2 (4.6)
Verbal memory 3433 (0%) 6.9 (2.3) 6.9 (2.3)
Abstract reasoning 3433 (0%) 45.4 (11.0) 45.4 (11.0)
Phonemic verbal fluency 3433 (0%) 16.0 (3.9) 16.0 (3.9)
Semantic verbal fluency 3433 (0%) 16.3 (4.1) 16.3 (4.1)
Birth year 3433 (0%) 1943.7 (5.0) 1943.7 (5.0)
Gender 3433 (0%)
 Men 72.2 72.2
Highest education qualification 3323 (3.2%)
 O level or lower 41.7 42.0
 A level or equivalent 24.8 24.7
 Degree level or higher 33.5 33.3
Employment grade 3433 (0%)
 Clerical/support (lowest) 12.1 12.1
 Professional/executive 42.2 42.2
 Administrative (highest) 45.7 45.7
Still in civil service 3400 (1.0%) 62.0 62.1
Job demand 3433 (0%)
 Low 24.4 24.4
 Middle 46.2 46.2
 High 29.4 29.4
Job decision latitude 3433 (0%)
 Low 24.8 24.8
 Middle 31.7 31.7
 High 43.5 43.5
Spouse’s employment status 3341(2.7%)
 Working spouse 50.3 50.1
 Non-working spouse 28.3 28.3
 No spouse 21.4 21.6
Smoking status 3312 (3.5%)
 Never-smoker 49.0 48.9
 Ex-smoker 41.6 41.8
 Current smoker 9.4 9.3
Alcohol consumption 3293 (4.1%)
 None last week 15.0 15.1
 ≤ 10 units last week 42.1 42.0
 > 10 units last week 42.9 42.9
GHQ depression (≥ 4) 3300 (3.9%)
 Yes 11.4 11.5
BMIb 3068 (10.6%) 26.6 (4.2) 26.7 (4.2)
 Normal (< 25 kg/m2) 37.5 37.2
 Overweight (25–29.9 kg/m2) 44.4 44.3
 Obese (≥ 30 kg/m2) 18.1 18.5
Blood pressureb 3232 (5.9%) 124.6 (16.0)c 124.7 (16.0)c
 High (≥ 140/90 mmHg) 18.7 18.9 75.0 (10.7)d 75.1 (10.7)d
Blood cholesterolb 3200 (6.8%) 5.7 (1.0) 5.7 (1.0)
 High (≥ 5 mmol/l) 77.0 77.1
CHD 3433 (0%) 7.5 7.5
Stroke 3433 (0%) 0.3 0.3
Cancer 3400 (0.1%) 3.5 3.5
Diabetes 3433 (0%) 21.6 21.6

aInformation for the cognitive outcomes and time-varying covariates (smoking status, alcohol consumption, GHQ depression, BMI, blood pressure, blood cholesterol, CHD, stroke, cancer, diabetes) were based on the last interview before retirement

bBMI, blood pressure, and blood cholesterol were used as continuous variables in regression models

cMean (SD) for systolic blood pressure

dMean (SD) for diastolic blood pressure

Sensitivity analyses

We conducted three sensitivity analyses to assess the reliability of our results and conclusions. Sensitivity analysis 1 aimed to assess potential bias to the results due to reverse causality. For this analysis, we excluded 500 participants from the analytic sample who retired due to health reasons or had a GHQ depression value of 4 or higher at the last interview before retirement. In addition, 172 participants for whom cognition was measured only twice (once before and once after retirement) were also excluded. Participants who moved from work to retirement via ‘unemployed/other’ (n = 278) were excluded from this sensitivity analysis, since they are likely to have higher levels of stress which may influence cognitive function. Some participants fulfilled several of the exclusion criteria, thus a total of 911 participants were excluded in this sensitivity analysis.

We compared the characteristics of ‘eligible participants but with missing cognition data (n = 258)’ and ‘the analytic sample (n = 3433)’, and found that they had several different demographic characteristics (online resources Table 1S). Therefore, it is possible that our analytic sample had different cognitive function compared to participants with missing cognitive data. Sensitivity analysis 2 aimed to assess the impact of missing cognitive data on results. This sensitivity analysis included these 258 participants and multiply imputed their missing cognitive measures.

Sensitivity analysis 3 assessed whether our results could be influenced by physical activity level, although one recently published Whitehall II study found no association between physical activity and cognitive decline [54]. We used the total physical activity level (< 8, 8–12, ≥ 12 h/week) at the last interview before retirement.

Results

Table 1 shows that the analytical sample includes 3433 participants of whom 72% were men. Their average retirement age was 61.2 years (SD = 4.6), which is slightly higher than the civil service occupational pension age (60 years). Descriptive information for the cognitive outcomes and time-varying covariates refers to the last interview before retirement. Most respondents’ (66.5%) highest educational qualification was lower than degree level. Most respondents were employed in the highest (‘administrative’, 45.7%) or second highest (‘professional/executive’, 42.2%) employment grade, and 62% of respondents were still working in the civil service (rather than working outside). 50.3% had a working spouse while 21.4% did not have a spouse. Nearly half of the sample were never-smokers, and 15% did not consume any alcohol in the past week. Eleven percent of respondents had a raised GHQ depression score. Approximately, one in five had hypertension, 77% had elevated levels of blood total cholesterol, and nearly three out of five were overweight (44.4%) or obese (18.1%). Imputed data showed very similar percentages and means as observed data.

Table 2 shows the fully adjusted models on retirement and cognitive function from piecewise linear regressions. The negative pre-retirement slope for verbal memory (− 0.103; 95% CI − 0.122, − 0.085) suggests that verbal memory was inversely associated with ‘years before retirement’, or in other words, verbal memory score declined by 0.103 every year before retirement. After retirement, verbal memory scores declined by 0.143 every year (95% CI − 0.162, − 0.124). The slope change (the difference between after vs. before retirement cognition slopes) was − 0.039 (95% CI − 0.058, − 0.021; p < 0.001), indicating that verbal memory test scores declined faster after retirement compared to before retirement. The % change was 38% [calculated as: -0.039÷-0.103×100%], suggesting that retirement was associated with, on average, 38% faster decline in verbal memory, independent of age-related decline. The results for other cognition domains (abstract reasoning, phonemic and semantic verbal fluency) showed that while there was age-related decline in cognitive function both before and after retirement, the differences in the slope of decline were not statistically significant at conventional levels (p = 0.180 for abstract reasoning; p = 0.867 for phonemic verbal fluency; p = 0.774 for semantic verbal fluency). Adding quadratic terms in the piecewise regressions did not improve model fit (Wald test p > 0.1, results are not shown). In the multivariate multilevel analysis including all cognitive domains, the test of heterogeneity confirmed that the effect of retirement on verbal memory was more pronounced compared to other domains of cognition (verbal memory vs. abstract reasoning p = 0.001; verbal memory vs. phonemic verbal fluency p = 0.028; verbal memory vs. semantic verbal fluency p = 0.070).

Table 2.

Changes of cognition trajectories before and after retirement (n = 3433)a

Before retirement After retirement Changeb
Slope (change per year) 95% CI p Slope (change per year) 95% CI p Slope (change per year) 95% CI p % change (%)c
Verbal memory − 0.103 − 0.122, − 0.085 < 0.001 − 0.143 − 0.162, − 0.124 < 0.001 − 0.039 − 0.058, − 0.021 < 0.001 38.0
Abstract reasoning − 0.579 − 0.643, − 0.513 < 0.001 − 0.547 − 0.616, − 0.477 < 0.001 0.032 − 0.015, 0.078 0.180 5.5
Phonemic verbal fluency − 0.219 − 0.250, − 0.189 < 0.001 − 0.217 − 0.249, − 0.184 < 0.001 0.002 − 0.025, 0.029 0.867 0.9
Semantic verbal fluency − 0.168 − 0.196, − 0.139 < 0.001 − 0.164 − 0.194, − 0.134 < 0.001 0.004 − 0.022, 0.029 0.774 2.3

aAdjusted for retirement age, birth cohort, highest educational qualification, gender, practice effects, spouse employment status, employment grades, still working in the civil service, job demands, job decision latitude, smoking status, alcohol consumption, depressive symptoms, systolic blood pressure, diastolic blood pressure, body mass index, total blood cholesterol, coronary heart disease, stroke, malignant cancers, and diabetes/intermediate hyperglycaemia

bCalculated as ‘slope after retirement’ minus ‘slope before retirement’

cCalculated as ‘slope change’ divided by ‘slope before retirement’ and multiplied by 100%

To visualise the results of verbal memory, trajectories of adjusted means, both before and after retirement, are shown in Fig. 2. As explained above, the decline in verbal memory accelerated after, compared to before, retirement. The trajectories for the other three domains are shown in the online resources (Figs. 1S to 3S), because there was no significant difference in the decline before and after retirement.

Fig. 2.

Fig. 2

Trajectories of verbal memory before and after retirement by averaging covariates

We then examined whether the association between retirement and cognitive function varied by employment grade. Including terms for the interaction between employment grade and slopes for verbal memory revealed that the interaction was borderline significant (Wald test p = 0.062, results are not shown). For better interpretation, we stratified the models for verbal memory by employment grade. Stratified results in Table 3 show that pre-retirement slopes were less negative among those with higher employment grades (− 0.084 for professional/executive, − 0.107 for administrative) compared to those in the clerical/support grade (− 0.156), suggesting that higher employment grade may be protective against verbal memory decline while people were still working. This ‘protective effect’ disappeared when individuals retired, as people had similar slopes of verbal memory after retirement, which was − 0.152 for clerical/support grade, − 0.142 for professional/executive grade, and − 0.144 for administrative grade. Those retiring from professional/executive (slope change = − 0.057; 95% CI − 0.086, − 0.029; % change = 67.9%) and administrative grades (slope change = − 0.037; 95% CI − 0.063, − 0.011; % change = 34.6%) experienced significant changes in their slopes of verbal memory, but those retired from the clerical/support grade did not. Figure 3 plots trajectories of verbal memory by employment grade. This highlights that people in higher grades start out with better verbal memory (i.e. higher intercept) and a slower rate of decline (i.e. less negative slope) while in work. After retirement, rates of decline were similar across employment grades, although verbal memory level remains higher among participants from higher employment grades. The interaction between sex and slopes for verbal memory was not statistically significant (Wald test p = 0.31), thus models were not stratified by sex.

Table 3.

Stratified results for verbal memory by employment grade (n = 3433)a

Before retirement After retirement Changeb
Slope (change per year) 95% CI p Slope (change per year) 95% CI p Slope (change per year) 95% CI p % change (%)c
Clerical/support(low) − 0.156 − 0.217, − 0.096 < 0.001 − 0.152 − 0.210, − 0.094 < 0.001 0.005 − 0.064, 0.073 0.893 3.2
Prof/exec(middle) − 0.084 − 0.112, − 0.057 < 0.001 − 0.142 − 0.171, − 0.112 < 0.001 − 0.057 − 0.086, − 0.029 < 0.001 67.9
Administrative(high) − 0.107 − 0.134, − 0.080 < 0.001 − 0.144 − 0.173, − 0.116 < 0.001 − 0.037 − 0.063, − 0.011 0.005 34.6

aAdjusted for retirement age, birth cohort, highest educational qualification, gender, practice effects, spouse employment status, still working in the civil service, job demands, job decision latitude, smoking status, alcohol consumption, depressive symptoms, systolic blood pressure, diastolic blood pressure, body mass index, total blood cholesterol, coronary heart disease, stroke, malignant cancers, and diabetes/intermediate hyperglycaemia

bCalculated as ‘slope after retirement’ minus ‘slope before retirement’

cCalculated as ‘slope change’ divided by ‘slope before retirement’ and multiplied by 100%

Fig. 3.

Fig. 3

Trajectories of verbal memory by employment grade

Three sensitivity analyses did not change the associations between retirement and cognitive outcomes. Results are shown in online resources Tables 2S to 4S.

Discussion

In this longitudinal study of 3433 individuals, we compared cognitive decline before and after retirement and examined whether trajectories varied depending on employment grade. We found that declines in verbal memory were faster during the 14 years after than during the 14 years before retirement. In the stratified analysis, we found that higher employment grade may be protective against verbal memory decline while people were still working, but this ‘protective effect’ was lost when individuals retired. The other domains of cognitive function, including abstract reasoning, phonemic verbal fluency, and semantic verbal fluency, were not affected by retirement, but declined steadily with age.

Our finding showing an adverse effect of retirement on verbal memory is consistent with most previous studies which used instrumental variables [1821] and also those which have applied other statistical methods. For instance, Wickrama and O’Neal [35] used growth curve analyses in the USA Health and Retirement Study (HRS), and found that individuals who retired between 1998 and 2002 had a faster memory decline between 2002 and 2006 compared to those who were working at both instances. In another HRS investigation, Clouston and Denier [22] showed similar findings for retirement and episodic memory (comprised of both verbal learning and verbal memory) by using longitudinal regression discontinuity methods to analyse trajectories between 1998 and 2012. A cross-national study by Adam et al. [55] used the stochastic frontier approach to estimate the episodic memory that individuals would reach if they were fully efficient for a given level of resources. They found an adverse effect of retirement on episodic memory and highlighted the positive impact non-professional activities at retirement and increased social contacts could have for episodic memory.

Our finding on the retirement-associated decline in verbal memory supports the ‘use it or lose it’ hypothesis suggesting that failing to keep mentally active may accelerate the rate of cognitive decline in post-retirement periods [17]. On a similar note, our findings are also consistent with the theory of ‘mental retirement’ proposed by Rohwedder and Willis [21], suggesting that the work environment could be more cognitively stimulating than the leisure environment as a retiree. Besides the direct effects of an absence of cognitive activities related to work, retirement may also affect cognitive function indirectly via loss of work-related forms of self-organisation, communications and collaborations [56], which are important factors potentially contributing to the maintenance of cognitive reserve [55, 57]. For example, social networks could be more extensive during employment, and accordingly, Börsch-Supan and Schuth [58] estimated that at least one-third of the decline in cognition after retirement could be attributed to a reduction in the size and composition of social networks.

Our results showing a significant effect of retirement for verbal memory but not for other cognitive domains suggest that retirement may affect some cognitive domains more than others. Age-related neuronal modifications that are at the root of Alzheimer’s disease have been observed to have heterogeneous effects on cognitive functioning. For example, episodic memory deficits are largely considered as a hallmark symptom of Alzheimer’s disease [59], but this is less the case for other domains of cognition. It may also be that verbal memory is a more sensitive indicator of cognitive decline than the other indicators. Few studies have assessed the effect of retirement on different domains of cognition. Using the USA Wisconsin Longitudinal Study, Denier et al. [60] found that those who had retired voluntarily or for family reasons had improved reasoning abilities, which is contrary to our findings. Denier et al. used the similarities construct of the Weschler Adult Intelligence Scale to measure reasoning abilities. Respondents were asked to relate two words; for instance, ‘How are an apple and orange alike?’ to which they should respond that both are fruits. In contrast, the AH4 questionnaire used in our study consists of both verbal and mathematical questions. Inconsistent results may originate from different assessments of cognitive functioning across studies. One SHARE study tested only memory and numeracy and found that both domains were negatively affected by retirement [58]. Also using SHARE data, Mazzonnaa and Peracchi found that retirement was negatively associated with verbal memory, orientation, and numeracy for both men and women. Retirement was not associated with verbal fluency, with the exception that retired women without a high-school degree showed a faster decline in verbal fluency [20]. Our Whitehall II study did not measure orientation or numeracy, but our results of the negative effect on memory and no influence on verbal fluency are generally consistent with these two SHARE studies. Roberts et al. [36] previously used Whitehall II to show that mean cognitive test scores increased between two assessments over 5 years, and discussed that this is possibly due to practice effects. They found that those retired increased less than those still working. Their findings could not be confirmed by our study using longer follow-up of Whitehall II, where we have taken account of practice effects by adjusting for the number of cognitive tests a participant has completed in previous phases.

Stratified analyses showed that higher employment grade may be protective against cognitive decline while people were still working, but this ‘protective effect’ went away when individuals retired. According to the cognitive reserve hypothesis, engagement in mentally challenging activities can yield additional neuronal resources that may prevent cognitive decline [57]. Higher grade jobs have higher levels of skill discretion implying more opportunities for the use of skills and variety of work [37], which suggests higher levels of mental processing than clerical/support jobs. Thus, our observation that employees in higher grades had slower decline during employment is plausible. This protection of higher grades no longer exists after retirement. As expected by the ‘use it or lose it’ hypothesis, the decline in verbal memory was similar in all participants irrespective of their pre-retirement employment grade.

Our findings on employment grade are in agreement with the observations from SHARE, showing that the average effect of retirement on cognition was negative, and the negative effect of retirement disappeared when the sample was restricted to people who worked in more physically demanding occupations [61]. Our stratified results are also in line with Finkel et al.’s work, which found that retirement from more complex jobs was related to a faster rate of cognitive decline in the longitudinal Swedish Adoption/Twin Study of Aging [62]. However, Fisher et al. [30] found that participants from occupations characterised by higher levels of mental demands showed slightly higher cognitive performance and less steep decline both before and after retirement, compared with individuals who were engaged in fewer mental demands. It is likely that employment grades may not only represent job mental demands but also serve as an indicator of broader working environments as well as post-retirement financial resources and social support. People in higher employment grades may have a stronger attachment to their work role, and thus retirement may be more detrimental to them because of this role loss. Future studies might investigate different preretirement occupational characteristics in order to understand the nature and mechanisms underlying the cognitive effects of retirement.

Our findings on employment grade should be interpreted cautiously because only 12.1% of our analytic sample was retired from clerical/support grade, and the interaction by employment grade was only marginally significant. Therefore, we cannot rule out the possibility that the different results found for clerical/support grade were due to selection bias. It is also worth pointing out that, even though individuals in higher grades had a faster rate of cognitive decline after retirement compared to before retirement, they still had higher average levels of cognition than people in the lowest grade, both before and after retirement. This suggests that although retirement seems to be more detrimental for those in higher grades, people in the lowest grade remain at greatest risk of developing cognition problems.

We found no significant sex differences in terms of the effect of retirement on verbal memory. However, less than 30% of our sample are women, with even fewer women in the higher grades, so it is possible that the study lacked the power to detect potential sex differences.

Strengths and limitations

The main strength of our study is the assessment of multiple cognitive domains and a long observation period both before and after retirement. To our knowledge, this is the first study on cognition with such extended periods of pre- and post-retirement measures.

It is also a strength that we could examine the rate of change in cognition before and after retirement rather than comparing levels of cognition. Cognitive decline could lead to retirement but the analysis reduced this problem by comparing slopes from long follow-up before and after for the same group of individuals. This way, our method could lower the risk of reverse causality. The consistent findings in the sensitivity analysis reassured us of our results. The use of a multilevel framework in this study was able to account for clustering of observations within participants. Missing values in the covariates were multiply imputed using chained equations, allowing us to use information from all cases. Predicted piecewise cognition trajectories showed the trajectories before and after retirement.

There are also several limitations to this study. First, a great challenge with this type of research is the possibility of reverse causality. Although this piecewise study has several strengths in attempting to reduce the influence of reverse causality, the possibility of reverse causality is still not fully eliminated. Loss-to-follow-up bias is also possible because we only included retired participants with repeated observations both before and after retirement. Some participants dropped out of the study earlier than others, and thus had fewer repeated measures of cognition, which may lead to nonignorable missingness. Also, the level of cognitive function was measured at every other wave rather than at each wave, which increased the likelihood of loss-to-follow-up for older participants. We adjusted for health conditions that are related to cognitive decline, but confounding from unknown characteristics is still possible. Random intercepts and random coefficients accounted for individual trajectories of cognitive function before and after retirement and captured such unobserved variability to some extent. However, even though the proportion of participants who died or were diagnosed with dementia is very small in this cohort, results may be biased by accelerating declines of cognition occurring before dementia [63] or prior to death [64, 65]. Our study focuses on the average slope change of cognitive function as a response to retirement. We did not examine whether post-retirement activities, such as voluntary work, social activities, and physical activities may modify the risk of cognitive decline. Further research may focus on factors explaining heterogeneity in declines in cognitive functioning after retirement.

The Whitehall II Study uses a sample of civil servants in the UK. Compared to the general population, their type of work may be more mentally challenging. For example, verbal memory may be especially important for their paperwork. Thus, their cognition trajectories cannot be regarded as being representative of the general population, although the sample covers the entire range of occupations from administrative to clerical/support, ensuring some level of variability. Moreover, the sample size at the two ends of the analytic period is smaller than those at other time points, leading to larger confidence intervals at the two ends. However, the estimated coefficients of our piecewise model are based on the overall linear trend of cognition, and showed a good fit between the estimated line and observed values at each time point.

Conclusion

In support of the ‘use it or lose it’ hypothesis, we found that retirement is associated with faster declines in verbal memory function over time, but has little impact on other domains of cognitive functions, such as abstract reasoning and verbal fluency. The smaller cognitive decline before retirement in employees from high employment grade jobs points to the potential benefits of cognitively stimulating activities associated with employment that could benefit older people’s memory.

Electronic supplementary material

Below is the link to the electronic supplementary material.

Acknowledgements

We thank all participating civil service departments and their welfare personnel and establishment officers; the Occupational Health and Safety Agency; the Council of Civil Service Unions; all participating civil servants in the Whitehall II Study; and all members of the Whitehall II Study Team. The Whitehall II Study Team comprises research scientists, statisticians, study coordinators, nurses, data managers, administrative assistants, and data entry staff, who make the study possible.

Funding

This work was funded by the Economic and Social Research Council and the Medical Research Council as part of the Lifelong Health and Well-Being (LLHW) initiative (Grant Number ES/L002892/1). The Whitehall II study is also supported by British Medical Research Council Grant (G0902037 & K013351). The funding organisations had no role in the design and conduct of the study; collection, management, analysis, and interpretation of data; and preparation, review, or approval of the manuscript. SAS was (in part) supported by the National Institute for Health Research (NIHR) Collaboration for Leadership in Applied Health Research and Care (CLAHRC) North Thames at Bart’s Health NHS Trust. The views expressed are those of the authors and not necessarily those of the NHS, the NIHR or the Department of Health.

Compliance with ethical standards

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that they have no conflict of interest.

References

  • 1.Salthouse T. Consequences of age-related cognitive declines. Annu Rev Psychol. 2012;63:201–226. doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-120710-100328. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Salthouse TA. Robust cognitive change. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2012;18:749–756. doi: 10.1017/S1355617712000380. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Aizpurua A, Koutstaal W. Aging and flexible remembering: contributions of conceptual span, fluid intelligence, and frontal functioning. Psychol Aging. 2010;25:193–207. doi: 10.1037/a0018198. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Schaie K. The course of adult intellectual development. Am Psychol. 1994;49:304–313. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.49.4.304. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Singer T, Verhaeghen P, Ghisletta P, Lindenberger U, Baltes PB. The fate of cognition in very old age: six-year longitudinal findings in the Berlin aging study (BASE) Psychol Aging. 2010;18:318–331. doi: 10.1037/0882-7974.18.2.318. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Murman DL. The impact of age on cognition. Semin Hear. 2015;36:111–121. doi: 10.1055/s-0035-1555115. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Silver M, Newell K, Hyman B, Growdon J, Hedley-Whyte ET, Perls T. Unraveling the mystery of cognitive changes in old age: correlation of neuropsychological evaluation with neuropathological findings in the extreme old. Int Psychogeriatr. 1998;10:25–41. doi: 10.1017/S1041610298005122. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Silver MH, Jilinskaia E, Perls TT. Cognitive functional status of age-confirmed centenarians in a population-based study. J Gerontol Ser B. 2001;56:134–140. doi: 10.1093/geronb/56.3.P134. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Fjell AM, McEvoy L, Holland D, Dale AM, Walhovd KB. What is normal in normal aging? Effects of aging, amyloid and Alzheimer’s disease on the cerebral cortex and the hippocampus. Prog Neurobiol Pergamon. 2014;117:20–40. doi: 10.1016/j.pneurobio.2014.02.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Gage FH. Neurogenesis in the adult brain. J Neurosci. 2002;22:612–613. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.22-03-00612.2002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Couillard-Després S. Hippocampal neurogenesis and ageing. Neurogenes Neural Plast. 2012;15:343–355. doi: 10.1007/7854_2012_232. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Cameron HA, Glover LR. Adult neurogenesis: beyond learning and memory. Annu Rev Psychol Ann Rev. 2015;66:53–81. doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-010814-015006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Amieva H, Jacqmin-Gadda H, Orgogozo J-M, Le Carret N, Helmer C, Letenneur L, et al. The 9 year cognitive decline before dementia of the Alzheimer type: a prospective population-based study. Brain. 2005;128:1093–1101. doi: 10.1093/brain/awh451. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Rabin LA, Smart CM, Amariglio RE. Subjective cognitive decline in preclinical Alzheimer’s disease. Annu Rev Clin Psychol Annu Rev. 2017;13:369–396. doi: 10.1146/annurev-clinpsy-032816-045136. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Stern Y. Cognitive reserve in ageing and Alzheimer’s disease. Lancet Neurol. 2012;11:1006–1012. doi: 10.1016/S1474-4422(12)70191-6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Stern Y. Cognitive reserve. Neuropsychologia Pergamon. 2009;47:2015–2028. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.03.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Hultsch DF, Hertzog C, Small BJ, Dixon RA. Use it or lose it: engaged lifestyle as a buffer of cognitive decline in aging? Psychol Aging Am Psychol Assoc. 1999;14:245–263. doi: 10.1037/0882-7974.14.2.245. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Behncke S. Does retirement trigger ill health? Health Econ. 2012;21:282–300. doi: 10.1002/hec.1712. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Bonsang E, Adam S, Perelman S. Does retirement affect cognitive functioning? J Health Econ. 2012;31:490–501. doi: 10.1016/j.jhealeco.2012.03.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Mazzonna F, Peracchi F. Ageing, cognitive abilities and retirement. Eur Econ Rev. 2012;56:691–710. doi: 10.1016/j.euroecorev.2012.03.004. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Rohwedder S, Willis RJ. Mental retirement. J Econ Perspect. 2010;24:119–138. doi: 10.1257/jep.24.1.119. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Clouston SAP, Denier N. Mental retirement and health selection: analyses from the U.S. Health and Retirement Study. Soc Sci Med. 2017;178:78–86. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2017.01.019. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Kajitani S, Sakata K, McKenzie C. Occupation, retirement and cognitive functioning. Ageing Soc. 2017;37:1568–1596. doi: 10.1017/S0144686X16000465. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Grotz C, Meillon C, Amieva H, Stern Y, Dartigues J-F, Adam S, et al. Why is later age at retirement beneficial for cognition? Results from a French population-based study. J Nutr Health Aging. 2016;20:514–519. doi: 10.1007/s12603-015-0599-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Dufouil C, Pereira E, Chêne G, Glymour MM, Alpérovitch A, Saubusse E, et al. Older age at retirement is associated with decreased risk of dementia. Eur J Epidemiol. 2014;29:353–361. doi: 10.1007/s10654-014-9906-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Wickrama K, O’Neal CW, Kwag KH, Lee TK. Is working later in life good or bad for health? An investigation of multiple health outcomes. J Gerontol Ser B Psychol Sci Soc Sci. 2013;68:807–815. doi: 10.1093/geronb/gbt069. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Lupton MK, Stahl D, Archer N, Foy C, Poppe M, Lovestone S, et al. Education, occupation and retirement age effects on the age of onset of Alzheimer’s disease. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2010;25:30–36. doi: 10.1002/gps.2294. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Coe NB, von Gaudecker H-M, Lindeboom M, Maurer J. The effect of retirement on cognitive functioning. Health Econ. 2012;21:913–927. doi: 10.1002/hec.1771. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Coe NB, Zamarro G. Retirement effects on health in Europe. J Health Econ. 2011;30:77–86. doi: 10.1016/j.jhealeco.2010.11.002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Fisher G, Stachowski A, Infurna F. Mental work demands, retirement, and longitudinal trajectories of cognitive functioning. J Occup Health Psychol. 2014;19:231–242. doi: 10.1037/a0035724. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Belbase A, Khan MR, Munnell AH, Webb A. Slowed or sidelined? The effect of “normal” cognitive decline on job performance among the elderly. Chestnut Hill: Center for Retirement Research at Boston College; 2015. [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Stafford M, Cooper R, Cadar D, Carr E, Murray E, Richards M, et al. Physical and cognitive capability in mid-adulthood as determinants of retirement and extended working life in a British cohort study. Scand J Work Environ Health. 2017;43:15–23. doi: 10.5271/sjweh.3589. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Karpansalo M, Manninen P, Kauhanen J, Lakka TA, Salonen JT. Perceived health as a predictor of early retirement. Scand J Work Environ Health. 2004;30:287–292. doi: 10.5271/sjweh.796. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Viswanathan A, Macklin EA, Betensky R, Hyman B, Smith EE, Blacker D. The influence of vascular risk factors and stroke on cognition in late life: analysis of the NACC cohort. Alzheimer Dis Assoc Disord. 2015;29:287–293. doi: 10.1097/WAD.0000000000000080. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Wickrama KK, O’Neal CW. The influence of working later in life on memory functioning. Adv Life Course Res. 2013;18:288–295. doi: 10.1016/j.alcr.2013.09.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Roberts B, Fuhrer R, Marmot M. Does retirement influence cognitive performance? The Whitehall II study. J Epidemiol Commun Health. 2010;65:958–963. doi: 10.1136/jech.2010.111849. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Marmot MG, Stansfeld S, Patel C, North F, Head J, White I, et al. Health inequalities among British civil servants: the Whitehall II study. Lancet. 1991;337:1387–1393. doi: 10.1016/0140-6736(91)93068-K. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.van Hooren SAH, Valentijn AM, Bosma H, Ponds RWHM, van Boxtel MPJ, Jolles J. cognitive functioning in healthy older adults aged 64–81: a cohort study into the effects of age, sex, and education. Aging Neuropsychol Cogn. 2007;14:40–54. doi: 10.1080/138255890969483. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Loretto W, Vickerstaff S. The domestic and gendered context for retirement. Hum Relat. 2013;66:65–86. doi: 10.1177/0018726712455832. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.McMunn A, Lacey R, Worts D, McDonough P, Stafford M, Booker C, et al. De-standardization and gender convergence in work–family life courses in Great Britain: a multi-channel sequence analysis. Adv Life Course Res. 2015;26:60–75. doi: 10.1016/j.alcr.2015.06.002. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Singh-Manoux A, Kivimaki M, Glymour MM, Elbaz A, Berr C, Ebmeier KP, et al. Timing of onset of cognitive decline: results from Whitehall II prospective cohort study. BMJ. 2012;344:d7622. doi: 10.1136/bmj.d7622. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Sabia S, Kivimaki M, Shipley MJ, Marmot MG, Singh-Manoux A. Body mass index over the adult life course and cognition in late midlife: the Whitehall II Cohort Study. Am J Clin Nutr. 2009;89:601–607. doi: 10.3945/ajcn.2008.26482. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Heim A. AH 4 group test of general intelligence. Windsor: NFER-Nelson Publishing Company Ltd; 1970. [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Borkowski JG, Benton AL, Spreen O. Word fluency and brain damage. Neuropsychologia. 1967;5:135–140. doi: 10.1016/0028-3932(67)90015-2. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Fleischmann M, Carr E, Stansfeld SA, Xue B, Head J. Can favourable psychosocial working conditions in midlife moderate the risk of work exit for chronically ill workers? A 20-year follow-up of the Whitehall II study. Occup Environ Med. 2017 doi: 10.1136/oemed-2017-104452. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Joensuu M, Kivimaki M, Pentti J, Virtanen M, Vaananen A, Vahtera J. Components of job control and mortality: the Finnish Public Sector Study. Occup Environ Med. 2014;71:536–542. doi: 10.1136/oemed-2014-102111. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Singh-Manoux A, Akbaraly TN, Marmot M, Melchior M, Ankri J, Sabia S, et al. Persistent depressive symptoms and cognitive function in late midlife: the Whitehall II study. J Clin Psychiatry Inserm. 2010;71:1379–1385. doi: 10.4088/JCP.09m05349gry. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Tunstall-Pedoe H, Kuulasmaa K, Amouyel P. Myocardial infarction and coronary deaths in the World Health Organization MONICA Project. Registration procedures, event rates, and case-fatality rates in 38. Circulation. 1994;90:583–612. doi: 10.1161/01.CIR.90.1.583. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Brunner EJ, Shipley MJ, Britton AR, Stansfeld SA, Heuschmann PU, Rudd AG, et al. Depressive disorder, coronary heart disease, and stroke: dose–response and reverse causation effects in the Whitehall II cohort study. Eur J Prev Cardiol. 2014;21:340–346. doi: 10.1177/2047487314520785. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Britton A, Milne B, Butler T, Sanchez-Galvez A, Shipley M, Rudd A, et al. Validating self-reported strokes in a longitudinal UK cohort study (Whitehall II): extracting information from hospital medical records versus the Hospital Episode Statistics database. BMC Med Res Methodol. 2012;12:83. doi: 10.1186/1471-2288-12-83. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.World Health Organization, International Diabetes Federation. Definition and diagnosis of diabetes mellitus and intermediate hyperglycemia: report of a WHO/IDF consultation [Internet]. Geneva: WHO Press; 2006 [cited 13 Jun 2017]. http://apps.who.int/iris/bitstream/10665/43588/1/9241594934_eng.pdf.
  • 52.Weisberg S. Applied linear regression. Hoboken: Wiley; 2005. [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Xue B, Head J, McMunn A. The associations between retirement and cardiovascular disease risk factors in China: a 20-year prospective study. Am J Epidemiol. 2017;185:688–696. doi: 10.1093/aje/kww166. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Sabia S, Dugravot A, Dartigues J-F, Abell J, Elbaz A, Kivimäki M, et al. Physical activity, cognitive decline, and risk of dementia: 28 year follow-up of Whitehall II cohort study. BMJ. 2017;357:j2709. doi: 10.1136/bmj.j2709. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Adam S, Bonsang E, Germain S, Perelman S. Retirement and cognitive reserve: a stochastic frontier approach applied to survey data 1. 2007. http://orbi.ulg.ac.be/bitstream/2268/35657/1/101%20WP_HECULG_20070402_Adam_Bonsang_Germain_Perelman.pdf. Accessed 19 Dec 2017.
  • 56.Wang M, Shi J. Psychological research on retirement. Annu Rev Psychol. 2014;65:209–233. doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-010213-115131. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Scarmeas N, Stern Y. Cognitive reserve and lifestyle. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol. 2010;25:625–633. doi: 10.1076/jcen.25.5.625.14576. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 58.Börsch-Supan A, Schuth M. Early retirement, mental health and social networks. In: Börsch-Supan A, Brandt M, Litwin H, Weber G, editors. Active ageing solidarity between generations Europe: first results from SHARE after economic crisis. Berlin: Walter de Gruyter; 2013. p. 337. [Google Scholar]
  • 59.Dubois B, Feldman HH, Jacova C, DeKosky ST, Barberger-Gateau P, Cummings J, et al. Research criteria for the diagnosis of Alzheimer’s disease: revising the NINCDS–ADRDA criteria. Lancet Neurol. 2007;6:734–746. doi: 10.1016/S1474-4422(07)70178-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 60.Denier N, Clouston SAP, Richards M, Hofer SM. Retirement and cognition: a life course view. Adv Life Course Res. 2017;31:11–21. doi: 10.1016/j.alcr.2016.10.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 61.Mazzonna F, Peracchi F. Unhealthy retirement? J Hum Resour. 2017;52:128–51. http://jhr.uwpress.org/content/52/1/128.full.pdf+html. Accessed 19 Dec 2017.
  • 62.Finkel D, Andel R, Gatz M, Pedersen NL. The role of occupational complexity in trajectories of cognitive aging before and after retirement. Psychol Aging. 2009;24:563–573. doi: 10.1037/a0015511. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 63.Cadar D, Piccinin AM, Hofer SM, Johansson B, Muniz-Terrera G. Education, occupational class, and cognitive decline in preclinical dementia. GeroPsyc Hogrefe AG. 2016;29:5–15. doi: 10.1024/1662-9647/a000138. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 64.Cadar D, Stephan BCM, Jagger C, Johansson B, Hofer SM, Piccinin AM, et al. The role of cognitive reserve on terminal decline: a cross-cohort analysis from two European studies: OCTO-Twin, Sweden, and Newcastle 85+, UK. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2016;31:601–610. doi: 10.1002/gps.4366. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 65.Muniz-Terrera G, van den Hout A, Piccinin AM, Matthews FE, Hofer SM. Investigating terminal decline: results from a UK population-based study of aging. Psychol Aging Am Psychol Assoc. 2013;28:377–385. doi: 10.1037/a0031000. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials


Articles from European Journal of Epidemiology are provided here courtesy of Springer

RESOURCES