Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2020 Feb 1.
Published in final edited form as: Emotion. 2018 Mar 26;19(1):171–177. doi: 10.1037/emo0000409

Dysregulated behavioral responses to hedonic probes among youth with depression histories and their high-risk siblings

Vanessa Panaite 1,2, Lauren M Bylsma 3, Maria Kovacs 3, Kimberly O’Leary 2, Charles J George 4, Ildikó Baji 5, István Benák 5, Roberta Dochnal 5, Enikő Kiss 5, Ágnes Vetró 5, Krisztina Kapornai 5, Jonathan Rottenberg 2
PMCID: PMC6158125  NIHMSID: NIHMS936658  PMID: 29578741

Abstract

Affect dysregulation in response to rewarding stimuli has been proposed as a vulnerability factor for major depressive disorder (MDD). However, it remains unclear how affective behavioral dynamics may be altered among individuals who are at high-risk for depression but not currently depressed. We examined the dynamics of affective facial behavior during hedonic probes among three groups of adolescents: remitted probands who had histories of childhood-onset MDD (n=187), never-depressed siblings of probands (high familial risk; n=207), and healthy controls (n=166). Participants’ happy and sad facial expressions were coded during three hedonic laboratory tasks: receiving a preferred prize, describing a positive autobiographical memory, and watching a humorous film. Happy and sad behavioral dynamics were indexed by mean level and time dependent reactivity, variability (mean of the squared successive differences, MSSD), and inertia (autocorrelation). Relative to controls, probands and siblings exhibited a more rapid decrease in happy behaviors and probands exhibited higher inertia of sad behaviors during hedonic probes. Both probands and siblings exhibited lower inertia of sad behaviors while receiving a desired prize, which highlights the importance of context variation in testing hypotheses. Overall, our study provides new evidence that hedonic behavioral dysregulation, as reflected in dynamic facial behavior, may highlight depression vulnerability.

Keywords: Early onset depression, remitted depression, familial depression, emotion, behavioral dynamics


Affective disturbance is central to major depressive disorder and is proposed as a vulnerability factor in children and adolescents (Clark, Watson, Mineka, 1994; Compas, Connor-Smith, & Jaser, 2004). Indeed, both a history of depression (Forbes, Shaw, & Dahl, 2007) and familial risk for depression (Rawal, Collishaw, Thapar, & Rice, 2013) have been associated with disrupted hedonic and negative emotional responses (Kellough, Beevers, Ellis, & Wells, 2008) to both positive and negative stimuli—albeit not consistently (e.g., Joorman, Talbot, Gotlib, 2007).

Reduced positive affective (PA) reactivity and deficient mood repair (i.e., ability to decrease sad affect) following positive stimuli have also been documented in youths with current or remitted depression (Kovacs, Bylsma, et al., 2015; Kovacs, Yaroslavsky, et al 2015). Since emotions are dynamic, a focus on the magnitude of emotional responses cannot adequately capture their temporal aspects. Dynamic indices such as variability and inertia (see Houben et al, 2015) can help elucidate potential abnormalities in affective dynamics in the context of depression or risk. While variability concerns the divergence of momentary affect from prior affect levels, inertia captures resistance to change in affective states and the moment-to-moment carryover of affect (Koval, Pe, Meers, & Kuppens, 2013).

Facial expressive behavior provides an unobtrusive window into affective dynamics (Gruber & Keltner, 2007). There are indications that behavioral stereotypy (i.e., expressive rigidity) is linked to both a history of depression (Hankin et al, 2012) and risk for future depression (Kuppens et al., 2012). Along these lines, offspring of depressed parents (a familial high-risk group) tend to exhibit fewer positive and more negative expressions than offspring of healthy parents (Jones et al., 2001). Depression in siblings is known to be a robust risk factor for the later development of depression in their (as yet) unaffected siblings (Kovacs et al., 2016). Therefore, it is important to understand whether high-risk siblings might also exhibit deficits in their affective behavioral dynamics that might predate the onset of depression, but no prior studies have investigated affective behavioral dynamics among high-risk siblings. Importantly, facial expressive dynamics do not rely on self-reported emotion, which may be less reliable among youths (Achenbach, McConaughy, & Howell, 1987). Unfortunately, the limited number of studies of facial behavior dynamics have predominantly focused on negative affect (e.g., Koval et al, 2012; Sheeber et al, 2012). The only study to examine positive affective behavioral dynamics in depressed youth found that depressed adolescents exhibited no differences in inertia of positive affective behaviors during rewarding social interaction tasks (Kuppens et al., 2010).

The Current Study

We examined both happy and sad facial expressive behaviors during three hedonic stimuli (receiving a desired prize, generation of positive autobiographical memories, watching a humorous film) among three groups of youth: probands with a history of childhood-onset MDD, never-depressed siblings of probands (high familial risk), and controls with no history of any major psychiatric disorder. Given theoretical and empirical support for lasting effects of early-onset depression, we hypothesized that depression risk status would impact happy expressive behavior to positive stimuli, such that probands would exhibit the least happy reactivity (and faster decreases of happy behavior), lowest happy variability, and lowest inertia of happy expressive behaviors. In contrast, we predicted that controls would exhibit the most reactivity, variability, and inertia of happy expressive behaviors, while high-risk siblings would exhibit a pattern of dynamic behaviors midway between the two groups, consistent with our prior pattern of findings for self-reported PA to hedonic stimuli in this same sample (Kovacs, Bylsma et al., 2016). We examined sadness behavioral dynamics as a point of comparison, but given the relative novelty of examining sadness behavior in hedonic contexts, we did not make specific predictions regarding sadness behavior.

Method

Participant Characteristics and Recruitment

This study was approved by the institutional review boards of the University of Pittsburgh and the Hungarian clinical research sites. The current sample included three groups of Hungarian youth, recruited from 23 outpatient mental health facilities: 186 remitted probands, 198 unaffected siblings of probands, and 164 healthy controls. Probands satisfied the following inclusion criteria: a history of a depression disorder, based on DSM-IV criteria (APA, 2000); age 7–14; absence of mental retardation or major medical disorder; have one full biological sibling aged 7–18; and one biological parent available to participate. Control youth recruited from public schools were free of any current or past major psychiatric disorder and were selected to match the probands on age and gender (see Tamas et al., 2007). Level of depressive symptoms was quantified with the Children’s Depression Inventory–2 (CDI-2) (Kovacs & MHS Staff, 2011) and level of anxiety symptoms with the Multidimensional Anxiety Scale for Children (MASC) (March, 2013).

Experimental Procedure

The current study reports on facial behavior responses to three hedonic probes in randomized order, briefly described below (see Kovacs, Bylsma, et al, 2016 for more detail). Unobtrusive webcams were used to capture participant behavior. The hedonic probes included the following: (1) Positive autobiographical memory (PAM, 120s), consisting of the recall of two positive events (60s each) from the past year; (2) Humorous film (186s), which was a segment from Mr. Bean, a slapstick-style comedy; and (3) Desired Prize (60s), where youth unexpectedly received a small prize they had previously ranked as preferred from a list of 7 prizes.

Behavioral coding

Happy and sad expressive behavior was coded using an adaptation of Emotional Behavior Coding System (Gross & Levenson, 1993). All behavior was coded for valence, duration, and intensity in 10s epochs throughout the tasks. Composite scores were computed to account for both behavior intensity and duration across the 10s. Happy and sad behavior was coded as a composite of intensity rated on a 4-point scale (from 0 = no behavior to 3 = strong behavior) and duration (1 to 10 seconds). Composite scores ranged from 0–30 and each composite of intensity and duration was computed as follows for happy and sad separately: e.g., slight_happyXduration+moderate_happyXduration+strong_happyXduration.

Statistical analyses

Given the nested structure of the data, we implemented hierarchical linear modeling (HLM) to examine group differences for each of our behavioral dynamics indices (reactivity, trajectory, variability, and inertia) using the SPSS MIXED procedure (Version 22; IBM, 2013). Preliminary analyses examined the potential impact of age, gender, SES, baseline PA, CDI and MASC scores as covariates predicting happy or sad behavior. Only significant covariates were retained in final models. Final happy behavior models included age and baseline PA as covariates, and final sad behavior models included, gender, baseline PA, CDI, and MASC as covariates. No intercept models were used for post hoc analyses of group effects.

Variability of behavior during each task was examined by using the mean estimate of MSSD at each time point nested within persons as the outcome variable (Koval et al, 2013)1. Behavioral inertia was modeled as the relationship between the prior timepoint (lagged variable) and the current timepoint (e.g., Kuppens et al., 2010). Reactivity was modeled as current affective behavior controlling for baseline.

Results

Demographic and clinical characteristics

Remitted probands were, on average older (M=17.11, SD=1.35), more likely to be male (64.5%), and from families with lower SES (indexed by parental education) (M=2.78, SD=1.17) when compared to control youth (Age: M=15.92, SD=2.13; Gender: 61.6% male; SES: M=3.73, SD=1.09) (ps<.05). Siblings were more likely to be female (46.5% male), and were approximately 1 year younger than probands (M=15.92, SD=2.15) (ps<.05). Probands reported more depressive symptoms on the CDI (M=9.28, SD=6.34), more anxiety symptoms on the MASC (M=31.79, SD=13.41), and lower baseline PA levels (M=3.27, SD=1.56) than controls (CDI:M=4.83, SD=4.29; MASC: M=28.75, SD=10.56; Baseline PA: M=4.33, SD=1.33; ps < .05). Siblings and probands did not differ on current depression scores and the three groups did not differ on baseline NA (ps<.05). However, siblings reported higher baseline PA (M=3.69, SD=1.36) and higher anxiety symptoms (M=34.07, SD=14.16) relative to probands. Finally, biological mothers of probands and siblings had fourfold odds of a lifetime depressive disorder relative to control mothers (32% vs. 9%, x2=31.7, p<.001, OR=4.8).

Happy behavioral reactivity

We predicted decreased happy behavioral reactivity during positive stimuli as a function of depression history, with probands displaying the least overall reactivity. However, the groups were indistinguishable in their overall emotional behavioral response to the three hedonic probes (ps >.05). Although our results reflected a group by task effect (B=−.03, SE=.01, t=−3.76, p<.001), post hoc analyses did not find group differences in happy behavioral reactivity for any of the specific tasks (ps>.05).

Happy behavioral dynamics

For the trajectory of happy behaviors over the course of each of the three hedonic tasks, group effects were consistent with hypotheses: a group effect was observed during hedonic probes, with post hoc analyses highlighting that both probands and siblings showed faster decreasing happy behavior over the course of each of the hedonic tasks relative to controls. No differences were observed between siblings and probands (p<.05). Since we did not find a task effect, we did not further investigate group effects within task.

Inconsistent with our prediction that increased depression risk would be associated with lower inertia of happy behaviors, no group differences were observed (ps>.05). We also predicted lower variability (MSSD), and while we did not find an overall group effect, our findings did highlight a group by task effect suggesting that variability fluctuated across tasks (B=−.23, SE=.07, t=−3.15, p<.01). However, post hoc analyses did not reveal any significant within task group effects (ps >.05). See Table 1.

Table 1.

Emotional behavioral reactivity, trajectory, inertia, and variability as a function of depression risk.

Parameter Estimate SE t Sig. 95% CI
LB UB
Happy Behavior
Trajectory
Group* Epoch −.021*** .005 −4.08 <.001 −.032 −.011
 Probands-Sibs* Epoch .016 .011 1.43 .152 −.006 .038
 Probands-Ctrl* Epoch −.045*** .012 −3.78 <.001 −.068 −.022
 Sibs-Ctrl* Epoch −.061*** .012 −5.24 <.001 −.084 −.038
Group* Task* Epoch <.001 .002 0.18 .858 −.003 .004
Sad Behavior
Reactivity
Group .041 .022 1.89 .060 −.002 .083
Group* Task .013*** .002 8.13 <.001 .010 .016
 PAM (Group) .025 .013 1.85 .065 −.002 .051
 Prize (Group) −.004 .012 −0.33 .742 −.027 .019
 Happy Film (Group) .068 .042 1.62 .105 −.014 .151
Trajectory
Group* Epoch .005*** .001 4.49 <.001 .003 .007
 Probands-Sibs* Epoch .004 .003 1.34 .182 −.002 .009
 Probands-Ctrl* Epoch .011*** .003 4.01 <.001 .005 .016
 Sibs-Ctrl* Epoch .007** .003 2.61 .009 .002 .012
Group* Task* Epoch <−.001 <.001 −0.36 .722 −.001 .001
Inertia
Group* ABt .148*** .007 21.31 <.001 .134 .162
  Probands-Sibs* ABt .240*** .011 21.34 <.001 .218 .262
  Probands-Ctrl* ABt .180*** .015 12.35 <.001 .151 .209
  Sibs-Ctrl* ABt −.060*** .016 −3.76 <.001 −.092 −.029
Group* Task* ABt −.022*** .002 −8.91 <.001 −.027 −.017
 PAM
  Group* ABt .296*** .015 20.31 <.001 .268 .325
  Probands-Sibs* ABt .604*** .040 14.95 <.001 .525 .684
  Probands-Ctrl* ABt .493*** .030 16.66 <.001 .435 .551
  Sibs-Ctrl* ABt −.112* .046 −2.46 .014 −.201 −.023
 Prize
  Group* ABt −.112*** .013 −8.34 <.001 −.138 −.085
  Probands-Sibs* ABt .041 .026 1.58 .113 −.010 .093
  Probands-Ctrl* ABt −.216*** .026 −8.19 <.001 −.268 −.164
  Sibs-Ctrl* ABt −.258*** .026 −9.87 <.001 −.309 −.206
 Happy Film
  Group* ABt .129*** .012 10.43 <.001 .105 .153
  Probands-Sibs* ABt .231*** .015 15.82 <.001 .202 .260
  Probands-Ctrl* ABt .080*** .020 3.94 <.001 .040 .120
  Sibs-Ctrl* ABt −.151*** .020 −6.88 <.001 −.194 −.108

Note:

*

p<.05,

**

p<.01,

***

p<.001;

CI = confidence interval; LB = lower bound; UB = upper bound; AB = affective behavior; Results are adjusted for the following covariates: Happy: Age, Base PA; Sad: Gender, CDI, MASC, Base PA (included in the final models as appropriate).

Sad behavioral reactivity

For sad behavioral reactivity, a significant group by task interaction indicated that findings varied across the three tasks (B=.01, SE=.002, t=8.13, p<.001). However, post hoc analyses did not reveal any significant within task group effects (ps >.05). See Table 1.

Sad behavior dynamics

In examining sad behavior over time across the three hedonic tasks, a significant group effect was noted (see Table 1). Post hoc analyses indicated that probands and siblings exhibited faster increasing sad behaviors relative to controls across all hedonic tasks, but were indistinguishable from each other. However, variability of sad behavior (MSSD) was unrelated to group status (ps>.05).

Analyses of sad behavior inertia during hedonic tasks yielded an overall group effect. Consistent with hypotheses, post hoc analyses revealed higher inertia among probands relative to both siblings and controls when tasks were combined. Unexpectedly, siblings showed the lowest overall inertia of sad behavior and significantly differed from both probands and controls. There was also a significant group by task interaction, with post hoc analyses revealing that probands showed higher inertia of sad behavior during PAM and the happy film, but lower inertia during prize, relative to controls. Within task analyses revealed that probands were indistinguishable from siblings during prize; however, probands exhibited higher inertia during PAM and happy film compared to siblings. See Table 1.

Discussion

Affective behavioral dynamics may provide an unobtrusive window into affective functioning that may be critical for understanding risk for depression. Our study was the first to examine positive and negative affective behavioral dynamics during hedonic laboratory probes among youth varying in their depression risk status. Overall, this study advances the field in four distinct ways.

First, our findings highlight sources of variation in affective behavioral dysregulation of happy behaviors. Curiously, despite prior findings that youth with remitted depression exhibit a dampened experiential response to positive stimuli (e.g., Kovacs, Bylsma, et al, 2015) they appeared very much like their healthy peers in their overall happy behavioral reactivity. However, novel findings highlighted more subtle alterations in behavioral dynamics. For example, we found evidence of a more rapid cessation of happy behaviors during a positive task for probands, which supports previous evidence of positive experiential dysregulation in remitted youth (Kovacs, Bylsma, et al., 2015). Our findings suggest that, indeed, a focus on the magnitude of emotional response alone may lead to misleading conclusions (e.g., Bloch et al, 2013; Schepman et al, 2012).

Second, while dysphoric affect has been the focus of studies investigating mood repair (probands reported deficient mood repair during PAM; Kovacs, Yaroslavsky, et al 2015) our examination of sad behavioral dynamics during multiple hedonic laboratory tasks is novel. Overall, we found that remitted youth exhibited inert sad behaviors that tended to persist over the course of our hedonic tasks. This pattern of rapid cessation of happy behaviors and high inertia of sad behaviors during positive contexts is consistent with prior evidence of negative affect inflexibility (Kuppens et al, 2012) and emotion regulation deficits (Kuppens et al, 2010; Bylsma et al., 2016) in previously depressed youth and fits well with models of depression that feature context insensitivity (Rottenberg et al, 2005). Findings also highlight the importance of examining both negative and positive affective dynamics to hedonic stimuli.

Third, our findings also highlighted variation among the tasks we employed, which indicates the importance of including multiple affective laboratory stimuli of varying strengths. Specifically, probands exhibited the lowest inertia of sad behavior relative to both controls and siblings during the prize while exhibiting greater inertia during the PAM and humorous film. Notably, while our manipulation checks and prior work (Kovacs, Bylsma, et al., 2016) have demonstrated that all our hedonic probes elicited significant increases in self-reported positive affect and behavior, the prize was the most potent task overall. Given that emotional expressive behaviors have been posited to trigger emotional experiences as much as communicate emotions (e.g., Eckman, 1997; Gruber & Keltner, 2007;), it is possible that our findings may be highlighting differences in how depression impacts emotional experiences versus emotion communication among youth with remitted depression. Specifically, probands appeared behaviorally more like healthy youth during laboratory tasks, possibly through interpersonal cues and intention to communicate positive emotions (while experiencing dampened positive emotions, Kovacs, Bylsma, et al, 2015). Alternatively, null results for some tasks may signal that certain aspects of hedonic functioning remain intact, such as brief enthusiasm to self-relevant rewards, especially given the brevity of the prize task. It may be that youth who are at greater risk for depression show more deficits in their positive affective dynamics for milder positive experiences, but are still able to experience positive affect for stronger hedonic probes. If they are less likely to experience such positive reactions, it may be that when they do occur they exhibit even stronger reactions relative to their peers. Indeed, there is some evidence that depressed adults experience greater decreases in negative affect in response to self-identified positive daily life events (Bylsma, Taylor-Clift & Rottenberg, 2011).

Fourth, our investigation of high-risk (but yet unaffected) siblings of youth with remitted depression was an important and novel feature of the current study. Siblings appeared much like the controls when investigating happy behavioral dynamics. Specifically, while we found that happy behaviors across tasks were indistinguishable from those of controls, siblings exhibited lower inertia of sad behaviors across tasks. Given our expectations that high-risk siblings would show some deficits, the observed patterns of behavioral dynamics of siblings relative to those of the remitted youth were surprising. Prior studies often report low or absent positive behaviors among infants or children at high familial risk for depression (due to a parental history of depression; e.g., Jones et al., 2001). However, our approach was distinct from prior studies in that we directly compared family members with and without a depression history. One possible explanation is that siblings also reported the highest current levels of anxiety symptoms, although we attempted to address this by covarying current anxiety symptoms in the analyses. While anxiety has been proposed as prodromal to depression in at risk youth (Kovacs, Gatsonis, Paulauskas, & Richards, 1989), recent work provides more insight into potential mechanisms, such that increased reactivity among youth with anxiety symptoms appeared to be protective (Morris, Bylsma, Yaroslavsky, Kovacs, & Rottenberg, 2015). Indeed, it may also be that the siblings represent a particularly resilient group, as they have not yet been affected by depression despite their increased risk. However, given heightened risk for depression later in life among youth with familial depression, continued monitoring of depression over time, especially given observed heightened depressive and anxiety symptoms among siblings, is warranted2.

In sum, depression risk was associated with alterations in affective behavioral dynamics. This was especially notable among remitted depressed youth and, to a lower extent, among unaffected siblings who also exhibited some possibly protective features. These findings are consistent with the idea that depression risk involves a loss of behavioral flexibility critical for psychological health (Houben, et al., 2015; Kashdan & Rottenberg, 2010).

Our current findings also highlight that the idea that depression is associated with reduced positive reactivity, as predicted by motivational (Gray, 1994) or emotion theories of depression (ECI; Rottenberg, 2005, 2017), may need to be to be qualified. It seems that in certain contexts, such as brief highly motivating rewards, high risk youth actually exhibit intact behavioral responses. This indicates that these youth may retain some capacity for adaptive hedonic functioning, which is consistent with naturalistic investigations of emotional reactivity showing evidence for a “mood brightening” effect characterized by large decreases in negative affect among depressed individuals in response to positive events (e.g., Bylsma, et al, 2011). Other affective scientists have also emphasized the important role of context in emotional reactivity and regulation. For example, Gross’ (2015) extended process model acknowledges the importance of temporal dynamics and context sensitivity in the development of emotional processes, such as hedonic functioning. Furthermore, Hollenstein and colleagues’ (2013) model of socioemotional flexibility highlights the importance of both proximal and distal contextual factors (rapid changes versus developmental changes) in affective functioning. Our findings reflect this potentially dynamic interplay between internal and external contextual factors; given that high risk youth showed larger changes in both positive and negative emotional behaviors during intense and brief reward contexts, which were not sustained during longer tasks, may reflect unsuccessful deployment of emotion regulation strategies over time (see Rottenberg, 2017 for a review). Future research should utilize multiple methods with high temporal precision longitudinally in order to further clarify how hedonic processes are altered in depression risk and how these processes change across development and with the onset of new episodes of depression, possibly guided by well-established models (e.g., Gross, 2015; Hollenstein et al., 2013).

Supplementary Material

1

Acknowledgments

We would like to thank: Dr. Peter Kuppens for feedback on an earlier draft and for consulting on approach to statistical analyses; Ena Begovic for help with data management. The study described in this article was funded by NIH grant MH-084938 and MH-056193 and Hungarian Scientific Research Fund Grant NN85285. The contents of this publication do not represent the views of the Department of Veterans Affairs or the United States Government.

Footnotes

1

Given that MSSD is composed of both variance and inertia (e.g., Jahng, Woods and Trull, 2008), we also conducted parallel analyses for variance, but there were no significant group effects.

2

Although out study speaks to mechanisms that are at play in the development and maintenance of depression, our design did not permit us to directly investigate the proximal risk mechanisms. For example, temperament is one variable that could explain restricted emotional behavioral dynamics and continued experience of low PA, even beyond depression remission. Indeed, in prior work, low positive emotionality predicted high depressive symptoms over time in young children (Dougherty, Klein, Durbin, Hayden, & Olino, 2010). Although our study did not assess the temperament of the youth, our youth with remitted depression reported the lowest baseline PA, potentially consistent with this explanation.

References

  1. American Psychiatric Association. DSM IV-TR. American Psychiatric Association; 2000. [Google Scholar]
  2. Bylsma LM, Yaroslavsky I, Rottenberg J, Kiss E, Kapornai K, Halas K, … Kovacs M. Familiality of mood repair responses among youth with and without histories of depression. Cognition and Emotion. 2016;30(4):807–816. doi: 10.1080/02699931.2015.1025707. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Clark LA, Watson D, Mineka S. Temperament, personality, and the mood and anxiety disorders. Journal of abnormal psychology. 1994;103(1):103. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Compas BE, Connor-Smith J, Jaser SS. Temperament, stress reactivity, and coping: Implications for depression in childhood and adolescence. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology. 2004;33(1):21–31. doi: 10.1207/S15374424JCCP3301_3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Dougherty LR, Klein DN, Durbin CE, Hayden EP, Olino TM. Temperamental positive and negative emotionality and children’s depressive symptoms: A longitudinal prospective study from age three to age ten. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology. 2010;29(4):462. [Google Scholar]
  6. Ekman P, Friesen WV. Unmasking the face: A guide to recognizing emotions from facial cues. 1975. [Google Scholar]
  7. Ekman P, Rosenberg EL, editors. What the face reveals: Basic and applied studies of spontaneous expression using the Facial Action Coding System (FACS) Oxford University Press; USA: 1997. [Google Scholar]
  8. Forbes EE, Shaw DS, Dahl RE. Alterations in reward-related decision making in boys with recent and future depression. Biological psychiatry. 2007;61(5):633–639. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2006.05.026. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Gross JJ, Levenson RW. Emotional suppression: physiology, self-report, and expressive behavior. Journal of personality and social psychology. 1993;64(6):970. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.64.6.970. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Gray JA. Three fundamental emotion systems. In: Ekman P, Davidons RJ, editors. The Nature of Emotion: Fundamental Questions. Oxford University Press; New York: 1994. pp. 243–247. [Google Scholar]
  11. Gross JJ. The extended process model of emotion regulation: Elaborations, applications, and future directions. Psychological Inquiry. 2015;26:130–137. doi: 10.1080/1047840X.2015.989751. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  12. Gross JJ, Levenson RW. Emotional suppression: physiology, self-report, and expressive behavior. Journal of personality and social psychology. 1993;64(6):970. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.64.6.970. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Gruber J, Keltner D. Emotional Behavior and Psychopathology: A Survey of Methods and Concepts. 2007. [Google Scholar]
  14. Hankin BL. Depression from childhood through adolescence: Risk mechanisms across multiple systems and levels of analysis. Current opinion in psychology. 2015;4:13–20. doi: 10.1016/j.copsyc.2015.01.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Hankin BL, Wetter EK, Flory K. Appetitive motivation and negative emotion reactivity among remitted depressed youth. Journal of Clinical Child & Adolescent Psychology. 2012;41(5):611–620. doi: 10.1080/15374416.2012.710162. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Hollenstein T, Anna Lichtwarck-Aschoff A, Potworowski G. A Model of Socioemotional Flexibility at Three Time Scales. Emotion Review. 2013;5(4):397–405. [Google Scholar]
  17. Houben M, Van Den Noortgate W, Kuppens P. The relation between short-term emotion dynamics and psychological well-being: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin. 2015;141(4):901. doi: 10.1037/a0038822. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Jones NA, Field T, Hart S, Lundy B, Davalos M. Maternal self-perceptions and reactions to infant crying among intrusive and withdrawn depressed mothers. Infant Mental Health Journal. 2001;22(5):576–586. [Google Scholar]
  19. Joormann J, Talbot L, Gotlib IH. Biased processing of emotional information in girls at risk for depression. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 2007;116:135–143. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.116.1.135. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Kashdan TB, Rottenberg J. Psychological flexibility as a fundamental aspect of health. Clinical Psychology Review. 2010;30(7):865–878. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2010.03.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Kellough JL, Beevers CG, Ellis AJ, Wells TT. Time course of selective attention in clinically depressed young adults: An eye tracking study. Behaviour Research and Therapy. 2008;46(11):1238–1243. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2008.07.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Kiss E, Gentzler AM, George C, Kapornai K, Tamás Z, Kovacs M, Vetró Á. Factors influencing mother–child reports of depressive symptoms and agreement among clinically referred depressed youngsters in Hungary. Journal of affective disorders. 2007;100(1):143–151. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2006.10.008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Kovacs M. The Interview Schedule for Children (ISC) Psychopharmacology Bulletin. 1985;21(4):991–994. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Kovacs M, Bylsma LM, Yaroslavsky I, Rottenberg J, George CJ, Kiss E, … Kapornai K. Positive affectivity is dampened in youths with histories of major depression and their never-depressed adolescent siblings. Clinical Psychological Science. 2016;4(4):661–674. doi: 10.1177/2167702615607182. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Kovacs M, Feinberg TL, Crouse-Novak MA, Paulauskas SL, Finkelstein R. Depressive disorders in childhood: I. A longitudinal prospective study of characteristics and recovery. Archives of General Psychiatry. 1984;41(3):229–237. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1984.01790140019002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Kovacs M, Gatsonis C, Paulauskas SL, Richards C. Depressive disorders in childhood. IV. A longitudinal study of comorbidity with and risk for anxiety disorders. Archives of General Psychiatry. 1989;46:776–782. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1989.01810090018003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Kovacs M, Staff MHS. Children’s Depression Inventory 2nd Edition (CDI 2): Technical manual. North Tonawanda, NY: Multi-Health Systems; 2011. [Google Scholar]
  28. Kovacs M, Yaroslavsky I, Rottenberg J, George CJ, Baji I, Benák I, … Kapornai K. Mood repair via attention refocusing or recall of positive autobiographical memories by adolescents with pediatric-onset major depression. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 2015;56(10):1108–1117. doi: 10.1111/jcpp.12376. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Koval P, Butler E, Hollenstein T, Lanteigne D, Kuppens P. Effects of cognitive reappraisal and expressive suppression on emotional inertia. 2013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Koval P, Kuppens P, Allen NB, Sheeber L. Getting stuck in depression: The roles of rumination and emotional inertia. Cognition & emotion. 2012;26(8):1412–1427. doi: 10.1080/02699931.2012.667392. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Kuppens P, Allen NB, Sheeber LB. Emotional inertia and psychological maladjustment. Psychological Science. 2010 doi: 10.1177/0956797610372634. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Kuppens P, Sheeber LB, Yap MB, Whittle S, Simmons JG, Allen NB. Emotional inertia prospectively predicts the onset of depressive disorder in adolescence. Emotion. 2012;12(2):283. doi: 10.1037/a0025046. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. March JS, Parker JDA, Sullivan K, Stallings P, Conners C. The Multidimensional Anxiety Scale for Children (MASC): Factor structure, reliability, and validity. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry. 1997;36(4):554–565. doi: 10.1097/00004583-199704000-00019. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Maziade MRMA, Roy MA, Fournier JP, Cliche D, Mérette C, Caron C, … Nicole L. Reliability of best-estimate diagnosis in genetic linkage studies of major psychoses: results from the Quebec pedigree studies. American Journal of Psychiatry. 1992;149:1674–1674. doi: 10.1176/ajp.149.12.1674. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Morris B, Bylsma L, Yaroslavsky I, Kovacs M, Rottenberg J. Reward learning in pediatric depression and anxiety: Preliminary findings in a high-risk sample. Depression and Anxiety. 2015;32:373–381. doi: 10.1002/da.22358. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Rawal A, Collishaw S, Thapar A, Rice F. A direct method of assessing underlying cognitive risk for adolescent depression. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology. 2013;41(8):1279–1288. doi: 10.1007/s10802-013-9760-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Schepman K, Taylor E, Collishaw S, Fombonne E. Face emotion processing in depressed children and adolescents with and without comorbid conduct disorder. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology. 2012;40(4):583–593. doi: 10.1007/s10802-011-9587-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Sheeber LB, Kuppens P, Shortt JW, Katz LF, Davis B, Allen NB. Depression is associated with the escalation of adolescents’ dysphoric behavior during interactions with parents. Emotion. 2012;12(5):913. doi: 10.1037/a0025784. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Tabachnick BG, Fidell LS. Using Multivariate Statistics. 6. Boston: Pearson; 2013. [Google Scholar]
  40. Tamás Z, Kovacs M, Gentzler AL, Tepper P, Gádoros J, Kiss E, Vetró Á. The relations of temperament and emotion self-regulation with suicidal behaviors in a clinical sample of depressed children in Hungary. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology. 2007;35(4):640–652. doi: 10.1007/s10802-007-9119-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

1

RESOURCES