Skip to main content
i-Perception logoLink to i-Perception
. 2018 Aug 21;9(4):2041669518765852. doi: 10.1177/2041669518765852

The Role of the Pupil, Corneal Reflex, and Iris in Determining the Perceived Direction of Gaze

Stuart Anstis 1,
PMCID: PMC6158516  PMID: 30275943

Abstract

In specially constructed movies depicting moving eyes, the pupils, irises, and corneal reflexes moved independently and sometimes in opposite directions. We found that the moving pupils or the corneal reflex, not the moving irises, determined the perceived direction of gaze (online Movie 1). When the pupils and irises moved in opposite directions, the one with the higher Michelson contrast determined the perceived direction of gaze (online Movie 2).

Keywords: iris, pupil, gaze, induced movement, eye movements, local motion


It is of social value to be able to judge the direction of another person’s gaze; a common everyday question is “Is that person looking at me?” And we are good at making such judgments (Gamer & Hecht, 2007; Kobayashi & Kohshima, 1997), although systematic errors can occur (Anstis, Mayhew, & Morley, 1969; George & Conty, 2008; Gibson & Pick, 1963). We wondered what visible parts of another person’s eye are used in judging where they are looking. These visible parts include the white sclera, the colored iris, and the black hole of the pupil. The small white corneal reflex is not a part of the eye but is a reduced reflection of the illumination.

In all previous publications on the judgments of eye movements, the pupil and the iris have moved in lockstep (as in real life), with a stationary corneal reflex. But our artificial movies teased apart the components of the moving eye that drove judgments of eye direction, by uncoupling the pupil, iris, and corneal reflex and allowing them to move independently in different directions, as never happens in real life. (The movements were kept small enough that the pupil remained within the iris.) Online Movie 1 shows six stimulus conditions. Fifteen naive observers were asked in which direction the eyes were moving in each condition, as the eyes swung back and forth horizontally. To facilitate reports, two small target letters oscillated back and forth in opposite directions above the eyes. Observers simply reported their percepts in the six conditions by writing down a string of six letters. For the observers, all target letters were the same size, but in online Movie 1, the winning letters have been enlarged. Labels, not visible to the observers, have also been added to each condition. Table 1 shows the results.

Table 1.

Movements of pupil, iris and reflex drive perceived directions of eye movements.

No. Pupil Iris Reflex Perceived direction Remarks %
1 x A Control 100
2 x x D Pupils move and win 87
3 X ←→? Irises move: Induced pupils sometimes win 60:40
4 X x H Corneal reflexes move and win 93
5 X x x J Eyelids move: Eyes seem to move in opposite direction. 80
6 x M No corneal reflex. Seen veridically 87

Table 1 shows that 80% to 100% of the observers reported that the perceived directions of eye movements in Conditions 1 to 6 were the letters A D? H J M. Condition 1 is a control condition in which the pupil and iris move together, as in real life. All observers veridically reported “A.” In Condition 2, only the pupils moved and drove the perceived eye movement direction (“D”). Only in Condition 3 (only irises moving) did the observers substantially disagree. The eyes appeared to move with the irises for 60% of the observers but with the induced movement (Anstis & Casco 2006; Leveille & Yazdanbakhsh, 2017) of the pupils for the other 40%.

Condition 4 (stationary pupil and iris, moving corneal reflex) gave quite a convincing impression of the whole eye moving (“H”).

In Condition 5 (eyelids moving), induced movement made the stationary pupil plus iris appear to move in the opposite direction (“J”).

Condition 6 was the same as the control Condition 1 but with the corneal reflex removed. The eyes now looked spooky and dead, but the percepts were still veridical (“M”).

Effects of contrast. In Condition 3, the observers split between attending to the real motion of the iris and to the induced motion of the pupil. So we now moved the irises and pupils back and forth in opposite directions at 0.5 Hz in online Movie 2 and again asked the observers to report the apparent direction of the eye movements. We now systematically varied the relative contrast of the iris and the pupil by varying the luminance of the achromatic iris. A method of constant stimuli yielded the point of subjective equality, at which the iris and pupil were equally likely to drive the perceived direction of eye movements. For three practiced observers, these points of subjective equality occurred when the ratios of the Michelson contrasts of the pupil/iris and the sclera/iris borders were 1.02, 1.08, and 1.08. These ratios are all close to unity, showing that whichever had the higher contrast, the pupil or the iris, drove the perceived eye movement. Nothing else mattered; for instance, although the small size of the pupils might have imparted induced movement into them (Anstis & Casco, 2006; Leveille & Yazdanbakhsh, 2017), this did not change judgments of eye movement direction.

Face eccentricity affects perceived gaze direction (Todorović, 2009). Dissociating pupil and iris now introduces a second layer of eccentricity. Thus, in Figure 1(a) (iris shifted left and pupil centered), gaze appears slightly shifted in the opposite direction, that is, pupil–iris, signaling gaze-right, dominates pupil–socket that signals gaze-straight-ahead. In Figure 1(b) (pupil shifted left and iris centered), gaze appears shifted further to the left. Pupil–socket and pupil–iris relations both indicate leftward gaze, and only iris-socket relation indicates straight gaze. In Figure 1(c) (pupil and iris both shifted left), gaze appears shifted strongly left. In this nested system, the pupil is shifted left with respect to iris, and both are shifted left with respect to socket. In sum, perceived gaze direction = pupil–socket eccentricity + pupil–iris eccentricity + iris–socket eccentricity.

Figure 1.

Figure 1.

Gaze appears to be shifted (a) slightly right, (b) left, and (c) strongly left (see text).

Movie 1.

Download video file (6.1MB, mov)
DOI: 10.1177/2041669518765852.M1

Click to play.

Movie 2.

Download video file (2.8MB, mov)
DOI: 10.1177/2041669518765852.M2

Dark iris: Iris wins. Light iris: Pupil wins.

Conclusion

When we artificially separated the movements of pupils and irises, the direction of gaze depended upon the pupils or the corneal reflex, not the irises. When the pupils and irises artificially moved in opposite directions, the one with the higher Michelson contrast won out.

Declaration of Conflicting Interests

The author(s) declared no potential conflicts of interest with respect to the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article.

Funding

The author(s) disclosed receipt of the following financial support for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article: This study was supported by a grant from the University of California, San Diego Department of Psychology.

References

  1. Anstis S., Casco C. (2006) Induced movement: The flying bluebottle illusion. Journal of Vision 6: 1087–1092. doi:10.1167/6.10.8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Anstis S. M., Mayhew J. W., Morley T. (1969) The perception of where a face or television “portrait” is looking. American Journal of Psychology 82: 474–489. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Gamer M., Hecht H. (2007) Are you looking at me? Measuring the cone of gaze. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 33: 705–715. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. George N., Conty L. (2008) Facing the gaze of others. Clinical Neurophysiology 38: 197–207. doi:10.1016/j.neucli.2008.03.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Gibson J. J., Pick A. D. (1963) Perception of another person’s looking behavior. American Journal of Psychology 76: 386–394. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Kobayashi H., Kohshima S. (1997) Unique morphology of the human eye. Nature 387: 767–768. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Leveille J., Yazdanbakhsh A. (2017) Induced motion. In: Shapiro A. G., Todorovic D. (eds) Oxford compendium of visual illusions, Oxford University Press, pp. 512–515. [Google Scholar]
  8. Todorović D. (2009) The effect of face eccentricity on the perception of gaze direction. Perception 38: 109–132. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

How to cite this article

  1. Anstis, S. (2018). The role of the pupil, corneal reflex, and iris in determining the perceived direction of gaze. i-Perception, 9(4), 1–4. doi: 10.1177/2041669518765852. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]

Articles from i-Perception are provided here courtesy of SAGE Publications

RESOURCES