Abstract
Host defences against avian brood parasites are the outcome of well-documented coevolutionary arms races, yet important questions about variation in hosts' antiparasitic response traits remain poorly understood. Why are certain defences employed by some species or individuals and not by others? Here, we propose that understanding variability in and the evolution of host defences can be facilitated by the study of the underlying physiological mechanisms. Specifically, because antiparasitic strategies involve behaviours that have been shown to be hormonally regulated in other contexts, we hypothesize that host responses to brood parasites are likely to be mediated by related endocrine mechanisms. We outline the hallmarks of the endocrine bases of parasite defence-related avian behaviours, review the current understanding of antiparasitic host tactics and propose testable hypotheses about the hormonal mechanisms that may mediate host defences. We consider these mechanisms in a life-history framework and discuss how endocrine factors may shape variation in host defences. By providing a hypothesis-driven mechanistic framework for defences against parasitism, this perspective should stimulate the study of their endocrine bases to enhance our understanding of the intricate arms races in avian host–parasite systems.
Keywords: avian brood parasitism, host defences, hormones
1. Introduction
Obligate avian brood parasites, which lay their eggs into the nests of other species, comprise a little more than 100 species (approx. 1% of birds), but parasitize over 950 (nearly 10%) of host bird species worldwide [1,2]. By definition, brood parasitism has a negative, often severe, effect on host fitness and, as a consequence, hosts often become involved in a coevolutionary battle with their parasites [3,4]. A large proportion of host taxa have evolved a variety of defences to reduce the incidence of parasitism and/or minimize the negative fitness consequences of successful parasitism [5]. These defences are a diverse collection of behavioural, morphological and physiological traits that can be broadly categorized as front-line defences, which minimize the probability of the parasite laying an egg into the host's nest; egg-stage defences, which minimize the probability of hatching of the parasitic egg; and nestling- and fledgling-stage defences, which minimize the fitness consequences of a successfully hatched parasite (reviewed in [4]).
Among the most important evolutionary questions in brood parasite–host biology include why hosts employ some but not other antiparasitic defences; why there are differences in defences between and within host species; how and how fast these defences evolve; and why some species are more successful in preventing parasitism than others through certain defences [3,6,7]. These questions have traditionally been addressed with hypotheses based on the strength of selection, evolutionary time, behavioural repertoire, and morphology [2,8]. For example, the ‘strategy blocking’ hypothesis predicts that success of one defence strategy may weaken the selection for other types of defences [9,10]. On the other hand, the ‘strategy facilitation’ hypothesis suggests that certain host defences may favour evolution of additional antiparasitic behaviours, resulting in multi-component defences [5], while the ‘evolutionary lag’ hypothesis suggests that some defences might not have had time to evolve [11,12]. Egg rejection probability has also been linked to morphology, e.g. selection for small bill sizes may preclude some hosts from ejecting eggs [13]. While these hypotheses explain some of the interspecific variation in host defences, important aspects of the variation in the magnitude and type of host defences remain unexplained [14].
The majority of host defences across front-line, egg and nestling stages appear to employ behaviours and life-history strategies that have been shown to have a strong endocrine basis in other behavioural and life-history contexts that are distinct from host–parasite interactions [15–17]. In this article, we propose that avian host defences against brood parasites are also mediated or directly regulated by major vertebrate hormones (such as testosterone, corticosterone, and prolactin) and argue that studying the hormonal mechanisms may inform the evolution and expression of host defences, as well as individual, sex and species differences in defence type and efficacy.
Hormones have unique properties that make them especially informative in the evolution of behaviour and life histories: hormones are sensitive to the environment, they often regulate more than one phenotype (hormonal pleiotropy), and form interconnected hormonal networks, where secretion of one hormone often affects another hormone [18]. These properties of hormonal signalling may both constrain and facilitate evolution and expression of host defences, and may shape the types of defences we observe in nature. Furthermore, hormones may underlie variation in host defences at the within-individual (plasticity), between-individual and across-species levels, potentially facilitating evolution of plastic defences to fixed responses [19]. A small number of studies have begun to uncover the hormonal responses of avian hosts to brood parasites at the within-species [20–23] and across-species levels [24–26]. To better understand the role that hormones may play in host defence evolution and variation, however, we need an overarching hypotheses-driven framework that generates testable predictions.
Here, we review the hormonal bases of behavioural traits that underlie major aspects of host defences (figure 1). We focus specifically on aggression, maternal behaviour and host offspring behaviour, which underlie the most widespread and well-studied behavioural defences at the front line [2], egg [27] and nestling stages [28]. We propose hypotheses about how hormones may regulate host-specific behaviours (table 1) and outline testable predictions for the patterns of covariation between host defences and hormones that we expect (electronic supplementary material, table S1). We conclude by conceptualizing hormone-mediated host defences in a more general life-history framework.
Figure 1.
Potential hormonal regulation of host defences against avian brood parasitism. Major vertebrate hormones have been shown to regulate specific aspects of behaviour and physiology that are also involved in host defence against brood parasites. CORT, corticosterone; T, testosterone; PROG, progesterone; PRL, prolactin; MT, mesotocin.
Table 1.
Mechanistic hypotheses for the hormonal basis of host defences. (T, testosterone; CORT, corticosterone; PRL, prolactin; MT, mesotocin; PROG, progesterone.)
defence stage | host defence | mechanistic hypothesis |
---|---|---|
front line | vigilance against brood parasites | CORT increases vigilance against brood parasites T decreases vigilance against brood parasites |
chronically high CORT in response to brood parasites has negative effect on host fitness | ||
aggression against brood parasites, blocking parasite access to nest | T and/or PROG regulate aggression towards brood parasites | |
pleiotropic functions of T impose trade-offs between aggressive phenotype and parental care | ||
egg and nestling stages | rejection of parasite eggs and nestlings, reduction of parental care towards parasitic nestlings | PRL, MT mediate response to egg, nestling and/or nest stimuli, and regulate the threshold for egg and nestling rejection and nest abandonment |
CORT and T downregulate host parental behaviour towards own and parasitic eggs and nestlings | ||
egg and nestling stages | host nestlings outcompete parasitic nest-mates | maternally deposited yolk T and CORT increases host offspring ability to compete with a parasitic nestling |
chronically high maternal CORT elevates yolk CORT and decreases host offspring ability to compete with parasitic nestling | ||
T and CORT secreted by host nestlings in response to parasitic nestling increases host offspring ability to compete |
2. Aggression towards brood parasites
One of the most noteable and widespread front-line defences against brood parasitism is the aggressive behaviour by the hosts towards the parasitic adults. Host species often aggressively mob, harass and physically attack adult parasites prior to parasitic egg laying [29,30] and, in some cases, can block parasites' access to the nest cup by sitting on the clutch and pecking at the intruder [31,32].
No study, to date, has investigated mechanisms underlying host aggression towards brood parasites. However, the hormonal control of aggression is well studied in other contexts. In birds, aggression has been most intensively studied in territorial interactions, where in many cases male and female aggression has been shown to be mediated by steroid hormones [15]. Testosterone (T) in peripheral circulation has been shown to have a permissive effect on the expression of aggression towards conspecifics [33], and it positively correlates with individual differences in aggression [34]. The effects of T on aggression may be sex-specific [35], e.g. in some species, aggression in females is instead mediated by progesterone (PROG) [36], which has also been shown to regulate non-territorial maternal aggression in rats [37]. In some cases, differences in aggression may be independent of circulating steroids and are instead explained by differential expression of steroid receptors in the brain [38], or by differences in the expression of aromatase, which converts T into oestradiol in the brain [39]. Finally, in other cases, conspecific aggression may be independent of steroid signalling and may be mediated more locally by serotonin and histamine signalling in the brain [40,41].
It remains unclear, however, if sex steroids regulate aggression in contexts other than those involving conspecific interactions, e.g. aggression against brood parasites or predators [42]. For example, in eastern bluebirds Sialia sialis, circulating T does not explain variation in aggression against heterospecific individuals [43]. In other taxa, however, T positively correlates with aggression against competitors as well as nest predators [44], implying that shared mechanisms may regulate aggression in different situations. Individual differences in aggressive behaviours across contexts are often positively correlated in many lineages, including birds [45,46]. This suggests that animals show a generalized aggression ‘behavioural syndrome’, and that aggression towards brood parasites may covary with territorial aggression. Behavioural syndromes are hypothesized to arise owing to shared mechanisms [42], therefore at least a partial overlap may exist between the mechanisms that control aggression towards brood parasites and aggression in conspecific contexts.
If T mediates aggression towards brood parasites, then individuals with higher T levels, or individuals with higher expression of aromatase and androgen or oestrogen receptors in the brain areas associated with aggression [47], should be more aggressive towards brood parasites. Alternatively, higher aggression towards brood parasites may be associated with lower PROG levels. Exposure to brood parasites is predicted to increase T in parasitized host individuals, and populations or species with higher brood parasitism levels should show higher T levels (or higher sensitivity to androgens, the brain areas associated with aggression). Supporting these predictions, Hahn et al. [22], in a first study of its kind, showed that eggs from parasitized nests had higher T levels relative to eggs from non-parasitized nests, suggesting that host mothers increase T in response to brood parasite sighting or recognizing parasitic eggs in their nests.
The regulation of aggression against brood parasites by sex steroids may have important consequences to host life histories. For example, individuals with higher T levels have been shown to invest less in parental care [48]. If aggression against brood parasites is mediated by circulating T, then this predicts that brood parasite hosts which are more aggressive should also invest less in parental care. This could lead to an interesting trade-off in mate choice, where females can choose between males who are good at defending nests against brood parasites, but are poor fathers, and males who are good fathers but are poor at nest defence.
Reduction in parental care may result in lower attachment to own eggs and offspring. If so, more aggressive individuals, or more territorial species, should be more likely to eject parasite eggs but perhaps also to mistakenly eject their own eggs compared with less aggressive individuals. Conversely, species or individuals with higher parental investment, which should have lower T [49], should be less aggressive towards brood parasites, and should more likely be an egg- and/or chick-acceptor species. Because aggression is probably regulated differently in males and females, these predictions are more likely to be observed within but not among sexes: whereas females have lower T, they are also often more aggressive than males against brood parasites [32].
These predictions assume that differences in aggression are regulated by circulating hormones, but it is important to note that that is not always the case [38]. However, some of these trade-offs may occur irrespective of the hormonal mechanisms, e.g. these behaviours may be traded-off in time. Furthermore, even if T does not regulate aggression against brood parasites, it may nevertheless affect host defences. For example, high T may lead to reduced vigilance against predators [50], and T-dependent behaviours, such as conspecific territorial aggression, may increase the probability of detection by brood parasites [51]. This predicts that high T individuals may actually experience higher levels of brood parasitism compared with low T individuals, contrary to the T-mediated aggression hypothesis that predicts the opposite pattern.
From a broader perspective, the behavioural syndrome hypothesis predicts that individuals from more highly territorial host species should be more aggressive towards brood parasites than group-living or colonial birds, and that species which are more aggressive against predators should be also more aggressive against brood parasites. Importantly, the former prediction is contradicted by studies which demonstrate that colonially and cooperatively breeding birds show greater aggression through more intense mobbing towards brood parasites [52,53] and by findings which show that more polygynous males are less aggressive against brood parasites [54]. This suggests that aggression against brood parasites and conspecifics may be disassociated, which may happen even if these behaviours are regulated by similar hormonal mechanisms (e.g. owing to changes in hormone receptor expression in the brain areas responsible for these behaviours [38]).
Context-general hormonally mediated aggression may also have important implications for the evolution of host aggression against brood parasites. For example, aggression against brood parasites may have evolved by co-opting the mechanisms of aggression against predators. Indeed, it is widely known that many brood parasites remove host eggs, resulting in partial predation of their clutches [3]. Host response to brood parasites may have evolved from a generalized response to a nest predator to a unique host defence against specific brood parasites, as shown in yellow warblers Setophaga petechia that show different defence strategies against brood parasites versus nest predators [32]. Unique behavioural responses to brood parasites according to this hypothesis may be more likely to arise in species that already possess the ability to differentiate between different predators or species where predator recognition is learned.
Another important hormone that may play a role in host nest defence against brood parasites is corticosterone (CORT), a hormone that is secreted in response to environmental stressors [17]. CORT has been shown to be positively correlated with vigilance behaviour [55,56], which is an important aspect of host defence against brood parasites [57,58]. Avian hosts that have been parasitized by a brood parasite show higher CORT levels [21]. Although it is as of yet unclear whether CORT increases in response to brood parasitism or an encounter with a brood parasite, increase in CORT in response the presence of brood parasites in the vicinity may cause host individuals be more vigilant against parasite attempts to access the nests, potentially leading to lower brood parasitism rates.
3. Maternal behaviour
If parasites succeed in laying an egg (or eggs) in a host's nest, many host species can recognize the foreign eggs and reject them [59,60]. In addition to rejecting eggs, hosts may also abandon a parasitized nest altogether, building a new nest elsewhere [11] or directly on top of the parasitized nest [60]. Some host species have also been shown to eject parasite nestlings upon hatching [61], or feed these nestlings less compared with their biological offspring [62,63].
Nest desertion and egg and nestling ejection are a stunning reversal from typical parental behaviour, specifically incubation and nestling provisioning, with the valence of typically associative stimuli (nests, eggs and nestlings) becoming negative. Maternal behaviour is strongly mediated by hormones [16], suggesting that hormones may also play a role in rejection of parasitic eggs or offspring. A limitation to this hormone-dependent maternal behaviour hypothesis is that systemic changes in hormone secretion are more likely to result in changes in behavioural and perceptual thresholds as opposed to directly causing an immediate response in response to specific stimuli. However, the time course of hosts' responses to parasitic eggs or nestlings can be as long as several days [64], suggesting that endocrine mechanisms may be involved in priming the neural mechanisms that control differential response of hosts to own and parasitic offspring.
The most likely hormones that may mediate the propensity of hosts to eject parasitic eggs or abandon their nests are those that regulate associative behaviours towards own offspring, such as incubation and maternal provisioning. In birds, prolactin (PRL) has been shown to regulate both of these behaviours and thus may mediate maternal responses to parasitic eggs or offspring. PRL plays an important role in the initiation and maintenance of incubation in many bird species [65,66]. In some, but not all, species, variation in PRL levels also correlates positively with parental behaviour during the nestling stage—individuals with higher PRL levels have higher offspring provisioning rates [67,68], and exposure to own young in birds increases PRL signalling in the hypothalamus [69]. An experimental increase in PRL can induce parental provisioning behaviour and reduce aggression towards nestlings, whereas disrupting PRL signalling has the opposite effect [70,71]. These results suggest that PRL may increase the sensitivity of individuals to associative egg or nestling stimuli, allowing the induction of parental behaviour towards offspring.
Another hormone that has been shown to play an important role in regulating maternal behaviour in birds is mesotocin (MT), an avian homologue to mammalian oxytocin. Inhibition of MT signalling in the brain results in reduction of brooding behaviour in chickens, and incubating chickens have a larger number of active MT neurons in the hypothalamus [72]. Exposure to young induces rapid activation of hypothalamic areas that contain MT-producing neurons in chickens [72].
While PRL and MT are not likely to directly regulate parasitic egg or offspring ejection, they may mediate behavioural and perceptual thresholds for parasitic egg or offspring rejection, or changes in maternal behaviour affecting the whole clutch (e.g. overall reduction in maternal behaviour or nest abandonment). PRL levels are known to decrease in response to egg removal [73] and in response to stressors [74]; therefore, sighting of a brood parasite near the nest, or viewing a poorly mimetic parasitic egg or nestling in one's own nest, may result in a drop in PRL or MT levels. If PRL or MT mediates associative responses to eggs, this may result in nest abandonment or reduce attachment to eggs in the nest. A lower attachment to eggs may effectively reduce the rejection threshold of perceived differences between own and parasitic eggs (or chicks) [75], resulting in a higher probability of egg (or chick) rejection. The hypothesis predicts that individuals that have higher PRL of MT levels (or decrease PRL or MT less in response to brood parasites) are less likely to abandon nests or eject parasitic eggs and nestlings compared with individuals with lower PRL or MT levels. Another prediction is that nest abandonment or egg rejection is less likely to occur during periods when PRL or MT levels are highest. This may explain why egg rejection most often occurs during egg laying (e.g. [27]) when PRL levels are lower compared with incubation [16]. Individual- or species-level variation in nest abandonment or egg and nestling rejection may also be owing to differential expression of PRL or MT receptors—individuals and species that have higher expression of PRL or MT receptors in the preoptic area and other hypothalamic nuclei should be more likely to abandon nests or eject eggs and nestlings. PRL and MT may also mediate provisioning of nestlings by hosts: recognition of parasitic nestlings in the nest may result in lower PRL or MT levels, resulting in lower provisioning rates per capita.
Changes in host parental behaviour may also be mediated by hormones that typically inhibit parental behaviour, such as T and CORT. As outlined above, high T levels can have a negative effect on parental care. For example, T reduces nestling provisioning behaviour in males [76,77] and incubation behaviour in females [78]. CORT has also been shown to reduce nestling provisioning [79,80]. Host adults that have been naturally parasitized show higher CORT [21] and, probably, higher T levels [22], which may result in a reduction in an associative response to own nests, eggs, of offspring, potentially leading the nest abandonment, increase in egg or nestling rejection probability or/and overall reduction of offspring provisioning.
While we argue that hormones may mediate egg and nestling rejection, hormones, including PRL and MT, may also be the reason why these behaviours have not evolved in all avian brood parasite hosts. Egg and nestling rejection require a fundamental departure from hormonally regulated parental care and attachment to eggs and young, which may make it more difficult to evolve defences against brood parasitic eggs and offspring and explain why so many species do not eject parasitic eggs despite the severely negative fitness consequences of raising a parasitic offspring.
4. Nestling behaviour and growth rate
In evicting brood parasite species, such as the common cuckoo Cuculus canorus, nest-mates have little chance of successfully competing with the foreign chick because they are often eliminated as eggs or hatchlings [81]. In turn, in non-evicting brood parasite systems, such as brown-headed cowbirds Molothrus ater, the battle between the hosts and parasites is most intense at the nestling phase, when the parasitic nestling competes with host nestlings for parental care [82]. Many hosts have evolved parental solicitation strategies by nestlings to limit the fitness loss owing to feeding the parasitic chick. At the species level, host nestlings from species with higher brood parasitism rates develop on average faster [83] and beg louder [84,85] than nestlings from species with lower parasitism rates. At the within-species level, nestlings from parasitized nests beg louder than those from non-parasitized nests ([86], but see [28]). This shows that brood parasitism can select for changes in the average host nestling phenotype over evolutionary time as well as result in plastic responses to specific parasitism events.
An increasing body of evidence suggests that nestling behavioural and metabolic phenotype can be regulated by maternally deposited yolk hormones [87]. For instance, yolk hormones can influence nestling growth rate: higher levels of yolk T stimulate nestling growth [88], whereas higher levels of CORT can have the opposite effect ([89] but see [90]). Yolk hormones can also have organizational effects on behaviour during the development of the embryo: high yolk T can result in long-term changes in behaviours, such as aggression [91]. Higher T and CORT levels in yolk can result in higher begging rates and intensified begging postures [87]. Importantly, the amount of hormones deposited in yolk is affected by the stress environment, e.g. stressed females have higher yolk CORT [92].
Maternally controlled differences in yolk hormones may underlie differences in the mean behaviour or growth rate of host nestlings across species, or changes in behaviour or growth rate in response to specific parasitism events [22]. At the between-species level, females from frequently parasitized species are predicted to deposit higher levels of T in their eggs compared with less frequently parasitized species, resulting in higher offspring growth rates and higher begging rates; indeed, small cowbird host species that pay a greater cost of competition with the parasitic nestling [82] have higher yolk T levels than do larger host species [25]. In addition to being exposed to higher T levels, embryos of host species may express higher levels of androgen receptors: increased sensitivity to T may lead to increased begging behaviour or growth rates. Yet another, thus far untested possibility is that species with higher parasitism rates may achieve higher begging rates and faster growth by changing the CORT levels in the yolk or the expression or glucocorticoid or mineralocorticoid receptors in the embryonic tissues. Because CORT can have different effects on nestling phenotype in different species, it is possible to predict both positive and negative relationships between CORT and nestling begging rate or growth.
Maternally derived yolk hormones may also mediate more immediate, plastic response to specific parasitic events or to high parasite density in the environment. Hahn et al. [22] showed that host eggs in nests parasitized by cowbirds have higher yolk T content than eggs from non-parasitized nests. They suggest that host females may increase yolk T either in response to observing a brood parasite that is prospecting its nest, or by recognizing a parasitic egg.
In addition to T, females may also be able to rapidly modify CORT levels in yolk in response to perceiving a brood parasite, increasing a future offspring begging rate. If, on the other hand, CORT negatively affects offspring growth, then such a response may result in a maladaptive reduction in host nesting growth rate or begging intensity. Females may also manipulate offspring phenotype via thyroid hormone (TH), which can regulate the growth of embryos or nestlings [93]. We are awaiting studies that experimentally investigate the role of TH on nestling phenotype, especially in the context of competitive growth and begging in broods of frequently parasitized hosts of non-evictor parasites.
In addition to maternally controlled phenotypes, host nestlings in parasitized nests may also respond to the parasitic nestling directly. European magpie Pica pica nestlings have higher CORT levels in nests parasitized by the great spotted cuckoo Clamator glandarius [20]. The authors suggest that increase in CORT might be either owing to the hunger of nestlings caused by increased competition of food, or may be in response to the increased begging displays by the parasitic nestlings. Higher CORT is linked to low body condition in other species [94] and could either increase glucose metabolism [17] or increase begging behaviour in nestlings [87,95], allowing them to become more competitive against parasitic nest-mates. A higher begging rate may also be achieved if nestlings elevated T in response to their parasitic nest-mates [87].
5. Parasites as stressors
While CORT may mediate adaptive responses to brood parasites, chronic elevation of CORT may result in negative fitness consequences [96]. CORT is a hormone that fulfils multiple functions, which may change depending on the levels of CORT in circulation [97] and the expression of its receptors in target tissues [98]. As suggested by Mark & Rubenstein [21], if brood parasites cause long-term elevation in CORT, this may result in negative fitness consequences to host fitness beyond the direct negative effects of brood parasitism. High CORT levels can have negative effects on body condition [99] and suppress immunity [100,101], and suppress growth [17]. If hosts mount a repeated stress response in response to brood parasitism or seeing a brood parasite near their nests, hosts may develop a lower body condition or suppressed immunity, potentially leading to increased ability to escape predators, provision offspring or resist infections. This may result in a negative effect on population growth rate independent of direct parasitism effects.
As shown by Hahn et al. [22], host hormonal responses to brood parasites may depend on the host species morphology or ecology—small hosts that suffer more severe negative fitness consequences of brood parasitism may mount a stronger stress response in response to brood parasites than larger host species, and as a result potentially suffer more negative fitness consequences. Along the same line, hosts that reject parasite eggs may mount a lower stress response than those that do not reject parasite eggs.
6. An integrative perspective
The multifaceted roles of hormones in behaviour and reproductive physiology as well as other organismal and life-history functions (such as self-maintenance, immunity and energetics) provide opportunities for both constraint and facilitation in the evolution and expression of host defences. As mentioned above, mechanisms that regulate maternal behaviour may constrain the evolution of host defences at the egg and nestling stages. On the other hand, the pleiotropic actions of CORT may allow the hosts to develop coordinated and complex defence responses, which may include increased vigilance against parasites, an increase in egg rejection propensity or reduction of feeding behaviour in parasitized nests, and increase in offspring begging rate. However, high CORT levels may also result in adverse effects to host fitness or offspring growth rate via yolk hormones. The effect of hormonal responses to brood parasitic adults or young may extend beyond host defences: high CORT levels may suppress reproduction, the immune system and may impose other somatic costs, reducing the probability of breeding in the future [21,101,102]. Host defences against brood parasites may thus need to be viewed in a more general framework of allostatic load [103].
T can likewise may both facilitate defence strategies as well as impose life-history trade-offs [76]. For example, high yolk T levels may facilitate the ability of the host offspring to compete for food against a brood parasitic nestling, as well as result in higher aggression against brood parasites in adulthood. On the other hand, high T phenotype that is advantageous in response to brood parasitism may be maladaptive in terms of survivorship, immunity and parental care [76].
Furthermore, CORT and T, and PRL, are sensitive to other environmental and social stimuli, such as stress [104] and competition [105]. Changes in hormone levels owing to factors that are not directly related to brood parasitism may nevertheless affect host defences that are mediated by these hormones (figure 1). For example, animals experiencing stressors that elevate CORT levels may show reduced attachment to eggs and nests, resulting in higher nest abandonment or higher propensity to reject eggs in response to parasitism. Understanding variation in host defences may therefore necessitate understanding the physiological state of the animal in response to the overall environment.
7. Future directions
The field of avian brood parasitism is ripe for mechanistic studies that test the endocrine mechanisms underlying host defences. The hypotheses outlined in this paper can be tested by adopting well developed and affordable [106] pharmacological techniques that have been used to experimentally manipulate hormone levels in wild animals in other systems (e.g. conspecific aggression [107]) and combining it with equally fine-tuned experimental techniques for studying host responses to parasites (reviewed in [4]). To understand the endocrine basis of host defences, it is imperative to focus both on hormones as well as their receptors, as variation in both can cause differences in behaviour [38,97,98]: studies on host endocrinology to date have focused mainly on variation in hormone levels, but receptor expression for these hormones in the target tissues may yield equally illuminating findings.
We emphasize that in addition to steroid hormones and PRL, other hormones, such as TH [93], catecholamines [17] and galanin [108], mediate the behavioural and reproductive phenotypes that we have discussed here and thus should be considered in future studies. Furthermore, hormones can act in a paracrine and autocrine fashion [109], and these effects may affect the relationships between peripheral hormones and behaviour. Finally, correlations between hormones, their receptors and behaviours may come about because of hormonal regulation of that behaviour, induction of hormonal changes by the behaviour, or unrelated third factors [110]. We therefore advocate for entertaining alternative hypotheses when interpreting relationships between physiology and host defences.
Lastly, many of the studies on host endocrinology so far have documented species differences in their responses to brood parasites [20,22–25]. Future studies should therefore embrace a comparative approach when investigating hormonal bases of host defences, as the costs and benefits of hormonally mediated phenotypes may differ between species [22].
8. Summary
The study of the arms-race between avian brood parasites and their hosts is a productive model system in behavioural ecology and evolutionary biology. Here, we argue that this system is also ripe for mechanistic studies on the proximal basis of host and parasite behaviours. Specifically, we suggest that understanding the hormonal basis of host defences has the potential to inform major questions in avian brood parasitic systems, such as why hosts employ some but not other defences against brood parasites, why individuals vary in their defence behaviours and how these defences evolve (table 1 and the electronic supplementary material, table S1).
Supplementary Material
Acknowledgements
We thank Sharon Gill, Abigail A. Kimmitt, members of the Hauber laboratory, and the journal's editors and referees for discussions and helpful comments.
Data accessibility
This article has no additional data.
Authors' contributions
M.A.-A. and M.E.H. drafted the manuscript. Both authors gave final approval for publication.
Competing interests
We declare we have no competing interests.
Funding
M.E.H. was funded by the US National Science Foundation (IOS-1456524) and the University of Illinois’ Harvey Jones Van Cleave Professorship.
References
- 1.Johnsgard PA. 1997. The avian brood parasites: deception at the nest. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
- 2.Feeney WE, Welbergen JA, Langmore NE. 2014. Advances in the study of coevolution between avian brood parasites and their hosts. Annu. Rev. Ecol. Evol. Syst. 45, 227–246. ( 10.1146/annurev-ecolsys-120213-091603) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Davies NB. 2000. Cuckoos, cowbirds, and other cheats. London, UK: Poyser. [Google Scholar]
- 4.Soler M. (ed.). 2017. Avian brood parasitism: behaviour, ecology, evolution and coevolution. Berlin, Germany: Springer International Publishing. [Google Scholar]
- 5.Kilner RM, Langmore NE. 2011. Cuckoos versus hosts in insects and birds: adaptations, counter-adaptations and outcomes. Biol. Rev. 86, 836–852. ( 10.1111/j.1469-185X.2010.00173.x) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Krüger O. 2007. Cuckoos, cowbirds and hosts: adaptations, trade-offs and constraints. Phil. Trans. R. Soc. B 362, 1873–1886. ( 10.1098/rstb.2006.1849) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Stoddard MC, Kilner RM. 2013. The past, present and future of ‘cuckoos versus reed warblers'. Anim. Behav. 85, 693–699. ( 10.1016/j.anbehav.2013.01.005) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Soler M. 2014. Long-term coevolution between avian brood parasites and their hosts. Biol. Rev. 89, 688–704. ( 10.1111/brv.12075) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Britton NF, Planqué R, Franks NR. 2007. Evolution of defence portfolios in exploiter–victim systems. Bull. Math. Biol. 69, 957–988. ( 10.1007/s11538-006-9178-5) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Grim T. 2011. Ejecting chick cheats: a changing paradigm? Front. Zool. 8, 14 ( 10.1186/1742-9994-8-14) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Hosoi SA, Rothstein SI. 2000. Nest desertion and cowbird parasitism: evidence for evolved responses and evolutionary lag. Anim. Behav. 59, 823–840. ( 10.1006/anbe.1999.1370) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Takasu F. 1998. Why do all host species not show defense against avian brood parasitism: evolutionary lag or equilibrium? Am. Nat. 151, 193–205. ( 10.1086/286111) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Rohwer S, Spaw CD. 1988. Evolutionary lag versus bill-size constraints: a comparative study of the acceptance of cowbird eggs by old hosts. Evol. Ecol. 2, 27–36. ( 10.1007/BF02071586) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Stokke BG, Moksnes A, Røskaft E. 2005. The enigma of imperfect adaptations in hosts of avian brood parasites. Ornithol. Sci. 4, 17–29. ( 10.2326/osj.4.17) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Maney DL, Goodson JL. 2011. Neurogenomic mechanisms of aggression in songbirds. Adv. Genet. 75, 83–119. ( 10.1016/B978-0-12-380858-5.00002-2) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Williams TD. 2012. Physiological adaptations for breeding in birds. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press. [Google Scholar]
- 17.Romero LM, Butler LK. 2007. Endocrinology of stress. Int. J. Comp. Psychol. 20, 89–95. [Google Scholar]
- 18.Zera AJ, Harshman LG, Williams TD. 2007. Evolutionary endocrinology: the developing synthesis between endocrinology and evolutionary genetics. Annu. Rev. Ecol. Evol. Syst. 38, 793–817. ( 10.1146/annurev.ecolsys.38.091206.095615) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Crispo E. 2007. The Baldwin effect and genetic assimilation: revisiting two mechanisms of evolutionary change mediated by phenotypic plasticity. Evolution 61, 2469–2479. ( 10.1111/j.1558-5646.2007.00203.x) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Ibáñez-Álamo JD, De Neve L, Roldán M, Rodríguez J, Trouvé C, Chastel O, Soler M. 2012. Corticosterone levels in host and parasite nestlings: is brood parasitism a hormonal stressor? Horm. Behav. 61, 590–597. ( 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2012.02.008) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Mark MM, Rubenstein DR. 2013. Physiological costs and carry-over effects of avian interspecific brood parasitism influence reproductive tradeoffs. Horm. Behav. 63, 717–722. ( 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2013.03.008) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Hahn DC, Wingfield JC, Fox DM, Walker BG, Thomley JE. 2017. Maternal androgens in avian brood parasites and their hosts: responses to parasitism and competition? Gen. Comp. Endocrinol. 240, 143–152. ( 10.1016/j.ygcen.2016.10.004) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Ruiz-Raya F, Soler M, Abaurrea T, Chastel O, Roncalli G, Ibáñez-Álamo JD. 2018. Hormonal responses to non-mimetic eggs: is brood parasitism a physiological stressor during incubation? Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 72, 153 ( 10.1007/s00265-018-2565-8) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Hauber ME, Pilz KM. 2003. Yolk testosterone levels are not consistently higher in the eggs of obligate brood parasites than their hosts. Am. Midl. Nat. 149, 354–362. ( 10.1674/0003-0031(2003)149%5B0354:YTLANC%5D2.0.CO;2) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Hahn DC, Hatfield JS, Abdelnabi MA, Wu JM, Igl LD, Ottinger MA. 2005. Inter-species variation in yolk steroid levels and a cowbird-host comparison. J. Avian Biol. 36, 40–46. ( 10.1111/j.0908-8857.2005.03040.x) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Hargitai R, Moskát C, Bán M, Gil D, López-Rull I, Solymos E. 2010. Eggshell characteristics and yolk composition in the common cuckoo Cuculus canorus: are they adapted to brood parasitism? J. Avian Biol. 41, 177–185. ( 10.1111/j.1600-048X.2009.04818.x) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Moskát C, Hauber ME. 2007. Conflict between egg recognition and egg rejection decisions in common cuckoo (Cuculus canorus) hosts. Anim. Cogn. 10, 377–386. ( 10.1007/s10071-007-0071-x) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Hauber ME. 2003. Lower begging responsiveness of host versus parasitic brown-headed cowbird (Molothrus ater) nestlings is related to species identity but not to early social experience. J. Comp. Psychol. 117, 24–30. ( 10.1037/0735-7036.117.1.24) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Welbergen JA, Davies NB. 2009. Strategic variation in mobbing as a front line of defense against brood parasitism. Curr. Biol. 19, 235–240. ( 10.1016/j.cub.2008.12.041) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Medina I, Langmore NE. 2016. Batten down the thatches: front-line defences in an apparently defenceless cuckoo host. Anim. Behav. 112, 195–201. ( 10.1016/j.anbehav.2015.12.006) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Canestrari D, Marcos JM, Baglione V. 2009. Cooperative breeding in carrion crows reduces the rate of brood parasitism by great spotted cuckoos. Anim. Behav. 77, 1337–1344. ( 10.1016/j.anbehav.2009.02.009) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Gill SA, Sealy SG. 1996. Nest defence by yellow warblers: recognition of a brood parasite and an avian nest predator. Behaviour 133, 263–282. ( 10.1163/156853996X00143) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Wingfield JC, Ball GF, Dufty AM Jr, Hegner RE, Ramenofsky M. 1987. Testosterone and aggression in birds. Am. Sci. 75, 602–608. [Google Scholar]
- 34.Johnsen TS. 1998. Behavioural correlates of testosterone and seasonal changes of steroids in red-winged blackbirds. Anim. Behav. 55, 957–965. ( 10.1006/anbe.1997.0642) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Elekonich MM, Wingfield JC. 2006. Seasonality and hormonal control of territorial aggression in female song sparrows (Passeriformes: Emberizidae: Melospiza melodia). Ethology 106, 493–510. ( 10.1046/j.1439-0310.2000.00555.x) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Goymann W, Wittenzellner A, Schwabl I, Makomba M. 2008. Progesterone modulates aggression in sex-role reversed female African black coucals. Proc. R. Soc. B 275, 1053–1060. ( 10.1098/rspb.2007.1707) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Albert DJ, Jonik RH, Walsh ML. 1992. Interaction of estradiol, testosterone, and progesterone in the modulation of hormone-dependent aggression in the female rat. Physiol. Behav. 52, 773–779. ( 10.1016/0031-9384(92)90413-V) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Rosvall KA, Bergeon Burns CM, Barske J, Goodson JL, Schlinger BA, Sengelaub DR, Ketterson ED. 2012. Neural sensitivity to sex steroids predicts individual differences in aggression: implications for behavioural evolution. Proc. R. Soc. B 279, 3547–3555. ( 10.1098/rspb.2012.0442) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Silverin B, Baillien M, Balthazart J. 2004. Territorial aggression, circulating levels of testosterone, and brain aromatase activity in free-living pied flycatchers. Horm. Behav. 45, 225–234. ( 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2003.10.002) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Norton WHJ, Stumpenhorst K, Faus-Kessler T, Folchert A, Rohner N, Harris MP, Callebert J, Bally-Cuif L. 2011. Modulation of Fgfr1a signaling in zebrafish reveals a genetic basis for the aggression-boldness syndrome. J. Neurosci. 31, 13 796–13 807. ( 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2892-11.2011) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Dennis RL, Chen ZQ, Cheng HW. 2008. Serotonergic mediation of aggression in high and low aggressive chicken strains. Poult. Sci. 87, 612–620. ( 10.3382/ps.2007-00389) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Koolhaas JM, de Boer SF, Coppens CM, Buwalda B. 2010. Neuroendocrinology of coping styles: towards understanding the biology of individual variation. Front. Neuroendocrinol. 31, 307–321. ( 10.1016/j.yfrne.2010.04.001) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Ambardar M, Grindstaff JL. 2017. Pre-GnRH and GnRH-induced testosterone levels do not vary across behavioral contexts: a role for individual variation. Gen. Comp. Endocrinol. 246, 51–62. ( 10.1016/j.ygcen.2017.03.009) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.O'Connor CM, Gilmour KM, Kraak GVD, Cooke SJ. 2011. Circulating androgens are influenced by parental nest defense in a wild teleost fish. J. Comp. Physiol. A 197, 711–715. ( 10.1007/s00359-011-0629-6) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Cain KE, Rich MS, Ainsworth K, Ketterson ED. 2011. Two sides of the same coin? Consistency in aggression to conspecifics and predators in a female songbird. Ethology 117, 786–795. ( 10.1111/j.1439-0310.2011.01932.x) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Huntingford FA. 1976. The relationship between anti-predator behaviour and aggression among conspecifics in the three-spined sickleback, Gasterosteus aculeatus. Anim. Behav. 24, 245–260. ( 10.1016/S0003-3472(76)80034-6) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Goodson JL. 2005. The vertebrate social behavior network: evolutionary themes and variations. Horm. Behav. 48, 11–22. ( 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2005.02.003) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.McGlothlin JW, Jawor JM, Ketterson ED. 2007. Natural variation in a testosterone-mediated trade-off between mating effort and parental effort. Am. Nat. 170, 864–875. ( 10.1086/522838) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Van Roo BL, Ketterson ED, Sharp PJ. 2003. Testosterone and prolactin in two songbirds that differ in paternal care: the blue-headed vireo and the red-eyed vireo. Horm. Behav. 44, 435–441. ( 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2003.07.001) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Marler CA, Moore MC. 1988. Evolutionary costs of aggression revealed by testosterone manipulations in free-living male lizards. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 23, 21–26. ( 10.1007/BF00303053) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Avilés JM, Parejo D. 2011. Host personalities and the evolution of behavioural adaptations in brood parasitic–host systems. Anim. Behav. 82, 613–618. ( 10.1016/j.anbehav.2011.07.025) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Feeney WE, Medina I, Somveille M, Heinsohn R, Hall ML, Mulder RA, Stein JA, Kilner RM, Langmore NE. 2013. Brood parasitism and the evolution of cooperative breeding in birds. Science 342, 1506–1508. ( 10.1126/science.1240039) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Brown M, Lawes MJ. 2007. Colony size and nest density predict the likelihood of parasitism in the colonial southern red bishop Euplectes orix—diderick cuckoo Chrysococcyx caprius system. Ibis 149, 321–327. ( 10.1111/j.1474-919X.2006.00633.x) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Trnka A, Prokop P. 2010. Does social mating system influence nest defence behaviour in great reed warbler (Acrocephalus arundinaceus) males? Ethology 116, 1075–1083. ( 10.1111/j.1439-0310.2010.01821.x) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Schoech SJ, Rensel MA, Heiss RS. 2011. Short- and long-term effects of developmental corticosterone exposure on avian physiology, behavioral phenotype, cognition, and fitness: a review. Curr. Zool. 57, 514–530. ( 10.1093/czoolo/57.4.514) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 56.Voellmy IK, Goncalves IB, Barrette M-F, Monfort SL, Manser MB. 2014. Mean fecal glucocorticoid metabolites are associated with vigilance, whereas immediate cortisol levels better reflect acute anti-predator responses in meerkats. Horm. Behav. 66, 759–765. ( 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2014.08.008) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57.Tewksbury JJ, Martin TE, Hejl SJ, Kuehn MJ, Jenkins JW. 2002. Parental care of a cowbird host: caught between the costs of egg-removal and nest predation. Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B 269, 423–429. ( 10.1098/rspb.2001.1894) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58.Davies N, Butchart SH, Burke T, Chaline N, Stewart IR. 2003. Reed warblers guard against cuckoos and cuckoldry. Anim. Behav. 65, 285–295. ( 10.1006/anbe.2003.2049) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 59.Rothstein SI. 1975. Mechanisms of avian egg-recognition: do birds know their own eggs? Anim. Behav. 23, 268–278. ( 10.1016/0003-3472(75)90075-5) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 60.Davies NB, Brooke ML. 1988. Cuckoos versus reed warblers: adaptations and counteradaptations. Anim. Behav. 36, 262–284. ( 10.1016/S0003-3472(88)80269-0) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 61.Sato NJ, Tokue K, Noske RA, Mikami OK, Ueda K. 2010. Evicting cuckoo nestlings from the nest: a new anti-parasitism behaviour. Biol. Lett. 6, 67–69. ( 10.1098/rsbl.2009.0540) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 62.Grim T. 2006. The evolution of nestling discrimination by hosts of parasitic birds: why is rejection so rare? Evol. Ecol. Res. 8, 785–802. [Google Scholar]
- 63.Schuetz JG. 2005. Reduced growth but not survival of chicks with altered gape patterns: implications for the evolution of nestling similarity in a parasitic finch. Anim. Behav. 70, 839–848. ( 10.1016/j.anbehav.2005.01.007) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 64.Antonov A, Stokke BG, Moksnes A, Røskaft E. 2009. Evidence for egg discrimination preceding failed rejection attempts in a small cuckoo host. Biol. Lett. 5, 169–171. ( 10.1098/rsbl.2008.0645) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 65.Sharp PJ, Macnamee MC, Sterling RJ, Lea RW, Pedersen HC. 1988. Relationships between prolactin, LH and broody behavior in bantam hens. J. Endocrinol. 118, 279–286. ( 10.1677/joe.0.1180279) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 66.Buntin JD. 1996. Neural and hormonal control of parental behaviour in birds. Adv. Study Behav. 25, 161–213. ( 10.1016/S0065-3454(08)60333-2) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 67.Schoech SJ, Mumme RL, Wingfield JC. 1996. Prolactin and helping behaviour in the cooperatively breeding Florida scrub-jay, Aphelocoma c. coerulescens. Anim. Behav. 52, 445–456. ( 10.1006/anbe.1996.0189) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 68.Smiley KO, Adkins-Regan E. 2016. Prolactin is related to individual differences in parental behavior and reproductive success in a biparental passerine, the zebra finch (Taeniopygia guttata). Gen. Comp. Endocrinol. 234, 88–94. ( 10.1016/j.ygcen.2016.03.006) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 69.Buntin L, Buntin JD. 2006. Patterns of Fos-like immunoreactivity in the brains of parent ring doves (Streptopelia risoria) given tactile and nontactile exposure to their young. 120, 651–664. ( 10.1037/0735-7044.120.3.651) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 70.Wang Q, Buntin JD. 1999. The roles of stimuli from young, previous breeding experience, and prolactin in regulating parental behavior in ring doves (Streptopelia risoria). Horm. Behav. 35, 241–253. ( 10.1006/hbeh.1999.1517) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 71.Smiley KO, Adkins-Regan E. 2018. Lowering prolactin reduces post-hatch parental care in male and female zebra finches (Taeniopygia guttata). Horm. Behav. 98, 103–114. ( 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2017.12.011) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 72.Thayananuphat A, Youngren OM, Kang SW, Bakken T, Kosonsiriluk S, Chaiseha Y, El Halawani ME. 2011. Dopamine and mesotocin neurotransmission during the transition from incubation to brooding in the turkey. Horm. Behav. 60, 327–335. ( 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2011.06.009) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 73.Bluhm CK, Phillips RE, Burke WH. 1983. Serum levels of luteinizing hormone, prolactin, estradiol and progesterone in laying and nonlaying mallards (Anas platyrhynchos). Biol. Reprod. 28, 295–305. ( 10.1095/biolreprod28.2.295) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 74.Angelier F, Chastel O. 2009. Stress, prolactin and parental investment in birds: a review. Gen. Comp. Endocrinol. 163, 142–148. ( 10.1016/j.ygcen.2009.03.028) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 75.Hauber ME, Moskát C, Bán M. 2006. Experimental shift in hosts' acceptance threshold of inaccurate-mimic brood parasite eggs. Biol. Lett. 2, 177–180. ( 10.1098/rsbl.2005.0438) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 76.Ketterson ED, Nolan VJ, Wolf L, Ziegenfus C. 1992. Testosterone and avian life histories: effects of experimentally elevated testosterone on behavior and correlates of fitness in the dark-eyed junco (Junco hyemalis). Am. Nat. 140, 980–999. ( 10.1086/285451) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 77.Peters A, Cockburn A, Cunningham R. 2002. Testosterone treatment suppresses paternal care in superb fairy-wrens, Malurus cyaneus, despite their concurrent investment in courtship. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 51, 538–547. ( 10.1007/s00265-002-0472-4) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 78.Rosvall KA. 2013. Life history trade-offs and behavioral sensitivity to testosterone: an experimental test when female aggression and maternal care co-occur. PLoS ONE 8, e54120 ( 10.1371/journal.pone.0054120) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 79.Silverin B. 1986. Corticosterone-binding proteins and behavioral effects of high plasma levels of corticosterone during the breeding period in the pied flycatcher. Gen. Comp. Endocrinol. 64, 67–74. ( 10.1016/0016-6480(86)90029-8) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 80.Horton BM, Holberton RL. 2009. Corticosterone manipulations alter morph-specific nestling provisioning behavior in male white-throated sparrows, Zonotrichia albicollis. Horm. Behav. 56, 510–518. ( 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2009.09.001) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 81.Anderson MG, Moskát C, Bán M, Grim T, Cassey P, Hauber ME. 2009. Egg eviction imposes a recoverable cost of virulence in chicks of a brood parasite. PLoS ONE 4, e7725 ( 10.1371/journal.pone.0007725) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 82.Hauber ME. 2003. Hatching asynchrony, nestling competition, and the cost of interspecific brood parasitism. Behav. Ecol. 14, 227–235. ( 10.1093/beheco/14.2.227) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 83.Remes˘ V. 2006. Growth strategies of passerine birds are related to brood parasitism by the brown-headed cowbird (Molothrus ater). Evolution 60, 1692–1700. ( 10.1554/06-170.1) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 84.Boncoraglio G, Saino N, Garamszegi LZ. 2009. Begging and cowbirds: brood parasites make hosts scream louder. Behav. Ecol. 20, 215–221. ( 10.1093/beheco/arn137) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 85.Briskie JV, Naugler CT, Leech SM. 1994. Begging intensity of nestling birds varies with sibling relatedness. Proc. R. Soc. B 258, 73–78. ( 10.1098/rspb.1994.0144) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 86.Pagnucco K, Zanette L, Clinchy M, Leonard ML. 2008. Sheep in wolf's clothing: host nestling vocalizations resemble their cowbird competitor's. Proc. R. Soc. B 275, 1061–1065. ( 10.1098/rspb.2007.1706) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 87.Groothuis TGG, Müller W, von Engelhardt N, Carere C, Eising C. 2005. Maternal hormones as a tool to adjust offspring phenotype in avian species. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 29, 329–352. ( 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2004.12.002) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 88.Andersson S, Uller T, Lõhmus M, Sundström F. 2004. Effects of egg yolk testosterone on growth and immunity in a precocial bird: effects of yolk testosterone in quail. J. Evol. Biol. 17, 501–505. ( 10.1111/j.1420-9101.2004.00706.x) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 89.Hayward LS, Wingfield JC. 2004. Maternal corticosterone is transferred to avian yolk and may alter offspring growth and adult phenotype. Gen. Comp. Endocrinol. 135, 365–371. ( 10.1016/j.ygcen.2003.11.002) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 90.Bowers EK, Bowden RM, Thompson CF, Sakaluk SK. 2016. Elevated corticosterone during egg production elicits increased maternal investment and promotes nestling growth in a wild songbird. Horm. Behav. 83, 6–13. ( 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2016.05.010) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 91.Partecke J, Schwabl H. 2008. Organizational effects of maternal testosterone on reproductive behavior of adult house sparrows. Dev. Neurobiol. 68, 1538–1548. ( 10.1002/dneu.20676) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 92.Saino N, Romano M, Ferrari RP, Martinelli R, Møller AP. 2005. Stressed mothers lay eggs with high corticosterone levels which produce low-quality offspring. J. Exp. Zoolog. A Comp. Exp. Biol. 303A, 998–1006. ( 10.1002/jez.a.224) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 93.Groothuis TG, Schwabl H. 2008. Hormone-mediated maternal effects in birds: mechanisms matter but what do we know of them? Phil. Trans. R. Soc. B 363, 1647–1661. ( 10.1098/rstb.2007.0007) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 94.Quillfeldt P, Masello JF, Strange IJ, Buchanan KL. 2006. Begging and provisioning of thin-billed prions, Pachyptila belcheri, are related to testosterone and corticosterone. Anim. Behav. 71, 1359–1369. ( 10.1016/j.anbehav.2005.09.015) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 95.Kitaysky AS, Wingfield JC, Piatt JF. 2001. Corticosterone facilitates begging and affects resource allocation in the black-legged kittiwake. Behav. Ecol. 12, 619–625. ( 10.1093/beheco/12.5.619) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 96.Romero LM, Dickens MJ, Cyr NE. 2009. The reactive scope model: a new model integrating homeostasis, allostasis, and stress. Horm. Behav. 55, 375–389. ( 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2008.12.009) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 97.Breuner CW, Wingfield JC. 2000. Rapid behavioral response to corticosterone varies with photoperiod and dose. Horm. Behav. 37, 23–30. ( 10.1006/hbeh.1999.1554) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 98.Baugh AT, Senft RA, Firke M, Lauder A, Schroeder J, Meddle SL, van Oers K, Hau M. 2017. Risk-averse personalities have a systemically potentiated neuroendocrine stress axis: a multilevel experiment in Parus major . Horm. Behav. 93, 99–108. ( 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2017.05.011) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 99.Breuner CW, Hahn TP. 2003. Integrating stress physiology, environmental change, and behavior in free-living sparrows. Horm. Behav. 43, 115–123. ( 10.1016/S0018-506X(02)00020-X) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 100.Dhabhar FS, McEwen BS. 1997. Acute stress enhances while chronic stress suppresses cell-mediated immunity in vivo: a potential role for leukocyte trafficking. Brain. Behav. Immun. 11, 286–306. ( 10.1006/brbi.1997.0508) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 101.Chin EH, Quinn JS, Burness G. 2013. Acute stress during ontogeny suppresses innate, but not acquired immunity in a semi-precocial bird (Larus delawarensis). Gen. Comp. Endocrinol. 193, 185–192. ( 10.1016/j.ygcen.2013.08.007) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 102.Hau M. 2007. Regulation of male traits by testosterone: implications for the evolution of vertebrate life histories. BioEssays 29, 133–q44. ( 10.1002/bies.20524) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 103.McEwen BS, Wingfield JC. 2003. The concept of allostasis in biology and biomedicine. Horm. Behav. 43, 2–15. ( 10.1016/S0018-506X(02)00024-7) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 104.Davies S, Noor S, Carpentier E, Deviche P. 2016. Innate immunity and testosterone rapidly respond to acute stress, but is corticosterone at the helm? J. Comp. Physiol. B 186, 907–918. ( 10.1007/s00360-016-0996-y) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 105.Wikelski M, Hau M, Wingfield JC. 1999. Social instability increases plasma testosterone in a year-round territorial neotropical bird. Proc. R. Soc. B 266, 551–556. ( 10.1098/rspb.1999.0671) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 106.Beck ML, Davies S, Moore IT, Schoenle LA, Kerman K, Vernasco BJ, Sewall KB. 2016. Beeswax corticosterone implants produce long-term elevation of plasma corticosterone and influence condition. Gen. Comp. Endocrinol. 233, 109–114. ( 10.1016/j.ygcen.2016.05.021) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 107.Adkins-Regan E. 2005. Hormones and animal social behavior. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press. [Google Scholar]
- 108.Wu Z, Autry AE, Bergan JF, Watabe-Uchida M, Dulac CG. 2014. Galanin neurons in the medial preoptic area govern parental behavior. Nature 509, 325–330. ( 10.1038/nature13307) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 109.Schmidt KL, Pradhan DS, Shah AH, Charlier TD, Chin EH, Soma KK. 2008. Neurosteroids, immunosteroids, and the Balkanization of endocrinology. Gen. Comp. Endocrinol. 157, 266–274. ( 10.1016/j.ygcen.2008.03.025) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 110.Vitousek MN, Zonana DM, Safran RJ. 2014. An integrative view of the signaling phenotype: dynamic links between signals, physiology, behavior and social context. Curr. Zool. 60, 739–754. ( 10.1093/czoolo/60.6.739) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Supplementary Materials
Data Availability Statement
This article has no additional data.