Abstract
Long-term depression (LTD) of parallel fibre/Purkinje cell synapses has been the favoured explanation for cerebellar motor learning such as classical eyeblink conditioning. Previous evidence against this interpretation has been contested. Here we wanted to test whether a classical conditioning protocol causes LTD. We applied a conditioning protocol, using a train of electrical pulses to the parallel fibres as the conditional stimulus. In order to rule out indirect effects caused by antidromic granule cell activation or output from Purkinje cells that might produce changes in Purkinje cell responsiveness, we focused the analysis on the first pulse in the conditional stimulus, that is, before any indirect effects would have time to occur. Purkinje cells learned to respond with a firing pause to the conditional stimulus. Yet, there was no depression of parallel fibre excitation after training.
Introduction
The textbook view of motor learning in the cerebellum is that it mainly involves long-term depression (LTD) of parallel fibre to Purkinje cell (pf/Pc) synapses1–3 and it has been widely assumed that eyeblink conditioning, a form of cerebellar motor learning, depends on LTD4–6. This view has recently been questioned because it cannot readily account for timing of conditional responses7–9 and because knockout mice with impaired LTD and rats in which LTD was pharmacologically prevented could still learn conditional responses10,11. It is conceivable, however, that conditioning normally involves LTD but that, if LTD is blocked, other mechanisms can compensate. Indeed, recent findings have suggested the likely existence of alternative learning mechanisms8,12,13. It is therefore of crucial importance to determine if a classical conditioning protocol actually causes LTD.
During conditioning, the conditional stimulus (CS), carried by the mossy and parallel fibres, acquires the ability to suppress simple spike firing in the Purkinje cells13,14. This Purkinje cell pause response is adaptively timed and drives the overt conditional blink response (CR)12,15
In an experiment where the CS consisted of a train of electrical stimulus pulses to the mossy fibres in decerebrate ferrets, it was observed that, except for the CR period of simple spike suppression, each stimulus pulse elicited a simple spike in the Purkinje cell with about the same probability before and after conditioning16, suggesting that no LTD had occurred. Against this interpretation, it could be argued that the individual mossy fibre stimuli could have successively activated different sets of granule cells4,5, and that a lack of reduced responsiveness of the Purkinje cell to the CS could reflect a lack of LTD only in a subset of synapses not involved in generating the CR.
As a more stringent test of the LTD hypothesis, we have previously studied Purkinje cell responses to pf stimuli8 (for details of the methodology, see ref.8). When a train of stimuli is directed to the pfs, each stimulus will excite the same population of fibres. If a particular stimulus in the CS train has the same excitatory effect on the Purkinje cell before and after conditioning, it will indicate that no LTD had occurred. A problem with this argument is that various indirect loops, say antidromic pf activation or Purkinje cell output, could modulate granule cell activity and cause a time varying activity in the pfs. However, this would not apply to the first pulse in the CS train, because there would not be sufficient time for such loops to influence granule cells. If the classical conditioning protocol actually causes any LTD, it should be visible as a reduced probability of the first stimulus pulse to elicit a spike in the Purkinje cell. To investigate this possibility, we performed a new analysis of these experiments.
Materials and Methods
All experiments were approved by the local Animal Experimentation Ethics Committee of Malmö-Lund, and all experiments were performed in accordance with relevant guidelines and regulations. Twelve male 1 year old ferrets were decerebrated and prepared for stimulation and recording in the cerebellum under anesthesia as previously described8,17. The experimental setup is shown in Fig. 1A. All recordings were made in a known blink-controlling area14 of cortical lobule HVI and each Purkinje cell was identified as controlling the eyelid by short-latency complex spikes in response to electrical periocular stimulation (Fig. 1B). When the CS consists of forelimb or mossy fiber stimulation these Purkinje cells consistently develop typical conditioned responses14 as shown in Fig. 1C. Here, the CS was a 100 Hz train of electrical stimuli (2–20 μA, 0.1 ms) applied directly to the parallel fibers. The location of parallel fiber stimulating electrodes was confirmed by eliciting simple spikes (Fig. 1D). The US consisted of two five-pulse 500 Hz trains of stimuli (30–400 μA, 0.1 ms) separated by 10 ms applied to ipsilateral climbing fibers. Parallel fibers and climbing fibers were stimulated with platinum tungsten electrodes (pulled and ground tips, 25 μm core diameter).
Two training protocols were used, one with a 300 ms CS, where the US was delivered 150 ms after CS onset and one with a 800 ms CS where the US was delivered 200 ms after CS onset. Using a CS that continues beyond the US serves two purposes. First, in standard protocols where the CS and US co-terminate the end of a Purkinje cell response might merely reflect the termination of the CS as opposed to reflecting learning of the interval between the CS and the US. Second, since each stimulus pulse excites the same population of parallel fibres it is of interest to assess the probability of spike elicitation beyond the CS-US interval where LTD but not conditioned responding would be expected (for references see7). The intertrial interval was 15 +/− 1 s (randomized). For further details concerning recording techniques and analysis, see8,17.
Results
Twelve Purkinje cells were trained with a CS that consisted of a 100 Hz train of stimulus pulses to the pfs followed by a US consisting of direct climbing fibre stimulation as described previously8 and as illustrated in Fig. 1A. To enable analysis of spike probability both before and after the expected US, this is the subset of cells in ref.8 where the CS continued beyond the US. In 7/12 cells the CS lasted 300 ms and the US was delivered at 150 ms. In 5/12 cells the CS lasted 800 ms and the US was delivered at 200 ms. In every case, this training protocol resulted in a typical Purkinje cell CR, that is a strong suppression of simple spike firing that reaches a maximum just before the expected US onset and ending shortly after the US, even though the CS continued for an additional 150 or 600 ms (Fig. 2A).
From the CS train, we selected three individual stimuli and determined the probability of a spike response 1–4 ms after each stimulus pulse over 10–20 trials. For comparison, we selected the first stimulus pulse (‘early’), the pulse delivered 100 ms before the US (‘middle’) and the pulse delivered 150 ms after the US (‘late’).
Although there was some variability between cells, the main results were clear (Fig. 2B–D). The average probability of spike responses of the Purkinje cell to the ‘early’ and ‘late’ pf stimuli were virtually identical before and after acquisition of the CR (‘early’: 0.42 +/− 0.09 SEM versus 0.41 +/− 0.13, ‘late’: 0.58 +/− 0.08 versus 0.49 +/− 0.09). A Wilcoxon matched-pairs signed rank test indicated no difference for either the ‘early’ impulse (W = 3; p = 0.92) or the ‘late’ impulse (W = 10; p = 0.70). Only the spike probability to an individual pf stimulus during the CR changed (0.69 +/− 0.07 versus 0.11 +/− 0.05, W = 78, p = 0.0005).
Discussion
The result show that, although the Purkinje cells exhibited clear conditioned pause responses, the conditioning protocol did not cause any measurable depression of the pf/Pc synapses.
It might be suggested that the reason that there was no LTD of the pf/Pc synapses activated by the first pf pulse, was that this set of synapses was activated too far ahead of the climbing fibre US, whereas later pf input, generated by the indirect loops, would be closer in time to the US. This suggestion is contradicted by the LTD literature, according to which the 150 ms CS–US interval employed in our experiments is well within the range for significant LTD7. Furthermore, as the CS is a direct train of repetitive stimuli to the same parallel fibers, the set of pf/Pc synapses activated by the first stimulus pulse would also be activated by all the subsequent pulses, and most of them would therefore be closer in time to the US. A substantial proportion of the synapses should therefore have undergone LTD and some degree of reduced spiking probability should have occurred. The fact that hundreds of CS-US presentations did not cause any measurable decrease at all in the probability of a spike after the first CS pulse, is strong evidence that no LTD occurred.
LTD can remove excitation but it cannot by itself suppress Purkinje cell firing below its resting level. It can only do so in combination with inhibitory input7,8,13. The present finding is consistent with recent reports suggesting that the Purkinje cell CR is generated by an intrinsic mechanism that does not involve the inhibitory interneurons8,12,13 so LTD is not needed.
Although we agree with most investigators that LTD probably has an important role in other forms of motor learning, the present results clearly support the conclusion, that a classical conditioning protocol does not cause any significant LTD and that, therefore, the learning of the Purkinje cell CR in classical conditioning is not due to LTD but involves a different kind of learning mechanisml7,8,13.
Acknowledgements
This work was supported by grants from the Swedish Research Council to The Linnaeus Centre for Cognition, Communication and Learning (349-2007-8695) and to G. Hesslow (09899) and from the Torsten Söderberg foundation.
Author Contributions
F.J., D.A.J. and G.H. designed research; F.J., A.R. and R.Z. performed research; F.J. analyzed data. G.H. and F.J. wrote the paper.
Competing Interests
The authors declare no competing interests.
Footnotes
Publisher's note: Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
References
- 1.Kandel, E. R. Principles of neural science (McGraw-Hill 2013).
- 2.Purves, D. Neuroscience (Sinauer Associates 2017).
- 3.Ito M, Sakurai M, Tongroach P. Climbing fibre induced depression of both mossy fibre responsiveness and glutamate sensitivity of cerebellar Purkinje cells. Journal of Physiology (London) 1982;324:113–134. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.1982.sp014103. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Yamazaki T, Tanaka S. Computational models of timing mechanisms in the cerebellar granular layer. Cerebellum. 2009;8:423–432. doi: 10.1007/s12311-009-0115-7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Medina JF, Mauk MD. Computer simulation of cerebellar information processing. Nature Neuroscience. 2000;3:1205–1211. doi: 10.1038/81486. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Lepora NF, Porrill J, Yeo CH, Dean P. Sensory prediction or motor control? Application of marr-albus type models of cerebellar function to classical conditioning. Front Comput Neurosci. 2010;4:140. doi: 10.3389/fncom.2010.00140. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Hesslow G, Jirenhed D-A, Rasmussen A, Johansson F. Classical conditioning of motor responses: What is the learning mechanism? Neural Networks. 2013;47:81–87. doi: 10.1016/j.neunet.2013.03.013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Johansson F, Jirenhed D-A, Rasmussen A, Zucca R, Hesslow G. Memory trace and timing mechanism localized to cerebellar Purkinje cells. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 2014;111:14930–14934. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1415371111. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Gallistel CR, Matzel LD. The neuroscience of learning: beyond the Hebbian synapse. Annu Rev Psychol. 2013;64:169–200. doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-113011-143807. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Welsh JP, et al. Normal motor learning during pharmacological prevention of Purkinje cell long-term depression. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2005;102:17166–17171. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0508191102. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Schonewille M, et al. Reevaluating the role of LTD in cerebellar motor learning. Neuron. 2011;70:43–50. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2011.02.044. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Johansson F, Carlsson HAE, Rasmussen A, Yeo CH, Hesslow G. Activation of a temporal memory in Purkinje cells by the mGluR7receptor. Cell reports. 2015;13:1741–1746. doi: 10.1016/j.celrep.2015.10.047. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Johansson F, Hesslow G, Medina JF. Mechanisms for motor timing in the cerebellar cortex. Curr Opin Behav Sci. 2016;8:53–59. doi: 10.1016/j.cobeha.2016.01.013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Jirenhed DA, Hesslow G. Are Purkinje Cell Pauses Drivers of Classically Conditioned Blink Responses? Cerebellum. 2016;15:526–534. doi: 10.1007/s12311-015-0722-4. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Jirenhed DA, Rasmussen A, Johansson F, Hesslow G. Learned response sequences in cerebellar Purkinje cells. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2017;114:6127–6132. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1621132114. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Jirenhed DA, Hesslow G. Time Course of Classically Conditioned Purkinje Cell Response is Determined by Initial Part of Conditioned Stimulus. Journal of Neuroscience. 2011;31:9070–9074. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1653-11.2011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Jirenhed DA, Bengtsson F, Hesslow G. Acquisition, extinction, and reacquisition of a cerebellar cortical memory trace. Journal of Neuroscience. 2007;27:2493–2502. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4202-06.2007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]