Skip to main content
Iranian Journal of Psychiatry logoLink to Iranian Journal of Psychiatry
. 2018 Jul;13(3):160–167.

Emotion Regulation in Generalized Anxiety and Social Anxiety: Examining the Distinct and Shared Use of Emotion Regulation Strategies

Imaneh Abasi 1, Behrouz Dolatshahi 2,*, Shirin Farazmand 2, Abbas Pourshahbaz 2, Shima Tamanaeefar 3
PMCID: PMC6178328  PMID: 30319698

Abstract

Objective: According to the transdiagnostic view, emotion regulation strategies are common among emotional disorders. Despite the vast majority of studies demonstrating the common role of emotion regulation strategies in emotional disorders including generalized anxiety disorder (GAD) and social anxiety disorder (SAD), distinct functions of these strategies are largely unknown. So, the aim of the present study was to assess the distinct and shared use of emotion regulation strategies in individuals with GAD and SAD symptoms.

Method : Participants were recruited from a community sample of Tehran using purposive sampling method. The sample (N = 346) consisted of 269 nonclinical individuals, 47 individuals with generalized anxiety symptoms, and 30 individuals with social anxiety symptoms. Informed consent was obtained from all the participants and they were asked to fill in a series of self -report questionnaires including GAD-IV, SIAS, DERS, EQ, ACS, and RRS. Data were analyzed by discriminant function analysis using SPSS-23.

Results: Findings revealed significant differences between the 3 groups in non-acceptance, focusing, shifting, brooding, and decentering (P<0.01). Results of discriminant function analysis showed 2 functions: the first accounted for 87.4% of the variance (Wilk`s Lambda = 0.81, df = 10, P<0.01, χ2 = 70.06), and the second for 12.6% of the variance (Wilk`s Lambda = 0.97, df = 4, P<0.05, χ2 = 9.43) of between group variability. Non-acceptance and focusing discriminated GAD and SAD the most.

Conclusion: The findings of the present study support both splitter and lumper approaches as there were some emotion regulation strategies common between GAD and SAD, including shifting, decentering, and brooding. However, some of them were distinct, such as focusing and non-acceptance. Nevertheless, there are still inconsistencies in research about the findings, and thus examining the pathways may illuminate these findings.

Key Words: Attention, Emotion Regulation, Generalized Anxiety Disorder, Rumination, Social Anxiety Disorder


Historically diagnostic classification has witnessed two diverse viewpoints: “splitter approach”, which assumes that boundaries are needed between different types of disorders (1), and “lumper approach” that emphasizes common dimensions across psychopathologies (2, 3). Transdiagnostic approach has been developed to seek a shared process across highly comorbid disorders, especially emotional disorders, such as depression, generalized anxiety disorder, and social anxiety disorder, which is in line with lumper point of view (4, 5) and National Institute of Mental Health (NIMH) recent project, Research Domain Criteria (6).

As emotional problems are prominent in various clinical conditions, recently emotion regulation, as a transdiagnostic common factor, has been evaluated by many studies in different disorders (7-9).

Emotion regulation is defined as an individual’s emotions and when and how he/she experiences and expresses them (10-12). Emotion regulation strategies are processes by which individuals modify their emotions; these strategies may be adaptive or maladaptive and are related to development and maintenance of a wide range of pathologies (7, 13-17).

Modal model of emotion regulation posits that there are 5 distinct emotion regulation processes that occur at 5 points in time. These emotion regulation strategies are categorized into 2 groups: (1) antecedent focused strategies, including situation selection, situation modification, attentional deployment, cognitive change; and (2) response focused strategies, such as response modulation (11, 12). Building upon the process model, Mennin and Fresco have assumed that regulatory strategies can be distinguished from less elaborative regulatory components (attending, being aware) to more elaborative regulatory components (reframing/ reappraisal, distancing) based on the degree of regulatory efforts (3).

The relationship between majority of emotional problems and maladaptive emotion regulation strategies is evident (18-21), however, their high associations with emotional disorders have been more scrutinized and reported (22, 23).

Among emotional disorders, generalized anxiety disorder and social anxiety disorder highly co-occur (24, 25). Furthermore, they are highly prevalent, chronic and impose problems in almost all aspects of life and reduce quality of life (26-29). Thus, assessing the role of emotion regulation strategies, especially ones that have been suggested to relate to dysfunctions in SAD and GAD, is pivotal. With respect to the mentioned emotion regulatory strategies in emotion regulation model proposed by Mennin and Fresco, several studies have demonstrated the relationship among SAD and GAD and rumination (30-34), decentering (35-37), non-acceptance (38), and attentional control (39-41).

According to the transdiagnostic and lumper approaches, these emotion regulation strategies are common among psychological disorders and a great number of studies have determined the contributing role of these strategies in various clinical disorders. Shared function of emotion regulation is not disputable, but the distinct role of emotion regulation strategies is ill-defined. Some recent movements have tried to clarify distinct roles of emotion regulation strategies in anxiety disorders including SAD and GAD (22, 38 and 42). Taking a middle ground between the 2 approaches (splitter and lumper) and in connection with previous researches, the purpose of the present study was to seek the distinct and shared use of emotion regulation strategies in individuals with SAD and GAD symptoms. Thus, the study questions are that which emotion regulation strategies are common and which are distinct between individuals with GAD and SAD.

Materials and Methods

Participants were recruited from a community sample of Tehran, Iran, using purposive sampling method. We can use any sample size for discriminant function analysis (DFA), but the sample size of the smallest group should exceed the number of predictive variables, for example, if we have 5 predictive variables, the sample size of the smallest group should exceed 5 (43). The sample (N = 346) consisted of 269 nonclinical individuals, 47 individuals with generalized anxiety symptoms, and 30 with social anxiety symptoms. Inclusion criteria were age ≥18 and participating voluntarily in the study. Exclusion criteria for the nonclinical population were scores equal or above 10 in GAD-IV, and 34 in SIAS. Cut off scores for questionnaires were, ≥10 (44), and ≥34 (45) for diagnosing GAD, and social anxiety, respectively.

Procedure

Participants were recruited by advertisements and in places where people gathered such as coffee shops and parks. The participants were informed about the purpose of the research and were asked to participate in the study that took place in the clinics in a quiet room. Participation was voluntary. After completing the informed consent, participants were asked to fill in a series of self -report questionnaires. After gathering the questionnaires, data of 500 participants were entered in to SPSS-23, and after data cleaning (removing missing and outliers), data of 480 participants remained. All individuals with exclusion criteria were removed from the analysis and at last 346 individuals remained.

Data Analysis

Data were analyzed using chi- square to assess the differences between groups in gender and education level, and descriptive analysis was used to obtain descriptive statistics. ANOVA was utilized to assess the differences between groups in age. Finally, discriminant function analysis was run to evaluate whether there were differences between groups in emotion regulation strategies and which ones most discriminated GAD and SAD.

Measures

Social Interaction Anxiety Scale (SIAS): SIAS is a 20-item self-report questionnaire, which is rated on a 5- point Likert scale, ranging from 0 (not at all characteristic or true of me) to 4 (extremely characteristic or true of me). Internal consistency of SIAS in social phobia sample, community sample, and undergraduate sample has been reported as 0.86, 0.95, and 0.85, respectively (45). Iranian psychometric properties has been demonstrated to have acceptable internal consistency (α = 0.90) and test-retest reliability (r = 0.79) (46). Internal consistency of SIAS in the present study was α = 0.88.

Generalized Anxiety Disorder (GAD-7(44): GAD-7 is a self-report 7- item scale that assesses the severity of generalized anxiety disorder and is rated on a 0 (not at all) to 3 (nearly every day) rating scale. Internal consistency and test-retest reliability have been reported to be α = 0.92 and r = 0.83, respectively. Cut off point of 10 has been identified with optimized sensitivity (89%) and specificity (82%) (44). Cronbach alpha of GAD-7 in the present research was α = 0.89.

Attentional Control Scale (ACS): ACS is a 20- item self-report questionnaire, which is rated on a 4- point Likert scale (1 = almost never to 4 = always) and assesses attentional control and attentional shifting; its internal consistency has reported to be α = 0.88 (47). Test-retest reliability for ACS varies from 0.45 to 0.73 and internal consistency for the total score is 0.61 (48). Coefficient alpha for the focusing, shifting, and the total score of ACS was α = 0.78, α = 0.66, and α = 0.77, respectively (49). Internal consistency of focusing and shifting subscales in the present study was 0.75 and 0.67, respectively.

The Ruminative Response Scale (RRS): RRS is a 22- item self-report questionnaire, which assesses the tendency to ruminate in response to depressed mood and is rated on a 4-point Likert-type scale, ranging from 0 to 3. Internal consistency (α = 0.89) and 5- month test- retest reliability have been reported to be acceptable (50, 51). Internal consistency of the Persian version of this scale is reported to be 0.81 (52). Internal consistency of Brooding subscale in the present study was α = 0.70.

Difficulties in Emotion Regulation Scale (DERS): DERS is a 36-item self-report scale and assesses individual’s typical tendencies for emotion regulation across several facets: (1) non-acceptance of emotional responses, (2) difficulties engaging in goal directed behavior, (3) impulse control difficulties, (4) lack of emotional awareness, (5) limited access to emotion regulation strategies, and (6) lack of emotional clarity. DERS demonstrates high internal consistency (α = higher than 0.80 for each subscale), good test-retest reliability (r = 0.88), and adequate construct and predictive validity (53). Psychometric properties of the Persian version has reported high internal consistency (α = 0.86), with acceptable correlation with Zuckerman-Kuhlman Personality Questionnaire (54). Non-acceptance subscale was used in the present research. Internal consistency of non-acceptance subscale in the present study was α = 0.84.

Experiences Questionnaire (EQ): EQ is an 11- item self-report questionnaire, which is rated based on a 7-point Likert scale, ranging from 1(never) to 7 (all the time) and assesses decentering. Internal consistency and test-retest reliability of this questionnaire have been demonstrated to be α = 0.89 and r = 0.87 (55). Internal consistency of decentering in Iranian population was acceptable, α = 0.82 (56). Internal consistency of EQ in the present study was α = 0.93.

Results

Descriptive Statistics

No significant differences were found between groups in age (F (2, 343) =1.65, P<0. 19), gender (P<0.13), and educational level (P<0.73). Mean age for GAD, SAD, and control groups was 35.87 (SD = 11.4), 39 (SD = 10.38), and 35.25 (SD = 10.63), respectively. Descriptive statistics of the demographic variables are presented in Table 1.

Table 1.

Descriptive Statistics of the Demographic Variables of Control, GAD and SAD Groups

Controls GAD SAD
Gender
Male 125 (46.5%) 21 (44.7%) 16 (53.3%)
Female 144 (53.5%) 26 (55.3%) 14 (46.7%)
Marital status
Single 82 (30.5%) 15 (31.9%) 10 (33.3%)
Married 183 (68%) 31 (66%) 17 (56.7%)
Divorced 3 (1.1%) 2 (6.7%)
Others 1 (0.4%) 1 (2.1%) 1 (3.3%)
Education level
Less than a high school diploma 30 (11.2%) 8 (17%) 6 (20%)
High school diploma 115 (42.4%) 20 (42.6%) 13 (43.3%)
Bachelor 81 (30.1%) 15 (31.9%) 6 (20%)
Master 37 (13.8%) 3 (6.4%) 3 (10%)
Doctoral 6 (2.2%) 1 (2.1%) 2 (6.7%)

Discriminant Function Analysis

Before performing discriminant function analysis, assumptions underlying DFA were examined and all assumptions, including absence of outliers, absence of missing data, linearity, normality, and absence of multicollinearity, were met. Discriminant function analysis using enter method revealed significant differences between groups in independent variables, including focusing, shifting, decentering, brooding, and non-acceptance, and these variables had significantly contributed to function (P<0.01). The summary results of equality of group means, along with the descriptive statistics of the study variables, are presented in Table 2. DFA revealed 2 functions and both were significant (F1 = P<0.01, F2 = P<0.05). The summary results of discriminant function analysis are demonstrated in Table 3. The 2 functions accounted for 87.4% and 12.6% of the between group variability, respectively. Standardized discriminant function coefficients showed the relative importance of each predictor in predicting each group from each function. Standardized discriminant function coefficients suggested that best predictors for distinguishing between GAD and SAD groups were focusing and non-acceptance, respectively. Overall, the discriminating power is acceptable and 59.2% of the original group cases were classified correctly.

Table 2.

Descriptive Statistics and Equality of Group Means of Emotion Regulation Strategies in Clinical and Control Groups

Mean (SD) Equality of Group Means
GAD SAD Control Wilks Lambda F (2, 343) sig
Focusing 24.08 (4.3) 23.53 (4.3) 27.06 (3.69) 0.89 20.95 0.01
Shifting 18.78 (3.69) 17.63 (3.74) 19.6 (3.81) 0.97 4.19 0.04
Decentering 34.72 (7.9) 34.46 (5.59) 38.07 (6.39) 0.95 8.33 0.01
Brooding 11.46 (2.12) 10.63 (2.52) 9.72 (2.64) 0.94 10.08 0.01
Non-acceptance 13.87 (5.26) 16.53 (4.57) 11.86 (4.67) 0.91 15.2 0.01

Table 3.

Summary Results of Discriminant Function Analysis of Emotion Regulation Strategies in Clinical and Control Groups

Function 1 Function 2
Structure matrix
Focusing 0.79 0.05
Shifting 0.33 -0.31
Decentering 0.49 0.10
Brooding -0.49 -0.61
Non-acceptance -0.63 0.57
Eigenvalue (variance %) 0.19 (87.4%) 0.02 (12.6%)
Canonical correlation 0.40 0.16
Wilks lambda 0.81 0.97
Chi-square 70.06 9.43
df 10 4
sig 0.01 0.05
Group centroids
GAD -0.72 -0.31
SAD -0.95 0.4
Nonclinical 0.23 0.01

Discussion

The aim of the present study was to examine the common and specific patterns in levels of emotion regulation strategies including focusing and shifting as subscales of attentional control, brooding, decentering, and non-accepting in GAD, SAD, and control groups. Wilk`s lambda showed significant differences among the 3 groups in emotion regulation strategies. Focusing and non-acceptance best discriminated the SAD and GAD groups.

Attentional control has been indicated in etiology and perpetuation of anxiety disorders, especially SAD and GAD that is in line with the results of the present research demonstrating its transdiagnostic role in psychopathology (40, 57 and 58). In fact, comorbidity of GAD and SAD may suggest the existence of shared dysfunction in the brain supporting the attentional control as a common emotion regulation strategy (59). Other emotion regulation strategies were significantly higher in SAD and GAD than the control group, which support the transdiagnostic nature of these variables and present findings that are consistent with those of previous studies, revealing the relationship among decentering, brooding and non-acceptance with GAD and SAD (31, 34, 35, 37, 38, 60, and 61).

Among the emotion regulation strategies examined, focusing and non-acceptance discriminated individuals with GAD symptoms and individuals with SAD symptoms. In line with the present study, it has been demonstrated that patients with generalized anxiety disorder, in contrast to patients with generalized social anxiety disorder, show greater impairments in the recruitment of regions implicated in top-down attentional control (59). Despite the vast majority of studies ranging from very primitive to very complicated that explored and demonstrated the role of attentional bias as a very closed concept to attentional control in emotional disorders (62-64), findings of the present study support the disorder-specific nature of attentional control.

The distinct role of acceptance in anxiety disorders has not been evaluated enough and there are not adequate studies to refer to. One study found that non-acceptance of emotion best predicts the comorbidity of GAD and SAD (38), which is somewhat contradictory to findings of the present research. This disagreement may be due to different sample groups and different methods of choosing the samples. Furthermore, individuals with SAD pay less attention to emotions than individuals with GAD (42) and lack of interactions with others hold them back from understanding their emotions, so they are less acceptance of their emotions (38).

Non-acceptance and attentional control as 2 major shared and yet distinct factors in anxiety disorders have captured the attention of the researchers and clinical psychologists who try to shed light on the mechanism and mediating functions of these variables in emotional disorders. Two well-known and qualified treatments that are widely used to help individuals suffering from anxiety and mood disorders and other psychological disorders are mindfulness and acceptance- based interventions, which largely emphasize improving acceptance and attention (65-68). Thus, given the importance of acceptance and attention in psychopathology and treatment of SAD and GAD, especially acceptance in SAD and attention in GAD, paying more attention to them and taking steps to use them in various studies on GAD and SAD are necessary and can illuminate vague points in recovering, improve quality of life, and increase productivity.

Overall, findings of the present study are consistent with those of the previous studies, indicating that transdiagnostic components are not cultural phenomena and are not different across various cultures. There were not any significant differences between the study variables in nonclinical and clinical groups, which strengthens the transdiagnostic nature of the study variables.

Limitation

A number of limitations in the present study suggest that findings should be interpreted cautiously. First, the present sample was selected from community individuals who had higher functioning and less impairment in different life domains and had less severe clinical symptoms. Therefore, future studies on clinical patients may yield more realistic and reliable findings. Second, exclusively relying on self-report measures may subject the findings to considerable bias, especially about emotion regulation related concepts, as sometimes it is difficult to grasp their meaning, so future studies may benefit from conducting multi-assessment procedures including physiological and neurobiological instruments to assess emotion regulation strategies. Third, this study was conducted on a restricted group of emotion regulation strategies. Thus, in future studies attention should be paid to different emotion regulation strategies between GAD and SAD, and also between other emotional disorders. Finally, due to the complicated contributory role of emotion regulation constructs in psychopathology, studying them as mediators and moderators may illuminate more straightforward and cost-effective conceptualization and treatment.

Conclusion

Focusing and non-acceptance were highlighted in distinguishing SAD and GAD symptoms in the present study. Moreover, as non-acceptance is much higher in individuals with SAD symptoms than in those with GAD symptoms, other related psychological factors responsible for this connection should be taken into consideration in future studies.

Acknowledgment

We thank all participants who contributed to the present study.

Conflict of Interest

The authors do not have conflicts of interest to disclose.

References

  • 1.Widiger TA, Clark LA. Toward DSM—V and the classification of psychopathology. Psychol Bull. 2000;126:946–963. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.126.6.946. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Barlow DH, Sauer-Zavala S, Carl JR, Bullis JR, Ellard KK. The nature, diagnosis, and treatment of neuroticism back to the future. Clinical Psychological Science. 2014;2:344–365. [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Mennin DS, Fresco DM. Emotion regulation therapy. Handbook of emotion regulation. 2014;2:469–490. [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Barrera TL, Smith AH, Norton PJ. Transdiagnostic CBT for Anxiety Disorders. The Wiley Handbook of Anxiety Disorders. 2014:787–803. [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Sheppes G, Suri G, Gross JJ. Emotion regulation and psychopathology. Annu Rev Clin Psychol. 2015;11:379–405. doi: 10.1146/annurev-clinpsy-032814-112739. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Insel TR, Cuthbert BN, Garvey MA, Heinssen R, Pine DS, Quinn K, et al. Research domain criteria (RDoC): toward a new classification framework for research on mental disorders. American Journal of Psychiatry. 2010;167:748–751. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2010.09091379. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Bardeen JR, Tull MT, Dixon-Gordon KL, Stevens EN, Gratz KL. Attentional Control as a Moderator of the Relationship between Difficulties Accessing Effective Emotion Regulation Strategies and Distress Tolerance. Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment. 2015;37:79–84. [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Burklund LJ, Craske MG, Taylor SE, Lieberman MD. Altered emotion regulation capacity in social phobia as a function of comorbidity. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci. 2015;10:199–208. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsu058. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Abasi I, Pourshahbaz A, Mohammadkhani P, Dolatshahi B. Mediation Role of Emotion Regulation Strategies on the Relationship Between Emotional Intensity, Safety and Reward Motivations with Social Anxiety Symptoms, Rumination and Worry: A Structural Equation Modeling. Iran J Psychiatry Behav Sci. 2017;11:e9640. [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Gross JJ. The emerging field of emotion regulation: an integrative review. Review of general psychology. 1998;2:271–299. [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Gross JJ, Thompson RA. Emotion regulation: Conceptual foundations. New York: Guilford Press; 2007. [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Gross JJ. Emotion regulation: Conceptual and empirical foundations. Handbook of emotion regulation. 2014;2:3–20. [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Aldao A, Nolen-Hoeksema S. The influence of context on the implementation of adaptive emotion regulation strategies. Behav Res Ther. 2012;50:493–501. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2012.04.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Nolen-Hoeksema S, Watkins ER. A heuristic for developing transdiagnostic models of psychopathology explaining multifinality and divergent trajectories. Perspect Psychol Sci. 2011;6:589–609. doi: 10.1177/1745691611419672. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Kever A, Pollatos O, Vermeulen N, Grynberg D. Interoceptive sensitivity facilitates both antecedent-and response-focused emotion regulation strategies. Personality and Individual Differences. 2015;87:20–23. [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Raio CM, Goldfarb EV, Lempert KM, Sokol-Hessner P. Classifying emotion regulation strategies. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2016;17:532. doi: 10.1038/nrn.2016.78. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Aldao A, Jazaieri H, Goldin PR, Gross JJ. Adaptive and maladaptive emotion regulation strategies: Interactive effects during CBT for social anxiety disorder. J Anxiety Disord. 2014;28:382–389. doi: 10.1016/j.janxdis.2014.03.005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Farris SG, Zvolensky MJ, Schmidt NB. Difficulties with emotion regulation and psychopathology interact to predict early smoking cessation lapse. Cognitive therapy and research. 2016;40:357–367. doi: 10.1007/s10608-015-9705-5. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Haynos AF, Roberto CA, Attia E. examining the associations between emotion regulation difficulties, anxiety, and eating disorder severity among inpatients with anorexia nervosa. Compr Psychiatry. 2015;60:93–98. doi: 10.1016/j.comppsych.2015.03.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Van Zutphen L, Siep N, Jacob GA, Goebel R, Arntz A. Emotional sensitivity, emotion regulation and impulsivity in borderline personality disorder: A critical review of fMRI studies. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2015;51:64–76. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2015.01.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Samson AC, Hardan AY, Lee IA, Phillips JM, Gross JJ. Maladaptive Behavior in Autism Spectrum Disorder: The Role of Emotion Experience and Emotion Regulation. J Autism Dev Disord. 2015;45:3424–3432. doi: 10.1007/s10803-015-2388-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Mennin DS, Fresco DM. Advancing emotion regulation perspectives on psychopathology: The challenge of distress disorders. Psychological Inquiry. 2015;26:80–92. doi: 10.1080/1047840X.2015.969624. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Kivity Y, Huppert JD. Does cognitive reappraisal reduce anxiety? A daily diary study of a micro-intervention with individuals with high social anxiety. J Consult Clin Psychol. 2016;84:269–283. doi: 10.1037/ccp0000075. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Mennin DS, Heimberg RG, Jack MS. Comorbid generalized anxiety disorder in primary social phobia: symptom severity, functional impairment, and treatment response. Journal of Anxiety disorders. 2000;14:325–343. doi: 10.1016/s0887-6185(00)00026-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Craske MG, Rauch SL, Ursano R, Prenoveau J, Pine DS, Zinbarg RE. What is an anxiety disorder? Depression and anxiety. 2009;26:1066–1085. doi: 10.1002/da.20633. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Aderka IM, Hofmann SG, Nickerson A, Hermesh H, Gilboa-Schechtman E, Marom S. Functional impairment in social anxiety disorder. J Anxiety Disord. 2012;26:393–400. doi: 10.1016/j.janxdis.2012.01.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Kessler RC, Petukhova M, Sampson NA, Zaslavsky AM, Wittchen HU. Twelve‐month and lifetime prevalence and lifetime morbid risk of anxiety and mood disorders in the United States. Int J Methods Psychiatr Res. 2012;21:169–184. doi: 10.1002/mpr.1359. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Tyrer P, Baldwin D. Generalised anxiety disorder. Lancet. 2006;368:2156–2166. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(06)69865-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Yonkers KA, Dyck IR, Warshaw M, Keller MB. Factors predicting the clinical course of generalised anxiety disorder. Br J Psychiatry. 2000;176:544–549. doi: 10.1192/bjp.176.6.544. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Michl LC, McLaughlin KA, Shepherd K, Nolen-Hoeksema S. Rumination as a mechanism linking stressful life events to symptoms of depression and anxiety: longitudinal evidence in early adolescents and adults. J Abnorm Psychol. 2013;122:339–352. doi: 10.1037/a0031994. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Ruscio AM, Gentes EL, Jones JD, Hallion LS, Coleman ES, Swendsen J. Rumination predicts heightened responding to stressful life events in major depressive disorder and generalized anxiety disorder. J Abnorm Psychol. 2015;124:17–26. doi: 10.1037/abn0000025. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Dar KA, Iqbal N. Worry and rumination in generalized anxiety disorder and obsessive compulsive disorder. The Journal of psychology. 2015;149:866–880. doi: 10.1080/00223980.2014.986430. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Penney ES, Abbott MJ. The impact of perceived standards on state anxiety, appraisal processes, and negative pre-and post-event rumination in social anxiety disorder. Cognitive Therapy and Research. 2015;39:162–177. [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Penney ES, Abbott MJ. Anticipatory and post-event rumination in social anxiety disorder: A review of the theoretical and empirical literature. Behaviour Change. 2014;31:79–101. [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Hayes-Skelton S, Graham J. Decentering as a common link among mindfulness, cognitive reappraisal, and social anxiety. Behav Cogn Psychother. 2013;41:317–328. doi: 10.1017/S1352465812000902. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Hoge EA, Bui E, Goetter E, Robinaugh DJ, Ojserkis RA, Fresco DM, et al. Change in decentering mediates improvement in anxiety in mindfulness-based stress reduction for generalized anxiety disorder. Cognit Ther Res. 2015;39:228–235. doi: 10.1007/s10608-014-9646-4. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Hayes-Skelton SA, Calloway A, Roemer L, Orsillo SM. Decentering as a potential common mechanism across two therapies for generalized anxiety disorder. J Consult Clin Psychol. 2015;83:395–404. doi: 10.1037/a0038305. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Mennin DS, McLaughlin KA, Flanagan TJ. Emotion regulation deficits in generalized anxiety disorder, social anxiety disorder, and their co-occurrence. J Anxiety Disord. 2009;23:866–871. doi: 10.1016/j.janxdis.2009.04.006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Morrison AS, Heimberg RG. Attentional control mediates the effect of social anxiety on positive affect. J Anxiety Disord. 2013;27:56–67. doi: 10.1016/j.janxdis.2012.10.002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Yiend J, Mathews A, Burns T, Dutton K, Fernández-Martín A, Georgiou GA, et al. Mechanisms of selective attention in generalized anxiety disorder. Clin Psychol Sci. 2015;3:758–771. doi: 10.1177/2167702614545216. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Thai N, Taber-Thomas BC, Pérez-Edgar KE. Neural correlates of attention biases, behavioral inhibition, and social anxiety in children: An ERP study. Dev Cogn Neurosci. 2016;19:200–210. doi: 10.1016/j.dcn.2016.03.008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Turk CL, Heimberg RG, Luterek JA, Mennin DS, Fresco DM. Emotion dysregulation in generalized anxiety disorder: A comparison with social anxiety disorder. Cognitive Therapy and Research. 2005;29:89–106. [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Meyers LS, Gamst G, Guarino AJ. Applied multivariate research: Design and interpretation. Sage; 2006. [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Spitzer RL, Kroenke K, Williams JB, Löwe B. A brief measure for assessing generalized anxiety disorder: the GAD-7. Arch Intern Med. 2006;166:1092–1097. doi: 10.1001/archinte.166.10.1092. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Heimberg RG, Mueller GP, Holt CS, Hope DA, Liebowitz MR. Assessment of anxiety in social interaction and being observed by others: The Social Interaction Anxiety Scale and the Social Phobia Scale. Behavior Therapy. 1992;23:53–73. [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Tavoli A, Allahyari A, Azadfallah P, Fathi Ashtiani A, Melyani M, Sahragard M. [Validity and Reliability of the Farsi Version of Social Interaction Anxiety Scale (SIAS) (Persian)] Iranian Journal of Psychiatry and Clinical Psychology. 2012;18:227–232. [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Derryberry D, Reed MA. Anxiety-related attentional biases and their regulation by attentional control. J Abnorm Psychol. 2002;111:225–236. doi: 10.1037//0021-843x.111.2.225. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Fajkowska M, Derryberry D. Psychometric properties of Attentional Control Scale: The preliminary study on a Polish sample. Polish Psychological Bulletin. 2010;41:1–7. [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Abasi I, Mohammadkhani P, Pourshahbaz A, Dolatshahi B. The Psychometric Properties of Attentional Control Scale and Its Relationship with Symptoms of Anxiety and Depression: A Study on Iranian Population. Iran J Psychiatry. 2017;12:109–117. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Nolen-Hoeksema S. Responses to depression and their effects on the duration of depressive episodes. J Abnorm Psychol. 1991;100:569–582. doi: 10.1037//0021-843x.100.4.569. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Nolen-Hoeksema S, Parker LE, Larson J. Ruminative coping with depressed mood following loss. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1994;67:92–104. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.67.1.92. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Asadi S, Abedini M, Poursharifi H, Nikokar M. [The Relationship between Intolerance of Uncertainty and Rumination with Worry on Student Population (Persian)] Journal of Clinical Psycology. 2013;4:83–92. [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Gratz KL, Roemer L. Multidimensional assessment of emotion regulation and dysregulation: Development, factor structure, and initial validation of the difficulties in emotion regulation scale. Journal of psychopathology and behavioral assessment. 2004;26:41–54. [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Asgari P, PASHA GR, Aminiyan M. [Relationship between emotion regulation, mental stresses and body image with eating disorders of women (Persian)] Andisheh Va Raftar (Applied Psychology) 2009;4:65–78. [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Fresco DM, Moore MT, van Dulmen MH, Segal ZV, Ma SH, Teasdale JD, et al. Initial psychometric properties of the experiences questionnaire: validation of a self-report measure of decentering. Behav Ther. 2007;38:234–246. doi: 10.1016/j.beth.2006.08.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56.Taherifar Z, Ferdowsi S, Mootabi F, Mazaheri MA, Fata L. The mediating role of emotion dysregulation strategies on the relationship between negative emotion intensity and safety motivation with generalized anxiety symptoms. [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Nitschke JB, Sarinopoulos I, Oathes DJ, Johnstone T, Whalen PJ, Davidson RJ, et al. Anticipatory activation in the amygdala and anterior cingulate in generalized anxiety disorder and prediction of treatment response. Am J Psychiatry. 2009;166:302–310. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2008.07101682. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 58.Heeren A, McNally RJ. An Integrative Network Approach to Social Anxiety Disorder: The Complex Dynamic Interplay among Attentional Bias for Threat, Attentional Control, and Symptoms. J Anxiety Disord. 2016;42:95–104. doi: 10.1016/j.janxdis.2016.06.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 59.Blair KS, Geraci M, Smith BW, Hollon N, DeVido J, Otero M, et al. Reduced dorsal anterior cingulate cortical activity during emotional regulation and top-down attentional control in generalized social phobia, generalized anxiety disorder, and comorbid generalized social phobia/generalized anxiety disorder. Biol Psychiatry. 2012;72:476–482. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2012.04.013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 60.Hong RY. Worry and rumination: Differential associations with anxious and depressive symptoms and coping behavior. Behav Res Ther. 2007;45:277–290. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2006.03.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 61.Norton AR, Abbott MJ, Norberg MM, Hunt C. A systematic review of mindfulness and acceptance‐based treatments for social anxiety disorder. J Clin Psychol. 2015;71:283–301. doi: 10.1002/jclp.22144. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 62.MacLeod C, Mathews A, Tata P. Attentional bias in emotional disorders. J Abnorm Psychol. 1986;95:15–20. doi: 10.1037//0021-843x.95.1.15. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 63.Carlbring P, Apelstrand M, Sehlin H, Amir N, Rousseau A, Hofmann SG, et al. Internet-delivered attention bias modification training in individuals with social anxiety disorder-a double blind randomized controlled trial. BMC psychiatry. 2012;12:66. doi: 10.1186/1471-244X-12-66. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 64.Berggren N, Derakshan N. Attentional control deficits in trait anxiety: why you see them and why you don’t. Biol Psychol. 2013;92:440–446. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2012.03.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 65.Vøllestad J, Nielsen MB, Nielsen GH. Mindfulness‐and acceptance‐based interventions for anxiety disorders: A systematic review and meta‐analysis. Br J Clin Psychol. 2012;51:239–260. doi: 10.1111/j.2044-8260.2011.02024.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 66.Dahlin M, Andersson G, Magnusson K, Johansson T, Sjögren J, Håkansson A, et al. Internet-delivered acceptance-based behaviour therapy for generalized anxiety disorder: A randomized controlled trial. Behav Res Ther. 2016;77:86–95. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2015.12.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 67.Yuen EK, Herbert JD, Forman EM, Goetter EM, Juarascio AS, Rabin S, et al. Acceptance based behavior therapy for social anxiety disorder through videoconferencing. J Anxiety Disord. 2013;27:389–397. doi: 10.1016/j.janxdis.2013.03.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 68.Roemer L, Williston SK, Eustis EH, Orsillo SM. Mindfulness and acceptance-based behavioral therapies for anxiety disorders. Curr Psychiatry Rep. 2013;15:410. doi: 10.1007/s11920-013-0410-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Articles from Iranian Journal of Psychiatry are provided here courtesy of Tehran University of Medical Sciences

RESOURCES