Skip to main content
eLife logoLink to eLife
. 2018 Oct 8;7:e34032. doi: 10.7554/eLife.34032

Motor thalamus supports striatum-driven reinforcement

Arnaud L Lalive 1, Anthony D Lien 1, Thomas K Roseberry 1,2,, Christopher H Donahue 1, Anatol C Kreitzer 1,2,3,
PMCID: PMC6181560  PMID: 30295606

Abstract

Reinforcement has long been thought to require striatal synaptic plasticity. Indeed, direct striatal manipulations such as self-stimulation of direct-pathway projection neurons (dMSNs) are sufficient to induce reinforcement within minutes. However, it’s unclear what role, if any, is played by downstream circuitry. Here, we used dMSN self-stimulation in mice as a model for striatum-driven reinforcement and mapped the underlying circuitry across multiple basal ganglia nuclei and output targets. We found that mimicking the effects of dMSN activation on downstream circuitry, through optogenetic suppression of basal ganglia output nucleus substantia nigra reticulata (SNr) or activation of SNr targets in the brainstem or thalamus, was also sufficient to drive rapid reinforcement. Remarkably, silencing motor thalamus—but not other selected targets of SNr—was the only manipulation that reduced dMSN-driven reinforcement. Together, these results point to an unexpected role for basal ganglia output to motor thalamus in striatum-driven reinforcement.

Research organism: Mouse

Introduction

Reinforcement refers to a process by which the frequency or intensity of a specific behavior increases over time. Standard models of reinforcement invoke dopamine-dependent corticostriatal plasticity as a mechanism underlying this form of associative learning (Schultz and Dickinson, 2000; Reynolds et al., 2001; Doya, 2007; Maia and Frank, 2011; Yagishita et al., 2014; Xiong et al., 2015). In these models, reinforcement occurs through plasticity of excitatory inputs to striatal action-related ensembles, which results in enhanced future recruitment of these motor programs in similar contexts. Yet other studies have found that activating targets of basal ganglia output can also be reinforcing (classic electrical studies reviewed in Wise, 1996); for recent optogenetic studies, see below) and reinforcement learning in songbirds and primates is proposed to involve basal ganglia regulation of cortical plasticity (Turner and Desmurget, 2010; Fee and Goldberg, 2011). Thus, it remains unclear to what extent striatum-driven reinforcement requires engagement of basal ganglia output projections to downstream targets in brainstem or thalamus.

The striatum contains two distinct classes of projection neurons—direct (dMSNs) and indirect pathway medium spiny neurons (iMSNs)—that regulate motivated behavior by increasing or suppressing movement, respectively (Albin et al., 1989; Kravitz et al., 2010). Numerous lines of evidence support a role for the striatum in reinforcement. Early intracranial self-stimulation experiments showed that striatal stimulation is sufficient to drive operant responding in rodents, in the absence of food reward (Phillips et al., 1976; White and Hiroi, 1998). In addition, striatal microstimulation biases choice in cue-guided decision tasks in primates (Nakamura and Hikosaka, 2006; Williams and Eskandar, 2006). Although some of these effects are due to dopamine release from axon stimulation, pathway-specific optogenetic stimulation later revealed that direct pathway stimulation is reinforcing, whereas indirect pathway stimulation drives avoidance (Hikida et al., 2010; Kravitz et al., 2012). Indeed, mice readily learned an operant task to self-stimulate dMSNs and formed a memory of the stimulation-paired apparatus. This suggests that dMSN stimulation is not just acutely rewarding but can drive long-lasting changes in the brain supporting learning and memory (Kravitz et al., 2012). Consistent with this, stimulation of direct and indirect pathways oppositely biases both choice and vigor in water-reinforced tasks (Tai et al., 2012; Yttri and Dudman, 2016). Similarly, in vivo recordings from primates and rodents show that activity in discrete populations of MSNs encodes reward (Hikosaka et al., 1989; Apicella et al., 1991; Oyama et al., 2010), actions and/or their values (Lauwereyns et al., 2002; Barnes et al., 2005; Pasupathy and Miller, 2005; Samejima et al., 2005; Lau and Glimcher, 2008; Kim et al., 2009; Kimchi and Laubach, 2009). Together, these results demonstrate that MSNs both integrate and support motor and reinforcement functions.

The involvement of regions downstream of the basal ganglia in motor control and reinforcement has also been extensively studied. Different brainstem targets of basal ganglia are associated with regulation of distinct aspects motor behavior: the superior colliculus is implicated in orienting (Hikosaka et al., 2000), the periaqueductal gray is involved in freezing (Gross and Canteras, 2012), and the mesencephalic locomotor region (MLR) is engaged for locomotion (Esposito and Arber, 2016), whereas Dorsal Raphe Nucleus (DRN) neurons are involved in mood regulation and state-dependent control of motivation (Cohen et al., 2015). Basal-ganglia-recipient motor thalamus, which consists of ventromedial (VM), ventral anterior and lateral (VAL) nuclei, is involved in motor performance and skill learning (Hikosaka et al., 2000; Graybiel, 2005; Turner and Desmurget, 2010; Bosch-Bouju et al., 2013; Goldberg et al., 2013; Kawai et al., 2015). Extensive intracranial self-stimulation studies have revealed that a multitude of sites across the brain, including different brainstem targets of basal ganglia, are sufficient to drive reinforcement (Wise, 1996). Recent optogenetic studies also showed that activation of specific cell populations in the MLR (Dautan et al., 2016; Xiao et al., 2016; Yoo et al., 2017) and DRN (Liu et al., 2014; McDevitt et al., 2014) is reinforcing. Additionally, electrical stimulation of motor thalamus (Clavier and Gerfen, 1982) supports operant behavior, and lesions impair striatum-dependent learning (Zis et al., 1984). However, it is poorly understood whether these basal ganglia output targets are necessary for striatum-driven reinforcement. Indeed, after almost 70 years of research on the anatomical substrates of reinforcement, it is still debated whether nuclei supporting self-stimulation operate in series within a single circuit, or belong to multiple, parallel systems (Routtenberg, 1971; Phillips, 1984; Wise and Bozarth, 1984).

Here, we took advantage of optogenetic dMSN self-stimulation in mice to probe both striatal plasticity and basal ganglia circuit mechanisms in striatum-driven reinforcement. In theory, repeated pairing of operant behavior with dMSN depolarization could locally induce Hebbian synaptic plasticity onto stimulated dMSNs and/or engage downstream circuits to drive reinforcement. We found that dMSN self-stimulation was not accompanied by measurable changes in synaptic strength of excitatory afferents, nor was it impaired by disrupting NMDA receptors in stimulated neurons. Furthermore, reinforcement could be elicited by direct suppression of SNr. In order to identify the contribution of basal ganglia target regions to striatal reinforcement, we combined dMSN self-stimulation with silencing of selected basal ganglia output targets. Although brainstem and thalamic targets were sufficient to drive reinforcement, only motor thalamus was necessary for dMSN self-stimulation. Thus, our results highlight an underappreciated circuit mechanism for striatum-driven reinforcement through recruitment of motor thalamus.

Results

dMSN self-stimulation supports reinforcement independently of NMDA receptors

We bilaterally injected Cre-dependent ChR2-eYFP virus and implanted optical fibers in the dorsomedial striatum (DMS) of D1-Cre mice (D1-ChR2) for selective self-stimulation of dMSNs, which represented the primary reinforcer motivating task performance (Figure 1a, Figure 1—figure supplement 1a). Mice learned to self-stimulate dMSNs by poking in an active (laser-paired) nosepoke (Figure 1b), whereas a second, inactive nosepoke had no effect. We characterized the self-stimulation behavior by varying different task parameters. Poke rate was sensitive to laser duration, stimulation pattern, effort, and contingency degradation, all of which are hallmarks of goal-directed behavior (Figure 1—figure supplement 2). For subsequent experiments, we chose one second of constant laser stimulation per nosepoke to optimally balance poke rate and total laser stimulation. Within a single session, D1-ChR2 mice began to make more nosepokes on the active side and rapidly neglected the inactive nosepoke (Figure 1c,d). In contrast, control mice (expressing only eYFP) produced fewer and equal numbers of nosepokes on both sides. Overall, no difference in total locomotion was observed during the session (Figure 1—figure supplement 2f). The total number of active nosepokes per session was stable over days (Figure 1e). Similarly, poking patterns did not change over time, showing stable inter-poke interval distributions and averages between day 1 and 4. Poking behavior could be divided in epochs of poking bursts, defined here by at least two nosepokes performed in <2 s, or an alternative criterion of <6 s. In either case, the number of bursts and pokes/bursts did not change over days (Figure 1—figure supplement 3). Additionally, when mice were subjected to an extinction session 24 hr after the last training session, D1-ChR2 mice made significantly more nosepokes on the previously-active side, despite the absence of dMSN stimulation (Figure 1g). In contrast, D1-eYFP mice showed no preference for either side. Together these results demonstrate that behavior was learned and stabilized within a single session, and that mice form a stable association (lasting at least 24 hr) between the nosepoke and the reinforcer (dMSN stimulation). Therefore, we focused our analysis on the first behavior session (see materials and methods).

Figure 1. dMSN self-stimulation elicits rapid and persistent NMDAR-independent reinforcement.

(a) Schematic of a sagittal brain section showing optogenetic stimulation of striatal dMSNs (left), and slice showing fluorescent dMSNs after striatal infusion of a Cre-dependent virus encoding eYFP in a D1-Cre mouse (right). (b) Schematic of the behavioral apparatus showing active (laser-paired) and inactive nosepokes. (c) Example cumulative plot of active nosepokes from a D1-ChR2 (blue) and D1-eYFP (black) mouse. (d) Average nosepokes during a single self-stimulation session for D1-ChR2 and D1-eYFP mice (n = 11 and 8, two-way ANOVA, interaction poke x group, F(1,17)=31.03, p<0.001, posthoc Sidak’s multiple comparisons test, ChR2 active vs eYFP active p<0.001, ChR2 active vs inactive p<0.001, eYFP active vs inactive p=0.921). (e) Average nosepokes during four consecutive daily sessions (n = 8, one-way ANOVA, F(1.908,13.36) = 1.237, p=0.319). (f) Distribution of (left) and average (right) inter-poke intervals for days 1 and 4 of dMSN self-stimulation (n = 8, paired t-test, p=0.0575). (g) Average nosepokes during an extinction session for D1-ChR2 and D1-eYFP mice (n = 8 and 8, two-way ANOVA, interaction poke x group, F(1,14)=47.86, p<0.0001, posthoc Sidak’s multiple comparisons test, ChR2 active vs eYFP active p<0.0001, ChR2 active vs inactive p<0.0001, eYFP active vs inactive p=0.5703). (h) Example cumulative plot of active nosepokes from D1-ChR2 mice expressing (D1-Cre, blue) or lacking (D1-Cre x NR1f/f, red) NMDA receptors in dMSNs. (i) Average nosepokes during a single self-stimulation session for D1-ChR2 mice with (‘D1’) or without (‘D1-NR1KO’) NMDA receptors in dMSNs (n = 9 and 7, two-way ANOVA, interaction poke x group, F(1,14)=0.638, p=0.438, posthoc Sidak’s multiple comparisons test, D1 active vs D1-NR1 active p=0.418). (j) Average distance travelled by D1-ChR2 mice with (‘D1’) or without (‘KO’) NMDA receptors in dMSNs (Mann Whitney U test, p=0.023).

Figure 1—source data 1. Source data for Figure 1.
DOI: 10.7554/eLife.34032.008

Figure 1.

Figure 1—figure supplement 1. Fiber placement and infusion sites.

Figure 1—figure supplement 1.

(a) Fiber tip placement in the striatum for D1-ChR2 (blue), D1-eYFP (white) and D1-NR1 (red) cohorts shown in Figure 1 (left), and intracellular slice recording from a dMSN expressing ChR2 from a D1-Cre x NR1f/f mouse injected with DIO-ChR2, showing absence of NMDA receptor-dependent current at +40 mV. (b) Fiber tip placement in the SNr for Arch3 (green) and eYFP (white) cohorts shown in Figure 3. (c) Fiber tip placement in VM for ChR2 (blue) and eYFP (white) cohorts shown in Figure 3. (d) Fiber tip placement in the DRN for ChR2 (blue) and eYFP (white) cohorts shown in Figure 3. (e) Fiber tip placement for axonal dMSN stimulation over the cerebral peduncle/anterior SNr (left), and coronal slices from mice infused with saline or FLEx-Caspase three in the DRN showing intact or absent 5HT expression in DRN, respectively (right), from mice used in Figure 4. (f) Fiber tip placement in the striatum (left) and infusion site (right) for D1-ChR2 mice infused saline (blue) or muscimol (red) used for self-stimulation or locomotion (grey) assays shown in Figure 5. (g) Left: Fiber tip placement for dMSN axonal stimulation over the cerebral peduncle (cp) in vGAT-cre mice infused with DIO-ChR2 in the striatum and DIO-ChR2 (blue) or DIO-eYFP (white) in the SNr, and coronal slice showing eYFP +dMSN fibers and fiber tip. Right: In the same mice, fiber tip placement for SNr terminal stimulation in VM, and coronal slice from a vGAT-Cre mouse infused with DIO-eYFP in the striatum and DIO-mCherry in the SNr, showing segregation of dMSN axons (green) en route to the SNr, and SNr terminals (red) in VM. From mice used in Figure 5.
Figure 1—figure supplement 2. Characterization of dMSN self-stimulation.

Figure 1—figure supplement 2.

(a) Relationship between laser duration and poke rate. Dotted box highlights chosen parameter for further experiments. (b) Relationship between stimulation pattern and poke rate (all stimuli delivered for 1 s). Dotted box highlights chosen parameter for further experiments. (c) Relationship between the number of nosepokes required to obtain laser (Fixed Ratio), poke rate, and laser exposure. (d) Average nosepoke rate before, during, and after a contingency degradation session (Deg), during which mice received an average of 5 × 1 s of laser per minute non-contingent on nosepokes (n = 8, one-way ANOVA, treatment F(1.405,9.832) = 10.11, p=0.007, posthoc Tukey’s multiple comparisons test, preTrain vs Deg p=0.016, Deg vs postTrain p=0.001). (f) Relationship between stimulation pattern and poke rate (all stimuli delivered for 1 s) for dMSN axon self-stimulation over the cerebral peduncle.
Figure 1—figure supplement 2—source data 1. Source data for Figure 1—figure supplement 2.
DOI: 10.7554/eLife.34032.005
Figure 1—figure supplement 3. dMSN self-stimulation pattern is stable over days.

Figure 1—figure supplement 3.

(a) Average number of poking bursts for days 1 and 4 of dMSN self-stimulation with criterion of inter poke interval (ipi) <2 s or <6 s (paired t-test,<2 s p=0.364;<6 s p=0.396). (b) Average number of nosepokes/bursts for days 1 and 4 of dMSN self-stimulation with criterion of inter poke interval (ipi) <2 s or <6 s (Wilcoxon signed rank test,<2 s p=0.383;<6 s p=0.383).
Figure 1—figure supplement 3—source data 1. Source data for Figure 1—figure supplement 3.
DOI: 10.7554/eLife.34032.007

Current models of basal ganglia reinforcement posit that synaptic plasticity of excitatory cortical inputs onto MSNs supports learning (Schultz and Dickinson, 2000; Doya, 2007; Maia and Frank, 2011). NMDA receptors have been implicated in the induction of various forms of synaptic plasticity in the striatum (Calabresi et al., 1992; Thomas et al., 2000; Shen et al., 2008), and interfering with striatal NMDA receptors disrupts action-outcome associations (Yin et al., 2005a). To assess a role for synaptic plasticity in dMSN reinforcement, we generated mice lacking NMDA receptors in dMSNs (D1-Cre x NR1f/f), in which various forms of plasticity are altered (Dang et al., 2006). However, these mice showed normal dMSN self-stimulation, despite a mild decrease in locomotion (Figure 1h–j, Figure 1—figure supplement 1a).

We next tested whether any trace of synaptic plasticity could be observed in stimulated (ChR2+) dMSNs after training. We trained D1-Cre x D1-tmt mice injected with Cre-dependent ChR2 or eYFP and then prepared acute slices for whole-cell recordings. We targeted neurons directly underneath the optic fiber tip (Figure 2a). We recorded: ChR2 +and ChR2- dMSNs (identified by tmt expression and presence or absence of ChR2-dependent photocurrent, respectively) in D1-ChR2 mice; eYFP +neurons in D1-eYFP mice, and tmt +dMSNs in naive D1-tmt mice. We examined mEPSCs, AMPA/NMDA ratios, paired-pulse ratio, and intrinsic excitability of dMSNs, but found no difference in any of these parameters after training (Figure 2). Similarly, we probed synaptic transmission and excitability in iMSNs (D1-tmt negative) after dMSN self-stimulation and did not observe any plastic changes (Figure 2—figure supplement 1). Taken together, these results prompted us to explore the involvement of circuitry downstream of striatum in reinforcement.

Figure 2. No measurable changes in excitatory synaptic transmission or excitability in dMSNs after self-stimulation.

(a) Coronal section of the striatum showing ChR2-eYFP expression in DMS, optic fiber track and recording area (dotted box), corresponding approximately to the portion of striatum illuminated during behavior. (b) Whole-cell recordings of dMSNs (tmt+) from trained D1-ChR2 mice (including ChR2 +and ChR2- neurons, identified by the presence or absence of a blue light-evoked photocurrent, inset) and naive mice (tmt+), showing average (left) and example traces (right) for mEPSC frequency and amplitude (frequency: Kruskal-Wallis test, p=0.505; amplitude, one-way ANOVA, F(2,32)=0.6141, p=0.547). (c,d) Whole-cell recordings of dMSNs (tmt+) from trained D1-ChR2 mice (including ChR2 +and ChR2- neurons), trained D1-eYFP mice (eYFP +neurons), or naive mice (tmt+), showing averages (left) and example traces (right) for AMPA/NMDA ratio and paired-pulse ratio (c) and excitability (d) [AMPA/NMDA ratio: n = 8,7,6,7; one-way ANOVA, F(3,24)=0.062, p=0.9793; paired-pulse ratio: n = 8,11,11,7; one-way ANOVA, F(3,32)=1.358, p=0.273; excitability: n = 6,8,14; two-way ANOVA, interaction current x group F(10,125)=0.3801, p=0.9533].

Figure 2—source data 1. Source data for Figure 2.
DOI: 10.7554/eLife.34032.012

Figure 2.

Figure 2—figure supplement 1. No measurable changes in excitatory synaptic transmission or excitability in iMSNs after dMSN self-stimulation.

Figure 2—figure supplement 1.

(a,b) Whole-cell recordings of putative iMSNs (tmt-) from trained D1-ChR2 (ChR2), D1-eYFP (eYFP) or naive mice (naive), showing average and example traces for AMPA/NMDA and paired-pulse ratios (a) and excitability (b) [AMPA/NMDA ratio: n = 7,6,5; one-way ANOVA, treatment F(2,15)=0.3426, p=0.715; Paired-pulse ratio: n = 10,6,5; Kruskal Wallis test, p=0.753; excitability: n = 11,6; two-way ANOVA, interaction current x group F(5,75)=0.438, p=0.820].
Figure 2—figure supplement 1—source data 1. Source data for Figure 2—figure supplement 1.
DOI: 10.7554/eLife.34032.011

Reinforcement through modulation of circuitry downstream of striatum

Given the widespread influence of the basal ganglia over multiple brain structures (Freeze et al., 2013; Oldenburg and Sabatini, 2015; Lee et al., 2016), we asked which specific downstream circuits could be involved in dMSN-driven reinforcement. Activation of inhibitory dMSNs leads to a net suppression of SNr neurons and disinhibition of downstream targets. First, we tested whether bypassing the striatum and directly inhibiting the SNr would be sufficient to recapitulate direct pathway-driven reinforcement. To do so, we bilaterally expressed Cre-dependent Arch3.0 in the SNr of vGAT-Cre mice. Surprisingly, these animals also learned to self-inhibit the SNr (Figure 3a–c, Figure 1—figure supplement 1b).

Figure 3. Optogenetic control of basal ganglia output neurons or their projection targets is reinforcing.

Figure 3.

(a) Schematic of a sagittal brain section showing optogenetic inhibition of SNr (left), and coronal slice showing Arch3-eYFP and TH expression after SNr infusion of a Cre-dependent construct in a vGAT-Cre mouse and fiber track (right). (b) Example cumulative plot of active nosepokes from a SNr-Arch3 (green) and SNr-eYFP (black) mouse. (c) Average nosepokes during a single self-inhibition session for SNr-Arch3 and SNr-eYFP mice (n = 7 and 10, two-way ANOVA, interaction poke x group, F(1,15)=13.01, p=0.003, posthoc Sidak’s multiple comparisons test, Arch3 active vs eYFP active p=0.007). (d) Schematic of a sagittal brain section showing optogenetic excitation of VM (left), and coronal slice showing ChR2-eYFP expression after VM infusion of CaMKIIα-ChR2 in a WT mouse (right). (e) Example cumulative plot of active nosepokes from a VM-ChR2 (blue) and VM-eYFP (black) mouse. (f) Average nosepokes during a single self-stimulation session for VM-ChR2 and VM-eYFP mice (n = 8 and 8, two-way ANOVA, interaction poke x group, F(1,14)=22.17, p=0.003, posthoc Sidak’s multiple comparisons test, ChR2 active vs eYFP active p<0.001). (g) Schematic of a sagittal brain section showing optogenetic excitation of DRN (left), and coronal slice showing ChR2-eYFP overlapping with 5HT expression after infusion of DIO-ChR2 in a SERT-Cre mouse and fiber track (right). (h) Example cumulative plot of active nosepokes from a DRN-ChR2 (blue) and DRN-eYFP (black) mouse. (i) Average nosepokes during a single self-stimulation session for DRN-ChR2 and DRN-eYFP mice (n = 4 and 6, two-way ANOVA, interaction poke x group, F(1,8)=38.09, p<0.001, posthoc Sidak’s multiple comparisons test, ChR2 active vs eYFP active p<0.001). (j) Schematic of a sagittal brain section showing optogenetic excitation of the MLR (left), and coronal slice showing ChR2-eYFP expression after MLR infusion (centered around the PPTg) of DIO-ChR2 in a vGLUT2-Cre mouse and fiber tracks (right). (k) Example cumulative plot of active nosepokes from an MLR-ChR2 responsive (resp, blue) and MLR-ChR2 non-responsive (non-resp, black) mouse. (l) Average nosepokes during a single self-stimulation session for MLR-ChR2 responsive and non-responsive mice (n = 2 and 3). Abbreviations: 5HT, 5-hydroxytryptamine; DRN, dorsal raphe nucleus; MLR, mesencephalic locomotor region, PPTg, pedunculopontine tegmentum; SNc, substantia nigra compacta; SNr, substantia nigra reticulata; TH, tyrosine hydroxylase; VM, ventromedial thalamus; VTA, ventral tegmental area.

Figure 3—source data 1. Source data for Figure 3.
DOI: 10.7554/eLife.34032.014

The SNr distributes basal ganglia output across several brain regions, including a canonical projection to ventromedial thalamus, traditionally recognized as part of the motor thalamus (Starr and Summerhayes, 1983; Klockgether et al., 1985; Deniau and Chevalier, 1992). It was recently shown that VM neurons send projections to and interact with prefrontal cortices, which are involved in decision-making and learning (Kuramoto et al., 2015; Guo et al., 2017). Therefore, we asked whether activation of VM would be sufficient to support operant behavior. We targeted VM neurons with CaMKIIα-ChR2-containing virus in WT mice. These mice showed robust self-stimulation of VM (Figure 3d–f, Figure 1—figure supplement 1c).

Brainstem targets of SNr, including the DRN (Liu et al., 2014; McDevitt et al., 2014; Cohen et al., 2015) and the pedunculopontine tegmentum, a part of the MLR (Xiao et al., 2016; Yoo et al., 2017), have also been associated with reinforcement. However, there is some uncertainty about the neuronal identity of cell populations involved. When targeting serotonergic neurons of the DRN, we observed self-stimulation in SERT-Cre mice injected with Cre-dependent ChR2 (Figure 3g–i, Figure 1—figure supplement 1d), but not in ePET-Cre mice (data not shown). In the MLR, glutamatergic neurons have been shown to receive innervation from SNr and drive both locomotion (Roseberry et al., 2016) and reinforcement (Yoo et al., 2017). We targeted these neurons with Cre-dependent ChR2 infusion into the MLR of vGLUT2-Cre mice and functionally validated expression by confirming increased locomotion upon stimulation (data not shown). When placed into the operant chamber, 2 of 5 mice showed clear self-stimulation behavior, whereas the other three showed no preference (Figure 3j–l, see materials and methods for responder criterion). Taken together, these results show that self-stimulation behavior can be elicited in both the DRN and MLR, raising the possibility that they are also engaged during dMSN self-stimulation to mediate reinforcement.

DRN serotonergic and MLR glutamatergic neurons are not required for dMSN-driven reinforcement

We next asked whether activation of DRN serotonergic neurons or MLR glutamatergic neurons is necessary for dMSN-driven reinforcement. To do so we combined dMSN self-stimulation with various approaches to silence these downstream nuclei. To test the role of DRN serotonin neurons, we virally expressed Cre-dependent Caspase three in SERT-Cre mice, leading to loss of SERT + cells (Figure 1—figure supplement 1e). Selective dMSN excitation was achieved by injecting hSyn-ChR2-eYFP virus into the striatum and placing optic fibers above the anterior tip of SNr, where only fibers from dMSNs are present (Figure 4a). This approach yielded reliable dMSN self-stimulation, similar to striatal cell body self-stimulation (Figure 1—figure supplement 2e). However, we observed no difference in the number of nosepokes performed by mice with lesioned or intact DRN (Figure 4b,c).

Figure 4. Silencing DRN serotonergic neurons or MLR glutamatergic neurons does not disrupt dMSN self-stimulation.

(a) Sagittal schematic showing injection of a hSyn-ChR2 construct in the striatum and DIO-Casp3 in the DRN of a SERT-Cre mouse, and optic fiber placement above dMSN axons. (b) Example cumulative plot of active nosepokes for dMSN axon stimulation in mice infused with saline (sal, blue) or DIO-Casp3 (casp, orange). (c) Average nosepokes during a single self-stimulation session for dMSN axon self-stimulation in mice with intact (sal) or lesioned (casp) serotonergic neurons in DRN (sal versus casp, two-way ANOVA, interaction pokes x stim F(1,8)=0.45, p=0.521, posthoc Sidak’s multiple comparisons test, sal active vs casp active p=0.76). (d) Sagittal schematic showing optogenetic stimulation of dMSNs and inhibition of MLR glutamatergic neurons (CaMKIIα-eNpHR3.0). (e) Example cumulative plot of active nosepokes from D1-ChR2 mice for dMSN stimulation alone (blue) or paired with MLR inhibition (green). (f) Average nosepokes during a single self-stimulation session for dMSN stimulation alone or paired with MLR inhibition, or MLR inhibition alone (D1 +versus D1+ and MLR-, two-way ANOVA, interaction pokes x stim F(1,10)=1.618, p=0.232, posthoc Sidak’s multiple comparisons test, D1 +active vs D1+ and MLR- active p=0.142; MLR-, Wilcoxon signed rank test, active vs inactive p=0.625).

Figure 4—source data 1. Source data forFigure 4.
DOI: 10.7554/eLife.34032.018

Figure 4.

Figure 4—figure supplement 1. MLR glutamatergic neurons are necessary for dMSN-driven locomotion.

Figure 4—figure supplement 1.

(a) Sagittal schematic showing optogenetic stimulation of dMSNs and inhibition of MLR glutamatergic neurons (CaMKIIα-eNpHR3.0). (b) Average distance travelled before (pre), during (laser) and after (post) dMSN stimulation with (green) or without (blue) MLR inhibition, and example tracks (right) (n = 6 mice, two-way ANOVA, interaction epoch x stim F(2,10)=10.15, p=0.004, posthoc Sidak’s multiple comparisons test, laser D1 vs D1 +MLR p<0.001).
Figure 4—figure supplement 1—source data 1. Source data for Figure 4—figure supplement 1.
DOI: 10.7554/eLife.34032.017

We then asked whether glutamatergic neurons in the MLR could be involved in dMSN-driven reinforcement. We expressed Cre-dependent ChR2 in the striatum of D1-Cre mice and eNpHR3.0 in the MLR under the CaMKIIα promoter to target glutamatergic neurons (Figure 4d). We confirmed that silencing MLR glutamatergic neurons reduced dMSN-evoked locomotion (Figure 4—figure supplement 1), as previously reported (Roseberry et al., 2016). However, we observed no difference in the total number of nosepokes in mice for dMSN stimulation alone or paired with inhibition of MLR glutamatergic neurons (Figure 4e,f).

VM supports dMSN-driven reinforcement

Given that the brainstem targets of basal ganglia output that we tested do not appear to be required for dMSN-mediated reinforcement, we next examined the role of SNr-recipient VM thalamus. In order to test whether VM activity contributes to dMSN-mediated reinforcement, we inhibited VM with muscimol, a GABAA receptor agonist, in D1-ChR2 mice prior to optogenetic self-stimulation (Figure 5a, Figure 1—figure supplement 1f). Overall, VM-muscimol mice self-stimulated less than saline-infused control mice (Figure 5b,c). In contrast, VM silencing had no effect on either spontaneous or dMSN-evoked locomotion (Figure 5d,e). To examine the requirement for VM with higher temporal resolution, we injected Cre-dependent ChR2-eYFP in the striatum and DIO-ChR2-eYFP or DIO-eYFP control virus in the SNr of vGAT-Cre mice (Figure 5f, Figure 1—figure supplement 1g). For dMSN-selective stimulation, we bilaterally implanted optic fibers above the cerebral peduncle, which contains axons of dMSNs on their way to the SNr. We also implanted a second set of fibers above VM, to specifically manipulate axon terminals of VM-projecting SNr neurons. This design enabled us to prevent VM disinhibition only during dMSN stimulation. Similar to what we observed with muscimol infusions, mice made fewer nosepokes when dMSN stimulation was paired with SNr terminal stimulation in VM, compared to control mice expressing only eYFP in the SNr (Figure 5g,h). SNr terminals were stimulated at 20 Hz, a frequency similar to basal SNr firing rates in mice (Freeze et al., 2013), and which alone was not reinforcing. Together, these results indicate that VM disinhibition is a critical component of the reinforcing circuit initiated by dMSN self-stimulation.

Figure 5. VM thalamus inhibition disrupts dMSN self-stimulation.

Figure 5.

(a) Schematic of a sagittal brain section showing optogenetic excitation of striatal dMSNs combined with muscimol infusion in VM, and coronal slice from a D1-Cre mouse showing ChR2-eYFP-expressing fibers from dMSNs en route to SNr (green) and infusion site in VM (DiI, orange). (b) Example cumulative plot of active nosepokes from D1-ChR2 mice infused with saline (sal, blue) or muscimol (mus, red). (c) Average nosepokes during a single self-stimulation session for D1-ChR2 mice infused with saline (sal) or muscimol (mus, n = 8 and 7, two-way ANOVA, interaction poke x group, F(1,13)=6.33, p=0.026, posthoc Sidak’s multiple comparisons test, sal active vs mus active p=0.0009, sal active vs inactive p<0.001, musactive vs inactive p=0.018). (d) Average spontaneous locomotion in D1-ChR2 mice infused in VM with muscimol or saline (left), and example tracks (right) (n = 3, Wilcoxon matched-pairs signed rank test, p=0.5). (e) Average distance travelled before (pre), during (laser) and after (post) dMSN stimulation in the same mice as in g (left), and example tracks (right) (two-way ANOVA, interaction laser x drug F(2,4)=0.873, p=0.485, main effect of laser F(2,4)=14.16, p=0.015, posthoc Tukey’s multiple comparisons test, sal pre vs laser p=0.002, mus pre vs laser p=0.001, sal laser vs mus laser p=0.994). (f) Schematic of a sagittal brain section showing injection of Cre-dependent ChR2 in the striatum and SNr of a vGAT-Cre mouse (left) and fiber placement above dMSN axons and in VM for optogenetic excitation of dMSN combined with excitation of SNr terminals in VM, respectively (right). (g) Example cumulative plot of active nosepokes for dMSN axon stimulation paired with SNr-eYFP (D1+, blue) or SNr-ChR2 terminal stimulation in VM (D1+ and SNr+, red). (h) Average nosepokes during a single self-stimulation session from the same mice as in e (n = 6 and 6, two-way ANOVA, interaction poke x group, F(1,10)=12.95, p=0.0049, posthoc Sidak’s multiple comparisons test, D1 +active vs D1+ and SNr +active p<0.001, D1 +active vs inactive p<0.001, D1+ and SNr +active vs inactive p=0.018; SNr+, Wilcoxon signed rank test, active vs inactive p=0.625).

Figure 5—source data 1. Source data for Figure 5.
DOI: 10.7554/eLife.34032.020

Finally, to better understand how basal ganglia output modulates VM, we performed ex vivo and in vivo recordings in VM during stimulation of basal ganglia circuitry. We first expressed ChR2 in the SNr of vGAT-Cre mice. We prepared acute slices of VM from the same mice and performed whole-cell recordings in VM that confirmed the inhibitory nature of this projection (Figure 6—figure supplement 1a). Next, we performed extracellular recordings from 163 VM neurons in awake, head-fixed mice while stimulating dMSNs for 1 s, mimicking task conditions (Figure 6a, Figure 6—figure supplement 1b). VM cells exhibited a variety of responses, from excitation to inhibition (Figure 6b), with the average Z-score of all units showing a net increase in firing during dMSN stimulation (Figure 6c). Within 20 ms of stimulation, most responsive VM neurons were excited (Figure 6d); these excited neurons displayed a shorter latency to change firing rate than inhibited cells. Within a 100 ms of stimulation, more cells were excited, and a significant fraction of neurons with decreased activity appeared (Figure 6e). Over 1 s of stimulation, a majority of neurons were excited, while a small fraction of cells did not show any significant change in firing (Figure 6f). There was no obvious spatial segregation of response type within VM (Figure 6—figure supplement 1c). These results demonstrate that dMSN activation increases activity in a majority of VM thalamus neurons and, taken together with the VM self-stimulation data (Figure 3) and inactivation data (Figure 5), further support a role for VM disinhibition in dMSN-driven reinforcement.

Figure 6. dMSN stimulation increases firing in VM thalamus.

(a) Sagittal schematic depicting dMSN optogenetic stimulation with simultaneous in vivo single-unit recording from VM in awake, head-fixed mouse. (b) Peri-event time histogram of all 163 cells recorded in VM, aligned to dMSN stimulation (blue bar, 1 s duration). Units are sorted by response type (increase [excit], no significant change [ns], decrease [inhib]) and modulation index (see below). (c) Average z-scored response of all VM neurons to dMSN stimulation (blue shading, 1 s duration, n = 163, baseline vs stim, Wilcoxon signed rank test, p=0.0006). (d–f) Detailed analysis of responses for the first 20 ms (d), first 100 ms (e) or full 1 s (f) of dMSN stimulation (blue shading). Average firing rate (top) and modulation index (bottom) for excited (red), inhibited (blue) or unmodulated units (grey). Latency to significant change in firing rate is shown in d, top inset. Bottom insets, pie charts showing fraction of excited (red), inhibited (blue) and unmodulated (grey) VM neurons during dMSN stimulation for each time window. See materials and methods for modulation index calculation.

Figure 6—source data 1. Source data for Figure 6.
elife-34032-fig6-data1.xlsx (405.1KB, xlsx)
DOI: 10.7554/eLife.34032.023

Figure 6.

Figure 6—figure supplement 1. Stimulation and recording sites for VM in vivo recordings.

Figure 6—figure supplement 1.

(a) Left, coronal section showing axonal eYFP expression in VM from a vGAT-Cre mouse expressing ChR2-eYFP in the SNr. Right, whole-cell recording at +40 mV in VM showing a GABAAR-mediated IPSC elicited by optical stimulation (blue tick) of ChR2-expressing terminals of SNr neurons, and absence of current at −70 mV (ECl). IPSC at +40 mV is blocked by picrotoxin (red trace). (b) Optic fiber tip placement in DMS for direct pathway stimulation during in vivo recordings in VM. (c) Coronal section of VM thalamus showing recording sites and modulation index.

Discussion

We used striatal dMSN self-stimulation as a model to dissect the circuit mechanisms underlying a simple form of striatum-driven reinforcement. Our results highlight several important aspects of basal ganglia function. First, we demonstrate that activation of striatal direct pathway projection neurons can engage a form of reinforcement that is independent of NMDAR-dependent plasticity in dMSNs. Second, we find that multiple downstream targets of basal ganglia output can mediate reinforcement, not all of which are required for dMSN self-stimulation. Finally, we identify an unexpected role for motor thalamus in supporting dMSN-driven reinforcement.

An alternative to classic mechanisms of reinforcement

Models of the basal ganglia typically propose that corticostriatal synaptic plasticity underlies reinforcement (Schultz and Dickinson, 2000; Doya, 2007; Maia and Frank, 2011). We observed here that dMSN self-stimulation plateaued during the first day of training and that mice formed a memory of the dMSN stimulation-paired nosepoke, consistent with our previous report (Kravitz et al., 2012). These results suggest that behavior was learned and acquired within a single session, presumably by engaging long-term plasticity. We initially hypothesized that the repeated pairing of a specific action (entry into the active nosepoke) with depolarization of ChR2-expressing dMSNs would drive NMDAR-dependent Hebbian plasticity at active (task-related) synapses onto stimulated dMSNs. This mechanism would thereby provide a synaptic substrate for reinforcement learning, through the task-dependent recruitment of action-promoting dMSNs. However, we observed that dMSN self-stimulation was unaltered by NMDAR knockout in dMSNs (Figure 1). Additionally, despite our ability to identify and record from task-relevant ChR2-expressing dMSNs, we did not observe changes in pre or postsynaptic excitatory transmission, or in neuronal excitability, across both dMSNs and iMSNs (Figure 2). In fact, previous reports suggest that simple types of reinforcement can occur without NMDAR-dependent striatal plasticity. For example, mice with dorsal striatum lesions can still learn and perform an action to obtain food rewards (Yin et al., 2005b). Similarly, altering MSN NMDAR composition by knocking out GluN2B, an important subunit for plasticity induction (Paoletti et al., 2013), does not prevent mice from learning a basic operant task (Brigman et al., 2013). Likewise, genetic deletion of NMDA receptors in MSNs does not prevent mice from learning simple action-outcome associations (Jin and Costa, 2010); although see Beutler et al., 2011). Altogether, these results are consistent with the idea that simple forms of reinforcement may not require corticostriatal synaptic plasticity, but rather take advantage of a different mechanism.

Multiple basal ganglia output targets for reinforcement

Several basal ganglia output targets in the brainstem have been associated with reinforcement. Recently, a series of optogenetic studies reported a role for both DRN serotonergic and non-serotonergic populations in reinforcement. Consistent with these reports, we observed that mice self-stimulated serotonergic neurons of the DRN (Figure 3). We observed reinforcement in SERT-Cre mice, which failed to sustain operant behavior in a previous report (Fonseca et al., 2015). In ePET-Cre mice, we did not observe reinforcement, consistent with one previous report (McDevitt et al., 2014), but inconsistent with another recent study (Liu et al., 2014). These discrepancies could be explained by variable targeting of DRN subregions and stimulation parameters. Despite overall mixed observations about which exact neuronal population is sufficient for reinforcement, a common result is that DRN activation is reinforcing through direct excitation of midbrain dopamine neurons (Liu et al., 2014; McDevitt et al., 2014). In our hands, ablation of DRN SERT-positive neurons did not affect dMSN stimulation (Figure 4).

Similarly, the MLR receives projections from the SNr and has been associated with both locomotor control and reinforcement (Inglis et al., 2000; Ryczko and Dubuc, 2013). Both glutamatergic and cholinergic populations of the MLR have been associated with reinforcement (Dautan et al., 2016; Xiao et al., 2016; Yoo et al., 2017), and we have previously shown that striatal dMSNs drive locomotion by disinhibiting MLR glutamatergic neurons (Roseberry et al., 2016). We observed variable levels of MLR glutamatergic neuron self-stimulation at sites that reliably drove locomotion (Figure 3). However, dMSN-driven reinforcement did not require MLR glutamatergic neurons (Figure 4). Similar to DRN, MLR self-stimulation of both glutamatergic and cholinergic populations seems to rely on direct excitation of midbrain dopamine neurons (Xiao et al., 2016; Yoo et al., 2017). Altogether, these results show that dMSN self-stimulation does not require DRN serotonergic or MLR glutamatergic neurons, suggesting that these nuclei belong to separate reinforcement circuits.

A role for motor thalamus in reinforcement

VM thalamus is traditionally classified as a motor nucleus relaying basal ganglia signals toward motor cortex (Starr and Summerhayes, 1983; Klockgether et al., 1985; Turner and Desmurget, 2010). VM and VA/VL nuclei of the thalamus receive basal ganglia outputs (Carter and Fibiger, 1978; Di Chiara et al., 1979) and project to motor cortical regions (Herkenham, 1979). However, VM differs with unique, widespread axonal projections to Layer I across many cortical areas including prefrontal and parietal cortices that are associated with more cognitive functions (Kuramoto et al., 2015).

Consistent with the disinhibitory architecture of the basal ganglia (Deniau and Chevalier, 1985; Albin et al., 1989), we observed an overall increase in VM activity upon dMSN stimulation (Figure 6). Latency to excitation was short (~10–20 ms), in line with di-synaptic disinhibition via the SNr. However, a significant fraction of cells showed a decrease in firing, highlighting a degree of heterogeneity among VM neurons. These differences in response type could be based on connectivity or biophysical properties of VM neurons. Alternatively, inhibition could arise from polysynaptic network effects like recruitment of inhibition (via the reticular thalamic nucleus) by VM excited neurons or through lateral disinhibition within the SNr among VM-projecting cells. Both scenarios would be consistent with the longer latencies to inhibition observed here. Nevertheless, our results demonstrate that dMSN stimulation significantly increases activity in the majority of VM neurons (Figure 6), which is critical for dMSN-driven reinforcement (see below).

Mimicking dMSN-driven VM disinhibition with optogenetic VM activation supported operant behavior, similar to dMSN self-stimulation and SNr self-inhibition (Figure 3), and consistent with VM electrical self-stimulation in rats (Clavier and Gerfen, 1982). In contrast to results obtained with silencing of brainstem SNr targets, both extended silencing of VM with muscimol infusion, or temporally-precise SNr terminal excitation time-locked to dMSN stimulation, decreased the total number of nosepokes mice performed to obtain dMSN stimulation (Figure 5). The latter approach argues for an acute role of VM disinhibition in reinforcement, rather than a global alteration of brain state. Therefore, it is possible that the magnitude of reinforcement (number of nosepokes) scales with that of VM disinhibition, similar to graded dMSN self-stimulation observed with increasing stimulation frequencies (Figure 1—figure supplement 2). Importantly, VM silencing did not affect either spontaneous locomotion in the open field or dMSN locomotor drive, consistent with a primary role of MLR glutamatergic output in locomotor control. Altogether, these results identify VM as a key recipient of basal ganglia reinforcement signals.

Silencing VM did not fully block dMSN self-stimulation. This may be due to incomplete VM silencing, or the participation of additional mechanisms. For example, mediodorsal thalamus, a basal ganglia-recipient, non-motor nucleus, has been implicated in action-outcome learning (Chudasama et al., 2001; Corbit et al., 2002; Corbit et al., 2003) and working memory (Parnaudeau et al., 2013; Bolkan et al., 2017), and could support striatal reinforcement. The SNr also targets the superior colliculus, which displays reward-related activity (Jeljeli et al., 2003; Doya, 2007), and MLR cholinergic population, which has been shown to drive reinforcement (Xiao et al., 2016). Additionally, dMSN activation, through inhibition of SNc-projecting SNr neurons (Mailly et al., 2001; Brazhnik et al., 2008), may disinhibit SNc dopamine neurons and thus increase DA release, which is sufficient for reinforcement (Phillips et al., 1976; Gratton et al., 1988; Ilango et al., 2014). Interestingly, rodent VM is embedded in circuits separate from dopamine (Groenewegen, 1988; Papadopoulos and Parnavelas, 1990; Watabe-Uchida et al., 2012), suggesting that the basal ganglia could engage both dopamine-dependent and dopamine-independent reinforcement mechanisms. In fact, previous reports have shown that dopamine-deficient mice can still learn to locate food in a T-maze (Robinson et al., 2005) or develop place preference for morphine or cocaine (Hnasko et al., 2005; Hnasko et al., 2007), providing evidence that alternate, dopamine-independent reinforcement mechanisms can occur.

VM lesions have been shown to impair both the acquisition and consolidation of active avoidance (Zis et al., 1984), a behavior that is believed to depend on the basal ganglia (Delacour et al., 1977; Hormigo et al., 2016). In songbirds, vocal learning requires an intact basal ganglia-thalamocortical loop (Scharff and Nottebohm, 1991; Fee and Goldberg, 2011). Other lesion studies in rodents have suggested a role for VM in spatial navigation (Jeljeli et al., 2003) and working memory (Bailey and Mair, 2005), and more specifically in sustaining cortical motor preparatory activity in a working memory task (Guo et al., 2017). Indeed, VM/VAL activation and inhibition increases and decreases cortical activity, respectively. Thus, it appears that basal-ganglia-recipient motor thalamus may be important for engaging cortical plasticity and learning.

Distinct circuits for locomotion and reinforcement

Gross manipulations of striatum, including cocaine administration and bulk dMSN stimulation drive both locomotion and reinforcement (Di Chiara et al., 1979; Inglis et al., 2000; Kravitz et al., 2010; Kravitz et al., 2012). However, it has not been clear whether those two distinct processes can be dissociated in downstream circuitry. Interestingly, direct stimulation of both VM thalamus and MLR glutamatergic neurons, two main targets of basal ganglia outputs, increases locomotion (Klockgether et al., 1986; Roseberry et al., 2016) and sustains operant responding (Clavier and Gerfen, 1982; Yoo et al., 2017). However, we demonstrate a dissociation of these two basal-ganglia-driven functions at the level of output targets: reinforcement relies on VM but not MLR glutamatergic neurons (Figures 4 and 5), whereas basal ganglia-driven locomotion depends on MLR glutamatergic neurons, but not VM (Figure 5, Figure 4—figure supplement 1). Future studies will be required to understand the exact nature of basal ganglia signals transferred to motor thalamus and how this information is integrated in thalamocortical circuits to support cognitive behavior.

Materials and methods

Subjects

All procedures were in accordance with protocols approved by the UCSF Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee. Mice were maintained on a 12 hr/12 hr light/dark cycle and fed ad libitum. Experiments were carried out during the light cycle. 166 wild-type (C57BL/6) or transgenic (gene name in italic) mice were used for experiments as follows: 44 D1-Cre mice (Drd1, 27 females, 17 males, GENSAT #030778-UCD) were used for dMSN self-stimulation experiments. four male D1-Cre mice were used for in vivo recordings in VM. three female D1-Cre mice were used for dMSN-driven locomotion. 30 D1-Cre mice crossed to D1-tmt mice (Jackson #016204) and 9 D1-tmt mice were used for slice recordings in the striatum. 7 D1-Cre x NR1f/f mice (Grin1tm2Stl, six females, one male, Jackson #005246) were used for dMSN self-stimulation. 17 vGAT-Cre mice (Slc32a1, nine females, eight males, Jackson #016962) were used for SNr self-inhibition. 12 vGAT-Cre (six females, six males) were used for dMSN axon self-stimulation paired with SNr terminal stimulation in VM. one male vGAT-Cre mouse was used for slice recordings. 16 wild-type mice (15 females, one male) were used VM self-stimulation. 5 vGLUT2-Cre mice (Slc17a6, two females, three males, Jackon 016963) were used for MLR self-stimulation. 18 Sert-Cre (Slc6a4, seven females, 11 males, Jackson 014554) mice were used for DRN self-stimulation and dMSN self-stimulation combined with DRN caspase lesions.

Surgical procedures

All surgeries were carried out in aseptic conditions while mice were anaesthetized with isoflurane (5% for induction, 0.5–1.5% afterward) and then placed in a stereotactic frame (Kopf). The scalp was opened and bilateral holes were drilled in the skull. In DMS, viral constructs were injected through a 33-gauge steel cannula (Plastics1) using a syringe pump (World Precision Instruments) at a rate of 100–200 nl/minute. In the SNr and MLR, viral constructs were injected through a 33-gauge needle on a 5 μL NanoFil syringe (WPI), mounted on a microsyringe pump (UMP3; WPI) and controller (Micro4; WPI), at a rate of 50–100 nl/minute. Needles were removed 5 min after the injection finished. The following volumes were injected: 500–1000 nl (DMS), 250 nl (SNr), 100 nl (VM), 500 nl (MLR, DRN, SNc). The following coordinates were used for viral injections and optic fiber implantation (AP, lateral, DV virus, DV fiber, in mm, from bregma and brain surface): DMS +0.6, 1.5, 2.5, 2; SNr −3.5, 1.4, 4; VM −1.3, 0.8, 3.7, 3.2; MLR −0.8 (from lambda), 1.2, 3.6, 3.2; fiber for dMSN axon stimulation (cerebral peduncle) −1.7, 2.75, 4.0, with 14-degree lateral angle; DRN −4.1, 0.3 (right hemisphere only), 3.3, 2.8; SNc −3.7, 1.5, 4.2, 3.4. For SNr terminal stimulation in VM, VM coordinates were used for fiber implantation. After viral injection, 200-μm-diameter optical fibers (Thorlabs #FT200UMT) glued into 1.25 mm ferrules (Thorlabs CFLC128-10) were lowered through the same holes, unless noted otherwise. Ferrules were then secured to the skull with a layer of dental adhesive (C and B Metabond, Parkell), and covered in UV-cure dental cement (flowable composite, Henry Schein) solidified with brief (20 s) UV light exposure (dental LED light lamp, MUW), while the mouse’s eyes were protected with cardboard glasses. The scalp was then sutured shut around the dental cement. Buprenorphine HCl (0.1 mg/kg, intraperitoneal injection) and Ketoprofen (5 mg/kg, subcutaneous injection) were used for postoperative analgesia. Mice were given 2–4 weeks for recovery and viral expression after surgery before behavioral training started.

Viral constructs

For Cre-dependent neuronal excitation and inhibition, we used an adeno-associated virus serotype 5 (AAV5) carrying Channelrhodopsin-2 (hChR2(H124R)) or eArch3.0, respectively, fused to enhanced yellow fluorescent protein (eYFP) in a double-floxed inverted open reading frame (DIO) under the control of the EF1α promoter (AAV5-EF1α-DIO-hChR2(H124R)-eYFP, AAV5-EF1α-DIO-eArch3.0-eYFP and AAV5-EF1α-DIO-eYFP for controls, University of Pennsylvania Vector Core, diluted 1:3 in saline (ChR2 and eYFP)). For MLR glutamatergic neurons inhibition, we expressed halorhodopsin 3.0 (eNpHR3.0) under the control of CaMKIIα (AAV5-CaMKIIα-eNpHR3.0-eYFP, University of North Carolina Vector Core). For VM excitation, we expressed Channelrhodopsin-2 under the control of CaMKIIα (AAV5-CaMKIIα-hChR2(H124R)-eYFP, University of North Carolina Vector Core). For DRN serotonergic neuron ablation, we expressed Caspase three in a flip-excision switch (AAV5-FLEx-EF1α-taCasp3-TEVp, University of North Carolina Vector Core). DIO constructs were allowed at least 2 weeks before experiments, and CamKIIα constructs at least 4 weeks.

Awake infusions in VM

For awake drug infusions in VM, mice were implanted with a custom stainless steel headbar for head fixation. The scalp was removed and skull scraped clean and dry using a scalpel. VM sites (AP, lateral, in mm, from bregma: −1.3, 0.8) were marked with sharpie pen on the skull and covered with a drop of silicone elastomer (Kwik-Cast; WPI). Cyanoacrylate glue (Vetbond, 3M) was lightly dabbed on the skull. For combined VM infusions and dMSN self-stimulation, holes above DMS were then drilled, the viral construct was injected and optic fibers implanted in DMS, and secured as described above. Then, the headbar was levelled flat and lowered to touch lambda, covered with dental adhesive (C and B Metabond, Parkell) and secured with UV-cure dental cement. Only a thin layer of was applied above the marked VM sites. After at least 2 weeks of recovery, mice were habituated to head fixation in a 3-cm-wide acrylic cylinder for 10 min twice a day, 3 days prior to the beginning of the experiment. On the last day of habituation, mice were anesthetized, dental cement and silicone above VM were removed, and holes were drilled on the marked VM locations. The craniotomy was then covered in silicone elastomere and mice were given 24–48 hr for recovery. For drug infusions, mice were headfixed, levelled flat, and a glass pipette mounted on a microinjector (Nanoject) was lowered 3.7 mm from brain surface through the craniotomy to deliver 100 nl of muscimol (0.1 mM in saline, Tocris) over 2 min in VM. The pipette was removed 2 min later and the same procedure was repeated on the other hemisphere. The craniotomy was covered in silicone elastomer and mice were immediately transferred to the behavior boxes for training. After the last behavior session, mice were infused with 20 nl of DiI (ThermoFischer) on each side and then perfused.

Behavioral experiments

Self-Stimulation

Apparatus

The self-stimulation apparatus consisted of a 10 × 20 cm box made of white acrylic, equipped with two nosepokes with infrared beams (MIX-engineering) on the same wall and a houselight (MedAssociates). For optogenetic excitation, we used 473 nm wavelength lasers (Shanghai Lasers) adjusted to 1 mW at the output of the fiber optic implant for continuous exposure or 10 mW for pulsed excitation (Master-8, A.M.P.I). For optogenetic inhibition, we used continuous 532 nm wavelength adjusted to 3–10 mW at the output of the fiber optic implant. Laser-emitted light was fed in 200 μm, 0.39NA optic fiber cable (Thorlabs), split through a 1-to-2 commutator (Doric) to two optic fiber cables connected to the mouse’s head for bilateral neuronal manipulation. Behavior was performed in the dark. Mouse position and nosepokes were tracked with Noldus software (Ethovision).

Self-stimulation

Sessions lasted 30 min. The night before the first training session, mice were food- and water-restricted. For the first training session, both nosepokes were baited with 200 nl of 10% sucrose-containing water, to ensure apparatus exploration. Mice were bilaterally connected to the laser with fiber optic cables and placed in the box. Poking in the laser-paired nosepoke (active) triggered the laser and the houselight, and poking in the other nosepoke (inactive) had no effect. The nosepoke-laser contingency was counterbalanced across mice and fixed throughout training. Mice could reinitiate a nosepoke as soon as the laser turned off, or after a similar lapse for the inactive side, by exiting and reentering the poke. After the first session, mice went back to unlimited water and food access for the rest of the training. Across all conditions, total number of nosepokes was stable from the first session onward. Mice were excluded if by day four the number of active nosepokes had declined by more than 50% or if mice performed <1 poke/min on average over four sessions. Laser activation patterns and durations were set as follows: dMSN cell bodies, 1 s continuous; dMSN axons, 1 s 40 Hz, SNr, 3 s continuous; VM, 3 s 20 Hz; MLR excitation 3 s 20 Hz; MLR inhibition, 1.1 s continuous, with dMSN stimulation starting 0.1 s later, for 1 s; SNr terminals in VM, 1 s 20 Hz, simultaneous to dMSN axon stimulation; DRN 3 s 40 Hz; SNc 1 s 40 Hz. Pulse duration, 5 ms.

Throughout the manuscript, self-stimulation data come from the first behavior session, except for Figure 1c,d,h,i, where data come from the first session with a 1 s laser/nosepoke contingency (second overall session).

The extinction session happened 24 hr after the 4th training session. Mice were placed in the apparatus for 15 min, during which the previously laser-paired nosepoke became inactive. Mice intended for ex vivo recordings were trained for 4 days and slices were prepared 24 hr later.

Locomotion

Spontaneous and dMSN-driven locomotion was measured in a 20 cm diameter circular open field arena made of transparent acrylic placed on a square of white yoga mat. Mice were habituated for 30 min to the open field 1 day prior to the experiment. After VM infusions, mice were placed in the open field for 5 min to measure baseline locomotion. For dMSN-driven locomotion, we repeated five cycles consisting of 10 s pre-stimulation, 10 s of bilateral optogenetic stimulation and 10 s post-stimulation epochs, followed by 30 s of recovery. Each mouse received both saline and muscimol infusions in VM in a random order, 24 hr apart. For dMSN stimulation paired with MLR glutamate neurons inhibition, the same protocol was applied, except that dMSN stimulation alone cycles were alternated with dMSN stimulation paired with bilateral MLR inhibition cycles, 5 cycles of each. Total distance travelled was summed for each epoch type, and averaged across mice.

Electrophysiology in acute slices

Mice were euthanized with a lethal dose of ketamine and xylazine followed by transcardial perfusion with 8 ml of ice cold artificial corticospinal fluid (aCSF) containing (in mM): glycerol (250), KCl (2.5), MgCl2 (2), CaCl2 (2), NaH2PO4 (1.2), HEPES (10), NaHCO3 (21) and glucose (5). Coronal slices (250 µM) containing the DMS or the SNr were then prepared with a vibratome (Leica) in the same solution, before incubation in 33°C recording aCSF containing (in mM): NaCl (125), NaHCO3 (26), NaH2PO4 (1.25), KCl (2.5), MgCl2 (1), CaCl2 (2), glucose (12.5), continuously bubbled with 95/5% O2/CO2. After 30 min of recovery, slices were either kept at room temperature or transferred to a recording chamber superfused with recording aCSF (2.5 ml/min) at 33°C. Whole‐cell current‐clamp recordings were obtained using an internal solution containing (in mM): KGluconate (130), NaCl (10), MgCl2 (2), CaCl2 (0.16), EGTA (0.5), HEPES (10). Voltage-clamp recordings were obtained using an internal solution containing (in mM): CsMeSO3 (120), CsCl (15), NaCl (8), EGTA (0.5), HEPES (10), Mg-ATP (2), Na-GTP (0.3), TEA-Cl (10), QX-314 (5). EPSCs were evoked with a monopolar stimulation electrode (glass pipet) placed in the striatum, between the recorded cell and the cortex, and isolated in presence of picrotoxin (100 μM). TTX (1 μM) was added to isolate miniEPSCs. Cells were held at −70 mV except for NMDAR currents, which were recorded at +40 mV (measured 50 ms after stimulus onset). dMSNs were identified by tomato fluorescence. ChR2-positive neurons were identified by the presence of a photocurrent evoked by 500 ms blue light. eYFP-positive neurons were identified by eYFP fluorescence. SNr terminal excitation was achieved by flashing 470 nm filtered LED (Thorlabs LED4C driven by a Prizmatix BLCC-2) light through a 40x immersion objective (1–5 ms pulse, 0.1–1 mW/cm2). Holding current was varied from −70 to +40 mV. In VM, currents evoked from SNr terminal stimulation were only observed at positive potential and were blocked by picrotoxin (100 μM). Stimulation was applied every 10 s. Holding currents were not corrected for junction potential. Recordings were obtained with 3–4 MW resistance pipettes pulled from glass capillaries (Harvard Apparatus GC150TF-10) on a puller (Zeitz). Picrotoxin (Tocris) was prepared at stock concentration in H2O, then diluted in aCSF while kynurenic acid (Sigma) was directly added to aCSF for bath application. Data was acquired with custom written code in Igor software, which can be found in the source code files linked to this article.

In vivo electrophysiology

D1-Cre mice were injected with DIO-ChR2 virus, implanted with optical fibers over DMS, and implanted with headbars as described above. Recordings were performed in head-fixed mice sitting in an acrylic tube. Mice were acclimated to head-fixation in several habituation sessions during the week before recording. On the day of recording, craniotomies (500 µm diameter) were performed over VM (0.7 mm posterior of Bregma, 0.8 lateral of midline) in the left and right hemispheres. The dura was left intact. Craniotomies were covered with a drop of surgilube followed by silicon elastomer and the animal was allowed to recover for at least 4 hr. Acute recordings were performed using a 64 channel silicon probe (H2 Cambridge Neurotech) mounted on a micromanipulator. The probe was inserted into the craniotomy and driven vertically to a depth of ~4 mm. To reduce movement, the craniotomy was covered with 2% agarose in saline and a layer of mineral oil. The probe was allowed to settle for at least 15 min before acquiring data. Continuous broadband data were acquired at 30 kHz using a SpikeGadgets acquisition system and Trodes software. Blue light was delivered to the DMS ipsilateral to the recording site using a fiber-coupled LED (M470F3, Thorlabs). 1 s of stimulation was delivered every 10 s for 100 trials. The power at the fiber tip was 1 mW. Probe shanks were coated with CM-DiI.

Analysis

Spike sorting was performed using MountainSort software (Chung et al., 2017). After a manual merging step, clusters with a clear refractory period in their autocorrelogram, noise overlap metric <0.03 (Chung et al., 2017), and amplitude distribution without a sharp cutoff at low amplitudes were considered single units. Only units whose peak signal was recorded on an electrode site within VM were included in the analysis. We report data from 163 units recorded across 8 hemispheres of 4 mice. Peri-event time histograms (PETH) aligned to the onset of stimulation were computed for each unit’s firing rate using 10 ms bins. Z-scored PETHs were computed using the average and standard deviation of the PETH in a 1000 ms baseline period preceding stimulus onset.

To determine whether a unit’s firing was significantly modulated by the stimulation, a paired t-test was performed to compare the firing rate in a 20 ms, 100 ms, or 1000 ms response window following stimulus onset with the firing rate in a 1000 ms baseline window preceding stimulus onset in each trial. Units with p<0.05 were considered significantly modulated for a given response window. Significantly modulated units were defined as excited if their average firing rate during the response window was greater than the baseline and inhibited if it was less. The modulation index was calculated as:

(AvgFiringRespWindow-AvgFiringBaselineWindow) / (AvgFiringRespWindow + AvgFiringBaselineWindow)

The latency to significant modulation was defined as the second time bin in which two consecutive bins of the PETH were outside of the 97% confidence level of the poisson distribution calculated from the average firing rate 1000 ms prior to stimulus onset (Abeles, 1982). Latency was not computed for units lacking two consecutive bins outside of the 97% confidence level within the first 100 ms after stimulus onset.

Histology

After recordings were completed, animals were prepared for histology as described below. To identify recording sites, 100 μm coronal sections were cut on a freezing microtome and DiI signal from the electrode was identified using fluorescence microscopy. Location of electrode sites relative to VM were determined by matching the histological images to the Paxinos mouse atlas.

Histology

Animals were euthanized with a lethal dose of ketamine and xylazine (400 mg ketamine plus 20 mg xylazine per kilogram of body weight, i.p.) and transcardially perfused with PBS, followed by 4% paraformaldehyde (PFA). Following perfusion, brains were transferred into 4% PFA for 16–24 hr and then moved to a 30% sucrose solution in PBS for 2–3 d (all at 4 deg C). Brains were then frozen and cut into 30 μm coronal sections with a sliding microtome (Leica Microsystems, model SM2000R) equipped with a freezing stage (Physitemp). Free-floating slides were blocked for 1 hr in 10% Normal Donkey Serum (NDS) in 0.5% phosphate-buffer saline in Tween 20 (PBST) then incubated overnight in primary antibody (1:500; Aves chicken anti-YFP, #GFP-1020; Pel-Freez rabbit anti-TH, P40101; ImmunoStar goat anti-5HT, 20079), 3% NDS in 0.5% PBST. The following day, they were washed 3 times for 10 min each in 0.5% PBST and incubated for 1 hr in secondary antibody (1:1000, Jackson ImmunoResearch Donkey anti-Chicken 488, #703-545-155; Invitrogen Alexa-fluor donkey anti-rabbit 647, A-31573; Invitrogen Alexa-fluor donkey anti goat 647, A21447), 3% NDS in 0.5% PBST. Slices were then incubated for 5 min with 1:2000 DAPI. After this, slides were washed for 10 min in 0.5% PBST and 2 more 10 min periods with 1:1 PBS. Slides were then washed with 0.05% lithium carbonate and alcohol, rinsed with diH2O, mounted and coverslipped with Cytoseal 60.

Slides were scanned on a VS120 semi-automated fluorescent slide scanner (Olympus Scientific Solutions Americas Corp, USA). Some figure images were acquired using a 6D high throughput microscope (Nikon, USA). All images shown were made brighter for better print quality using Photoshop function ‘Vibrance’, ‘Brightness’ and ‘Contrast’ to change LUT. No detail was lost during this manipulation. Manual registration of slices was performed by scaling whole slice images in Adobe Illustrator, using the Paxinos mouse atlas (Academic Press, Orlando, FL) panels as a background reference.

Statistics

All statistics were calculated with Prism 7 (Graphpad) or Igor Pro (WaveMetrics). Distribution normality was tested for each data set to determine whether parametric or non-parametric tests would be adequate. Based on the number of groups and independent variables, we used t-test, Mann-Whitney U-test, 1-way and 2-way ANOVAs, followed by posthoc tests correcting for multiple comparisons. Only two-tailed tests were used. Tests and p-values are mentioned in figure legends. p-value<0.05 was considered significant (p<0.05*, p<0.01**, p<0.001***). Results are reported as mean +- SEM. Error bars represent SEM. When possible, sample size was calculated based on pilot cohort (powerandsamplesize.com) with a power of 0.8 and alpha of 0.05). All behavioral experiments were independently repeated at least twice. All data supporting the findings can be found in the source data file linked to this article.

Acknowledgements

We thank D Schulte and B Margolin for technical assistance. We thank J Berke and members of the Kreitzer lab for useful discussions. This research was supported by the Swiss National Science Foundation (ALL) and the NIH (ACK).

Funding Statement

The funders had no role in study design, data collection and interpretation, or the decision to submit the work for publication.

Funding Information

This paper was supported by the following grants:

  • Schweizerischer Nationalfonds zur Förderung der Wissenschaftlichen Forschung to Arnaud L Lalive.

  • National Institutes of Health U01 NS094342 to Anatol C Kreitzer.

  • National Institutes of Health P01 DA010154 to Anatol C Kreitzer.

  • National Institutes of Health R01 NS064984 to Anatol C Kreitzer.

Additional information

Competing interests

No competing interests declared.

Author contributions

Conceptualization, Resources, Formal analysis, Supervision, Funding acquisition, Validation, Investigation, Visualization, Writing—original draft, Project administration, Writing—review and editing.

Investigation, Methodology.

Conceptualization, Formal analysis, Funding acquisition, Validation, Investigation, Visualization, Writing—original draft, Project administration, Writing—review and editing.

Investigation, Methodology.

Conceptualization, Resources, Supervision, Funding acquisition, Investigation, Methodology, Writing—original draft, Writing—review and editing.

Ethics

Animal experimentation: This study was performed in strict accordance with the recommendations in the Guide for the Care and Use of Laboratory Animals of the National Institutes of Health. All of the animals were handled according to approved institutional animal care and use committee (IACUC) protocols (AN144957) of the University of California, San Francisco. All surgery was performed under isoflurane anesthesia, and every effort was made to minimize suffering.

Additional files

Transparent reporting form
DOI: 10.7554/eLife.34032.024

Data availability

All data generated or analysed during this study are included in the manuscript and supporting files.

References

  1. Abeles M. Quantification, smoothing, and confidence limits for single-units' histograms. Journal of Neuroscience Methods. 1982;5:317–325. doi: 10.1016/0165-0270(82)90002-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Albin RL, Young AB, Penney JB. The functional anatomy of basal ganglia disorders. Trends in Neurosciences. 1989;12:366–375. doi: 10.1016/0166-2236(89)90074-X. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Apicella P, Ljungberg T, Scarnati E, Schultz W. Responses to reward in monkey dorsal and ventral striatum. Experimental Brain Research. 1991;85:491–500. doi: 10.1007/BF00231732. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Bailey KR, Mair RG. Lesions of specific and nonspecific thalamic nuclei affect prefrontal cortex-dependent aspects of spatial working memory. Behavioral Neuroscience. 2005;119:410–419. doi: 10.1037/0735-7044.119.2.410. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Barnes TD, Kubota Y, Hu D, Jin DZ, Graybiel AM. Activity of striatal neurons reflects dynamic encoding and recoding of procedural memories. Nature. 2005;437:1158–1161. doi: 10.1038/nature04053. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Beutler LR, Eldred KC, Quintana A, Keene CD, Rose SE, Postupna N, Montine TJ, Palmiter RD. Severely impaired learning and altered neuronal morphology in mice lacking NMDA receptors in medium spiny neurons. PLoS One. 2011;6:e28168. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0028168. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Bolkan SS, Stujenske JM, Parnaudeau S, Spellman TJ, Rauffenbart C, Abbas AI, Harris AZ, Gordon JA, Kellendonk C. Thalamic projections sustain prefrontal activity during working memory maintenance. Nature Neuroscience. 2017;20:987–996. doi: 10.1038/nn.4568. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Bosch-Bouju C, Hyland BI, Parr-Brownlie LC. Motor thalamus integration of cortical, cerebellar and basal ganglia information: implications for normal and parkinsonian conditions. Frontiers in Computational Neuroscience. 2013;7:163. doi: 10.3389/fncom.2013.00163. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Brazhnik E, Shah F, Tepper JM. GABAergic afferents activate both GABAA and GABAB receptors in mouse substantia nigra dopaminergic neurons in vivo. Journal of Neuroscience. 2008;28:10386–10398. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2387-08.2008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Brigman JL, Daut RA, Wright T, Gunduz-Cinar O, Graybeal C, Davis MI, Jiang Z, Saksida LM, Jinde S, Pease M, Bussey TJ, Lovinger DM, Nakazawa K, Holmes A. GluN2B in corticostriatal circuits governs choice learning and choice shifting. Nature Neuroscience. 2013;16:1101–1110. doi: 10.1038/nn.3457. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Calabresi P, Pisani A, Mercuri NB, Bernardi G. Long-term potentiation in the striatum is unmasked by removing the Voltage-dependent magnesium block of NMDA receptor channels. European Journal of Neuroscience. 1992;4:929–935. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.1992.tb00119.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Carter DA, Fibiger HC. The projections of the entopeduncular nucleus and globus pallidus in rat as demonstrated by autoradiography and horseradish peroxidase histochemistry. The Journal of Comparative Neurology. 1978;177:113–123. doi: 10.1002/cne.901770108. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Chudasama Y, Bussey TJ, Muir JL. Effects of selective thalamic and prelimbic cortex lesions on two types of visual discrimination and reversal learning. European Journal of Neuroscience. 2001;14:1009–1020. doi: 10.1046/j.0953-816x.2001.01607.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Chung JE, Magland JF, Barnett AH, Tolosa VM, Tooker AC, Lee KY, Shah KG, Felix SH, Frank LM, Greengard LF. A fully automated approach to spike sorting. Neuron. 2017;95:1381–1394. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2017.08.030. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Clavier RM, Gerfen CR. Intracranial self-stimulation in the thalamus of the rat. Brain Research Bulletin. 1982;8:353–358. doi: 10.1016/0361-9230(82)90072-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Cohen JY, Amoroso MW, Uchida N. Serotonergic neurons signal reward and punishment on multiple timescales. eLife. 2015;4:e06346. doi: 10.7554/eLife.06346. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Corbit LH, Ostlund SB, Balleine BW. Sensitivity to instrumental contingency degradation is mediated by the entorhinal cortex and its efferents via the dorsal hippocampus. The Journal of Neuroscience. 2002;22:10976–10984. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.22-24-10976.2002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Corbit LH, Muir JL, Balleine BW. Lesions of mediodorsal thalamus and anterior thalamic nuclei produce dissociable effects on instrumental conditioning in rats. European Journal of Neuroscience. 2003;18:1286–1294. doi: 10.1046/j.1460-9568.2003.02833.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Dang MT, Yokoi F, Yin HH, Lovinger DM, Wang Y, Li Y. Disrupted motor learning and long-term synaptic plasticity in mice lacking NMDAR1 in the striatum. PNAS. 2006;103:15254–15259. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0601758103. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Dautan D, Souza AS, Huerta-Ocampo I, Valencia M, Assous M, Witten IB, Deisseroth K, Tepper JM, Bolam JP, Gerdjikov TV, Mena-Segovia J. Segregated cholinergic transmission modulates dopamine neurons integrated in distinct functional circuits. Nature Neuroscience. 2016;19:1025–1033. doi: 10.1038/nn.4335. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Delacour J, Echavarria MT, Senault B, Houcine O. Specificity of avoidance deficits produced by 6-hydroxydopamine lesions of the nigrostriatal system of the rat. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology. 1977;91:875–885. doi: 10.1037/h0077367. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Deniau JM, Chevalier G. Disinhibition as a basic process in the expression of striatal functions. II. The striato-nigral influence on thalamocortical cells of the ventromedial thalamic nucleus. Brain Research. 1985;334:227–233. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(85)90214-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Deniau JM, Chevalier G. The lamellar organization of the rat substantia nigra pars reticulata: distribution of projection neurons. Neuroscience. 1992;46:361–377. doi: 10.1016/0306-4522(92)90058-A. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Di Chiara G, Porceddu ML, Morelli M, Mulas ML, Gessa GL. Evidence for a GABAergic projection from the substantia nigra to the ventromedial thalamus and to the superior colliculus of the rat. Brain Research. 1979;176:273–284. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(79)90983-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Doya K. Reinforcement learning: Computational theory and biological mechanisms. HFSP Journal. 2007;1:30–40. doi: 10.2976/1.2732246/10.2976/1. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Esposito MS, Arber S. Motor control: illuminating an enigmatic midbrain locomotor center. Current Biology. 2016;26:R291–R293. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2016.02.043. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Fee MS, Goldberg JH. A hypothesis for basal ganglia-dependent reinforcement learning in the songbird. Neuroscience. 2011;198:152–170. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2011.09.069. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Fonseca MS, Murakami M, Mainen ZF. Activation of dorsal raphe serotonergic neurons promotes waiting but is not reinforcing. Current Biology. 2015;25:306–315. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2014.12.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Freeze BS, Kravitz AV, Hammack N, Berke JD, Kreitzer AC. Control of basal ganglia output by direct and indirect pathway projection neurons. Journal of Neuroscience. 2013;33:18531–18539. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1278-13.2013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Goldberg JH, Farries MA, Fee MS. Basal ganglia output to the thalamus: still a paradox. Trends in Neurosciences. 2013;36:695–705. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2013.09.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Gratton A, Hoffer BJ, Gerhardt GA. Effects of electrical stimulation of brain reward sites on release of dopamine in rat: an in vivo electrochemical study. Brain Research Bulletin. 1988;21:319–324. doi: 10.1016/0361-9230(88)90247-X. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Graybiel AM. The basal ganglia: learning new tricks and loving it. Current Opinion in Neurobiology. 2005;15:638–644. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2005.10.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Groenewegen HJ. Organization of the afferent connections of the mediodorsal thalamic nucleus in the rat, related to the mediodorsal-prefrontal topography. Neuroscience. 1988;24:379–431. doi: 10.1016/0306-4522(88)90339-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Gross CT, Canteras NS. The many paths to fear. Nature Reviews Neuroscience. 2012;13:651–658. doi: 10.1038/nrn3301. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Guo ZV, Inagaki HK, Daie K, Druckmann S, Gerfen CR, Svoboda K. Maintenance of persistent activity in a frontal thalamocortical loop. Nature. 2017;545:181–186. doi: 10.1038/nature22324. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Herkenham M. The afferent and efferent connections of the ventromedial thalamic nucleus in the rat. The Journal of Comparative Neurology. 1979;183:487–517. doi: 10.1002/cne.901830304. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Hikida T, Kimura K, Wada N, Funabiki K, Nakanishi S. Distinct roles of synaptic transmission in direct and indirect striatal pathways to reward and aversive behavior. Neuron. 2010;66:896–907. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2010.05.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Hikosaka O, Sakamoto M, Usui S. Functional properties of monkey caudate neurons. III. Activities related to expectation of target and reward. Journal of Neurophysiology. 1989;61:814–832. doi: 10.1152/jn.1989.61.4.814. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Hikosaka O, Takikawa Y, Kawagoe R. Role of the basal ganglia in the control of purposive saccadic eye movements. Physiological Reviews. 2000;80:953–978. doi: 10.1152/physrev.2000.80.3.953. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Hnasko TS, Sotak BN, Palmiter RD. Morphine reward in dopamine-deficient mice. Nature. 2005;438:854–857. doi: 10.1038/nature04172. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Hnasko TS, Sotak BN, Palmiter RD. Cocaine-conditioned place preference by dopamine-deficient mice is mediated by serotonin. Journal of Neuroscience. 2007;27:12484–12488. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3133-07.2007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Hormigo S, Vega-Flores G, Castro-Alamancos MA. Basal ganglia output controls active avoidance behavior. The Journal of Neuroscience. 2016;36:10274–10284. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1842-16.2016. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Ilango A, Kesner AJ, Keller KL, Stuber GD, Bonci A, Ikemoto S. Similar roles of substantia nigra and ventral tegmental dopamine neurons in reward and aversion. The Journal of Neuroscience. 2014;34:817–822. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1703-13.2014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Inglis WL, Olmstead MC, Robbins TW. Pedunculopontine tegmental nucleus lesions impair stimulus--reward learning in autoshaping and conditioned reinforcement paradigms. Behavioral Neuroscience. 2000;114:285–294. doi: 10.1037/0735-7044.114.2.285. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Jeljeli M, Strazielle C, Caston J, Lalonde R. Effects of ventrolateral-ventromedial thalamic lesions on motor coordination and spatial orientation in rats. Neuroscience Research. 2003;47:309–316. doi: 10.1016/S0168-0102(03)00224-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Jin X, Costa RM. Start/stop signals emerge in nigrostriatal circuits during sequence learning. Nature. 2010;466:457–462. doi: 10.1038/nature09263. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Kawai R, Markman T, Poddar R, Ko R, Fantana AL, Dhawale AK, Kampff AR, Ölveczky BP. Motor cortex is required for learning but not for executing a motor skill. Neuron. 2015;86:800–812. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2015.03.024. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Kim H, Sul JH, Huh N, Lee D, Jung MW. Role of striatum in updating values of chosen actions. Journal of Neuroscience. 2009;29:14701–14712. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2728-09.2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Kimchi EY, Laubach M. Dynamic encoding of action selection by the medial striatum. Journal of Neuroscience. 2009;29:3148–3159. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5206-08.2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Klockgether T, Schwarz M, Turski L, Wolfarth S, Sontag KH. Rigidity and catalepsy after injections of muscimol into the ventromedial thalamic nucleus: an electromyographic study in the rat. Experimental Brain Research. 1985;58:559–569. doi: 10.1007/BF00235872. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Klockgether T, Schwarz M, Turski L, Sontag KH. The rat ventromedial thalamic nucleus and motor control: role of N-methyl-D-aspartate-mediated excitation, GABAergic inhibition, and muscarinic transmission. The Journal of Neuroscience. 1986;6:1702–1711. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.06-06-01702.1986. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Kravitz AV, Freeze BS, Parker PR, Kay K, Thwin MT, Deisseroth K, Kreitzer AC. Regulation of parkinsonian motor behaviours by optogenetic control of basal ganglia circuitry. Nature. 2010;466:622–626. doi: 10.1038/nature09159. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Kravitz AV, Tye LD, Kreitzer AC. Distinct roles for direct and indirect pathway striatal neurons in reinforcement. Nature Neuroscience. 2012;15:816–818. doi: 10.1038/nn.3100. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Kuramoto E, Ohno S, Furuta T, Unzai T, Tanaka YR, Hioki H, Kaneko T. Ventral medial nucleus neurons send thalamocortical afferents more widely and more preferentially to layer 1 than neurons of the ventral anterior-ventral lateral nuclear complex in the rat. Cerebral Cortex. 2015;25:221–235. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bht216. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Lau B, Glimcher PW. Value representations in the primate striatum during matching behavior. Neuron. 2008;58:451–463. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2008.02.021. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Lauwereyns J, Watanabe K, Coe B, Hikosaka O. A neural correlate of response bias in monkey caudate nucleus. Nature. 2002;418:413–417. doi: 10.1038/nature00892. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Lee HJ, Weitz AJ, Bernal-Casas D, Duffy BA, Choy M, Kravitz AV, Kreitzer AC, Lee JH. Activation of direct and indirect pathway medium spiny neurons drives distinct Brain-wide responses. Neuron. 2016;91:412–424. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2016.06.010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Liu Z, Zhou J, Li Y, Hu F, Lu Y, Ma M, Feng Q, Zhang JE, Wang D, Zeng J, Bao J, Kim JY, Chen ZF, El Mestikawy S, Luo M. Dorsal raphe neurons signal reward through 5-HT and glutamate. Neuron. 2014;81:1360–1374. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2014.02.010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Maia TV, Frank MJ. From reinforcement learning models to psychiatric and neurological disorders. Nature Neuroscience. 2011;14:154–162. doi: 10.1038/nn.2723. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Mailly P, Charpier S, Mahon S, Menetrey A, Thierry AM, Glowinski J, Deniau JM. Dendritic arborizations of the rat substantia nigra pars reticulata neurons: spatial organization and relation to the lamellar compartmentation of striato-nigral projections. The Journal of Neuroscience. 2001;21:6874–6888. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.21-17-06874.2001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. McDevitt RA, Tiran-Cappello A, Shen H, Balderas I, Britt JP, Marino RAM, Chung SL, Richie CT, Harvey BK, Bonci A. Serotonergic versus nonserotonergic dorsal raphe projection neurons: differential participation in reward circuitry. Cell Reports. 2014;8:1857–1869. doi: 10.1016/j.celrep.2014.08.037. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Nakamura K, Hikosaka O. Facilitation of saccadic eye movements by postsaccadic electrical stimulation in the primate caudate. Journal of Neuroscience. 2006;26:12885–12895. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3688-06.2006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  63. Oldenburg IA, Sabatini BL. Antagonistic but not symmetric regulation of primary motor cortex by basal ganglia direct and indirect pathways. Neuron. 2015;86:1174–1181. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2015.05.008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  64. Oyama K, Hernádi I, Iijima T, Tsutsui K. Reward prediction error coding in dorsal striatal neurons. Journal of Neuroscience. 2010;30:11447–11457. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1719-10.2010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  65. Paoletti P, Bellone C, Zhou Q. NMDA receptor subunit diversity: impact on receptor properties, synaptic plasticity and disease. Nature Reviews Neuroscience. 2013;14:383–400. doi: 10.1038/nrn3504. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  66. Papadopoulos GC, Parnavelas JG. Distribution and synaptic organization of dopaminergic axons in the lateral geniculate nucleus of the rat. The Journal of Comparative Neurology. 1990;294:356–361. doi: 10.1002/cne.902940305. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  67. Parnaudeau S, O'Neill PK, Bolkan SS, Ward RD, Abbas AI, Roth BL, Balsam PD, Gordon JA, Kellendonk C. Inhibition of mediodorsal thalamus disrupts thalamofrontal connectivity and cognition. Neuron. 2013;77:1151–1162. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2013.01.038. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  68. Pasupathy A, Miller EK. Different time courses of learning-related activity in the prefrontal cortex and striatum. Nature. 2005;433:873–876. doi: 10.1038/nature03287. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  69. Phillips AG, Carter DA, Fibiger HC. Dopaminergic substrates of intracranial self-stimulation in the caudate-putamen. Brain Research. 1976;104:221–232. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(76)90615-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  70. Phillips AG. Brain reward circuitry: a case for separate systems. Brain Research Bulletin. 1984;12:195–201. doi: 10.1016/0361-9230(84)90189-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  71. Reynolds JN, Hyland BI, Wickens JR. A cellular mechanism of reward-related learning. Nature. 2001;413:67–70. doi: 10.1038/35092560. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  72. Robinson S, Sandstrom SM, Denenberg VH, Palmiter RD. Distinguishing whether dopamine regulates liking, wanting, and/or learning about rewards. Behavioral Neuroscience. 2005;119:5–15. doi: 10.1037/0735-7044.119.1.5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  73. Roseberry TK, Lee AM, Lalive AL, Wilbrecht L, Bonci A, Kreitzer AC. Cell-Type-Specific control of brainstem locomotor circuits by basal ganglia. Cell. 2016;164:526–537. doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2015.12.037. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  74. Routtenberg A. Forebrain pathways of reward in Rattus norvegicus. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology. 1971;75:269–276. doi: 10.1037/h0030927. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  75. Ryczko D, Dubuc R. The multifunctional mesencephalic locomotor region. Current pharmaceutical design. 2013;19:4448–4470. doi: 10.2174/1381612811319240011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  76. Samejima K, Ueda Y, Doya K, Kimura M. Representation of action-specific reward values in the striatum. Science. 2005;310:1337–1340. doi: 10.1126/science.1115270. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  77. Scharff C, Nottebohm F. A comparative study of the behavioral deficits following lesions of various parts of the zebra finch song system: implications for vocal learning. The Journal of Neuroscience. 1991;11:2896–2913. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.11-09-02896.1991. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  78. Schultz W, Dickinson A. Neuronal coding of prediction errors. Annual Review of Neuroscience. 2000;23:473–500. doi: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.23.1.473. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  79. Shen W, Flajolet M, Greengard P, Surmeier DJ. Dichotomous dopaminergic control of striatal synaptic plasticity. Science. 2008;321:848–851. doi: 10.1126/science.1160575. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  80. Starr MS, Summerhayes M. Role of the ventromedial nucleus of the thalamus in motor behaviour--I. Effects of focal injections of drugs. Neuroscience. 1983;10:1157–1169. doi: 10.1016/0306-4522(83)90106-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  81. Tai LH, Lee AM, Benavidez N, Bonci A, Wilbrecht L. Transient stimulation of distinct subpopulations of striatal neurons mimics changes in action value. Nature Neuroscience. 2012;15:1281–1289. doi: 10.1038/nn.3188. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  82. Thomas MJ, Malenka RC, Bonci A. Modulation of long-term depression by dopamine in the mesolimbic system. The Journal of Neuroscience. 2000;20:5581–5586. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.20-15-05581.2000. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  83. Turner RS, Desmurget M. Basal ganglia contributions to motor control: a vigorous tutor. Current Opinion in Neurobiology. 2010;20:704–716. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2010.08.022. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  84. Watabe-Uchida M, Zhu L, Ogawa SK, Vamanrao A, Uchida N. Whole-brain mapping of direct inputs to midbrain dopamine neurons. Neuron. 2012;74:858–873. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2012.03.017. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  85. White NM, Hiroi N. Preferential localization of self-stimulation sites in striosomes/patches in the rat striatum. PNAS. 1998;95:6486–6491. doi: 10.1073/pnas.95.11.6486. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  86. Williams ZM, Eskandar EN. Selective enhancement of associative learning by microstimulation of the anterior caudate. Nature Neuroscience. 2006;9:562–568. doi: 10.1038/nn1662. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  87. Wise RA, Bozarth MA. Brain reward circuitry: four circuit elements "wired" in apparent series. Brain Research Bulletin. 1984;12:203–208. doi: 10.1016/0361-9230(84)90190-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  88. Wise RA. Addictive drugs and brain stimulation reward. Annual Review of Neuroscience. 1996;19:319–340. doi: 10.1146/annurev.ne.19.030196.001535. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  89. Xiao C, Cho JR, Zhou C, Treweek JB, Chan K, McKinney SL, Yang B, Gradinaru V. Cholinergic mesopontine signals govern locomotion and reward through dissociable midbrain pathways. Neuron. 2016;90:333–347. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2016.03.028. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  90. Xiong Q, Znamenskiy P, Zador AM. Selective corticostriatal plasticity during acquisition of an auditory discrimination task. Nature. 2015;521:348–351. doi: 10.1038/nature14225. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  91. Yagishita S, Hayashi-Takagi A, Ellis-Davies GC, Urakubo H, Ishii S, Kasai H. A critical time window for dopamine actions on the structural plasticity of dendritic spines. Science. 2014;345:1616–1620. doi: 10.1126/science.1255514. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  92. Yin HH, Knowlton BJ, Balleine BW. Blockade of NMDA receptors in the dorsomedial striatum prevents action-outcome learning in instrumental conditioning. European Journal of Neuroscience. 2005a;22:505–512. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2005.04219.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  93. Yin HH, Ostlund SB, Knowlton BJ, Balleine BW. The role of the dorsomedial striatum in instrumental conditioning. European Journal of Neuroscience. 2005b;22:513–523. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2005.04218.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  94. Yoo JH, Zell V, Wu J, Punta C, Ramajayam N, Shen X, Faget L, Lilascharoen V, Lim BK, Hnasko TS. Activation of pedunculopontine glutamate neurons is reinforcing. The Journal of Neuroscience. 2017;37:38–46. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3082-16.2016. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  95. Yttri EA, Dudman JT. Opponent and bidirectional control of movement velocity in the basal ganglia. Nature. 2016;533:402–406. doi: 10.1038/nature17639. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  96. Zis VP, Spyraki C, Papadopoulos G. Acquisition and extinction of L-maze and conditioned avoidance behaviours following kainic acid-induced lesions of the ventromedial thalamic nuclei in rats. Brain Research Bulletin. 1984;12:617–623. doi: 10.1016/0361-9230(84)90141-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Decision letter


In the interests of transparency, eLife includes the editorial decision letter and accompanying author responses. A lightly edited version of the letter sent to the authors after peer review is shown, indicating the most substantive concerns; minor comments are not usually included.

Thank you for submitting your article "Motor Thalamus Supports Striatum-Driven Reinforcement" for consideration by eLife. Your article has been reviewed by Michael Frank as the Senior Editor and Reviewing Editor, and three reviewers. The following individual involved in review of your submission has agreed to reveal his identity: D James Surmeier (Reviewer #1).

The reviewers have discussed the reviews with one another and the Reviewing Editor has drafted this decision to help you prepare a revised submission.

Summary:

This study uses optogenetic approaches to assess the role of circuitry downstream to the basal ganglia (BG) for mediating reinforcement. First, the authors demonstrate that stimulation of direct pathway medium spiny neurons (dMSN) is highly reinforcing but ostensibly not dopamine dependent. They moved on to directly stimulate BG output nucleus, substantia nigra reticulata (SNr), and find that its direct inhibition is also mildly reinforcing. SNr projection targets, including thalamus and brainstem were also directly stimulated to assess if they can support reinforcement. Similar to some previous reports, the authors find that stimulation at these sites is indeed reinforcing. Interestingly, this study also suggests that thalamic, but not brainstem, nuclei is directly involved in reinforcement produced following dMSN stimulation.

Essential revisions:

All involved found the study to be highly interesting and provocative. However, they also expressed a few major reservations that would need to be addressed.

1) There was particular concern among all three reviewers about the claims regarding dopamine independence. Their respective comments on this issue are pasted below, but the key point is that to establish this properly would require much more extensive experiments (as well as other statistics, see comments from reviewer 3) than possible for a revision at eLife. Nevertheless, we all agreed that the dopamine-independence is not critical for the key contribution of this study, which is mainly to show that the thalamic output is at least partially involved in dMSN-driven reinforcement. Hence, we suggest that you strongly temper the conclusions regarding DA-independence.

Reviewer 1:

It is not clear that the reinforcing effects of dMSN stimulation are independent of dopamine (DA). Neural networks, like neurons, are non-linear. The implicit assumption made by the authors is that partial inhibition of DA signaling should result in partial inhibition of dMSN stimulation induced behavior. I don't see any compelling reason to believe this is true. Given the novelty of the conclusions drawn by the authors, the burden of proof is on their shoulders. The author's assertion is based upon two negative observations: nosepokes were not affected by (1) cocktail of a D1R (SCH23390) and D2R (eticlopride) antagonists administered i.p. and (2) deletion of NR1 subunits from dMSNs. The D1R/D2R cocktail produced a very modest reduction in motor behavior and in self-stimulation of SNc. For this to be convincing, there must be no 'spare' receptors in the circuitry mediating the behavior. Second, not all of the effects of DA on reinforcement need to be mediated by NMDARs – DA receptors also change the intrinsic excitability of basal ganglia neurons; only long-term memory of reinforcing events is thought require long-term potentiation of glutamatergic synapses and NMDARs (behavior over days was examined (Results section) but not discussed; why?). At the very least, the authors should determine how dMSN optical stimulation interacts with forms of reinforcement that are well known to depend upon DA. For example, does nosepoking for dMSN stimulation affect nose-poking for sucrose? Previous work by Tai et al., and Yttri and Dudman suggests it should, implicating DA signaling.

DA-dependent synaptic plasticity may be necessary for long-term memories associated with goal directed behavior, but not necessarily the short-term memory examined in this study. It would be worthwhile testing animals on successive days of training to determine if there is in fact a memory created by dMSN stimulation.

Reviewer 2:

- The DA independence of D1MSN stimulation is corroborated by the dMSN-NR1-KO mice. Indeed, while the authors have reported this before (Kravitz 2012), and cite that study for DA antagonist injections, they do not explicitly indicate the route of infusions (i.e IP, intracranial) and whether it is in the direct vicinity of the optical fibers. I suspect this was an IP infusion as in the 2012 study, but it would be interesting to see the authors clearly, and directly demonstrate the DA independence of this effect. That is, without concentration confounds (movement related) and localization to str (systemic vs local).

- VM may instantiate some increased propensity of re-doing an action (similar to 'activation' role of DA) as part of the intrinsic reinforcing nature of ICSS. But, whether this develops across days or its similarity to learning that requires striatal LTP can only be clarified if that authors analyze and show effects of multi-day stimulations and other behavioral metrics in addition to total # of nosepokes.

Reviewer 3:

Statistical power is an issue throughout the paper, particularly when claiming

- Positive effects with an n of 2 (MLR. This after also throwing out over half of the data because it didn't fit), or

- Lack of effect (antagonists, receptor KO's had no effect). Eye-balling the data in Figure 2g (antagonist), I roughly get saline control (u= 325, std= 165) and antag (u=235, std = 95), with n = 7 in each group.

First, failure to detect an effect *does not* mean the two distributions are equivalent or that there is no effect. This is, sadly, a common mistake and to say so requires a different statistical test.

Second, any measure of power makes it clear that, with this small sample size and large variance, it would be highly likely to commit a type 2 error – failure to detect a difference that is there. From this estimation, the authors would need to double their sample size to get even 70% power.

In fact, although it is difficult to ascertain, it appears that be increasing power by combining this result with the data from 2A of the author's 2010 paper (albeit with a slightly different antagonist), the effect of antagonists is significant, rather than merely a strong trend across multiple papers.

This, in combination with the smallest concentration of eticlopride I've seen used (the standard for awake behaving rodents is at least 0.5 mg/kg, not 0.2) casts some doubt on this, their first major finding.

2) Also, from multiple reviewers: The electrophysiological consequences of optical stimulation of dMSNs on downstream targets are not systematically examined but simply inferred. The paper is about the circuitry mediating the effects of dMSN stimulation. Yet, outside of a few recordings from VM neurons, there is no attempt to determine the electrophysiological consequences of optogenetic stimulation on key circuit elements. This also is true in other parts of the study (e.g., SNr terminal stimulation is assumed to have a simple effect on VM neurons). More work needs to be done characterizing the thalamic response. Characterizing an area with 13 neurons, and making broad claims with 6 of those 13, is troubling. In supplemental Figure 5 there are very modest differences between excitation and inhibition of VM neurons by dMSN stimulation. In fact, excitation won by only 3 cells out of 13! Parametric Z-scores cannot be used to assay population behavior with a sample size of 13. The experiment illustrated in Figure 4F-H is elegant, but the nose-pokes evoked by terminal stimulation are twice those evoked by striatal stimulation in the previous figures. This makes it difficult to evaluate. Moreover, what is to be made of the change in slope, rather than the shift in curves seen in panel B?

Moreover, the questions the authors could answer would greatly and easily strengthen the impact of the paper – not only to give a better sense of the thalamic response (there appears to be evidence of excitatory responses early, middle and late follow stim – but it's impossible to tell with such a small sample size), but to get an idea how the thalamus responds during behavior. The one recording was done in a head-fixed mouse that passively received stimulation. Answering anything along the lines of how do the neurons respond during unstimulated nose pokes, how does the response change over a session, and how/why do the two populations of early excite and early inhib+late excite come about would be wonderful to see. I think this falls in line with the earlier comment concerning the assumption of simple linearity. That a brain region responds when another is stimulated says almost nothing about its potential to modulate learning or behavior. In my opinion, this could be done in a week with a few animals, taking the recording from troubling weakness to outstanding strength.

3) The experiments performed do not exclude a role for the MLR in mediating the effects of dMSN stimulation. None of the experiments address the potential role of cholinergic or GABAergic neurons in this region. The cholinergic neurons here have widespread projections, including to the thalamus. Moreover, it is not clear that the optogenetic inhibition of glutamatergic neurons in this region included those projecting to the SNc and basal ganglia. So, the assertion that the brainstem circuits don't play a role needs to be qualified, or else the cholinergic inputs studies.

4) In Figure 1D, dMSN stimulation routinely promotes ~400 pokes/30 minute session (ignoring a couple of outliers), and Figure 2 indicates that the direct stimulation of projection targets individually contribute ~ 100-200 pokes/ 30 minutes. Finally, disruption of Vm with muscimol or activation of GABAergic input from SNr reduce nosepoke responding to ~150 pokes/30 minutes. Altogether these results suggest a capacity of DRN and MLR for reinforcement, but the authors claim that "dMSN self stimulation does not engage DRN or MLR for reinforcement". What structure, then, is responsible for the remaining reinforcement when SNr-> VM is activated coincideRt-pcrnt with dMSN activation. In other words, VM thalamus modulates the reinforcing effects of dMSN stimulation, though inhibition only blunts, not eliminates the effect. This is potentially and due to very interesting non-VM mechanisms, and it would be a great addition for the authors to address it.

5) Situating these results in context of a previous extensive ICSS literature. The reinforcing effects of ICSS have been shown to occur throughout the brain, including cerebellum, PFC, LC, Raphe, brainstem (including putative MLR), thalamus – and under the control of either DA, Ach, 5HT, and/or noradrenaline. ICSS 'reinforced' behavior had been studied in depth in the late 60's and 70's and continues to today, with particularly excellent work done in the early 70's by Routtenberg et al. The absence of any citation of this extensive literature, save for one focusing on the BG and dopamine by Phillips, (1976), is problematic given that this paper focuses on ICSS (even though it does study a more specific component related to dMSN stimulation and downstream targets).

Given this, I would like the authors to read some of the seminal work on ICSS throughout the brain in order to better understand the context (that most positive ICSS is observed outside of the BG and is DA-independent was common knowledge in 1975). Wise, 1996 would be a wonderful start, and his list of positive ICSS locations is below. The authors re-discovery of non-DA ICSS everywhere is not exciting – it happens even in primary sensory cortex. However, their dissociation and mapping of the pathways that execute striatum's reinforcing and motor roles is certainly valuable. I would recommend a reworking of the manuscript to take this into account.

The authors' overcoming "the requirement for dopamine in reinforcement (Discussion section)" is a terrible straw man, one that the authors identify as already having been torn down in the very next paragraph. Phillips himself suggested that ICSS may be intrinsically reinforcing, rather than being a part of a brain's native RL mechanism.

From Wise, 1996 (Figure 1): I. medial forebrain bundle sites, including Ihe anterior, posterior, and lateral hypothalamus; 2. ventromedial hypothalamus; 3. substantia nigra (zona compacta) and ventral tegmental area;4. midline mesencephalon, including the regions of the dorsal and medial raphe nuclei; 5. region of locus coeruleus; 6. deep cerebellar nuclei and decussation of the brachium conjunctivum; 7. regions of the mesencephalic and motor nuclei of the trigeminal nerve; 8. nucleus of the solitary tract; 9. olfactory bulb; 10. olfactory tubercle; 11. medial frontal cortex; 12. sulcal frontal cortex; 13. anterior cingulate cortex; 14. entorhinal cortex; 15. hippocampus; 16. amygdala; 17. medial and lateral septal regions; 18. nucleus accumbens; 19. caudate nucleus: and 20. dorso-medial thalamus.

eLife. 2018 Oct 8;7:e34032. doi: 10.7554/eLife.34032.027

Author response


All involved found the study to be highly interesting and provocative. However, they also expressed a few major reservations that would need to be addressed.

1) There was particular concern among all threRt-pcre reviewers about the claims regarding dopamine independence. Their respective comments on this issue are pasted below, but the key point is that to establish this properly would require much more extensive experiments (as well as other statistics, see comments from reviewer 3) than possible for a revision at eLife. Nevertheless, we all agreed that the dopamine-independence is not critical for the key contribution of this study, which is mainly to show that the thalamic output is at least partially involved in dMSN-driven reinforcement. Hence, we suggest that you strongly temper the conclusions regarding DA-independence.

We have taken the reviewers’ comments into account and decided to remove the DA antagonist experiment from the manuscript. Accordingly, we no longer draw any specific conclusions regarding the role of DA in dMSN-driven reinforcement and have rewritten sections across the whole manuscript. Instead, we use the data presented in Figure 1 to introduce the hypothesis of a downstream circuit mechanism for reinforcement.

Reviewer 1:

It is not clear that the reinforcing effects of dMSN stimulation are independent of dopamine (DA). Neural networks, like neurons, are non-linear. The implicit assumption made by the authors is that partial inhibition of DA signaling should result in partial inhibition of dMSN stimulation induced behavior. I don't see any compelling reason to believe this is true. Given the novelty of the conclusions drawn by the authors, the burden of proof is on their shoulders. The author's assertion is based upon two negative observations: nosepokes were not affected by (1) cocktail of a D1R (SCH23390) and D2R (eticlopride) antagonists administered i.p. and (2) deletion of NR1 subunits from dMSNs. The D1R/D2R cocktail produced a very modest reduction in motor behavior and in self-stimulation of SNc. For this to be convincing, there must be no 'spare' receptors in the circuitry mediating the behavior. Second, not all of the effects of DA on reinforcement need to be mediated by NMDARs – DA receptors also change the intrinsic excitability of basal ganglia neurons; only long-term memory of reinforcing events is thought require long-term potentiation of glutamatergic synapses and NMDARs (behavior over days was examined (Results section) but not discussed; why?). At the very least, the authors should determine how dMSN optical stimulation interacts with forms of reinforcement that are well known to depend upon DA. For example, does nosepoking for dMSN stimulation affect nose-poking for sucrose? Previous work by Tai et al., and Yttri and Dudman suggests it should, implicating DA signaling.

The reviewer raises a number of valid points. We agree that much more work would be required to convincingly demonstrate DA independence. We have therefore removed the DA antagonist experiments from the manuscript.

DA-dependent synaptic plasticity may be necessary for long-term memories associated with goal directed behavior, but not necessarily the short-term memory examined in this study. It would be worthwhile testing animals on successive days of training to determine if there is in fact a memory created by dMSN stimulation.

We have now added data into Figure 1 showing that our protocol does elicit a long-term (>24 hour) memory of the previously-paired nosepoke. We also added new data showing that despite this learning, no observable changes are observed in mEPSC frequency, mEPSC amplitude, paired-pulse ratio, AMPA/NMDA ratio, or intrinsic excitability in stimulated dMSNs recorded in slices after training (Figure 2) or in neighboring iMSNs (Figure 2—figure supplemRt-pcrent 1).

Reviewer 2:

- The DA independence of D1MSN stimulation is corroborated by the dMSN-NR1-KO mice. Indeed, while the authors have reported this before (Kravitz 2012), and cite that study for DA antagonist injections, they do not explicitly indicate the route of infusions (i.e IP, intracranial) and whether it is in the direct vicinity of the optical fibers. I susRt-pcrpect this was an IP infusion as in the 2012 study, but it would be interesting to see the authors clearly, and directly demonstrate the DA independence of this effect. That is, without concentration confounds (movement related) and localization to str (systemic vs local).

As described above, we have decided to remove the DA antagonist experiments from the manuscript.

- VM may instantiate some increased propensity of re-doing an action (similar to 'activation' role of DA) as part of the intrinsic reinforcing nature of ICSS. But, whether this develops across days or its similarity to learning that requires striatal LTP can only be clarified if that authors analyze and show effects of multi-day stimulations and other behavioral metrics in addition to total # of nosepokes.

We now show dMSN self-stimulation over multiple days (Figure 1—figure supplement 3). We analyzed multiple aspects of the self-stimulation (poking patterns, interpoke intervals) and found that behavior on consecutive days was similar to the first session. This suggests that behavior was learned and stabilized within a single session.

Reviewer 3:

Statistical power is an issue throughout the paper, particularly when claiming

- Positive effects with an n of 2 (MLR. This after also throwing out over half of the data because it didn't fit), or

- Lack of effect (antagonists, receptor KO's had no effect). Eye-balling the data in Figure 2g (antagonist), I roughly get saline control (u= 325, std= 165) and antag (u=235, std = 95), with n = 7 in each group.

First, failure to detect an effect *does not* mean the two distributions are equivalent or that there is no effect. This is, sadly, a common mistake and to say so requires a different statistical test.

Second, any measure of power makes it clear that, with this small sample size and large variance, it would be highly likely to commit a type 2 error – failure to detect a difference that is there. From this estimation, the authors would need to double their sample size to get even 70% power.

In fact, although it is difficult to ascertain, it appears that be increasing power by combining this result with the data from 2A of the author's 2010 paper (albeit with a slightly different antagonist), the effect of antagonists is significant, rather than merely a strong trend across multiple papers.

This, in combination with the smallest concentration of eticlopride I've seen used (the standard for awake behaving rodents is at least 0.5 mg/kg, not 0.2) casts some doubt on this, their first major finding.

We agree that we do not present a strong case for dopamine independence and thus we have removed these results from the manuscript and toned down these conclusions.

Regarding MLR glutamatergic neurons self-stimulation, we have removed the statistical test and explain here the classification of responders versus non-rRt-pcresponders. Our aim was to show that these neurons, which control locomotion and mediate dMSNdriven locomotion (Roseberry et al., 2016, and our manuscript, Figure 4—figure supplement 1), can also, in some instances, elicit reinforcement. Indeed, all 5 mice showed an increase in locomotion upon stimulation (data not shown). However, only 2 of them consistently self-stimulated (responsive). The other 3 were classified as non-responsive (<1 poke/minute on average over 4 sessions or >50% decrease by session 4, a selection criterion applied to all conditions across the paper. This criterion is mentioned in the ‘transparent reporting’ file, submitted along with the manuscript, and can now be found in the methods section). Given that at least some of these mice showed reinforcement seems sufficient to support further investigation of the role of MLR glutamatergic neurons in dMSN-driven reinforcement (which is the point of this Figure).

2) Also, from multiple reviewers: The electrophysiological consequences of optical stimulation of dMSNs on downstream targets are not systematically examined but simply inferred. The paper is about the circuitry mediating the effects of dMSN stimulation. Yet, outside of a few recordings from VM neurons, there is no attempt to determine the electrophysiological consequences of optogenetic stimulation on key circuit elements. This also is true in other parts of the study (e.g., SNr terminal stimulation is assumed to have a simple effect on VM neurons). More work needs to be done characterizing the thalamic response. Characterizing an area with 13 neurons, and making broad claims with 6 of those 13, is troubling. In supplemental Figure 5 there are very modest differences between excitation and inhibition of VM neurons by dMSN stimulation. In fact, excitation won by only 3 cells out of 13! ParameRt-pcrtric Z-scores cannot be used to assay population behavior with a sample size of 13. The experiment illustrated in Figure 4F-H is elegant, but the nose-pokes evoked by terminal stimulation are twice those evoked by striatal stimulation in the previous figures. This makes it difficult to evaluate. Moreover, what is to be made of the change in slope, rather than the shift in curves seen in panel B?

Moreover, the questions the authors could answer would greatly and easily strengthen the impact of the paper – not only to give a better sense of the thalamic response (there appears to be evidence of excitatory responses early, middle and late follow stim – but it's impossible to tell with such a small sample size), but to get an idea how the thalamus responds during behavior. The one recording was done in a head-fixed mouse that passively received stimulation. Answering anything along the lines of how do the neurons respond during unstimulated nose pokes, how does the response change over a session, and how/why do the two populations of early excite and early inhib+late excite come about would be wonderful to see. I think this falls in line with the earlier comment concerning the assumption of simple linearity. That a brain region responds when another is stimulated says almost nothing about its potential to modulate learning or behavior. In my opinion, this could be done in a week with a few animals, taking the recording from troubling weakness to outstanding strength.

We performed additional in vivo recordings and gathered data from 163 neurons to characterize VM activity during dMSN stimulation in head-fixed, awake mice. To better mimic behavior, we recorded VM responses during a 1second stimulation window (versus only 100ms in the previous version). These results are now presented in Figure 6. After 20ms of dMSN stimulation, most responsive VM neurons were excited. After 100ms of stimulation, a significant fraction of neurons with decreased activity appeared. After 1second, a majority of neurons were excited. Overall, these results fit with our model of dMSN-driven VM disinhibition for reinforcement. Additionally, we now discuss the heterogeneous response properties of VM neurons during dMSN stimulation. We believe head-fixed recordings are not only optimal but also necessary to properly isolate VM responses during dMSN stimulation from any movement-related activity. Given the known role of VM in motor control, recording from freely-moving animals is highly likely to confound dMSN-driven activity with spontaneous movement related activity. In order to further link specific VM activity patterns with reinforcement versus movement, future studies should take advantage of head-fixed operant tasks combined with in vivo recordings. Although extremely interesting, we believe that these experiments fall beyond the scope of our manuscript.

Regarding the experiments shown in Figure 5f-H (previously Figure 4F-H):

although dMSN axonal self-stimulation was stronger than cell body self-stimulation, its combination with SNr terminal excitation in VM led to an approximate 50% decrease, which is similar to the muscimol effect on cell body self-stimulation shown in Figure 5.

Regarding Figure 5B and G, we apologize that the chosen examples did not accurately reflect the average behavior. We do observe a change in slope with both pharmacological and optogenetic silencing approaches, which fits with a decrease in poking rate / total number of nosepokes. We replaced this example with another trace which better represents the average result.

3) The experiments performed do not exclude a role for the MLR in mediating the effects of dMSN stimulation. None of the experiments address the potential role of cholinergic or GABAergic neurons in this region. The cholinergic neurons here have widespread projections, including to the thalamus. Moreover, it is not clear that the optogenetic inhibition of glutamatergic neurons in this region included those projecting to the SNc and basal ganglia. So, the assertion that the brainstem circuits don't play a role needs to be qualified, or else the cholinergic inputs studies.

We apologize for the lack of clarity. Our experiments are strictly limited to glutamatergic neurons of the MLR, which we functionally define here as sufficient for locomotion and necessary for dMSN-driven locomotion. We have updated the text to be more specific.

4) In Figure 1D, dMSN stimulation routinely promotes ~400 pokes/30 minute session (ignoring a couple of outliers), and Figure 2 indicates that the direct stimulation of projection targets individually contribute ~ 100-200 pokes/ 30 minutes. Finally, disruption of Vm with muscimol or activation of GABAergic input from SNr reduce nosepoke responding to ~150 pokes/30 minutes. Altogether these results suggest a capacity of DRN and MLR for reinforcement, but the authors claim that "dMSN self stimulation does not engage DRN or MLR for reinforcement". What structure, then, is responsible for the remaining reinforcement when SNr-> VM is activated coincident with dMSN activation. In other words, VM thalamus modulates the reinforcing effects of dMSN stimulation, though inhibition only blunts, not eliminates the effect. This is potentially and due to very interesting non-VM mechanisms, and it would be a great addition for the authors to address it.

Several possibilities could explain the partial decrease in self-stimulation during VM manipulation. First, it is possible that the small volume and concentration of muscimol that we used does not fully silence VM. Additionally, the SNr also projects to other regions than the ones examined in this study, like the thalamic mediodorsal nucleus, involved in learning and working memory (Parnaudeau et al., 2013). The SNr targets the superior colliculus, which displays reward-related activity (Ikeda and Hikosaka, 2003; Weldon et al., 2007), and MLR cholinergic population, which has been shown to drive reinforcement (Xiao et al., 2016). Lastly, as the reviewers have pointed out, our resRt-pcrRt-pcrRt-pcrults do not fully discard a role for DA in dMSN self-stimulation. Identifying the remaining component for dMSN-driven reinforcement is a highly interesting question, that we now discuss in the manuscript and would like to identify in future studies.

5) […] Given this, I would like the authors to read some of the seminal work on ICSS throughout the brain in order to better understand the context (that most positive ICSS is observed outside of the BG and is DA-independent was common knowledge in 1975). Wise, 1996 would be a wonderful start, and his list of positive ICSS locations is below. The authors re-discovery of non-DA ICSS everywhere is not exciting – it happens even in primary sensory cortex. However, their dissociation and mapping of the pathways that execute striatum's reinforcing and motor roles is certainly valuable. I would recommend a reworking of the manuscript to take this into account.

The authors' overcoming "the requirement for dopamine in reinforcement (Discussion section)" is a terrible straw man, one that the authors identify as already having been torn down in the very next paragraph. Phillips himself suggested that ICSS may be intrinsically reinforcing, rather than being a part of a brain's native RL mechanism.

From Wise, 1996 (Figure 1): I. medial forebrain bundle sites, including Ihe anterior, posterior, and lateral hypothalamus; 2. ventromedial hypothalamus; 3. substantia nigra (zona compacta) and ventral tegmental area;4. midline mesencephalon, including the regions of the dorsal and medial raphe nuclei; 5. region of locus coeruleus; 6. deep cerebellar nuclei and decussation of the brachium conjunctivum; 7. regions of the mesencephalic and motor nuclei of the trigeminal nerve; 8. nucleus of the solitary tract; 9. olfactory bulb; 10. olfactory tubercle; 11. medial frontal cortex; 12. sulcal frontal cortex; 13. anterior cingulate cortex; 14. entorhinal cortex; 15. hippocampus; 16. amygdala; 17. medial and lateral septal regions; 18. nucleus accumbens; 19. caudate nucleus: and 20. dorso-medial thalamus.

We agree and now discuss our data within the framework of past ICSS literature.

Associated Data

    This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

    Supplementary Materials

    Figure 1—source data 1. Source data for Figure 1.
    DOI: 10.7554/eLife.34032.008
    Figure 1—figure supplement 2—source data 1. Source data for Figure 1—figure supplement 2.
    DOI: 10.7554/eLife.34032.005
    Figure 1—figure supplement 3—source data 1. Source data for Figure 1—figure supplement 3.
    DOI: 10.7554/eLife.34032.007
    Figure 2—source data 1. Source data for Figure 2.
    DOI: 10.7554/eLife.34032.012
    Figure 2—figure supplement 1—source data 1. Source data for Figure 2—figure supplement 1.
    DOI: 10.7554/eLife.34032.011
    Figure 3—source data 1. Source data for Figure 3.
    DOI: 10.7554/eLife.34032.014
    Figure 4—source data 1. Source data forFigure 4.
    DOI: 10.7554/eLife.34032.018
    Figure 4—figure supplement 1—source data 1. Source data for Figure 4—figure supplement 1.
    DOI: 10.7554/eLife.34032.017
    Figure 5—source data 1. Source data for Figure 5.
    DOI: 10.7554/eLife.34032.020
    Figure 6—source data 1. Source data for Figure 6.
    elife-34032-fig6-data1.xlsx (405.1KB, xlsx)
    DOI: 10.7554/eLife.34032.023
    Transparent reporting form
    DOI: 10.7554/eLife.34032.024

    Data Availability Statement

    All data generated or analysed during this study are included in the manuscript and supporting files.


    Articles from eLife are provided here courtesy of eLife Sciences Publications, Ltd

    RESOURCES