Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2018 Oct 14.
Published in final edited form as: Curr Opin Behav Sci. 2017 Aug 5;19:7–12. doi: 10.1016/j.cobeha.2017.07.009

The positivity effect: a negativity bias in youth fades with age

Laura L Carstensen 1, Marguerite DeLiema 2
PMCID: PMC6186441  NIHMSID: NIHMS954281  PMID: 30327789

Abstract

Relative to younger adults, older adults attend to and remember positive information more than negative information. This shift from a negativity bias in younger age to a preference for positive information in later life is termed the ‘positivity effect.’ Based on nearly two decades of research and recent evidence from neuroscience, we argue that the effect reflects age-related changes in motivation that direct behavior and cognitive processing rather than neural or cognitive decline. Understanding the positivity effect, including conditions that reduce and enhance it, can inform effective public health and educational messages directed at older people.

Introduction

In 2003 Charles et al. [1] reported findings from a study in which young, middle-aged, and older adults were asked to view images that varied by emotional valence. Some of the images were negative, some positive and others neutral. Compared to the youngest participants, middle-aged participants displayed a modest preference in memory for positive over negative images and elderly participants were far more likely to recall positive images than negative. Earlier research had demonstrated an age-related preference for emotional material over other types of information (e.g., [2]) but none had shown differential processing of positive and negative material.

The observation was striking given its juxtaposition with a large literature documenting a negativity bias in younger people. Infants reliably orient to negative stimuli more than positive, and scores of studies have shown that children detect and remember threatening stimuli better than non-threatening stimuli (e.g., [35]). Social psychologists have widely documented the attention-grabbing properties of negative information in young adults (mostly college students) and shown that negative events are more likely to be remembered and retold in the social transmission of stories than positive events [6]. In a now classic paper titled, ‘Bad is stronger than good,’ Baumeister and colleagues [7] argued that a negativity bias in humans is so reliable that it can be considered a fundamental principle of human behavior. It makes logical sense: attending to the lion in the brush more than the puppy in the grass likely holds evolutionary advantages [8].

Yet since Charles et al. [1] was published, scores of studies have documented an age-associated reversal in preferences for negative over positive stimuli in attention and in memory. Coined, the positivity effect, the pattern refers to a shift from a negativity bias early in life to a positivity bias that emerges in middle and late adulthood (see [9]). Studies have examined the positivity effect in attention, short-term memory [10], autobiographical memory [11,12], and even working memory [13] using a wide range of experimental paradigms, from eye-tracking [14,15,16•] to neuroimaging [17••, 18]. The effect has been shown in many different contexts including attention to emotional faces [19], recall of facial expressions [20], memory for health information [21,22••], focusing more on positive than negative old age stereotypes [23], and the interpretation of socially ambiguous situations [24•].

Abundant empirical evidence for the positivity effect led to broad acceptance of the observation. Underlying mechanisms, on the other hand, continue to be debated. In this review, we describe initial findings and recent studies on the positivity effect and make the case that the body of literature is most coherent when viewed through the lens of motivated cognition.

Socioemotional selectivity theory

The positivity effect was first identified through tests of hypotheses grounded in socioemotional selectivity theory (SST). SST is a life-span theory of motivation that postulates systematic changes in goals as a function of perceived time horizons [25]. SST maintains that when time horizons are vast and nebulous, as they typically are in youth, goals tend to concern exploration and learning. In contrast, as time horizons grow limited, as they typically do with age, goals shift to ones realized in the present and which tend to focus on savoring, emotional meaning, and satisfaction. Because perceived time is strongly correlated with chronological age, changes in goals appear systematically as people move through adulthood. And because goals direct cognitive resources, aging is associated with changes in preferences, decisions, and even what we see, hear, and remember.

Postulates of SST have been examined quite extensively in life-span developmental psychology and gerontology. Most of the early work addressed preferences for social partners and social network composition [26,27]. Shrinking social networks associated with aging had long been thought to reflect deaths and disabilities. Tests of SST instead revealed a proactive pruning process over the life course — discarding peripheral partners and selectively retaining a stable core of emotionally close social partners [28]. Moreover, this selective narrowing of the social world is associated with greater emotional balance [29]. In a parallel line of research, experiments have revealed that preferences for social partners shift as a function of time horizons. When time horizons are statistically controlled, age differences were eliminated [26,27].

Empirical investigation of goal-directed cognitive processing began relatively recently. Fung and Carstensen [2] found that older people better remembered advertisements when products were framed in emotionally meaningful terms than when positioned as products that facilitate exploration. Importantly, they also demonstrated that these age differences were eliminated by expanding time horizons. Extending research on SST further to cognitive processing, Charles et al. [1] observed age differences in memory as a function of valence. And recently, Barber et al. [30] demonstrated that the positivity effect can be effectively produced in younger people by experimentally limiting future time horizons.

Exploring alternative explanations

Although the age-related preference for positive material was identified by tests of hypotheses derived from SST, explorations of alternatives have helped to refine the concept. Two of the most viable alternatives — namely, cognitive decline and neural degradation — have been largely ruled out. Dynamic integration theory [31] posits that general age-related declines in processing capacity also affect the processing of emotion. Specifically, DIT maintains that negative information is more complex and thus more difficult to integrate into cognitive-affective systems than positive information leading to greater overall memory for, and attention to, positive stimuli. Whereas there may be empirical evidence for aspects of DIT, it does not appear to account for the positivity effect. Reasoning from DIT, the effect would be expected to be more pronounced in individuals with relatively poor cognitive functioning, for example, and less pronounced in those with higher levels of functioning. Mather and Knight [32] observed the opposite relationship, however (see also [33]): Older people with the highest levels of executive control show the strongest evidence of the positivity effect. The competing explanations were recently tested in a delayed recall task with Alzheimer’s disease patients, healthy older adults and young adults. Consistent with the motivational perspective, the effect was observed in healthy older adults but not in patients with Alzheimer’s disease [34•].

Similarly, the positivity effect would be expected to be degraded under conditions of cognitive load. Yet, there is considerable empirical evidence that cognitive load reduces the positivity effect [32,35,36,37,38•].

Given age-related changes in brain function and structure, neural degradation offered another viable alternative. This was a particularly intriguing alternative because compared to a normal control group and patients with lesions in other brain areas, patients with lesions in the amygdala rated negative images as less arousing than other groups rated them even though they assigned similar valence ratings to the images [39]. Based on this study, Cacioppo et al. [40] suggested that aging may be associated with decreased responsiveness of the amygdala which is important to affective processing, thereby giving rise to the positivity effect. At this point, neuroimaging and behavioral studies suggest that structural and functional integrity of the emotion regions are well maintained with age [17••,41]. Even though amygdala activation is selectively diminished in response to negative stimuli, such as in response to sad or angry facial expressions [42,43], there are no age differences in response to positive [44] or novel stimuli [45]. Using a monetary incentive delay task, older participants displayed reduced activation to potential losses compared to young adults but comparable activation to gains [46,47]; see also [48]. Importantly, Sakaki et al. [18] observed greater coupling between amygdala and medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC) in older adults relative to their younger counterparts during a task involving recall of emotional faces suggesting greater association with higher order cognitive processing and decision making.

Failures to replicate the positivity effect

Shortly after Charles et al. [1] was published, our research group continued to replicate the phenomenon [12,19] and evidence from other laboratories began to accrue [4952]. Yet there were also failures to replicate using very similar methods to ours [53,54]. We began to search for systematic differences that might account for inconsistencies and recognized a subtle but theoretically important difference in approach. In our research, participants simply viewed stimuli and recall was later assessed with an incidental memory test. In experiments that failed to observe the effect, participants were asked to operate on the stimuli while viewing and in some cases were required to make judgments about the material. Again, a coherent picture began to emerge. SST maintains that chronically activated goals shift with age. We suspected that our experimental approaches tapped into ubiquitous goals in daily life; in other words, all things being equal, older people favor positive material. In studies where experimenters assigned new goals, however, and participants were asked to attend specifically to certain types of information and operate on the information (for example, [55]), preferential processing of positive materials was diminished (see [9]).

A recent meta-analysis of one hundred studies found overall support for the positivity effect as well as moderating effects across a range of conditions that theoretically influence the strength of the effect [56••]. They concluded that the positivity effect is more pronounced in studies where experimental constraints are not imposed on cognitive processing and weakest in studies that require active engagement with experimental requirements (viz., externally-imposed goals).

Is the positivity effect an explicit strategy or goal directed cognitive processing?

Some researchers have treated the positivity effect as an emotion regulation strategy [57]. As originally conceptualized, however, the positivity effect reflects goal-directed cognitive processing not a deliberative strategy. The former presumes that goals about emotional satisfaction and meaning direct attention to goal-relevant (viz., positive) stimuli. The latter presumes that positivity represents the strategic management of negative emotional states [58,59]. Given abundant evidence that older people experience relatively low levels of negative emotions in everyday life, the empirical basis for the conception of the positivity effect as emotion regulation is weak. Although favoring positive stimuli in cognitive processing may indeed have positive downstream consequences for emotional well-being, the emotional benefits are a byproduct of goal-directed cognition. Conceptual clarity is important when assessing the reliability of the positivity effect and its relationship to strategies.

Is the positivity effect adaptive?

As noted above, there are likely many advantages to focusing on positive more than negative information in daily life. One can imagine circumstances, however, when preferential treatment of positive information may be disadvantageous. Older people are the most targeted age group in financial scams [60]. Presented with clever but fraudulent enticements, a focus on positive over negative could increase risk. Along the same lines, failing to process negative information after receiving a serious medical diagnosis could be maladaptive. More research is needed to identify circumstances that place older people at risk. Findings from one recent study, however, suggest that the positivity effect is not observed when negative information is goal-relevant. English and Carstensen [22••] examined the effect in a sample that comprised relatively healthy and relatively unhealthy older adults. The positivity effect was observed in both groups when they assessed material related to buying a car. Yet only healthy adults, and not those in poor health, showed a positivity preference when reviewing health-relevant information. In other words, when the stakes are high and negative material is goal-relevant, the positivity effect may not be observed (see also, [61]).

In addition to emotional well-being, one recent study suggests that the positivity effect is associated with improved health. Kalokerinos et al. [62] tested the link between positive and negative image recall and immune system functioning at three time points. Recalling more positive images at time 1 was associated with fewer indicators of immunosenescence at times 2 and 3, measured as higher CD4 counts and lower CD4 t-cell activation.

The positivity effect also may point to ways that messaging to older adults can be more effective. Notthoff and Carstensen [63•] found that older adults who were informed about the benefits of walking walked more than those who were informed about the negative consequences of failing to walk. In an eye-tracking study, older cigarette smokers spent less time viewing graphic tobacco warning labels than younger smokers [64] suggesting that to reduce smoking among older people, warning labels should communicate the benefits of quitting rather than show graphic images of the dangers of tobacco.

Conclusion

The discovery of a positivity effect in cognitive processing, along with subsequent challenges from the scientific community, set in motion a line of research that speaks to the utility of theory and the value of resolving issues related to replicability. The conditions under which the positivity effect appears and fails to appear have refined and supported the argument that it reflects motivated cognition driven by developmental changes in goals. The fluidity of the effect across experimental conditions speaks against more intractable explanations such as neural impairment or cognitive decline.

At a more conceptual level, the negativity bias in children and young adults appears to dissipate with age. Rather than speak against the evolutionary adaptiveness of the negativity bias in early life, the positivity effect may point to a developmental shift that contributes to the survival and reproduction in the group. For example, post-reproductive members of groups who are emotionally stable and focus attention on positive information may hold benefits for the success of the larger group [65]. This area of research has yet to be explored.

Of course, many other questions remain about the possible utility of the positivity effect. In particular, more research is needed to understand the contexts and conditions where knowledge about motivated cognition can improve public health messaging and, more broadly, communications targeting elderly adults.

Acknowledgments

Funding

This work was supported by a grant to Laura L. Carstensen from the National Institute on Aging [NIA R37-8816].

Footnotes

This review comes from a themed issue on Emotion-memory interactions

Edited by Mara Mather and Michael Fanselow

For a complete overview see the Issue and the Editorial

Conflicts of interest

Nothing declared.

References

  • 1.Charles ST, Mather M, Carstensen LL. Aging and emotional memory: the forgettable nature of negative images for older adults. J Exp Psychol. 2003;132:310–324. doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.132.2.310. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Fung HH, Carstensen LL. Sending memorable messages to the old: age differences in preferences and memory for advertisements. J Pers Soc Psychol. 2003;85:163–178. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.85.1.163. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Baltazar NC, Shutts K, Kinzler KD. Children show heightened memory for threatening social actions. J Exp Child Psychol. 2012;112:102–110. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2011.11.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Kinzler KD, Shutts K. Memory for mean over nice: The influence of threat on children’s face memory. Cognition. 2008;107:775–783. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2007.09.005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.LoBue V. More than just another face in the crowd: superior detection of threatening facial expressions in children and adults. Develop Sci. 2009;12:305–313. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2008.00767.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Bebbington K, MacLeod C, Ellison TM, Fay N. The sky is falling: evidence of a negativity bias in the social transmission of information. Evol Hum Behav. 2017;38:92–101. doi: 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2016.07.004. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Baumeister RF, Bratslavsky E, Finkenauer C, Vohs KD. Bad is stronger than good. Rev Gen Psychol. 2001;5:323–370. doi: 10.1037/1089-2680.5.4.323. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Vaish A, Grossmann T, Woodward A. Not all emotions are created equal: the negativity bias in social-emotional development. Psychol Bull. 2008;134:383–403. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.134.3.383. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Reed AE, Carstensen LL. The theory behind the age-related positivity effect. Front Psychol. 2012;3:1–9. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00339. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Mammarella N, Di Domenico A, Palumbo R, Fairfield B. When green is positive and red is negative: aging and the influence of color on emotional memories. Pychol Aging. 2016;31:914–926. doi: 10.1037/pag0000122. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Cuddy LL, Sikka R, Silveira K, Bai S, Vanstone A. Music-evoked autobiographical memories (MEAMs) in Alzheimer disease: evidence for a positivity effect. Cogent Psychol. 2017;4:1–20. doi: 10.1080/23311908.2016.1277578. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Kennedy Q, Mather M, Carstensen LL. The role of motivation in the age-related positivity effect in autobiographical memory. Psychol Sci. 2004;15:208–214. doi: 10.1111/j.0956-7976.2004.01503011.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Mikels JA, Larkin GL, Reuter-Lorenz PA, Carstensen LL. Divergent trajectories in the aging mind: changes in working memory for affective versus visual information with age. Pychol Aging. 2005;20:542–553. doi: 10.1037/0882-7974.20.4.542. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Isaacowitz DM, Harris JA. Middle-aged adults facing skin cancer information: fixation, mood, and behavior. Pychol Aging. 2014;29:342–351. doi: 10.1037/a0036399. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Isaacowitz DM, Livingstone KM, Harris JA, Marcotte SL. Mobile eye tracking reveals little evidence for age differences in attentional selection for mood regulation. Emotion. 2015;15:151–161. doi: 10.1037/emo0000037. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16•.Sasse LK, Gamer M, Büchel C, Brassen S. Selective control of attention supports the positivity effect in aging. PLoS ONE. 2014;9:e104180. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0104180. . The positivity effect was more pronounced when stimuli were self-relevant to older participants. The study provides further evidence that the effect reflects selective allocation of cognitive resources based on chronically activated goals. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17••.Mather M. The affective neuroscience of aging. Annu Rev Psychol. 2016;67:213–238. doi: 10.1162/jocn_a_00392. . This paper synthesizes research on age-associated changes in structure and function of the amygdala and ventromedial prefrontal cortex, and how these changes interact with other brain regions to affect processing of emotional stimuli, emotion regulation, and recognition of facial expressions. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Sakaki M, Nga L, Mather M. Amygdala functional connectivity with medial prefrontal cortex at rest predicts the positivity effect in older adults memory. J Cognitive Neurosci. 2013;25:1206–1224. doi: 10.1162/jocn_a_00392. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Mather M, Carstensen LL. Aging and attentional biases for emotional faces. Psychol Sci. 2003;14:409–415. doi: 10.1111/1467-9280.01455. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Sava AA, Krolak-Salmon P, Delphin-Combe F, Cloarec M, Chainay H. Memory for faces with emotional expressions in Alzheimer’s disease and healthy older participants: positivity effect is not only due to familiarity. Aging Neuropsychol C. 2017;24:1–28. doi: 10.1080/13825585.2016.1143444. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Shamaskin AM, Mikels JA, Reed AE. Getting the message across: age differences in the positive and negative framing of health care messages. Pychol Aging. 2010;25:746–751. doi: 10.1037/a0018431. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22••.English T, Carstensen LL. Does positivity operate when the stakes are high? Health status and decision making among older adults. Pychol Aging. 2015;30:348–355. doi: 10.1037/a0039121. . Older adults in relatively poor health showed less positivity than healthy older adults when reviewing information related to health decisions. Positivity emerged regardless of health status for decisions that were unrelated to health, suggesting that personal relevance and contextual factors determine whether or not positivity emerges. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Zhou L, Lu J, Chen G, Dong L, Yao Y. Is there a paradox of aging: when the negative aging stereotype meets the positivity effect in older adults. Exp Aging Res. 2017;43:80–93. doi: 10.1080/0361073X.2017.1258254. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24•.Mikels JA, Shuster MM. The interpretative lenses of older adults are not rose-colored—just less dark: aging and the interpretation of ambiguous scenarios. Emotion. 2016;16:94–100. doi: 10.1037/emo0000104. . Participants were presented with ambiguous scenarios and instructed to complete the stories according to their own interpretations. Older adults continued the scenarios using less negative words than younger adults. Findings suggest that age-related positivity also occurs in spontaneous speech generated in response to ambiguity. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Carstensen LL. The influence of a sense of time on human development. Science. 2006;312:1913–1915. doi: 10.1126/science.1127488. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Fredrickson BL, Carstensen LL. Choosing social partners: How old age and anticipated endings make us more selective. Pychol Aging. 1990;5:335–347. doi: 10.1037/0882-7974.5.3.335. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Fung HH, Carstensen LL, Lutz A. The influence of time on social preferences: implications for life-span development. Pychol Aging. 1999;14:595–604. doi: 10.1037/0882-7974.14.4.595. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Lang FR, Carstensen LL. Time counts: future time perspective, goals and social relationships. Pychol Aging. 2002;17:125–139. doi: 10.1037/0882-7974.17.1.125. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.English T, Carstensen LL. Selective narrowing of social networks across adulthood is associated with improved emotional experience in daily life. Int J Behav Dev. 2014;38:195–202. doi: 10.1177/0165025413515404. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Barber SJ, Opitz PC, Martins B, Sakaki M, Mather M. Thinking about a limited future enhances the positivity of younger and older adults recall: support for socioemotional selectivity theory. Mem Cognition. 2016;44:869–882. doi: 10.3758/s13421-016-0612-0. . Researchers manipulated future time perspective in older and younger participants using a writing exercise. Regardless of chronological age, thinking about a limited future enhanced the positivity effect in recall, providing evidence that time horizons affect goal prioritization and processing of emotional information. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Labouvie-Vief G, Grühn D, Studer J. Dynamic integration of emotion and cognition: equilibrium regulation in development and aging. In: Lerner RM, Lamb ME, Freund AM, editors. The Handbook of Life-Span Development. Vol. 2. Wiley; 2010. pp. 79–115. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Mather M, Knight M. Goal-directed memory: the role of cognitive control in older adults emotional memory. Pychol Aging. 2005;20:554–570. doi: 10.1037/0882-7974.20.4.542. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Petrican R, Moscovitch M, Schimmack U. Cognitive resources, valence, and memory retrieval of emotional events in older adults. Pychol Aging. 2008;23:585–594. doi: 10.1037/a0013176. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34•.Kalenzaga S, Lamidey V, Ergis AM, Clarys D, Piolino P. The positivity bias in aging: motivation or degradation? Emotion. 2016;16:602–610. doi: 10.1037/emo0000170. . Young adults, patients with Alzheimer’s disease (AD), and unimpaired older adults were told to passively view or to memorize emotional and neutral words. Unimpaired older adults demonstrated a larger positivity bias than adults with AD in delayed recall trials. The positivity bias was greater when task instructions required participants to operate on the material. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Knight M, Seymour TL, Gaunt JT, Baker C, Nesmith K, Mather M. Aging and goal-directed emotional attention: distraction reverses emotional biases. Emotion. 2007;7:705–714. doi: 10.1037/1528-3542.7.4.705. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Kryla-Lighthall N, Mather M. The role of cognitive control in older adults emotional well-being. In: Bengtson V, Gans D, Putney N, Silverstein M, editors. Handbook of Theories of Aging. 2nd. Springer; 2009. pp. 323–344. [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Nashiro K, Sakaki M, Mather M. Age differences in brain activity during emotion processing: reflections of age-related decline or increased emotion regulation. Gerontology. 2012;58:156–163. doi: 10.1037/a0013176. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38•.Noh SR, Isaacowitz DM. The effects of varying contextual demands on age-related positive gaze preferences. Pychol Aging. 2015;30:356–368. doi: 10.1037/a0039233. . Using an eye-gaze paradigm, researchers tested the positivity effect under conditions of low, medium, and high visual and auditory noise. Older adults’ positive gaze preferences were reduced when they first viewed emotional faces within high levels of visual noise that disrupted attentional control. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Berntson GG, Bechara A, Damasio H, Tranel D, Cacioppo JT. Amygdala contribution to selective dimensions of emotion. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci. 2007;2:123–129. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsm008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Cacioppo JT, Berntson GG, Bechara A, Tranel D, Hawkley LC. Could an aging brain contribute to subjective well-being? In: Todorov A, Fiske ST, Prentice DA, editors. The value added by a social neuroscience perspective In Social Neuroscience: Toward Understanding the Underpinnings of the Social Mind. Oxford University Press; 2011. pp. 249–262. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Jiang J, Sachdev P, Lipnicki DM, Zhang H, Liu T, Zhu W, Suo C, Zhuang L, Crawford J, Reppermund S, et al. A longitudinal study of brain atrophy over two years in community-dwelling older individuals. NeuroImage. 2014;86:203–211. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.08.022. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Fischer H, Sandblom J, Gavazzeni J, Fransson P, Wright CI, Bäckman LL. Age-differential patterns of brain activation during perception of angry faces. Neurosci Lett. 2005;386:99–104. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2005.06.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Tessitore A, Hariri AR, Fera F, Smith WG, Das S, Weinberger DR, Mattay VS. Functional changes in the activity of brain regions underlying emotion processing in the elderly. Psychiat Res-Neuroim. 2005;139:9–18. doi: 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2005.02.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Mather M, Canli T, English T, Whitfield S, Wais P, Ochsner K, John G, Carstensen LL. Amygdala responses to emotionally valenced stimuli in older and younger adults. Psychol Sci. 2004;15:259–263. doi: 10.1111/j.0956-7976.2004.00662.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Wright CI, Wedig MM, Williams D, Rauch SL, Albert MS. Novel fearful faces activate the amygdala in healthy young and elderly adults. Neurobiol Aging. 2006;27:361–374. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2005.01.014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Samanez-Larkin GR, Gibbs SEB, Khanna K, Nielsen L, Carstensen LL, Knutson B. Anticipation of monetary gain but not loss in healthy older adults. Nature Neurosci. 2007;10:787–791. doi: 10.1038/nn0907-1222b. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Wu CC, Samanez-Larkin GR, Katovich K, Knutson B. Affective traits link to reliable neural markers of incentive anticipation. Neuroimage. 2014;84:279–289. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.08.055. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Spaniol J, Bowen HJ, Wegier P, Grady C. Neural responses to monetary incentives in younger and older adults. Brain Res. 2015;1612:70–82. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2014.09.063. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Isaacowitz DM, Wadlinger HA, Goren D, Wilson HR. Is there an age-related positivity effect in visual attention? A comparison of two methodologies. Emotion. 2006;6:511–516. doi: 10.1037/1528-3542.6.3.511. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Schlagman S, Schulz J, Kvavilashvili L. A content analysis of involuntary autobiographical memories: examining the positivity effect in old age. Memory. 2006;14:161–175. doi: 10.1080/09658210544000024. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Nikitin J, Freund AM. Age and motivation predict gaze behavior for facial expressions. Pychol Aging. 2011;26:695–700. doi: 10.1037/a0023281. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Tomaszczyk JC, Fernandes MA, Macleod CM. Personal relevance modulates the positivity bias in recall of emotional pictures in older adults. Psychon B Rev. 2008;15:191–196. doi: 10.3758/PBR.15.1.191. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Grühn D, Smith J, Baltes PB. No aging bias favoring memory for positive material: evidence from a heterogeneity-homogeneity list paradigm using emotionally toned words. Pychol Aging. 2005;20:579–588. doi: 10.1037/0882-7974.20.4.579. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Leclerc CM, Kensinger E. Effects of age on detection of emotional information. Pychol Aging. 2008;23:209–215. doi: 10.1037/0882-7974.23.1.209. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Leal SL, Noche JA, Murray EA, Yassa MA. Positivity effect specific to older adults with subclinical memory impairment. Learn Mem. 2016;23:415–421. doi: 10.1101/lm.042010.116. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56••.Reed AE, Chan L, Mikels JA. Meta-analysis of the age-related positivity effect: age differences in preferences for positive over negative information. Pychol Aging. 2014;29:1–15. doi: 10.1037/a0035194. . This is the first meta-analysis on the positivity effect. Results indicate that the positivity effect is reliable and moderated by processing constraints. The effect is larger in attention and memory when participants passively view emotional stimuli compared to operating on the stimuli, and also larger when age differences between young and old participants are wide. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Isaacowitz DM, Blanchard-Fields F. Linking process and outcome in the study of emotion and aging. Perspect Psychol Sci. 2012;7:3–17. doi: 10.1177/1745691611424750. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 58.Allard ES, Kensinger EA. Cognitive emotion regulation in adulthood and old age: positive gaze preferences across two strategies. Aging Neuropsychol C. 2017:1–18. doi: 10.1080/13825585.2017.1279265. [DOI] [PubMed]
  • 59.Zebrowitz LA, Boshyan J, Ward N, Gutchess A, Hadjikhani N. The older adult positivity effect in evaluations of trustworthiness: emotion regulation or cognitive capacity? PloS One. 2017;12:e0169823. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0169823. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 60.FINRA Investor Education Foundation. Financial fraud susceptibility in the United States: Research report from a 2012 survey. Washington, DC: 2013. [Google Scholar]
  • 61.Juang C, Knight BG. Age differences in interpreting ambiguous situations: the effects of content themes and depressed mood. J Gerontol B-Psychol. 2015;71:1024–1033. doi: 10.1093/geronb/gbv037. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 62.Kalokerinos EK, von Hippel W, Henry JD, Trivers R. The aging positivity effect and immune function: positivity in recall predicts higher CD4 counts and lower CD4 activation. Pychol Aging. 2014;29:636–641. doi: 10.1037/a0037452. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 63•.Notthoff N, Carstensen LL. Positive messaging promotes walking in older adults. Pychol Aging. 2014;29:329–341. doi: 10.1037/a0036748. . This study tested the effect of framing valence on health behaviors in younger and older adults. For older adults, positively-framed messages more effectively promoted walking than negatively-framed messages over a 28-day period. Young adults were unaffected by framing valence. Results point to the benefits of positive messaging to motivate health behaviors among older people. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 64.Roberts ME, Peters E, Ferketich AK, Klein EG. The Age-related positivity effect and tobacco warning labels. Tob Reg Sci. 2016;2:176–185. doi: 10.18001/TRS.2.2.8. http://dx.doi.org/10.18001/TRS.2.2.8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 65.Carstensen LL, Löckenhoff CE. Aging, emotion and evolution: The bigger picture. In: Ekman P, Campos JJ, Davidson RJ, de Waal FBM, editors. Emotions inside out: 130 years after Darwin’s the expression of the emotions in man and animals. Vol. 1000. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences; 2003. pp. 152–179. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES