Skip to main content
Schizophrenia Bulletin logoLink to Schizophrenia Bulletin
. 2018 Apr 19;44(Suppl 2):S468–S479. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sby044

The Network Structure of Schizotypal Personality Traits

Eduardo Fonseca-Pedrero 1,2,#,, Javier Ortuño 1, Martin Debbané 3,4, Raymond C K Chan 5,6, David Cicero 7, Lisa C Zhang 8, Colleen Brenner 8, Emma Barkus 9, Richard J Linscott 10, Thomas Kwapil 11, Neus Barrantes-Vidal 12, Alex Cohen 13, Adrian Raine 14,15,16, Michael T Compton 17, Erin B Tone 18, Julie Suhr 19, Felix Inchausti 20, Julio Bobes 2,21, Axit Fumero 22, Stella Giakoumaki 23, Ioannis Tsaousis 23, Antonio Preti 24, Michael Chmielewski 25, Julien Laloyaux 26,27,28, Anwar Mechri 29, Mohamed Aymen Lahmar 29, Viviana Wuthrich 30, Frank Larøi 26,27,28, Johanna C Badcock 31, Assen Jablensky 31, Adela M Isvoranu 32, Sacha Epskamp 32, Eiko I Fried 32,33,#
PMCID: PMC6188518  PMID: 29684178

Abstract

Elucidating schizotypal traits is important if we are to understand the various manifestations of psychosis spectrum liability and to reliably identify individuals at high risk for psychosis. The present study examined the network structures of (1) 9 schizotypal personality domains and (2) 74 individual schizotypal items, and (3) explored whether networks differed across gender and culture (North America vs China). The study was conducted in a sample of 27001 participants from 12 countries and 21 sites (M age = 22.12; SD = 6.28; 37.5% males). The Schizotypal Personality Questionnaire (SPQ) was used to assess 74 self-report items aggregated in 9 domains. We used network models to estimate conditional dependence relations among variables. In the domain-level network, schizotypal traits were strongly interconnected. Predictability (explained variance of each node) ranged from 31% (odd/magical beliefs) to 55% (constricted affect), with a mean of 43.7%. In the item-level network, variables showed relations both within and across domains, although within-domain associations were generally stronger. The average predictability of SPQ items was 27.8%. The network structures of men and women were similar (r = .74), node centrality was similar across networks (r = .90), as was connectivity (195.59 and 199.70, respectively). North American and Chinese participants networks showed lower similarity in terms of structure (r = 0.44), node centrality (r = 0.56), and connectivity (180.35 and 153.97, respectively). In sum, the present article points to the value of conceptualizing schizotypal personality as a complex system of interacting cognitive, emotional, and affective characteristics.

Keywords: schizotypy, schizotypal personality, psychosis, network, SPQ, mental disorders

Introduction

A clear and accurate picture of schizotypal traits is important if we are to understand the various manifestations of psychosis spectrum liability1 and to reliably identify individuals at high risk for psychosis.2–4 Schizotypal traits and schizotypal personality disorder (SPD) have been identified as potential risk factors for the onset of psychotic disorders.5–7 For instance, independent follow-up studies have shown that individuals who report schizotypal traits are at a greater risk for transition to psychosis than are those who do not endorse schizotypal characteristics.5 In samples with high genetic risk, schizotypal traits improve the individualized prediction of schizophrenia onset above and beyond the predictive capacity of neuroanatomical and neurocognitive variables.8,9 In high-risk samples, schizotypal traits have also demonstrated psychosis-predictive value.10–12 Thus, understanding subclinical psychotic experiences and traits may help elucidate relevant etiological mechanisms, risk indicators, and protective factors for psychosis spectrum disorders.6,13

At the phenomenological level, the psychosis phenotype is distributed along a severity continuum that ranges from psychological well-being to psychosis spectrum disorders.2,14 Subclinical psychotic manifestations are commonly known as psychotic-like experiences or schizotypal traits.15 The prevalence and expression of these phenomena vary according to country income and ethnicity16–18 as well as gender and age.15,19–22 For instance, African American students tend to have significantly higher scores on positive and negative schizotypy measures than Caucasian students.23 Cross-national studies show differences on schizotypal traits among residents of several European countries (eg, UK, Switzerland, Italy, and Spain)24,25 as well as between American and Spanish samples.26,27

Although there is no universal agreement regarding the latent structure of schizotypal personality—for instance, debates continue as to whether the construct is dimensional or categorical28—the factor modeling literature has consistently identified multiple facets with a minimum of 3 dimensions (ie, cognitive-perceptual, interpersonal, and disorganized),29 similar to those found in patients with psychosis.30 In recent years, a novel conceptual framework has gained attention in clinical psychology and psychiatry: That mental disorders (and other psychological constructs such as personality, intelligence, or attitudes) are emergent properties that arise from causal relations among symptoms.31–35 This contrasts with current classification systems (eg, DSM and ICD) and common research practices where symptoms are understood as passive indicators of underlying diseases. The possibility that symptoms or traits are correlated because of direct causal associations is largely overlooked.36,37

The network approach overcomes these limitations and provides an alternative way to conceptualize psychological processes and constructs such as psychosis by considering them as complex systems.38–40 Statistical network models have been added to the analytic toolbox in psychological research, with the goal to identify these structural relationships among variables.38,39 In this article, we use the conceptual framework of network theory, and related methods of network psychometrics,41 to model schizotypal personality as a complex system of interacting cognitive, emotional, and affective traits. This is consistent with recent developments in the field42: researchers used network analysis to investigate the impact of environmental risk factors (cannabis use, developmental trauma, and urban environment) on psychosis expression and to estimate the network structures of a wide range of psychotic symptoms.36,37,43–46 Recently, network models have also been used to analyze psychotic-like experiences in a large US sample.47

So far, the network structure of schizotypal personality traits has not been investigated. Given recent concerns about the stability, accuracy, and replicability of network models,48–50 such analyses should best be carried out in large samples. To this end, we used state-of-the-art network modeling techniques to estimate the network of self-reported schizotypal traits, assessed via the Schizotypal Personality Questionnaire (SPQ),51 in a dataset gathered from 27001 participants in studies conducted in 12 countries and across 21 sites. In particular, we conducted 4 sets of analysis. First, we estimated the network structure of 9 domains assessed by the SPQ, which broadly reflect the DSM-5 SPD criteria. The goal of this analysis is to provide novel insights as to how domains relate to each other. Second, we estimated networks of all 74 individual SPQ items. Third, we used graph theoretical measures, such as predictability and expected influence (EI), to interpret the network structures. Finally, we estimated and compared network structures between women and men, and between participants from North America and China.

Method

Participants

Table 1 provides a summary of the demographic characteristics of samples that provided data for the omnibus dataset. Item-level data were obtained from 21 sites across 12 countries (United States of America, United Kingdom, China, Belgium, Spain, Italy, Tunisia, Australia, New Zealand, Canada, Mauritius, and Greece). See supplementary material for the procedure of data collection.

Table 1.

Demographic Characteristics of the Sample

Study Country Main Researcher n Sampling/Procedure Mean Age (SD) Age Range Males, n (%)
1 United States Cicero 3162 College 20 (3.7) 16–55 997 (31.5)
2 United States Kwapil 1556 College 19.5 (2.9) 16–54 363 (23.3)
3 Spain Fonseca-Pedrero 1123 College 20.2 (2) 18–29 224 (19.9)
4 United States Compton 1190 College 20.9 (4) 16–52 284 (23.9)
5 United States Chmielewski 556 College 102 (18.3)
6 Mauritius Raine 1201 Birth cohort 23.4 (1.2) 21–27 688 (57.3)
7 Italian Preti 649 College 24.3 (3.5) 19–38 305 (47)
8 Australia Wuthrich 445 College 22.6 (6.3) 17–53 126 (28.3)
9 United States Cohen 1458 College 19.3 (2.2) 16–53 531 (36.4)
10 Belgium Larøi 357 General 25 (10.3) 17–55 110 (38.8)
11 Australia Badcock 342 General 36.1 (11.6) 17–55 182 (53.2)
12 Belgium Laloyaux 536 General 24.9 (8.1) 18–55 135 (25.2)
13 Tunisia Mechri 458 College 20.4 (1.4) 18–29 137 (29.9)
14 New Zealand Linscott 1648 College 20.1 (3.1) 17–51 515 (30.3)
15 United Kingdom Barkus 774 General 21.6 (4.4) 17–49 291 (37.6)
16 Australia Barkus 1144 College 326 (28.5)
17 United States Suhr 1169 College 299 (27.3)
18 China Chan 4907 College 19.7 (1.6) 16–24 2973 (60.6)
19 Canada Zhang 1849 College 20.8 (2.9) 18–53 562 (30.4)
20 United States Zhang 1386 MTurk 31.9 (9.5) 18–55 586 (42.3)
21 Greek Tsaousis 1041 General 32.4 (9.9) 18–55 390 (37.5)

The overall sample consisted of 27001 participants (n = 4251 drawn from the general population). The mean age was 22.12 years (SD = 6.28; range 16–55 years), 15.2% (n = 4113) of participants did not provide age. Only 3.3% (n = 849) of the sample was over 35 years. Participant included 37.5% (n = 10126) men and 60.6% (n = 16368) women; 1.9% (n = 507) did not specify gender. All demographic information is available in table 1. Studies were reviewed and approved by institutional review boards or ethics committees of the jurisdictions in which studies were conducted. All participants provided written informed consent before participation. Studies were conducted in accordance with the guidelines of the Declaration of Helsinki.52

Consistent with prior publications on this dataset,29 we deleted from the initial sample those participants with more than 2 missing values on the 74 SPQ items. Based on the SPSS 22.0 missing value analysis module,53 the relatively few missing values in the data were replaced by regression-based estimates to which an error component was added.

Instruments

The SPQ measures a broad range of schizotypal traits—originally it targeted 9 subordinate traits that are based on the operational definition of DSM-III-R SPD.54 These domains also represent the main features of DSM-5 SPD criteria.55 The 74 items of the SPQ are distributed across 9 subscales, each containing 7–9 items: odd beliefs or magical thinking, unusual perceptual experiences, ideas of reference, paranoid ideation/suspiciousness, excessive social anxiety, no close friends, constricted affect, odd or eccentric behavior, and odd speech. The psychometric properties have been examined in a number of nation- or region-specific studies.29 All individual SPQ items are listed in the supplementary material, and we distinguish the 9 subscales by different colors in the network figures below.

In the present study, we used the SPQ versions adapted and validated for each country: English version,51 Spanish,56 Italian,57 Chinese,58 Arabic,59 French,60 Creole,61 and Greek.62

Data Analyses

In our primary analysis, we estimated an SPQ domain network and an SPQ item network in the full sample (n = 27001). In a secondary analysis, we compared networks of women (n = 16368) and men (n = 10126), and of North American (n = 12326) and Chinese (n = 4907) study participants. For all networks, we investigated 2 graph theoretical measures: EI and predictability.

All analyses were carried out in R version 3.163 in R-Studio 1.0.136, and are described below in detail. R-packages and version numbers are listed in the supplementary materials.

General Network Estimation

A network consists of nodes (in our case the SPQ domains/items) and edges (unknown statistical relationships between nodes that need to be estimated). For the domains, which were constructed by summing items per domain and then standardizing the resulting variable, we estimated a Gaussian Graphical Model (GGM64); for the binary items, we estimated an Ising model.65 Both models result in conditional dependence relations which are akin to partial correlations: if 2 nodes are connected in the resulting graph via an edge, they are statistically related after controlling for all other variables in the network; if they are unconnected, they are conditionally independent. Both models entail the estimation of a large number of parameters but have the goal of describing the network structures parsimoniously. To avoid obtaining false positive associations among items, the models, therefore, use regularization to shrink all edge weights, setting many exactly to zero.66 This approach circumvents the problem of estimating spurious relationships and results in a sparse network structure. A detailed explanation of the 2 models can be found elsewhere.65 We interpret both models differently. In the domain network, we interpret edges as putative causal associations. That is, if A and B are connected, we hypothesize that this connection comes from A→B, A←B, or A↔B. For the item-level network, we interpret edges purely statistically (as regularized partial correlations) and not as putative causal pathways. This is because the SPQ contains many items too similar to regard them as separate variables, and the more likely explanation for some edges is that items measure the same construct.49 For instance, items 18 and 59 both ask a very similar question about whether people feel that others “have it in” for them, and the resulting edge is likely not because endorsing item 18 causes the endorsement of item 59.

For network inference, we estimated 2 measures: EI and predictability. EI is the sum of all edges of a node.67 We use EI instead of strength centrality68 (that has been used in prior work) because strength centrality uses the sum of absolute weights (ie, negative edges are turned into positive edges before summing), which distorts the interpretation if negative edges are present (such as in the present article). Predictability, on the other hand, is an absolute measure of interconnectedness: it provides us with the variance of each node that is explained by all its neighbors.69 Predictability can be understood as an upper bound of controllability: assuming that all undirected edges connected to a node point toward this node, predictability quantifies how much impact neighbors have on a focal node by intervening on them. In the figures, dark areas in the circle around nodes can be interpreted akin to R2 (% of explained variance, in case of the Ising model, above the marginals).69

Network Stability.

To test network stability and accuracy, we used bootstrapping routines implemented in the R-package bootnet.48 Given the combination of sample size and number of nodes that leads to considerable computational burden and is so far unparalleled in the psychological network literature, we performed bootstrap analyses on a high-performance computer cluster, parallelized over 100 multicore units each running 10 bootstrap samples.

Comparison of Subsamples.

Because the degree of regularization is dependent on sample size, it is difficult to compare networks estimated on different sample sizes, which was the case for the group comparisons. We, therefore, subsampled the larger datasets down to the same size as the smaller one 10 times, computed an Ising model each time, and averaged these 10 models into one final network model. We compared the resulting networks in terms of (1) similarity of adjacency matrices (ie, network structures) and (2) similarity of EI estimates by correlating these parameters across the networks. We used Pearson correlations if the distribution of parameters met assumptions of multivariate normality and Spearman correlations in the cases where normality was violated. Further, we compared SPQ total scores across the subsamples.

Note that it would have been preferable to use the Network Comparison Test (https://cran.r-project.org/web/packages/NetworkComparisonTest/) (NCT), a permutation test that investigates whether networks differ from each other. Unfortunately, the test was developed for considerably smaller samples with much fewer items, and we could not use the NCT here due to the prohibitive computational burden. It would be possible to run the test similar to the bootstrapping routines on a high-performance computer cluster if the parallelization of the NCT to multiple cores had been worked out yet, which is not yet the case.

Supplementary Materials.

We make all model output (eg, network parameters, item means, centrality, connectivity) available in the supplementary materials, along with all R codes that were used to compute the analyses.

Results

Network Structure of 9 Schizotypal Domains

As figure 1 shows, the estimated network was interconnected, with strong edges between the domains “no close friends” and “constricted affect” (0.50), “odd/magical beliefs” and “unusual perceptions” (0.37), and “ideas of reference” and “suspiciousness” (0.36). Interestingly, we also obtained 2 negative edges: between “ideas of reference” and “no close friends” (−0.11), and between “social anxiety” and “odd/magical beliefs” (−0.09). The most central nodes in terms of standardized EI (ie, the sum of edges connected to a node) were “unusual perceptions,” “constricted affect,” and “odd speech”; social anxiety was the least central domain. Predictability (variance of a node explained by its neighbors) ranged from 31% (odd/magical beliefs) to 55% (constricted affect), and average predictability was 43.7% (figure 2).

Fig. 1.

Fig. 1.

Gaussian graphical model of 9 schizotypal domains. Blue edges are positive associations, and red edges are negative ones. Thickness and saturation of edges depict the strength of associations. The filled part of the circle around each node depicts predictability: the variance of the nodes explained by all its neighbors.

Fig. 2.

Fig. 2.

Expected influence of the 9 domains schizotypal network depicted in figure 1.

General Network Structure of 74 Schizotypal Items

The full network of 74 SPQ items is depicted in figure 3. Five results are noteworthy. First, items within each of the 3 higher order dimensions were more closely associated with each other than with items of other dimensions. The average edge weights for the within-domain associations were 0.15, 0.15, and 0.27 for positive, interpersonal, and disorganization, respectively. Average edge weights across domains were 0.04 for all 3 domains (eg, all weights from items in the positive domain to items outside of the positive domain).

Fig. 3.

Fig. 3.

Ising model of 74 SPQ items. Blue edges are positive associations, and red edges are negative ones. Thickness and saturation of edges depict the strength of associations. The filled part of the circle around each node depicts predictability: the variance of the nodes explained by all its neighbors. The numbers refer to the SPQ items provided in supplementary material.

Second, we found a similar, although more pronounced, result for the 9 domains: within-scale item relations with 0.33, 0.45, 0.47, 0.26, 0.61, 0.31, 0.37, 0.24, and 0.44 were considerably stronger than associations from items in one of these subscales to all other items (all between 0.04 and 0.07). The subscale “odd/eccentric behavior” had the strongest average inter-item association (0.61), “constricted affect” the lowest (0.24).

Third, items 57 (“I tend to keep in the background on social occasions” from the no close friend subscale) and 73 (“I tend to keep my feelings to myself” from the constricted affect subscale) showed numerous negative edges.

Fourth, node predictability varied considerably across SPQ items, ranging from 3% in node 22 to 59% in node 38. The average predictability of SPQ items was 27.8%, implying that substantial variability remained unexplained. Node predictability varied across the 9 subscales and ranged from a mean of 15.0% for unusual perceptual experiences to 39.8% for excessive social anxiety.

Fifth, items that stood out in terms of EI (larger or smaller than 1.5 standard deviations) were, in decreasing order: 59 (2.64, suspiciousness), 69 (2.13, odd speech), 23 (1.94, odd behavior), 71 (1.58, excessive social anxiety), 1 (−1.97, ideas or reference), 10 (−2.03, ideas of reference), 20 (−2.07, excessive social anxiety), 54 (−2.39, excessive social anxiety), and 49 (−2.55, no close friends) (figure 4).

Fig. 4.

Fig. 4.

Expected influence of the full 74 SPQ item network depicted in figure 3.

Network Structure of Schizotypal Items across Gender

The estimated networks by gender are depicted in figure 5. The connectivity (sum of all absolute edge values) of both networks was very similar, with values of 195.59 and 199.70 for men and women, respectively. Investigating the similarity of the network structures, we found that the adjacency matrices (the edge weights) were substantially inter-correlated, with a Spearman correlation coefficient of 0.74. EI estimates were correlated 0.90 across the networks. Differences of the networks in terms of EI estimates are summarized in figure 6; only item 59 (suspiciousness) was noticeably different across networks, with a difference of 1.80 (male network EI for item 59: 3.26; female network: 1.47). Mean SPQ items differences by gender are shown in supplementary material.

Fig. 5.

Fig. 5.

Ising models of women (left) and men (right). Blue edges are positive associations, and red edges are negative ones. Thickness and saturation of edges depict the strength of associations. The numbers refer to the SPQ items provided in supplementary material.

Fig. 6.

Fig. 6.

Differences of the expected influence estimates for men minus women and North America (“West”) minus China (“East”). For example, the positive value on item 59 implies that it was more central in the Ising model estimated in men than the Ising model estimated in women.

Network Structure of Schizotypal Items across Country

Figure 7 shows networks for North American and Chinese participants. The North American network was substantially denser (connectivity, ie, sum of all absolute edge values = 180.35) than the Chinese network (153.97). The Spearman correlation coefficient of the network structures was 0.44, which was substantially lower than the correlation between male and female networks. EI estimates were correlated 0.56 across the networks. Differences between the networks (EI estimate West − EI estimate East) are summarized in figure 6. Items 37 (2.7), 68 (2.05), 57 (1.97), 54 (−2.77), and 44 (−2.87) showed the largest differences.

Fig. 7.

Fig. 7.

Ising models of North America (left) and China (right). Blue edges are positive associations, and red edges are negative ones. Thickness and saturation of edges depict the strength of associations. The numbers refer to the SPQ items provided in supplementary material.

Network Stability

The results of the stability and accuracy analysis48 available in the supplementary materials indicated that all networks were accurately estimated. The domain network was more accurately estimated than any other psychological between-subjects network in the prior literature: confidence intervals around edge weights were very small, the stability coefficient for EI was at the maximum obtainable value of 0.75, and early all edge weight comparisons were significant (ie, 528 out of all possible 561 edge weights comparisons indicated significant differences between edges weights).

Discussion

To our knowledge, this is the first study to examine the empirical network structure of schizotypal domains and traits. We use the SPQ to assess the items in a large sample of 27001 individuals across 12 countries. We are not aware of any network analysis in clinical psychology or psychiatry with a sample size comparable to the one used in the present work. In the following, we propose to understand schizotypal personality as a complex system of cognitive, emotional, and behavioral traits, and argue that psychological network methodology can aid in uncovering this complexity.

Several findings deserve a closer look. First, the 9 schizotypal domains were strongly interconnected. In particular, the relationship between nodes showed a 3-cluster named cognitive-perceptual, interpersonal, and disorganized. This network structure was quite congruent with the 3-dimensional model proposed in this arena.29 In addition, predictability values ranged from 31% (odd/magical beliefs) to 55% (constricted affect), where the average predictability was 43.7%. The average predictability found was high compared to previous network literature.69 In the domain network, we interpret schizotypal traits as putative causal associations. The network perspective proposes that correlations among schizotypal domains are due to causal connections among behaviors, beliefs, and feelings. Here, we found that odd speech and odd behavior domains, or magical thinking and unusual perceptions domains were more interconnected in the general schizotypal network than others. These findings can be considered within the network model of onset of psychotic disorder proposed by van Os and Linscott.70 The onset for the outcome of these disorders can be understood in part as different psychotic-like experiences and traits that causally impact each other over time. This is congruent with previous research that demonstrated how negative/disorganized symptoms predicted positive symptoms71 or how hallucinations gave rise to delusions.72 These findings are also consistent with the concept of emergence. As Lenzenweger4 pointed out, mental disorders represent complex configural outcomes of multiple interacting systems that cannot be reduced to a mere collection of constituent parts. These findings allow for a deeper understanding of the nature of interactions that take place among the schizotypal traits that contribute to psychosis liability.

Second, in the item-level network, and similar to domain-level, variables showed relations both within and across domains, although within-domain associations were generally stronger. Moreover, overall predictability in the full network was 27.8%, meaning that a substantial portion of the variance of the SPQ items cannot be explained by the nodes in the estimated network. This value is lower than the average predictability found at domain level. Of note, predictability was higher for the facets of excessive social anxiety, ideas of reference, suspiciousness, odd speech, and odd or eccentric behavior than for the facets of unusual perceptual experiences, constricted effect, odd beliefs or magical thinking, and no close friends. These results may indicate that important variables that could contribute to trait/symptom development are missing in the estimated model. In addition, disorders within the psychosis spectrum are thought to arise from a crucial interplay between genes and environment73—it would, therefore, be expected that part of the missing predictability here is due to the absence of genetic or environmental components in the network. Some of these findings both domain and item level are consistent with previous research using the network framework in patients with psychosis and nonclinical samples.36,37,43–47 For instance, Murphy et al47 found in a study of psychotic-like experiences (PE) in a large US sample that the network of symptom severity ratings revealed strong interconnectivity between PEs, and that paranoia nodes were among the most central in the network.

Third, estimated networks of men and women were similar, as were node centrality and connectivity. In spite of small differences in the network structures, connectivity and centrality estimates in the present study were similar for both men and women, indicating that differences may not lie within the inter-item associations. Research in the field of psychotic disorders often identifies different symptom profiles for men and women, with men presenting with poorer premorbid functioning and worse course of illness.74–76 In nonclinical populations, however, findings have been more inconsistent, with women seeming to score higher on measures of positive schizotypal features and men seeming to score higher on measures of negative schizotypal features.15,20–22,77 To date, due to limited data, it was not possible to compare networks of men and women for patients. We hope future research could address this issue—if the network structures and connectivity are found to be different for men and women within a patient population.

Fourth, at the country level, North American and Chinese participants networks showed lower similarity than the gender comparison both in terms of structure, node centrality, and connectivity. In addition, the North American network estimated for SPQ items was substantially denser than the network for Chinese participants, which is particularly interesting in light of recent work showing that groups with more densely connected networks are likely to have more adverse outcomes in the future (reviewed in Fried et al40). Previous research has demonstrated that the prevalence and expression of these subclinical psychotic phenomena (eg, psychotic-like experiences, schizotypal traits) varies according to place, culture, income, and ethnic minority groups16–18 as well as gender and age,15,19–21 but has also found that schizotypal traits similarly cluster for Chinese and Western samples.78 Even though our results show a similar network structure for the 2 populations, we also pinpoint previously unidentified differences between Western and Eastern countries. It is important to note that our results are exploratory and no other studies to date have compared schizotypal traits between North American and Chinese samples, limiting the degree to which we can situate our findings in the extant literature. Bhugra et al79,80 found that Asian patients diagnosed with schizophrenia display better premorbid functioning than do Caucasian patients; future research could investigate whether the symptom network is also less densely connected in Chinese than in North American patient samples, potentially supporting the idea of a more resilient network structure. Broadly, however, we believe that comparison of schizotypal traits among people from different cultures has the potential to provide us with information on cultural differences in social and affective functioning42 that could ultimately prove highly valuable in clinical practice.

The results of the present study should be interpreted in the light of the following limitations. First, the majority of the participants were college students and this characteristic may affect the generalization of the results to other populations of interest. Second, the study is subject to the problems inherent to any research based on self-reports like the effect of stigmatization, the possibility of misunderstanding of some items, the lack of introspection of some participants, and that of social desirability. Third, the infrequency scale to screen out participants who responded in a random manner was not systematically employed in all samples. Fourth, it was not possible to use the novel network comparison test to investigate statistical differences across networks because the test cannot currently deal with a very large number of participants. Instead, and consistent with prior publications (eg,49,81), we report the correlation of the network structures as a measure of similarity. Fifth, as discussed in more detail elsewhere, psychological networks model single items that are prone to measurement error.49 While some of the issues surrounding measurement error are unresolved,82,83 it is noteworthy that the average predictability in the main network presented in this study was 27.4%, which is somewhat lower than the average predictability identified in a re-analysis of 25 datasets from 18 prior psychopathology network studies.69 Interestingly, across all analyzed studies, psychosis had the lowest average predictability of 28%, remarkably similar to our result. The authors of the reanalysis concluded that not only excluding important variables from the network can result in lower average predictability (a lot of variance of the nodes remains unexplained), but also measurement error. Sixth, as highlighted in the methods section, an edge between 2 items of very similar content cannot be understood as a putative causal pathway. Because the SPQ contains many item pairs that are very similar in nature, especially relations within the domains should only be interpreted statistically, ie, as regularized partial correlation coefficients. Edges across domains, however, are consistent with standard network-theoretical interpretations. In addition, these domains are common across almost all schizotypal and psychosis (risk) inventories; thus, we hope this analysis will enable future research to investigate how the domain network structure obtained here replicates or generalizes using different measuring instruments and samples. Finally, the between-subjects network analysis of one large aggregated dataset in cross-sectional data means that we can neither draw strong conclusions about the dynamic nature of associations, nor know whether the network structure generalizes to within-person processes.

Conclusions

This study is the first to comprehensively examine the network structure of the self-reported schizotypal traits using a large multinational sample. The results are consistent with the conceptual notion of schizotypal personality as a complex network structure of cognitive, emotional, and behavioral traits. This study also offers a deeper understanding of the subclinical psychosis expression or schizotypy (psychosis liability).

Even though it is only at the beginning of the road, the research in the domain of psychopathology has embraced network theory and methodology as a pair of matching tools with the goal to shed light on the complexity of the psychosis spectrum phenotype and, ultimately, to contribute to advancements in clinical practice. We hope that this will open new avenues for nosology, etiological research, assessment strategies, prevention, and treatment.

Supplementary Material

Supplementary data are available at Schizophrenia Bulletin online.

Supplementary Material

Acknowledgments

E.F.P. was supported by the Spanish Ministry of Science and Innovation (MICINN) (PSI2014-56114-P), the Instituto Carlos III, Center for Biomedical Research in the Mental Health Network (CIBERSAM), 2015 edition of the BBVA Foundation Grants for Researchers and Cultural Creators and AYUDAS FUNDACIÓN BBVA A EQUIPOS DE INVESTIGACION CIENTIFICA 2017. E.I.F. was supported by the European Research Council Consolidator grant no. 647209. M.D. was supported by the Swiss National Science Foundation (100019_159440). R.C.K.C. was supported by the National Basic Research Program of China (Precision Psychiatry Programme: 2016YFC0906402), the Beijing Training Project for Leading Talents in S&T (Z151100000315020), the Beijing Municipal Science & Technology Commission grant (Z161100000216138), and the CAS/SAFEA International Partnership Programme for Creative Research Teams (Y2CX131003). S.G. and I.T. were supported by the “ARISTEIA II” Action of the Operational Programme Education and Lifelong Learning and was co-funded by the European Social Fund (ESF) and National Resources (grant number KA 2990). A.M.I. was supported by the Netherlands Organisation for Scientific Research (NWO) Talent.

References

  • 1. Meehl PE. Schizotaxia, schizotypy, schizophrenia. Am Psychol. 1962;17:827–838. [Google Scholar]
  • 2. Kwapil TR, Barrantes-Vidal N. Schizotypy: looking back and moving forward. Schizophr Bull. 2015;41(suppl 2):S366–S373. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3. Keshavan MS, DeLisi LE, Seidman LJ. Early and broadly defined psychosis risk mental states. Schizophr Res. 2011;126:1–10. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4. Lenzenweger MF. Schizotypy and Schizophrenia: The View from Experimental Psychopathology. New York: Guilford Press; 2010. [Google Scholar]
  • 5. Debbané M, Eliez S, Badoud D, Conus P, Flückiger R, Schultze-Lutter F. Developing psychosis and its risk states through the lens of schizotypy. Schizophr Bull. 2015;41(suppl 2):S396–S407. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6. Barrantes-Vidal N, Grant P, Kwapil TR. The role of schizotypy in the study of the etiology of schizophrenia spectrum disorders. Schizophr Bull. 2015;41(suppl 2):S408–S416. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7. Fusar-Poli P, Carpenter WT, Woods SW, McGlashan TH. Attenuated psychosis syndrome: ready for DSM-5.1?Annu Rev Clin Psychol. 2014;10:155–192. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8. Zarogianni E, Storkey AJ, Johnstone EC, Owens DG, Lawrie SM. Improved individualized prediction of schizophrenia in subjects at familial high risk, based on neuroanatomical data, schizotypal and neurocognitive features. Schizophr Res. 2017;181:6–12. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9. Tandon N, Montrose D, Shah J, Rajarethinam RP, Diwadkar VA, Keshavan MS. Early prodromal symptoms can predict future psychosis in familial high-risk youth. J Psychiatr Res. 2012;46:105–110. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10. Salokangas RK, Dingemans P, Heinimaa M, et al. ; EPOS group Prediction of psychosis in clinical high-risk patients by the Schizotypal Personality Questionnaire. Results of the EPOS project. Eur Psychiatry. 2013;28:469–475. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11. Flückiger R, Ruhrmann S, Debbané M, et al. . Psychosis-predictive value of self-reported schizotypy in a clinical high-risk sample. J Abnorm Psychol. 2016;125:923–932. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12. Mason O, Startup M, Halpin S, Schall U, Conrad A, Carr V. Risk factors for transition to first episode psychosis among individuals with ‘at-risk mental states’. Schizophr Res. 2004;71:227–237. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13. Fonseca Pedrero E, Debbané M. Schizotypal traits and psychotic-like experiences during adolescence: an update. Psicothema. 2017;29:5–17. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14. van Os J, Reininghaus U. Psychosis as a transdiagnostic and extended phenotype in the general population. World Psychiatry. 2016;15:118–124. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15. Linscott RJ, van Os J. An updated and conservative systematic review and meta-analysis of epidemiological evidence on psychotic experiences in children and adults: on the pathway from proneness to persistence to dimensional expression across mental disorders. Psychol Med. 2013;43:1133–1149. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16. Nuevo R, Chatterji S, Verdes E, Naidoo N, Arango C, Ayuso-Mateos JL. The continuum of psychotic symptoms in the general population: a cross-national study. Schizophr Bull. 2012;38:475–485. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17. McGrath JJ, Saha S, Al-Hamzawi A, et al. . Psychotic experiences in the general population: a cross-national analysis based on 31,261 respondents from 18 countries. JAMA Psychiatry. 2015;72:697–705. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18. Cicero DC. Measurement invariance of the Schizotypal Personality Questionnaire in Asian, Pacific Islander, white, and multiethnic populations. Psychol Assess. 2016;28:351–361. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19. van Os J, Linscott RJ, Myin-Germeys I, Delespaul P, Krabbendam L. A systematic review and meta-analysis of the psychosis continuum: evidence for a psychosis proneness-persistence-impairment model of psychotic disorder. Psychol Med. 2009;39:179–195. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20. Miettunen J, Jääskeläinen E. Sex differences in Wisconsin Schizotypy Scales—a meta-analysis. Schizophr Bull. 2010;36:347–358. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21. Fonseca-Pedrero E, Lemos-Giráldez S, Paino M, Sierra-Baigrie S, Muñiz J. Phenotypic expression of schizotypal traits in an adolescent population. J Pers Disord. 2012;26:539–550. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22. Bora E, Baysan Arabaci L. Effect of age and gender on schizotypal personality traits in the normal population. Psychiatry Clin Neurosci. 2009;63:663–669. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23. Chmielewski PM, Fernandes LO, Yee CM, Miller GA. Ethnicity and gender in scales of psychosis proneness and mood disorders. J Abnorm Psychol. 1995;104:464–470. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24. Fonseca-Pedrero E, Ortuño-Sierra J, Sierro G, et al. . The measurement invariance of schizotypy in Europe. Eur Psychiatry. 2015;30:837–844. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25. Ortuño-Sierra J, Badoud D, Knecht F, et al. . Testing measurement invariance of the schizotypal personality questionnaire-brief scores across Spanish and Swiss adolescents. PLoS One. 2013;8:e82041. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26. Kwapil TR, Ros-Morente A, Silvia PJ, Barrantes-Vidal N. Factor invariance of psychometric schizotypy in Spanish and American samples. J Psychopathol Behav Assess. 2012;34:145–152. [Google Scholar]
  • 27. Fonseca-Pedrero E, Cohen A, Ortuño-Sierra J, de Álbeniz AP, Muñiz J. Dimensional structure and measurement invariance of the Schizotypal Personality Questionnaire—Brief Revised (SPQ-BR) scores across American and Spanish samples. J Pers Disord. 2017;31:522–541. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28. Everett KV, Linscott RJ. Dimensionality vs taxonicity of schizotypy: some new data and challenges ahead. Schizophr Bull. 2015;41(suppl 2):S465–S474. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29. Fonseca-Pedrero E, Debbané M, Ortuño-Sierra J, et al. . The structure of schizotypal personality traits: a cross-national study. Psychol Med. 2018;48:451–462. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30. Liddle PF. The symptoms of chronic schizophrenia. A re-examination of the positive-negative dichotomy. Br J Psychiatry. 1987;151:145–151. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31. McNally RJ, Robinaugh DJ, Wu GWY, et al. . Mental disorders as causal systems : a network approach to posttraumatic stress disorder. Clin Psychol Sci. 2014;3:1–14. [Google Scholar]
  • 32. Fried EI. Problematic assumptions have slowed down depression research: why symptoms, not syndromes are the way forward. Front Psychol. 2015;6:309. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33. Schmittmann VD, Cramer AOJ, Waldorp LJ, Epskamp S, Kievit RA, Borsboom D. Deconstructing the construct: a network perspective on psychological phenomena. New Ideas Psychol. 2013;31:43–53. [Google Scholar]
  • 34. Mõttus R, Allerhand M. The underlying trait and network approaches. In: Zeigler-Hill V, Shackelford T, eds. SAGE Handbook of Personality and Individual Differences: Volume 1. The Science of Personality and Individual Differences. London: SAGE; 2017:1–22. [Google Scholar]
  • 35. Fonseca-Pedrero E. Análisis de redes: ¿una nueva forma de comprender la psicopatología?Rev Psiquiatr Salud Ment. 2017;10:206–215. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36. van Rooijen G, Isvoranu A, Meijer C, et al. . A symptom network structure of the psychosis spectrum. Schizophr Res. 2017;189:75–83. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37. Isvoranu AM, Borsboom D, van Os J, Guloksuz S. A network approach to environmental impact in psychotic disorder: brief theoretical framework. Schizophr Bull. 2016;42: 870–873. doi:10.1093/schbul/sbw049. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38. Borsboom D, Cramer AO. Network analysis: an integrative approach to the structure of psychopathology. Annu Rev Clin Psychol. 2013;9:91–121. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39. Borsboom D. A network theory of mental disorders. World Psychiatry. 2017;16:5–13. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40. Fried EI, van Borkulo CD, Cramer AOJ, Boschloo L, Schoevers RA, Borsboom D. Mental disorders as networks of problems: a review of recent insights. Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol. 2016;58:7250–7257. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41. Epskamp S, Maris G, Waldorp LJ, Borsboom D. Network psychometrics. In: Irwing P, Hughes D, Booth T, eds. Handbook of Psychometrics. New York, NY: Wiley; 2018. [Google Scholar]
  • 42. Cohen AS, Mohr C, Ettinger U, Chan RC, Park S. Schizotypy as an organizing framework for social and affective sciences. Schizophr Bull. 2015;41(suppl 2):S427–S435. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43. Isvoranu AM, van Borkulo CD, Boyette LL, Wigman JTW, Vinkers CH, Borsboom D. A Network Approach to Psychosis: Pathways between Childhood Trauma and Psychotic Symptoms. Schizophr Bull. 2016;(Advance Access):1–10. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44. Levine SZ, Leucht S. Identifying a system of predominant negative symptoms: network analysis of three randomized clinical trials. Schizophr Res. 2016;178:17–22. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45. Wigman JTW, de Vos S, Wichers M, van Os J, Bartels-Velthuis AA. A transdiagnostic network approach to psychosis. Schizophr Bull. 2017;43:122–132. doi:10.1093/schbul/sbw095. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46. Klippel A, Viechtbauer W, Reininghaus U, et al. . The cascade of stress: a network approach to explore differential dynamics in populations varying in risk for psychosis. Schizophr Bull. 2018;44:328–337. doi:10.1093/schbul/sbx037. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47. Murphy J, McBride O, Fried E, Shevlin M. Distress, impairment and the extended psychosis phenotype: a network analysis of psychotic experiences in a US general population sample. Schizophr Bull. 2017. doi:10.1093/schbul/sbx134. [Epub ahead of print]. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48. Epskamp S, Borsboom D, Fried EI. Estimating psychological networks and their accuracy: a tutorial paper. Behav Res Methods. 2018;50:195–212. doi:10.3758/s13428-017-0862-1. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49. Fried EI, Cramer AOJ. Moving forward: challenges and directions for psychopathological network theory and methodology. Perspect Psychol Sci. 2017;12:999–1020. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50. Fried EI, Eidhof MB, Palic S, et al. . Replicability and generalizability of Posttraumatic Stress Disorder (PTSD) networks: a cross-cultural multisite study of PTSD symptoms in four trauma patient samples. Clin Psychol Sci. 2018. doi:10.1177/2167702617745092. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51. Raine A. The SPQ: a scale for the assessment of schizotypal personality based on DSM-III-R criteria. Schizophr Bull. 1991;17:555–564. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52. World Medical Association. World medical association declaration of Helsinki: ethical principles for medical research involving human subjects. J Am Med Assoc. 2013;310:2191–2194. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53. IBM Corp Released. IBM SPSS Statistics for Windows, Version 22.0. Armonk, NY: IBM Corp; 2013. [Google Scholar]
  • 54. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (3rd Ed. Revised) (DSM-III-R). Washington, DC: APA;1987. [Google Scholar]
  • 55. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders: DSM-5. Washington DC: American Psychiatric Association; 2013:991. doi:10.1176/appi.books.9780890425596.744053. [Google Scholar]
  • 56. Fonseca-Pedrero E, Fumero A, Paino M, et al. . Schizotypal personality questionnaire: new sources of validity evidence in college students. Psychiatry Res. 2014;219:214–220. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 57. Fossati A, Raine A, Carretta I, Leonardi B, Maffei C. The three-factor model of schizotypal personality: invariance across age and gender. Pers Individ Dif. 2003;35:1007–1019. [Google Scholar]
  • 58. Chen WJ, Hsiao CK, Lin CC. Schizotypy in community samples: the three-factor structure and correlation with sustained attention. J Abnorm Psychol. 1997;106:649–654. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 59. Lahmar ML, Gassab L, Beltaief F, Mechri A. Psychometric properties of the Arabic version of the schizotypal personality questionnaire in Tunisian university students. Tunis Med. 2014;92:318–322. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 60. Dumas P, Bouafia S, Gutknecht C, Saoud M, Dalery J, d’Amato T. Validation of the French version of the raine schizotypal personality disorder questionnaire–categorial and dimensional approach to schizotypal personality traits in a normal student population. Encephale. 2000;26:23–29. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 61. Reynolds CA, Raine A, Mellingen K, Venables PH, Mednick SA. Three-factor model of schizotypal personality: invariance across culture, gender, religious affiliation, family adversity, and psychopathology. Schizophr Bull. 2000;26:603–618. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 62. Tsaousis I, Zouraraki C, Karamaouna P, Karagiannopoulou L, Giakoumaki SG. The validity of the schizotypal personality questionnaire in a Greek sample: tests of measurement invariance and latent mean differences. Compr Psychiatry. 2015;62:51–62. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 63. R Core Team. R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing. Vienna, Austria: R Foundation for Statistical Computing; 2016. [Google Scholar]
  • 64. Epskamp S, Fried E. A tutorial on regularized partial correlation networks. Psychol Methods. 2018. doi:10.1037/met0000167 [Epub ahead of print]. https://arxiv.org/pdf/1607.01367.pdf. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 65. van Borkulo CD, Borsboom D, Epskamp S, et al. . A new method for constructing networks from binary data. Sci Rep. 2014;4:5918. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 66. Ravikumar P, Wainwright MJ, Lafferty JD. High-dimensional Ising model selection using ℓ 1 -regularized logistic regression. Ann Stat. 2010;38:1287–1319. [Google Scholar]
  • 67. Robinaugh DJ, Millner AJ, McNally RJ. Identifying highly influential nodes in the complicated grief network. J Abnorm Psychol. 2016;125:747–757. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 68. Opsahl T, Agneessens F, Skvoretz J. Node centrality in weighted networks: generalizing degree and shortest paths. Soc Networks. 2010;32:245–251. [Google Scholar]
  • 69. Haslbeck JMB, Fried EI. How predictable are symptoms in psychopathological networks? A reanalysis of 18 published datasets. Psychol Med. 2017;47:2767–2776. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 70. van Os J, Linscott RJ. Introduction: the extended psychosis phenotype–relationship with schizophrenia and with ultrahigh risk status for psychosis. Schizophr Bull. 2012;38:227–230. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 71. Dominguez MD, Saka MC, can Saka M, Lieb R, Wittchen HU, van Os J. Early expression of negative/disorganized symptoms predicting psychotic experiences and subsequent clinical psychosis: a 10-year study. Am J Psychiatry. 2010;167:1075–1082. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 72. Krabbendam L, Myin-Germeys I, Hanssen M, et al. . Hallucinatory experiences and onset of psychotic disorder: evidence that the risk is mediated by delusion formation. Acta Psychiatr Scand. 2004;110:264–272. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 73. van Os J, Kenis G, Rutten BP. The environment and schizophrenia. Nature. 2010;468:203–212. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 74. Abel KM, Drake R, Goldstein JM. Sex differences in schizophrenia. Int Rev Psychiatry. 2010;22:417–428. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 75. Canuso CM, Pandina G. Gender and schizophrenia. Psychopharmacol Bull. 2007;40:178–190. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 76. Maric N, Krabbendam L, Vollebergh W, de Graaf R, van Os J. Sex differences in symptoms of psychosis in a non-selected, general population sample. Schizophr Res. 2003;63:89–95. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 77. Raine A. Sex differences in schizotypal personality in a nonclinical population. J Abnorm Psychol. 1992;101:361–364. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 78. Wang Y, Neumann D, Shum DH, Chan RC. A cross-validation study of clustering of schizotypy using a non-clinical Chinese sample. Psychiatry Res. 2012;200:55–58. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 79. Bhugra D, Corridan B, Rudge S, Leff J, Mallett R. Social factors and first onset schizophrenia among Asians and whites. Int J Soc Psychiatry. 1999;45:162–170. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 80. Bhugra D, Corridan B, Rudge S, Leff J, Mallett R. Early manifestations, personality traits and pathways into care for Asian and white first-onset cases of schizophrenia. Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol. 1999;34:595–599. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 81. Rhemtulla M, Fried EI, Aggen SH, Tuerlinckx F, Kendler KS, Borsboom D. Network analysis of substance abuse and dependence symptoms. Drug Alcohol Depend. 2016;161:230–237. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 82. Borsboom D, Fried EI, Epskamp S, et al. . Replicability of psychopathology networks: the right question but the wrong answer. A comment on “Evidence that psychopathology symptom networks have limited replicability” by Forbes, Wright, Markon, and Krueger. J Abnorm Child Psychol. 2017;126(7):989–999. doi: 10.1037/abn0000306. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 83. Forbes MK, Wright AGC, Markon KE, Krueger RF. Evidence that psychopathology symptom networks have limited replicability. J Abnorm Psychol. 2017;126:969–988. doi:10.1037/abn0000276. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

Supplementary Material

Articles from Schizophrenia Bulletin are provided here courtesy of Oxford University Press

RESOURCES