Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2018 Oct 17.
Published in final edited form as: Curr Epidemiol Rep. 2017 May 30;4(2):133–144. doi: 10.1007/s40471-017-0113-0

Age-Based Reproductive Healthcare Stereotype Threat (HCST) as a Stressor Affecting Prenatal Mental Health in Pregnant Women of Advanced Maternal Age: Measurement, Process, Outcomes, and Interactions with Ethnicity/Race, SES, and Other Social Identities

Cleopatra M Abdou 1
PMCID: PMC6191845  NIHMSID: NIHMS880640  PMID: 30345220

Abstract

Purpose of Review

Reproductive health, and pregnancy more specifically, is the first critical link between generations. Beginning with this first critical link, pregnancy acts as a domino, affecting the expression of genes and determining the lifespan mental and physical health and reproductive potential of offspring and, likely, of subsequent generations. Given the powerful intergenerational domino that is pregnancy, the development of innovative models to enhance reproductive health and outcomes is an invaluable public health investment.

Recent Findings

While U.S. pregnancy and birth outcomes have improved dramatically since the 1960s— including substantial progress within the past 15 years, largely catalyzed by the Healthy People initiative—group-based disparities remain. What’s more, social change and medical advancements have led to an evolving window of female reproductive age. Despite becoming more common, being an older expectant mother remains a stigmatized social identity. The concept of healthcare stereotype threat (HCST) is introduced in relation to reproductive health. Stereotype threat is a situational predicament in which an individual who possesses a stigmatized social identity fears confirming negative group-based stereotypes. HCST is a healthcare-specific form of stereotype threat, arising out of stereotypes that are salient in healthcare settings. It is hypothesized that the experience of age-based reproductive HCST is an overlooked stressor affecting prenatal mental and physical health among women of advanced maternal age.

Summary

The hypothesized process of age-based reproductive HCST is described; outcomes and consequences are discussed; interactions with ethnicity/race, SES, and other aspects of social identity are considered; and strategies for prevention and intervention are explored, including active and passive shifts in three areas: (1) internally, in both providers and patients; (2) in the external environment, or the physical healthcare setting itself; and (3) within interpersonal interactions that occur within healthcare settings, particularly between physicians and patients. Implications for gynecological, infertility, and obstetric care and improving the reproductive outcomes of older women are discussed.

Keywords: reproductive health, infertility, pregnancy, advanced maternal age, birth outcomes, stress, mental health, healthcare stereotype threat, stereotype threat, health care, health disparities

Introduction

Women’s reproductive health is not just a women’s health issue. It is not even merely a health issue. It is better classified as a societal issue given it’s far-reaching and lasting imprint on our society [14]. As such, the development of innovative models that better explain variability in reproductive outcomes is an invaluable public health investment. In 1997, Shiono and colleagues [5] found that 46 well-known sociodemographic risk factors accounted for only 10% of the variance in birth outcomes among African American, Chinese, Dominican, Mexican, Puerto Rican, and White women. Two decades later, reproductive outcomes and disparities remain a public health priority and a scientific puzzle [68]. Adding to the complexity of reproductive health, the demographic distribution of women of reproductive age is shifting to encompass older and older ages as a product of increased schooling and medical advancements, including in the area of assistive reproductive technologies (ART) [9, 10]. Whereas 1% of births were to women aged 35 and older in 1970, 8% of births were to women aged 35 and older in 2006 [11]. Older women, particularly those who have undergone some form of ART, bring to the experience of pregnancy their own unique set of risk and resilience factors. Evolving and innovative models of reproductive health, then, are not just a worthwhile investment, but also deeply necessary—both for the purpose of solving the intergenerational puzzle that is reproductive health and for the purpose of evolving with the changing reproductive age bracket. Reproductive healthcare stereotype threat (HCST), on the basis of age and other factors, may be an important new lens for understanding and interrogating reproductive health differences and disparities1. The term HCST was recently coined and presented as an overlooked psychosocial determinant of healthcare and larger health experiences and disparities [1417]. The idea of HCST grew out of the broader stereotype threat literature, which demonstrates that individuals who belong to stigmatized social groups (i.e., groups for whom popularly-held negative stereotypes exist) experience the threat of being judged by, and/or of personally confirming, negative group-based stereotypes when operating in stereotype-relevant domains [18, 19]. For instance, females indicate less interest in math [2022], leadership roles [23], and other traditionally masculine domains [2427] when female stereotypes are made salient. HCST—a healthcare-specific form of stereotype threat—is defined as the threat of being judged by, and/or of personally confirming through one’s own actions, behaviors, or outcomes, negative group-based stereotypes that are salient in healthcare settings [15, 16]. Notably, the types of healthcare-relevant stereotypes that are called to mind differ with the particular dimension(s) of social identity under threat. For example, with respect to ethnicity/race-based HCST, negative health-relevant stereotypes include inferior intelligence, lower status, greater likelihood of engaging in risky behaviors and lifestyle choices, and being less deserving or needing of the best available care [1417, 2830]. For instance, a stunning survey of medical students and physicians demonstrated that African American patients are perceived as possessing innate biological differences and that these perceptions lead to inferior healthcare delivery on the part of physicians (e.g., African Americans were rated as having less sensitive nerve endings than Whites, which likely contributes to the less aggressive pain treatment commonly received by African Americans) [30]. In contrast to ethnicity/race-based HCST, age-based HCST is likely the product of a very different set of stereotypes, including being past one’s prime and lower likelihood of being in good health. I propose that age-based HCST has particular relevance for reproductive health and healthcare experiences. For older women experiencing pregnancy or seeking to become pregnant—particularly women who meet the criterion for advanced maternal age (i.e., 35 years of age or older)—prominent medical and sociocultural messages about precipitously declining fertility, a ticking biological clock, and exponentially heightened risks to both mother and child can result in age-based reproductive HCST, which may have important consequences for prenatal mental and physical health. Please refer to Figure 1 for a conceptual model detailing the hypothesized process and outcomes of age-based reproductive HCST. The purpose of this review is to introduce the theory of age-based reproductive HCST. It is proposed that age-based reproductive HCST is an important contributor to reproductive health and healthcare experiences among older women, with special attention to prenatal mental health. The emerging literature on HCST and the broader literature on the general phenomenon of stereotype threat are reviewed. These literatures are then integrated with the reproductive health literature.

Figure 1.

Figure 1

A Process- and Outcome-Focused Model of Age-Based Reproductive Healthcare Stereotype Threat (HCST).

Interactions among the stereotype, the threat, and perceived and objective realities

It is important to note that, in the conceptualization of age-based reproductive HCST, it is not suggested that heightened maternal-fetal risk with advanced maternal age is not real. Drawing a parallel with ethnicity/race-based HCST, for instance, it is empirically documented that African Americans have a greater risk, relative to Whites and most other groups, of developing a wide range of physical health conditions, including obesity and hypertension [2, 31] It is also well-documented that African Americans, Latinos, and other people of color disproportionately live in poverty [32]. Images depicting African Americans and Latinos as being at greater risk of engaging in harmful lifestyle choices that lead to these adverse health outcomes and/or being of lower SES, then, can be stereotypical—in that they are overgeneralizations, resistant to disconfirming evidence, and overlook important individual differences—even when they reflect reality to at least some degree. Similarly, it is well-documented that advanced maternal age is associated with adverse fertility, pregnancy, and birth outcomes, including, among other things, decreased fertility [33] and increased risk of miscarriage [34], gestational hypertension [35], preeclampsia [35], and chromosomal abnormalities [34]. Whereas a woman in her 20s has a 20–25% likelihood of conceiving during any given menstrual cycle, a woman in her 30s has a 15–20% likelihood of conceiving during any given menstrual cycle, and a woman in her 40s has a 5% likelihood of conceiving during any given menstrual cycle [36]. Female fertility typically peaks around age 24, and begins to decline between the ages of 25 and 32 [37, 38].

By contrast, the relationship of maternal age to prenatal and postpartum mental health is more complex. The factors that put expectant mothers at risk for outcomes such as pregnancy anxiety and postpartum depression are highly correlated with age, with many risk factors being far more common among younger women, but a few robust predictors becoming more prevalent with advanced maternal age. For instance, unintended pregnancy, the lack of a stable relationship with the baby’s father or other sources of social support, and financial instability and other forms of stress are reliable predictors of pregnancy anxiety and postpartum depression [3, 4, 3945]. Both in terms of lived realities and popular stereotypes, these social and economic circumstances are more commonly associated with younger mothers. However, past pregnancy loss, as well as past and present pregnancy complications, are also robust predictors of prenatal and postpartum mental health challenges, including pregnancy anxiety and postpartum depression and its variants (i.e., postpartum blues, depression, and psychosis) [14, 39, 44]. Therefore, not only are older women more likely to experience difficulty becoming pregnant and carrying a pregnancy to term, they may also be more likely to suffer from pregnancy anxiety and postpartum depression directly as a result of being older and the perceived and actual risks associated with this older age. These prenatal mental and physical health experiences, in turn, affect postpartum mental and physical health, as well as pregnancy and birth outcomes that have been shown to affect offspring in early childhood and throughout the lifecourse and even into subsequent generations [14].

Thus, age-based HCST is put forth as a theory of the psychosocial dimensions of reproductive health outcomes, both mental and physical, and differences and disparities among older women. This psychosocial process also takes into account the tremendous individual variability in overall health, fertility potential, and prenatal health, including susceptibility to prenatal and postpartum anxiety, blues, depression, and psychosis. It also accounts for the arbitrary aspects of the age 35 cut-off for meeting criterion for advanced maternal age. The psychosocial process of age-based reproductive HCST may interact with, but in no way diminishes the importance of, the realities of human biology—one of which is that the reproductivespan of human females is limited in both length and quality.

Pregnancy: A universal and critical domino

Pregnancy is a universal experience. Pregnancy is a health event that occurs with great frequency across time and space. It also happens within a restricted timeframe (10 months; rather than nine as is commonly believed) and under reasonably well-defined parameters. The outcomes of pregnancy are clear and relatively easy to measure [46, 47]. They pertain to both mother and neonate antenatally, as well as during labor, delivery, and the first weeks to year of the infant’s life.

Pregnancy can be thought of as a domino because it leads to a cascade of outcomes— which are by and large positive, but can also be challenging—that affect the woman and offspring, as well as their larger family and society [48, 49]. In addition, the cascade of outcomes that is pregnancy, including the birth of a healthy child, affect the woman for the rest of her individual lifecourse. Even more strikingly, this domino ripples across multiple generations. Reproduction is the first link between generations. Regarding the intergenerational transmission of health, the outcomes of a woman’s pregnancy—including both the mental and physical health outcomes that are likely to be affected by age-based HCST among women of advanced maternal age—affect fetal development in utero, birth outcomes, early childhood outcomes in offspring, the future reproductive potential of offspring, susceptibility to disease and other markers of aging in offspring, and, therefore, reproductivespan, healthspan, and lifespan in offspring and their offspring.

Age-based HCST as a stressor affecting prenatal mental health

Given the significance and sensitivity of the prenatal period, it is no surprise that a large literature has examined links of physical and psychological stressors to pregnancy outcomes [14, 6, 3941, 5054]. Indeed, general stressors as well as those specific to certain aspects of social identity—particularly ethnicity/race and social class—have demonstrated links to a wide range of prenatal (e.g., pregnancy anxiety), labor (e.g., preterm labor and other medical complications), birth (e.g., birthweight), and postpartum (e.g., depression) outcomes [14, 6, 42, 50, 51, 55]. However, to my knowledge, neither stereotype threat nor the more specific application that is the focus of this paper, HCST, have been examined in relation to pregnancy or other aspects of fertility.

Stereotype threat has been most commonly studied in African Americans and females [18, 19, 5658]. Further, stereotype threat research has focused on performance domains, particularly academics and leadership, because a central tenet of stereotype threat theory is that the experience of stereotype threat creates conditions of cognitive load, resulting in performance decrements [57, 59]. For example, African Americans have been shown to perform poorly relative to Whites on standardized tests only when under conditions of stereotype threat, such as when having to check a box identifying their ethnicity/race at the start of the testing session [60]. When the possibility of stereotype threat is eliminated or reduced in the testing environment, the African American-White disparity in test performance disappears [18, 61, 62]. Similarly, explicitly countering female stereotypes removes gender differences in performance in stereotypically masculine domains, such as math and leadership [6365].

The general phenomenon of stereotype threat is regarded as a psychosocial stressor, and, as with other stressors, stereotype threat has immediate physiological consequences, which have implications for mental and physical health [66]. The general experience of stereotype threat has been experimentally linked to increases in negative affect [59] and blood pressure and other physiological indicators of stress [28, 67]. Although stereotype threat has previously been linked to immediate physiological consequences, little is known, empirically-speaking, about the long-term mental and physical health consequences of stereotype threat given the traditional cross-sectional experimental paradigm that is used to study stereotype threat. It is hypothesized that, over time, individuals with stigmatized identities disidentify with, or disengage from, the stereotyped domain in order to avoid the psychological, social, and performance consequences of stereotype threat, including poor self-esteem and/or poor performance [22, 68], Nevertheless, this long-term process of disidentification largely remains an open empirical question.

HCST is one of the first applications of stereotype threat theory that examines the longer-term, rather than only immediate, mental and physical health consequences of the threat of being judged by negative group stereotypes in stereotype-relevant domains [16]. HCST is also the first application of stereotype threat specifically in relation to healthcare and health experiences and disparities, as well as to the specific stereotypes that are salient in healthcare settings [15,16]. Further, HCST is a critical application of stereotype threat theory that examines domains that are directly critical to livelihood [15, 16]. To date, it is unclear how the process of stereotype threat-induced disidentification unfolds in domains that are critical to livelihood, including the domain of healthcare. HCST has been linked to underutilization of preventive care, physician distrust, and heightened anxiety in healthcare settings [15, 16]. HCST has also been hypothesized to lead to avoidance of healthcare and delayed care in the face of ambiguous or chronic health problems [15, 16]. The very solution to age-based health risks, including age-based reproductive health risks, is healthcare. Therefore, it seems unlikely that age-based reproductive HCST leads to delay or avoidance of prenatal care. Nevertheless, it likely does lead to greater anxiety related to prenatal care.

Previous work has demonstrated that HCST is predictive of immediate [15] and longer-term [16] indicators of health under both experimental and observational conditions. Using observational survey methods, HCST has been linked to downstream indicators of, and disparities in, mental and physical health in older adulthood, including depression, poor self-rated health, and hypertension [16]. Importantly, HCST has also been linked to adverse healthcare outcomes, which in and of themselves have potentially important and cumulative health implications, including use of preventive care, physician distrust, dissatisfaction with healthcare, and greater healthcare-specific anxiety [15]. If HCST is, in fact, experienced during pregnancy on the basis of age and/or other factors as predicted (ethnicity/race-based HCST and SES-based HCST [15, 69], as well as weight-based HCST and religion-based HCST, are likely also related to pregnancy and other reproductive outcomes and disparities in important ways), this is a critical application of HCST specifically and stereotype threat theory in general. This also raises important questions about the long-term consequences—as in the 10 months of pregnancy, but also as in the lifespan and intergenerational consequences—of the HCST process.

As previously mentioned, no empirical studies have examined any type of HCST (i.e., age-based, ethnicity/race-based, or otherwise) in pregnant, or trying to conceive, samples to date. A 2014 [15] experimental study demonstrated that when primed with negative stereotypical images of African American women’s reproductive health, highly identified African American women were more likely than other African American women and all White women to experience ethnicity/race-based HCST. It seems plausible that a parallel process occurs in relation to reproductive health, and pregnancy specifically, on the basis of age.

Changes in reproductive potential over time and intersections with ethnicity/race, SES, and other aspects of social identity

Although the experience of declining fertility with age is universal to human females, the rate of change is not uniform across individuals [70, 71]. It is also not uniform across population groups or subgroups [7274]. In addition, as the 2014 HCST study suggests [15], it is highly likely that age-based reproductive stereotypes are likely compounded by stereotypes arising out of the other potentially stigmatized aspects of identity with which they intersect, including ethnicity/race, SES, sexual orientation, and body composition. It is interesting to note, however, that both the stereotype and the reality of older mothers is that they are more educated and affluent. Further, it is often the case that mothers of color, including African Americans, certain Asian subgroups (e.g., Filipinas), Latinas, and Middle Easterners and North Africans, are stereotyped as having more children and having them earlier in life [11]. Therefore, popular stereotypical images of advanced maternal age and its consequences don’t account for women of color to the same degree that they do White women. Yet, because of their lower social standing and ascribed value in our society [31, 32], and the psychophysiological weathering and premature aging that results from this lower social status [14, 42, 43, 50, 51, 53, 54], women of color are thought to experience more rapid declines in their fertility with age [53, 54]. As a result, women of color, and their children, may actually be most vulnerable to age-based HCST and its downstream healthcare and reproductive, physical, and mental health consequences. Further, women of color may have fewer family or peer examples of women successfully having children later in life, thereby further compounding the effects of age-based HCST. Sexual orientation and gender identity present additional, important layers of complexity, as same-sex couples typically require medical intervention (e.g., at-home insemination with donor sperm) to achieve pregnancy, regardless of age or fertility status. This presents unique practical and social challenges, including the need to discuss identity with doctors.

Susceptibility to age-based reproductive HCST

People are most susceptible to stereotype when they are a) highly identified with the stereotyped identity and/or b) highly identified with, or invested in, the stereotyped domain [75, 76]. For example, African Americans and females for whom being African American or female is a central aspect of the self will be more susceptible to stereotype threat and its deleterious psychological, social, and performance consequences when operating in domains in which popular negative stereotypes of African Americans or females are common [58, 75, 77]. Similarly, African Americans or females who are highly invested in the stereotyped domain—for example, academic success or success in math, the sciences, and/or leadership roles—are most susceptible to stereotype threat and its cascade of deleterious consequences [66, 7880]. Therefore, in terms of susceptibility produced by the stereotyped identity, women for whom age is a central aspect of identity (even if just in relation to their fertility, pregnancy, and broader reproductive experiences) will be more susceptible to HCST. On the other hand, women for whom age is not a central aspect of identity, in general or in relation to reproduction, may be less susceptible to the prenatal healthcare or health consequences of age-based reproductive HCST. Similarly, a woman whose subjective, or felt, age is lower than her objective, or actual, age may view the age-based stereotypes of high-risk pregnancy to be less relevant to her, thereby reducing the likelihood that she will reproductive HCST on the basis of her age. In terms of susceptibility produced by the stereotyped domain, women who are highly identified, or invested in, the role of motherhood may be more likely to experience age-based HCST. Personally and culturally, the importance of becoming a mother and having biological children in terms of self-definition and status in family and community likely also produce increased susceptibility to age-based reproductive HCST for older women hoping to become pregnant in the near future or who are already pregnant [81]. This has important and interesting implications for women who have invested thousands of dollars and months (if not years) of their lives in assistive reproductive technologies, as in the case of in vitro fertilization. This consideration also has implications for women experiencing primary infertility, versus those who are experiencing secondary infertility (i.e., women who are experiencing difficulty conceiving or carrying a pregnancy to term following the natural conception and birth of one or more biological children [82]). For instance, it may be that older women seeking to get pregnant for the first time are more susceptible to age-based reproductive HCST, whereas older women seeking to get pregnant for the second, third, or fourth time may be protected to some degree by already having a history of successfully conceiving and delivering a healthy baby and by already possessing the role of mother.

Unique features of age-based HCST

To date, HCST has most commonly been studied as a function of ethnicity/race and among African Americans as compared to Whites. As of this writing, a small number of studies have also examined HCST among Latinos and other socially stigmatized groups (e.g., older adults), as well as on the basis of multiple potentially stigmatized aspects of identity. In addition to ethnicity/race, these include socioeconomic status (SES), gender, age, and body composition (i.e., normal weight, overweight, obese) [16]. The psychosocial experience of age-based HCST, when compared to the experience of HCST on the basis of other potentially stigmatized aspects of identity, possesses key unique features that should be taken under consideration. Critically, age changes over time. This is similar to weight, which often changes over time; and also possibly SES, which—although less likely—can change over time. This is in contrast to dimensions of identity that are generally stable over time (e.g., ethnicity/race, gender, sexual orientation). Second, age—like SES, religion, or sexual orientation—may be concealable or, at least, not readily visible [8389]. Again, this is in contrast to ethnicity/race or gender, which generally are readily visible (whether the visible perceived identity accurately matches the self-identity is another question altogether). This is both interesting and important because the ability to conceal older age may suggest a youthfulness that is not just skin deep. In fact, there are several lines of evidence linking youthfulness and longevity to fertility [1, 9092]. Research on centenarians has demonstrated that women who have their last child after the age of 33 had twice the odds of living to age 95 than women who had their last child by age 30 or younger [90]. Patterns such as these are likely, at least in part, attributable to increased likelihood of delayed childbearing among more affluent women, who also have longer life expectancies because they are more affluent (i.e., the SES-health gradient). However, this may also suggest that women who remain fertile for longer are more youthful at a biological level and, therefore, have longer life lifespans in addition to reproductivespans [1, 9092].

Implications for practice, institutional and public health policy, and future research

There is a complex constellation of medical, psychobiological, and sociocultural factors that affect the ability to become and stay pregnant, as well as to experience a pregnancy in which both mother and infant are mentally and physically healthy. I propose that reproductive HCST on the basis of age (and other potentially stigmatized aspects of identity as well) is an overlooked force among these determinants of healthy pregnancy. There are a number of critical implications if age-based HCST is, in fact, a potent force in the prenatal mental and physical health outcomes of women with advanced maternal age and other women who worry that they have waited too long to have children. Growing evidence suggests that physicians, quite literally, cannot empathize with the pain of African Americans and likely other minority patients [30]. This phenomenon (i.e., difficulty empathizing with patients who are different from self or otherwise stigmatized) is likely in some ways similar and in some ways different for female patients, including pregnant women with advanced maternal age. As of 2017, a majority (64%) of practicing obstetricians continues to be male [93]. In addition, the realities of male biology are very different from that of female biology. Taken together, these two factors likely indicate that it is difficult for obstetricians to empathize with their patients. Although not specific to gynecological, infertility, and/or obstetric care, multiple recent studies have demonstrated that patients of female physicians fare better mentally and emotionally than patients of male physicians [9495]. However, the ability of male obstetricians to empathize with their pregnant patients may be affected, not just by firsthand experience, but also by vicarious experience. Male physicians may experience, through people they care for deeply (their mothers, sisters, wives, daughters, and friends) what it is like to be a female in the American healthcare system. This may translate—in at least some cases and to at least some degree—to a greater ability to empathize with female patients despite the lack of firsthand experience with the stereotypes and HCST experienced by females.

Conclusion and Recommendations for Medical Practice and Future Research

Unlike other social determinants of health and healthcare experiences, including poverty and discrimination, stereotype threat is highly modifiable. Specifically, addressing implicit and explicit stereotypes in the environment can remove stereotype threat and its deleterious consequences [60, 63, 66, 96, 97]. Early evidence suggests that HCST, similarly, is highly modifiable through both internal, environmental, and interpersonal shifts that address patients, providers, and the larger medical setting (please see web-based HCST training, available through Anthem insurance, for practical strategies for implementing these ideas in medical practice: https://www.anthem.com/hcst/) [98]. For example, symbolic and deliberate messages of age diversity in obstetric offices and other prenatal care facilities may make age-related reproductive health stereotypes less salient, thereby reducing the experience of age-based HCST and its cascade of deleterious psychosocial, affective, behavioral, cognitive, psychophysiological, and relational consequences. An obstetric office where it is common to see older expectant mothers may be one such symbolic message that could alleviate age-based HCST and its consequences. In terms of deliberate messages, public health messages in the form of posters, brochures, or other print materials that women encounter in the course of prenatal care, which contain negative stereotypes, or are shame- or fear-based, may increase the likelihood that pregnant women of advanced maternal age experience age-based HCST. On the other hand, public health messages that convey positive messages about the health potential of older expectant mothers and their children may help to reduce age-based HCST and its negative mental as well as physical health consequences. Cross-sectional and longitudinal experimental and observational research are needed to interrogate and clarify the role of age-based HCST (and other forms of HCST) in healthcare and health experiences and disparities among the evolving segment of the human female population of reproductive age.

Acknowledgments

I am grateful to National Institutes of Health/National Institute on Aging grant P30AG043073 for the generous support that made this work possible.

Footnotes

Compliance with Ethical Guidelines

Conflict of Interest

Cleopatra M. Abdou declares no potential conflict of interest.

Human and Animal Rights and Informed Consent

This article contains no studies with human or animal subjects performed by any of the authors.

SpringerLink Header: Social Epidemiology (A. Aiello, Section Editor) Topical Collection on Social Epidemiology

1

Differences and disparities are distinguished here. A health difference is one that is expected. For instance, female fertility rates differ by age. This is an expected and natural difference (even as it is evolving to some degree). Disparities are differences in health that are unnecessary, avoidable, and unjust [12, 13]. For instance, the high rate of adverse birth outcomes among African American women, as compared to White women of the same age, is a disparity.

References

  • 1**.Abdou CM, Dominguez TP, Myers HF. Maternal familism predicts birthweight and asthma symptoms three years later. Soc Sci Med. 2013;76:28–38. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2012.07.041. The epidemiological paradox and other health literatures have hypothesized that cultural resources contribute to better outcomes during pregnancy and early life, but few studies have tested this empirically. This study investigated whether the nonmaterial cultural resource of familism was protective against low birthweight and asthma symptoms three years later in African American, Latino, and White families, most of whom were facing socioeconomic disadvantage. Familism, independent of ethnicity and lifespan family socioeconomic position, was found to be associated with better outcomes—namely, higher birthweight and fewer asthma symptoms by age three. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2**.Abdou CM. Minority aging before birth and beyond: Life span and lntergenerational adaptation through positive resources. In: Whitfield K, Baker T, editors. Handbook of Minority Aging. Springer Publishing Company; 2013. pp. 9–24. This chapter introduces Aging Before Birth and Beyond, a lifespan and intergenerational model of human development It also directly links this lifespan and intergenerational view of aging to minority health and health disparities It discusses the importance of positive psychology, and nonmaterial cultural resources in particular, for advancing understanding of heterogeneity in minority health This discussion is guided by the Culture and Social Identity Health Theory (CSIH), a theoretical framework for examining independent and interactive effects of culture and social identities on health The four premises of this theory include 1) social identities impose powerful constraints on culture by making certain beliefs, attitudes, and experiences more salient, 2) culture can have direct effects on health that are independent of links to social identity, 3) culture often exerts its effect on health by moderating social identity-based (e.g., ethnic and socioeconomic) disparities, and 4) intrapersonal and interpersonal processes—including the psychosocial process of healthcare stereotype threat—underlie both culture and social identity effects on health. [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Abdou CM, Schetter CD, Jones F, et al. Community perspectives: Mixed-methods investigation of culture, stress, resilience, and health. Ethn Dis. 2010;20(Suppl 2):41–48. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Abdou CM, Dunkel Schetter C, Campos B, et al. Communalism predicts prenatal affect, stress, and physiology better than ethnicity and socioeconomic status. Cultural Diversity and Ethnic Minority Psychology. 2010;16(3):395–403. doi: 10.1037/a0019808. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Shiono PH, Rauh VA, Park M, Lederman SA, Zuskar D. Ethnic differences in birthweight: The role of lifestyle and other factors. Am J Public Health. 1997;87(5):787–793. doi: 10.2105/ajph.87.5.787. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Lu MC, Halfon N. Racial and ethnic disparities in birth outcomes: A life-course perspective. Matern Child Health J. 2003;7(1):13–30. doi: 10.1023/a:1022537516969. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Kramer MS, Seguin L, Lydon J, Goulet L. Socio-economic disparities in pregnancy outcome: Why do the poor fare so poorly? Paediatr Perinat Epidemiol. 2000;14(3):194–210. doi: 10.1046/j.1365-3016.2000.00266.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Anachebe NF, Sutton MY. Racial disparities in reproductive health outcomes. Obstet Gynecol. 2003;188(4):S37–S42. doi: 10.1067/mob.2003.245. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Jain T, Missmer SA, Hornstein MD. Trends in embryo-transfer practice and in outcomes of the use of assisted reproductive technology in the United States. N Engl J Med. 2004;350(16):1639–1645. doi: 10.1056/NEJMsa032073. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.de Mouzon J, Goossens V, Bhattacharya S, et al. Assisted reproductive technology in Europe, 2006: Results generated from European registers by ESHRE. Human Reproduction. 2010;124 doi: 10.1093/humrep/deq124. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11**.Mathews TJ, Hamilton BE. Mean age of mothers is on the rise: United States, 2000–2014. NCHS Data Brief. 2016;232(232) Presents key findings from the National Vital Statistics System on the mean age of mothers. Importantly, mother’s age is associated with the total number of births, as well as birth defects, multiple births, and other birth outcomes. During the period from 2000–2014, age at first birth has increased from 24.9 to 26.3 years. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Carter-Pokras O, Baquet C. What is a “health disparity”? Public Health Reports. 2002;117:426–34. doi: 10.1093/phr/117.5.426. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Whitehead M. The concepts and principles of equity and health. Health Promotion International. 1991;6:217–28. [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Abdou CM, Fingerhut AF, Davies PG. The effect of stereotype threat on physician trust. Poster presentation for the international association for cross-cultural psychology meeting. International Journal of Psychology. 2008;43:3–4. This was the first study to suggest that there is a need to apply the social science theory of stereotype threat to the health sciences and, specifically, to the domains of healthcare and broader health disparities African American and White women were randomly assigned to either a Neutral Condition or a Healthcare Stereotype Threat Condition, which contained negative stereotypical images of African American women’s sexuality The images utilized were from real-world album covers Women in both conditions were then presented with a set of ambiguous symptoms and a photograph and brief biographical description of a hypothetical physician, Dr David Campbell The women were then asked to make a series of healthcare decisions related to the set of ambiguous symptoms. [Google Scholar]
  • 15**.Abdou CM, Fingerhut AW. Stereotype threat among Black and White women in health care settings. Cultural Diversity and Ethnic Minority Psychology. 2014;20(3):316. doi: 10.1037/a0036946. This was the first experimental study conducted with a community sample to test the concept of healthcare stereotype threat. In a virtual health care setting, African American and White women randomly assigned to the Healthcare Stereotype Threat Condition were exposed to negative stereotypical images of African American women’s reproductive health. This study found that highly identified African American women in the Healthcare Stereotype Threat Condition reported more healthcare-specific anxiety. This study utilized real-life public health campaigns (in the form of posters on the wall of the virtual waiting and examination rooms). Findings suggest that public health campaigns that reinforce negative stereotypes may inadvertently invoke healthcare stereotype threat, thereby creating conditions under which vulnerable or high-risk groups may actually be less likely to receive the care they need in a timely fashion. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16**.Abdou CM, Fingerhut AW, Jackson JS, Wheaton FV. Healthcare stereotype threat in older adults in the Health and Retirement Study. Am J Prev Med. 2016;50(2):191–198. doi: 10.1016/j.amepre.2015.07.034. This study is the first to examine associations of healthcare stereotype threat to health and healthcare utilization among older adults. Data were collected on a subsample of Health and Retirement Study participants. Results showed that healthcare stereotype threat predicted poorer mental and physical health (i.e., poor self-rated health, hypertension and depressive symptoms). It was also associated with poorer healthcare outcomes, including physician distrust, dissatisfaction with healthcare, and lower likelihood of using preventative care. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Burgess DJ, Warren J, Phelan S, Dovidio J, Van Ryn M. Stereotype threat and health disparities: What medical educators and future physicians need to know. Journal of General Internal Medicine. 2010;25(2):169–177. doi: 10.1007/s11606-009-1221-4. This theoretical piece presents an application of Stereotype threat Theory to the study of health disparities that is consisted with the theory of healthcare stereotype threat presented and tested by Abdou and colleagues (Abdou, Fingerhut, & Davies, 2008; Abdou & Fingerhut, 2014; Abdou, Fingerhut, Jackson, & Wheaton, 2016). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Steele CM, Aronson J. Stereotype threat and the intellectual test performance of African Americans. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1995;69(5):797. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.69.5.797. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Steele CM, Aronson J. Contending with a stereotype: African-American intellectual test performance and stereotype threat. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1995;69:797–811. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.69.5.797. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Eccles JS. Gender roles and women’s achievement-related decisions. Psychol Women Q. 1987;11(2):135–172. [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Eccles JS, Jacobs JE. Social forces shape math attitudes and performance. Signs. 1986:367–380. [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Schmader T, Johns M, Barquissau M. The costs of accepting gender differences: The role of stereotype endorsement in women’s experience in the math domain. Sex Roles. 2004;50(11–12):835–850. [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Davies PG, Spencer SJ, Steele CM. Clearing the air: Identity safety moderates the effects of stereotype threat on women’s leadership aspirations. J Pers Soc Psychol. 2005;88(2):276. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.88.2.276. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Carr PB, Steele CM. Stereotype threat affects financial decision making. Psychol Sci. 2010;21(10):1411–1416. doi: 10.1177/0956797610384146. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Murphy MC, Steele CM, Gross JJ. Signaling threat: How situational cues affect women in math, science, and engineering settings. Psychol Sci. 2007;18(10):879–885. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.01995.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Gupta VK, Bhawe NM. The influence of proactive personality and stereotype threat on women’s entrepreneurial intentions. Journal of Leadership & Organizational Studies. 2007;13(4):73–85. [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Burgess DJ, Joseph A, van Ryn M, Carnes M. Does stereotype threat affect women in academic medicine? Acad Med. 2012;87(4):506–512. doi: 10.1097/ACM.0b013e318248f718. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Aronson J, Burgess D, Phelan SM, Juarez L. Unhealthy interactions: The role of stereotype threat in health disparities. Am J Public Health. 2013;103(1):50–56. doi: 10.2105/AJPH.2012.300828. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Burgess DJ, Van Ryn M, Crowley-Matoka M, Malat J. Understanding the provider contribution to race/ethnicity disparities in pain treatment: Insights from dual process models of stereotyping. Pain Medicine. 2006;7(2):119–134. doi: 10.1111/j.1526-4637.2006.00105.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Hoffman KM, Trawalter S, Axt JR, Oliver MN. Racial bias in pain assessment and treatment recommendations, and false beliefs about biological differences between Blacks and Whites. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2016;113(16):4296–4301. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1516047113. This study examines whether false beliefs about biological differences between Blacks and whites is related to pain assessment. Results revealed that both White laypersons and White medical students perceive African Americans as possessing innate biological differences. These false beliefs are hypothesized to help explain disparities in pain assessment and treatment. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Centers for Disease Control and Prevention (CDC) CDC health disparities and inequalities report: United States. Suppl 3. Vol. 62. MMWR; 2013. 2013. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Macartney S, Bishaw A, Fontenot K. Poverty rates for selected detailed race and Hispanic groups by state and place 2007–2011. 2013:11–17. [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Navot D, Bergh R, Williams MA, et al. Poor oocyte quality rather than implantation failure as a cause of age-related decline in female fertility. The Lancet. 1991;337(8754):1375–1377. doi: 10.1016/0140-6736(91)93060-m. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Cleary-Goldman J, Malone FD, Vidaver J, et al. Impact of maternal age on obstetric outcome. Obstetrics & Gynecology. 2005;105(5, Part 1):983–990. doi: 10.1097/01.AOG.0000158118.75532.51. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Roberts CL, Algert CS, Morris JM, Ford JB, Henderson-Smart DJ. Hypertensive disorders in pregnancy: A population-based study. Med J Aust. 2005;182(7):332–335. doi: 10.5694/j.1326-5377.2005.tb06730.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Broekmans FJ, Knauff EAH, te Velde ER, Macklon NS, Fauser BC. Female reproductive ageing: current knowledge and future trends. Trends in Endocrinology & Metabolism. 2007;18:58–65. doi: 10.1016/j.tem.2007.01.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Leridon H. Human fertility. University of Chicago Press; 1977. [Google Scholar]
  • 38.van Noord-Zaadstra BM, Looman CW, Alsbach H, Habbema JD, te Velde ER, Karbaat J. Delaying childbearing: effect of age on fecundity and outcome of pregnancy. BMJ. 1991;302:1361–65. doi: 10.1136/bmj.302.6789.1361. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Dunkel Schetter C. Psychological science on pregnancy: Stress processes, biopsychosocial models, and emerging research issues. Annu Rev Psychol. 2011;62:531–558. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.031809.130727. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Collins NL, Dunkel-Schetter C, Lobel M, Scrimshaw SC. Social support in pregnancy: Psychosocial correlates of birth outcomes and postpartum depression. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1993;65(6):1243. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.65.6.1243. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Wadhwa PD, Sandman CA, Porto M, Dunkel-Schetter C, Garite TJ. The association between prenatal stress and infant birth weight and gestational age at birth: A prospective investigation. Obstet Gynecol. 1993;169(4):858–865. doi: 10.1016/0002-9378(93)90016-c. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Dominguez TP, Dunkel-Schetter C, Glynn LM, Hobel C, Sandman CA. Racial differences in birth outcomes: The role of general, pregnancy, and racism stress. Health Psychology. 2008;27(2):194. doi: 10.1037/0278-6133.27.2.194. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Giscombé CL, Lobel M. Explaining disproportionately high rates of adverse birth outcomes among african americans: The impact of stress, racism, and related factors in pregnancy. Psychol Bull. 2005;131(5):662. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.131.5.662. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44*.Biaggi A, Conroy S, Pawlby S, Pariante CM. Identifying the women at risk of antenatal anxiety and depression: A systematic review. J Affect Disord. 2016;191:62–77. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2015.11.014. This study presents the findings of a systematic literature review on studies that identify risk factors for antenatal anxiety and depression. The most important factors identified include lack of a partner/social support, abuse/domestic violence, mental illness, unplanned/unwanted pregnancy, stress, and pregnancy complications and loss. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45*.Rubertsson C, Hellström J, Cross M, Sydsjö G. Anxiety in early pregnancy: Prevalence and contributing factors. Archives of Women’s Mental Health. 2014;17(3):221–228. doi: 10.1007/s00737-013-0409-0. This study explores prevalence of, and risk factors for, antenatal anxiety in early pregnancy. Approximately 16% of respondents in a population-based community sample of pregnant women experienced anxiety symptoms in early pregnancy. The following risk factors were identified: age <25 years, non-native language, low education, unemployment, nicotine use before pregnancy, and history of depression or anxiety. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46*.Ide M, Papapanou PN. Epidemiology of association between maternal periodontal disease and adverse pregnancy outcomes–systematic review. J Clin Periodontol. 2013;40(s14) doi: 10.1111/jcpe.12063. This is a review of the literature on maternal periodontal disease and adverse pregnancy outcomes A modest association was found between maternal periodontitis and low birth weight, pre-term birth, and pre-eclampsia However, there was substantial heterogeneity across studies in methodological characteristics, including how periodontitis was defined. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Deshpande N, James N, Kucirka L, et al. Pregnancy outcomes in kidney transplant recipients: A systematic review and Meta-Analysis. American Journal of Transplantation. 2011;11(11):2388–2404. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-6143.2011.03656.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Nomaguchi KM, Milkie MA. Costs and rewards of children: The effects of becoming a parent on adults’ lives. Journal of Marriage and Family. 2003;65(2):356–374. [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Umberson D, Pudrovska T, Reczek C. Parenthood, childlessness, and well-being: A life course perspective. Journal of Marriage and Family. 2010;72(3):612–629. doi: 10.1111/j.1741-3737.2010.00721.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Campos B, Schetter CD, Abdou CM, Hobel CJ, Glynn LM, Sandman CA. Familialism, social support, and stress: Positive implications for pregnant Latinas. Cultural Diversity and Ethnic Minority Psychology. 2008;14(2):155. doi: 10.1037/1099-9809.14.2.155. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Hilmert CJ, Schetter CD, Dominguez TP, et al. Stress and blood pressure during pregnancy: Racial differences and associations with birthweight. Psychosom Med. 2008;70(1):57–64. doi: 10.1097/PSY.0b013e31815c6d96. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52**.Flagg LA, Needham BL, Locher JL. Neighborhood disadvantage, preconception health behaviors and infant birthweight: A preliminary study. Int J Contemp Sociol. 2014;51(1):7–25. This study tests whether diet, physical activity, and BMI mediate the association between neighborhood disadvantage/disorder and birth outcomes. Results using data from the National Longitudinal Study of Adolescent Health (Add Health) did not find these factors to be mediators of the association. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Geronimus A. The weathering hypothesis and the relationship of maternal age to birth outcome: Evidence and speculations. Ethnic Dis. 1992;2:207–221. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54*.Geronimus AT. Deep integration: Letting the epigenome out of the bottle without losing sight of the structural origins of population health. Am J Public Health. 2013;103(S1):S56–S63. doi: 10.2105/AJPH.2013.301380. This is a review of the weathering hypothesis and the developmental origins of health and disease hypothesis and argues that both can contribute to advancing understanding of population health inequality. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Rini CK, Dunkel-Schetter C, Wadhwa PD, Sandman CA. Psychological adaptation and birth outcomes: The role of personal resources, stress, and sociocultural context in pregnancy. Health Psychology. 1999;18(4):333. doi: 10.1037//0278-6133.18.4.333. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56.Spencer SJ, Steele CM, Quinn DM. Stereotype threat and women’s math performance. J Exp Soc Psychol. 1999;35(1):4–28. [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Steele CM, Spencer SJ, Aronson J. Contending with group image: The psychology of stereotype and social identity threat. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology. 2002;34:379–440. [Google Scholar]
  • 58.Steele CM. A threat in the air: How stereotypes shape intellectual identity and performance. Am Psychol. 1997;52(6):613. doi: 10.1037//0003-066x.52.6.613. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 59.Schmader T, Johns M, Forbes C. An integrated process model of stereotype threat effects on performance. Psychol Rev. 2008;115(2):336. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.115.2.336. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 60.Alter AL, Aronson J, Darley JM, Rodriguez C, Ruble DN. Rising to the threat: Reducing stereotype threat by reframing the threat as a challenge. J Exp Soc Psychol. 2010;46(1):166–171. [Google Scholar]
  • 61.Aronson J, Fried CB, Good C. Reducing the effects of stereotype threat on African American college students by shaping theories of intelligence. J Exp Soc Psychol. 2002;38(2):113–125. [Google Scholar]
  • 62.Marx DM, Goff PA. Clearing the air: The effect of experimenter race on target’s test performance and subjective experience. British Journal of Social Psychology. 2005;44(4):645–657. doi: 10.1348/014466604X17948. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 63.Johns M, Schmader T, Martens A. Knowing is half the battle: Teaching stereotype threat as a means of improving women’s math performance. Psychol Sci. 2005;16(3):175–179. doi: 10.1111/j.0956-7976.2005.00799.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 64.Martens A, Johns M, Greenberg J, Schimel J. Combating stereotype threat: The effect of self-affirmation on women’s intellectual performance. J Exp Soc Psychol. 2006;42(2):236–243. [Google Scholar]
  • 65.McIntyre RB, Lord CG, Gresky DM, Ten Eyck LL, Frye GJ, Bond CF., Jr A social impact trend in the effects of role models on alleviating women’s mathematics stereotype threat. Current Research in Social Psychology. 2005;10(9):116–136. [Google Scholar]
  • 66**.Spencer SJ, Logel C, Davies PG. Stereotype threat. Annu Rev Psychol. 2016;67:415–437. doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-073115-103235. This review of stereotype threat brings together approximately two decades of research on this topic. It covers sources of stereotype threat, mediators and moderators of its effects (underlying mechanisms), and consequences of stereotype threat. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 67.Blascovich J, Spencer SJ, Quinn D, Steele C. African Americans and high blood pressure: The role of stereotype threat. Psychol Sci. 2001;12(3):225–229. doi: 10.1111/1467-9280.00340. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 68.Kiefer AK, Sekaquaptewa D. Implicit stereotypes, gender identification, and math-related outcomes: A prospective study of female college students. Psychol Sci. 2007;18(1):13–18. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.01841.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 69.Rosenthal L, Lobel M. Explaining racial disparities in adverse birth outcomes: Unique sources of stress for Black American women. Social Science & Medicine. 2011;72(6):977–983. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2011.01.013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 70*.Bongaarts J, Potter RE. Fertility, biology, and behavior: An analysis of the proximate determinants. Academic Press; 1983. [Google Scholar]
  • 71.Baird DT, Collins J, Egozcue J, et al. Fertility and ageing. Hum Reprod Update. 2005;11(3):261–276. doi: 10.1093/humupd/dmi006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 72.Huddleston HG, Cedars MI, Sohn SH, Giudice LC, Fujimoto VY. Racial and ethnic disparities in reproductive endocrinology and infertility. Obstet Gynecol. 2010;202(5):413–419. doi: 10.1016/j.ajog.2009.12.020. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 73.Butts SF, Seifer DB. Racial and ethnic differences in reproductive potential across the life cycle. Fertil Steril. 2010;93(3):681–690. doi: 10.1016/j.fertnstert.2009.10.047. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 74.Harlow SD, Paramsothy P. Menstruation and the menopausal transition. Obstet Gynecol Clin North Am. 2011;38(3):595–607. doi: 10.1016/j.ogc.2011.05.010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 75.Schmader T. Gender identification moderates stereotype threat effects on women’s math performance. J Exp Soc Psychol. 2002;38(2):194–201. [Google Scholar]
  • 76.McGlone MS, Aronson J. Stereotype threat, identity salience, and spatial reasoning. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology. 2006;27(5):486–493. [Google Scholar]
  • 77.Nguyen HD, Ryan AM. Does stereotype threat affect test performance of minorities and women? A meta-analysis of experimental evidence. J Appl Psychol. 2008;93(6):1314. doi: 10.1037/a0012702. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 78.Lewis NA, Sekaquaptewa D. Beyond test performance: A broader view of stereotype threat. Current Opinion in Psychology. 2016;11:40–43. [Google Scholar]
  • 79.Pennington CR, Heim D, Levy AR, Larkin DT. Twenty years of stereotype threat research: A review of psychological mediators. PloS One. 2016;11(1):e0146487. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0146487. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 80.Smith LV, Cokley K. Stereotype threat vulnerability: A psychometric investigation of the social identities and attitudes scale. Measurement and Evaluation in Counseling and Development. 2016;49(2):145–162. [Google Scholar]
  • 81.McQuillan J, Greil AL, Shreffler KM, Tichenor V. The importance of motherhood among women in the contemporary United States. Gender & Society. 2008;22:477–96. doi: 10.1177/0891243208319359. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 82.World Health Organization. Infertility: a tabulation of available data on prevalence of primary and secondary infertility. Geneva: World Health Organization; 1991. [Google Scholar]
  • 83.Smart L, Wegner DM. The hidden costs of hidden stigma. The social psychology of stigma. 2000:220–242. [Google Scholar]
  • 84.Quinn DM, Chaudoir SR. Living with a concealable stigmatized identity: The impact of anticipated stigma, centrality, salience, and cultural stigma on psychological distress and health. J Pers Soc Psychol. 2009;97(4):634. doi: 10.1037/a0015815. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 85.Goffman E. Stigma: Notes on a spoiled identity. In: Jenkins JH, editor. Carpenter. 1963. [Google Scholar]
  • 86.Quinn DM. Concealable versus conspicuous stigmatized identities. Stigma and group inequality: Social Psychological Perspectives. 2006:83–103. [Google Scholar]
  • 87.Beals KP, Peplau LA, Gable SL. Stigma management and well-being: The role of perceived social support, emotional processing, and suppression. Person Soc Psychol Bull. 2009;35(7):867–879. doi: 10.1177/0146167209334783. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 88.Quinn DM, Earnshaw VA. Understanding concealable stigmatized identities: The role of identity in psychological, physical, and behavioral outcomes. Social Issues and Policy Review. 2011;5(1):160–190. [Google Scholar]
  • 89.Quinn DM, Earnshaw VA. Concealable stigmatized identities and psychological well-being. Social and Personality Psychology Compass. 2013;7(1):40–51. doi: 10.1111/spc3.12005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 90**.Sun F, Sebastiani P, Schupf N, et al. Extended maternal age at birth of last child and women’s longevity in the Long Life Family Study. Menopause. 2015;22(1):26–31. doi: 10.1097/GME.0000000000000276. This study investigates the link between maternal age at last child’s birth and maternal survival. Based on a case-control study of data from the Long Life Family Study (LLFS), there was a significant association between older maternal age (last child >33years) and survival to the top 5th percentile of survival within ones birth cohort. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 91.Muller HG, Chiou JM, Carey JR, Wang JL. Fertility and life span: Late children enhance female longevity. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci. 2002;57(5):B202–6. doi: 10.1093/gerona/57.5.b202. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 92.Gagnon A, Smith KR, Tremblay M, Vézina H, Paré P, Desjardins B. Is there a trade-off between fertility and longevity? A comparative study of women from three large historical databases accounting for mortality selection. Am J Hum Biol. 2009;21(4):533–540. doi: 10.1002/ajhb.20893. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 93.Lewin T. Women’s health is no longer a man’s world. New York Times. 2001 Available from: http://www.nytimes.com/2001/02/07/us/women-s-health-is-no-longer-a-man-s-world.html.
  • 94.Tsugawa Y, Jena AB, Figueroa JF, Orav EJ, Blumenthal DM, Jha AK. Comparison of hospital mortality and readmission rates for Medicare patients treated by male vs female physicians. JAMA Internal Medicine. 2016 doi: 10.1001/jamainternmed.2016.7875. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 95.Dahrouge S, Seale E, Hogg W, et al. A comprehensive assessment of family physician gender and quality of care. Med Care. 2016;54(3):277–286. doi: 10.1097/MLR.0000000000000480. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 96.Fries-Britt S, Griffin K. The black box: How high-achieving Blacks resist stereotypes about Black Americans. Journal of College Student Development. 2007;48(5):509–524. [Google Scholar]
  • 97.Woodcock A, Hernandez PR, Schultz PW. Diversifying science: Intervention programs moderate the effect of stereotype threat on motivation and career choice. Social Psychological and Personality Science. 2016;7(2):184–192. doi: 10.1177/1948550615608401. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 98.Abdou CM, Amano T. Reducing health care stereotype threat. Anthem Website. https://www22.anthem.com/hcst/. Updated 2016. Accessed 3/01/2017.

RESOURCES