Abstract
Background
The dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC) is involved with allocating attentional resources to maintain postural control. However, it is unknown whether age‐related structural and functional declines of the DLPFC may impair postural control during sensory manipulation. In this study, we aim to understand the effects of aging on the DLPFC when sensory cues were removed or presented inaccurately (i.e., increased sensory complexity) during the sensory orientation test (SOT).
Methods
Twenty young (18–25 years) and 18 older (66–73 years) healthy adults were recruited to undertake the SOT, which consisted of six conditions aimed at removing or disrupting the visual, vestibular, and proprioceptive senses. During these six SOT conditions, functional near‐infrared spectroscopy (fNIRS), consisting of eight transmitter‐receiver optode pairs (four channels over the left and right DLPFC), was used to measure hemodynamic responses (i.e., changes in oxy‐ [O2Hb] and deoxyhemoglobin [HHb]) from the bilateral DLPFC.
Results
Our results show an increase in bilateral DLPFC activation (i.e., increase in O2Hb and concomitant smaller decrease in HHb) with increasing sensory complexity in both young and older adults. The increase in left and right DLPFC activation during more complex sensory conditions was greater, which was concomitant with reduced balance performance in older adults compared to younger adults. Furthermore, we observed a right lateralized DLPFC activation in younger adults. Finally, a significant positive association was observed between balance performance and increased bilateral DLPFC activation particularly for SOT conditions with greater sensory disruptions.
Conclusion
Our findings highlight the involvement of the DLPFC in maintaining postural control, particularly during complex sensory tasks, and provide direct evidence for the role of the DLPFC during postural control of a clinically relevant measure of balance.
Keywords: aging, neuroimaging, postural control, prefrontal cortex, sensory orientation test
1. INTRODUCTION
Human postural control is reliant on visual, vestibular, and proprioceptive senses to maintain a stable upright stance (Blumle, Maurer, Schweigart, & Mergner, 2006). In particular, aging is accompanied by a reduced capacity to respond to external perturbations during postural control, particularly when sensory cues are hindered or inaccurate (de Dieuleveult, Siemonsma, van Erp, & Brouwer, 2017). This is in part due to an age‐related decline in neuromuscular function (i.e., changes in muscle morphology and neuromuscular transmission; Clark et al., 2010; Gomes, Reis, Neves, Petrella, & de Abreu, 2013; Takacs, Carpenter, Garland, & Hunt, 2013) and reduced cognitive and visuospatial processing abilities (Horak, 2006; Mihara, Miyai, Hatakenaka, Kubota, & Sakoda, 2008). Accordingly, biomechanical analyses of static posturographic measures, such as those obtained from the sensory organization test (SOT) have provided strong evidence for a shift in sensory utilization during postural control with aging (Camicioli, Panzer, & Kaye, 1997; Cohen, Heaton, Congdon, & Jenkins, 1996).
While understanding the biomechanical changes during sensory manipulation provide some insight on how postural control alters with age, little is known about how the brain is involved in processing sensory cues to maintain upright posture. Utilizing a top‐down approach, Allali et al. (2014) used functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) to show age‐related differences in cortical activation patterns in the right orbitofrontal cortex and left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC) during an imagined locomotion task. However, the ecological validity of using an imagined task during fMRI is questionable as it only provides an implied measure of brain response during actual upright body movements or postural control. Recent systematic reviews have highlighted the ability to apply fNIRS as a neuroimaging technique to quantify the cortical control of static and dynamic forms of balance and gait (Herold, Wiegel, et al., 2017; Wittenberg, Thompson, Nam, & Franz, 2017). In this context, fNIRS may offer an advantageous alternative to fMRI due to reduced physical constraints and greater portability (Herold, Wiegel et al., 2017; Wittenberg et al., 2017). The concept of fNIRS as well as fMRI is based on the assumption that brain activity leads to an increase in regional cerebral blood flow due to neurovascular coupling mechanisms, which is reflected in the fNIRS hemodynamic response signal by an increase in oxyhemoglobin [O2Hb] and decrease in deoxyhemoglobin [HHb] concentration levels (Ferrari & Quaresima, 2012). Karim, Fuhrman, Sparto, Furman, and Huppert (2013) were the first to successfully use fNIRS to measure cortical hemodynamic responses from the frontal, temporal, and parietal regions of the brain in young healthy participants during the SOT. Their results showed bilateral activation in the temporal–parietal areas (i.e., superior temporal and supramarginal gyri) when both vision and proprioceptive information were degraded. Greater cortical activation in the primary sensorimotor cortex (pre‐ and postcentral gyri) and supplementary motor area during an unstable standing task (i.e., balance board) has also since been demonstrated in young healthy adults (Herold, Orlowski, Bormel, & Muller, 2017). Employing a similar fNIRS paradigm with the SOT as Karim et al. (2013), Lin, Barker, Sparto, Furman, and Huppert (2017) showed an increase in frontal‐parietal and occipital activation when vestibular and visual information were used predominantly during the SOT in older adults compared to middle‐aged adults. However, no differences in postural sway between middle‐aged and older adults were shown. Taken together, these studies provide strong evidence for an age‐related difference in frontal–parietal cortical activation during postural control that can be measured ecologically with fNIRS.
From a motor control perspective, the DLPFC has been implicated in postural control and balance (Fujita, Kasubuchi, Wakata, Hiyamizu, & Morioka, 2016; Mihara et al., 2008), such that the DLPFC is involved with integrating sensory cues and allocating attentional resources during postural control (Woollacott & Shumway‐Cook, 2002). Particularly related to aging, recent fNIRS studies have shown an age‐related reduction in effective (Huo et al., 2018) and functional connectivity (Wang et al., 2016) between the left and right DLPFC during postural changes between older and young adults. However, these studies did not employ clinical measures such as the SOT to manipulate the presentation of sensory cues. While preliminary data have shown differences in frontal cortical activation during the SOT between middle‐aged and older adults (Lin et al., 2017), age‐related differences in DLPFC activation between young and older adults relevant to the SOT have not been examined.
In this context, we investigated the age‐related differences in bilateral DLPFC activation during sensory manipulation of a balance task (SOT) in healthy young and older adults. Unlike the study by Lin et al. (2017) that used a frontal–parietal optode montage, we employed an eight‐channel optode montage to optimize coverage specific to the left and right DLPFC (LDLPFC and RDLPFC). We hypothesized that older adults would experience greater bilateral DLPFC activation during the SOT conditions where sensory cues are removed or perturbed. These findings will provide direct evidence for the role of the DLPFC during postural control of a clinically relevant measure of balance and enhance our understanding of the cortical mechanisms associated with balance in older adults.
2. METHODS
2.1. Participants
Twenty young (aged 18‐25 years, 10M/10F) and 18 older (aged 66‐73 years, 10M/8F) sedentary, otherwise healthy, adults were recruited. Participants were excluded if they (a) scored less than 25 on the Mini‐Mental State Examination (MMSE); (b) had a known vestibular impairment; (c) had a visual impairment not rectified by wearing a visual aid (e.g., glasses); (d) suffered from a lower‐limb musculoskeletal injury in the last 6 months; (e) had a previous diagnosis of acquired brain injury or stroke; (f) were on medication for any mood or neurological disorders; (g) suffer severe motion sickness; (h) prone to fainting; and (i) had a known diagnosis of any neurodevelopmental or neurodegenerative disorder. Written informed consent was obtained from all participants. All procedures were performed in accordance with the ethical standards of the Deakin University human research ethics committee (2016‐045) and the Declaration of Helsinki.
2.2. SOT setup and data processing
The Neurocom Balancemaster system (Neurocom International Inc, Oregon, USA) was used to SOT, which comprised of six different sensory conditions in quiet stance, and measured the center of pressure (COP) path in centimeters (cm). The test began with condition 1 (eyes open in a static stance), and sequentially moves through each condition which progressively becomes more challenging by either distracting or removing visual and/or proprioceptive feedback. The visual surround and force platform were sway‐referenced, which referred to the tilting of the support surface and/or visual surround in response to movement of the participant's COP. If a participant has poor anterior–posterior control over their COP, the force plate and visual surround will move correspondingly in an anterior–posterior orientation. During the sway‐referenced conditions, erroneous sensory information was presented in an attempt to disconcert the participant, while the force platform measured the participant's ability to compensate by using other senses to maintain balance equilibrium. Three 20‐s trials were conducted for each sensory condition with an intertrial rest period of 60 s. During each rest period, all participants were allowed to readjust their foot position if necessary and was asked to remain standing still upright for at least 30 s with their eyes opened and focused onto a fixation cross at eye‐level in front of them. The sensory conditions included the following:
Condition 1 (COND1)—Eyes open, unmoving surface, and visual surround;
Condition 2 (COND2)—Eyes closed, unmoving surface;
Condition 3 (COND3)—Eyes open, unmoving surface, and sway‐referenced visual surround;
Condition 4 (COND4)—Eyes open, sway‐referenced surface, and fixed visual surround;
Condition 5 (COND5)—Eyes closed, sway‐referenced surface;
Condition 6 (COND6)—Eyes open, sway‐referenced surface, and visual surround.
Following each trial, an equilibrium score was computed, which quantified postural stability during each trial of the six sensory conditions. The equilibrium score compared the participant's anterior–posterior sway during each trial to a theoretical sway stability limit of 12.5 degrees. A participant swaying to the limits of stability will receive a low score, while a score closer to 100 indicates good stability and minimum sway. To quantify postural control for each sensory condition, the averaged equilibrium score across three trials within each sensory condition was used.
2.3. FNIRS setup, data collection, and processing
The fundamental basis for fNIRS is that most biological tissue such as skin and bone are transparent to NIR light, while O2Hb and HHb are better absorbers of NIR light in the spectrum of 700–900 nm (Isac & Matthes, 1938; Isac, Matthes, & Yamanaka, 1938; Matthes, 1938; Severinghaus, 2007). The principle of fNIRS is based on the absorption rate of O2Hb and HHb using the modified Beer–Lambert law (MBLL) (Delpy et al., 1988) at two different wavelength of NIR light between transmitter (Tx) and receiver (Rx) pairs placed over the scalp. Therefore, brain maps can be developed by spatially arranging the NIR light Tx–Rx pairs on the head so that specific brain region(s) of interest can be determined (Boas, Dale, & Franceschini, 2004). In this study, an eight‐channel continuous‐wave NIRS system (Oxymon Mk III, Artinis Medical Systems, The Netherlands) was used to measure fNIRS signals using two wavelengths at 750 and 850 nm during the six sensory conditions of the SOT. Hemodynamic responses were measured from the bilateral DLPFC using four pairs of Tx and Rx over the forehead region (Figure 1). Each receiver measures time‐multiplexed NIR light from two transmitter sources located at a distance of 3 cm, resulting in a total of eight channels (four channels on the left and right DLPFC) of O2Hb and HHb. For each participant, an individual differential path‐length factor (DPF) was used to account for age differences according to Scholkmann and Wolf (2013). All signals were sampled at a rate of 10 Hz. Figure 1 shows the placement of the Tx and Rx over the forehead. To identify the functional brain region of interest in which the fNIRS optodes are measuring from, we employed a MATLAB‐based toolbox (fOLD—fNIRS optode location decider) as reported by Zimeo Morais, Balardin, and Sato (2018). The fOLD toolbox provides an accurate estimate of spatial brain mapping of electroencephalography (EEG) 10–20 systems on associated Brodmann areas (Rorden & Brett, 2000). Based on this method, we have identified the LDLPFC and RDLPFC as the regions of interest in this study.
Measures of O2Hb and HHb from all eight channels were exported to and processed using HOMER2, a MATLAB‐based toolbox (MATLAB 2015b, The MathWorks Inc, Massachusetts, USA) to analyze fNIRS data (Huppert, Diamond, Franceschini, & Boas, 2009). As described by Brigadoi et al. (2014), all raw data were first converted to changes in optical density (OD). Channels with low signal‐to‐noise ratio were removed using the enPruneChannels function. As all channels displayed excellent signal‐to‐noise ratio, none of the channels were excluded from the analysis. A motion artifact detection algorithm (hmrMotionArtifactByChannel; AMPThresh = 0.40, SDThresh = 5.0, tMotion = 0.5, and tMask = 1.0) was then used to identify motion artifacts on a channel‐by‐channel basis that exceed the prespecified thresholds in the OD time series. After motion artifact identification was performed, a motion artifact correction method using the principal component analysis (PCA) filter was applied to any active channel that exhibited a signal change greater than the prespecified thresholds (hmrMotionArtifactPCArecurse; AMPThresh = 0.40, SDThresh = 5.0, tMotion = 0.5, tMask = 1.0, nSV = 0.97, and maxlter = 5). A bandpass filter (hmrBandpassFilt; high‐pass = 0.01 and low‐pass = 0.50) was then applied to filter out low‐ or high‐frequency noise. The OD data were then converted into concentration changes using the MBLL (Delpy et al., 1988) using the hmrOD2Conc function (ppf = 6.0, 6.0) and block averaged (hmrBlockAvg; trange = −5.0, 25.0) to provide an indication of average change of three trials for all eight channels during each sensory condition. A figure of the data processing pipeline may be found in the Supporting Information Appendix S1 (Supporting Information Figure S1). The change in hemodynamic response for the LDLPFC (four channels) and RDLPFC (four channels) was averaged to determine hemispheric differences in cortical activation between young and older adults. Additionally to determine hemispheric lateralization, a laterality index of O2Hb and HHb, as described by Seghier (2008) for fMRI studies, was calculated for all conditions in young and older adults. The formula used to calculate the laterality index is as follows:
Based on the formula above, negative values up to −1 indicated RDLPFC dominance, while positive values up to +1 indicated LDLPFC dominance. An overall average of all eight channels was also calculated to provide an overall measure of DLPFC activation during each sensory condition. While O2Hb measures are typically used to represent cerebral activation, it is also sensitive to systemic changes in blood pressure, skin blood flow, and heart rate compared to HHb (Kirilina et al., 2012). For this reason, we considered changes in both O2Hb and HHb as true indicators of cerebral activation.
2.4. Experimental design
Participants attended one testing session lasting approximately 90 minutes at the Clinical Exercise Learning Centre, Deakin University. Prior to the testing session, individual demographical characteristics such as height (kg), weight (cm), education status (years of formal education), and the MMSE was taken. An fNIRS headcap was fitted, and the participants were harnessed onto the Balancemaster system. The harness was tightened to a point that prevented falling but did not restrict anterior–posterior movement. To prevent any practice effect that may influence balance and hemodynamic responses, only one practice trial for each sensory condition was performed. Upon completion of the practice trials, the testing session commenced with all sensory conditions sequentially performed. To exclude the potential for an order effect on hemodynamic response, a subsample of five young participants repeated the test on a separate day with the order of sensory condition presented randomly.
2.5. Statistical analysis
Data were screened for normality using histograms and Shapiro–Wilks test. Independent t tests were used to compare demographics between young and older adults. A repeated‐measures analysis of covariance (ANCOVA) was used to examine the effect of age (between group) and sensory condition (within group), on equilibrium score, changes in O2Hb and HHb and laterality index, controlling for education as a covariate. For changes in O2Hb and HHb, separate ANCOVAs were conducted for left and right hemispheres. Greenhouse–Geisser correction was applied if the assumption of sphericity was violated. Where significant interactions were found, Bonferroni adjustment was applied to post hoc pairwise comparisons. Linear regressions were used to determine whether the change in overall change in O2Hb of the PFC was associated with balance, as indexed by the equilibrium score at each sensory condition for both age‐groups. Regression models were additionally adjusted for education level and MMSE score. Fisher's r‐to‐z transformation compared whether the association between O2Hb and equilibrium score differed between young and older adults. Cohen's d effect sizes were compared between the means of the subsample and main study results for both O2Hb and HHb (see Supporting Information Figure S2 and Table S1 for results). Alpha level of p < 0.05 was used to determine the level of significance. Analyses were conducted using SPSS Statistics V.22.0 (IBM Corp, NY, USA).
3. RESULTS
3.1. Participant characteristics
Table 1 displays differences in demographics between the young and older adults. The participants in the young group had a higher level of education than those in the older group. No differences in weight, height, or MMSE scores were observed.
Table 1.
Young (n = 20) | Older (n = 18) | T test p‐values | |
---|---|---|---|
Gender (M/F) | 12/8 | 10/8 | – |
Age (years) | 21.5 ± 3.5 | 69.5 ± 3.4 | <0.001 |
Height (cm) | 176.3 ± 12.6 | 170.6 ± 11.5 | 0.75 |
Weight (kg) | 74.7 ± 17.4 | 77.4 ± 15.7 | 0.87 |
MMSE score (/30) | 29.5 ± 0.9 | 27.7 ± 1.0 | 0.66 |
Education status (years) | 15.7 ± 2.3 | 10.4 ± 3.1 | 0.04 |
Boldface indicates between‐group significances of p<0.05.
3.2. Balance performance (equilibrium score)
All equilibrium scores for young and older adults are shown in Table 2. There was an effect for CONDITION (F 2.60,91.14 = 1062.48, p < 0.001) and CONDITION x AGE interaction (F 3.88,135.68 = 129.67, p < 0.001). Compared to condition 1 (eyes open, unmoving surface, and visual surround) both younger and older adults showed decreases in equilibrium scores for conditions 2 through to 6 (eyes closed conditions and those using the sway‐referenced surface; all p < 0.001). For conditions 3–6 (sway‐referenced surface), older adults showed a greater decrease (p < 0.001) in equilibrium scores when compared to the younger group.
Table 2.
Condition | Equilibrium scores | Between‐group p‐values | |
---|---|---|---|
Young | Old | ||
1 | 96.00 ± 1.62 | 94.56 ± 2.23 | 0.027 |
2 | 94.30 ± 1.34 | 93.89 ± 2.00 | 0.457 |
3 | 94.05 ± 1.32 | 89.56 ± 2.36 | <0.001 |
4 | 84.60 ± 1.70 | 80.89 ± 1.60 | <0.001 |
5 | 69.90 ± 2.75 | 62.17 ± 1.65 | <0.001 |
6 | 65.70 ± 2.18 | 58.44 ± 2.55 | <0.001 |
3.3. Changes in LDLPFC and RDLPFC between sensory conditions
Figure 2a,b shows the changes in O2Hb and HHb in the LDLPFC and RDLPFC respectively during each of the six SOT sensory conditions for younger and older adults. For the LDLPFC, a main effect for CONDITION and an AGE x CONDITION interaction was observed in both O2Hb (CONDITION = F 3.58,125.23 = 60.33, p < 0.001; AGE x CONDITION = F 3.58,125.23 = 25.35, p < 0.001) and HHb (CONDITION = F 3.58,125.23 = 15.55, p < 0.001; AGE x CONDITION = F 3.58,125.23 = 12.45, p < 0.001). No significant difference was observed for condition 1 between younger and older adults (p = 0.154); however, from conditions 2 to 6, older adults had a significantly greater increase (p < 0.001) in O2Hb compared to younger adults and compared to condition 1. Additionally, a significantly greater increase (p < 0.001) in O2Hb was observed in younger adults from conditions 4 to 6 compared to condition 1. For HHb, no significant differences were observed between conditions 1 to 3 and between age‐groups. However, from conditions 4 to 6, both younger and older adults had a significant reduction in HHb compared to condition 1 (all p < 0.001) and between each other (p < 0.001).
An effect for CONDITION and AGE x CONDITION interaction was observed in the RDLPFC for O2Hb (CONDITION = F 3.58,125.23 = 66.64, p < 0.001; AGE x CONDITION = F 3.58,125.23 = 10.54, p < 0.001) and HHb (CONDITION = F 3.58,125.23 = 14.54, p < 0.001; AGE x CONDITION = F 3.58,125.23 = 11.58, p < 0.001). In the RDLPFC, no significant difference was found in condition 1 (p = 0.177) and condition 6 (p = 0.105) between younger and older adults. However, from conditions 2 to 5, a significantly greater increase (p < 0.001) in O2Hb was observed in older compared to younger adults. Compared to condition 1, a greater increase (p < 0.001) in O2Hb was observed from conditions 2 to 6, while younger adults showed a significant increase (p < 0.001) in O2Hb on from conditions 4 to 6. For HHb, significant between‐group differences were observed only from conditions 4 to 6, with older adults demonstrating a greater reduction (p < 0.01) in HHb compared to younger adults. Compared to condition 1, a significant reduction (p < 0.01) in HHb was observed in younger and older adults from conditions 4 to 6, while conditions 2 and 3 were not significantly different from condition 1 in both groups.
3.4. Changes in laterality index between conditions
Figure 2c shows the laterality index for O2Hb and HHb between sensory conditions. An effect of CONDITION and AGE x CONDITION interaction was found in O2Hb (CONDITION = F 3.58,125.23 = 35.67, p < 0.001; AGE x CONDITION = F 3.58,125.23 = 21.33, p < 0.001) and HHb (CONDITION = F 3.58,125.23 = 21.45, p < 0.001; AGE x CONDITION = F 3.58,125.23 = 12.54, p < 0.001). In older adults, the laterality index for O2Hb and HHb showed no differences across all conditions. However, in younger adults, the laterality index for O2Hb in conditions 2–6 were significantly lower (i.e., toward −1, indicating RDLPFC lateralization) compared to condition 1 (p < 0.001), and compared to older adults (all p < 0.001). For HHb, within‐group comparisons showed that the laterality index in younger adults was lower (indicating a greater RDLPFC lateralization) from conditions 3 to 6 compared to condition 1 (p < 0.001), while between‐group comparisons showed a significant RDLPFC lateralization effect from conditions 4 to 6 when compared to older adults (p < 0.001).
3.5. Changes in overall DLPFC activation between conditions
Figure 2d shows the overall change in O2Hb and HHb of the DLPFC between sensory conditions. For overall O2Hb, there was an effect for condition (F 3.58,125.23 = 62.36, p < 0.001) and condition by age interaction (F 3.58,125.23 = 20.93, p < 0.001). During condition 1, no differences between O2Hb levels in younger and older adult were observed (p = 0.160); however, older adults had greater changes in O2Hb levels during conditions 2 through to 6 (p < 0.001) compared to young adults. Compared to sensory condition 1, older adults had an increase in O2Hb for all conditions (p < 0.001), whereas young adults had increases in conditions 3– 6 (all p < 0.001).
For overall HHb, there was a condition (F 3.88,135.68 = 4.76, p = 0.001) and condition by age interaction (F 3.88,135.68 = 5.89, p < 0.001). There were no differences in change in HHb between older adults in condition 1 (p = 0.665); however, older adults had a greater decrease in HHb in conditions 2 through to 6. Compared with condition 1, no changes in HHb for condition 2 and 3 were observed for young adults (p = 0.999). Younger adults had a decrease in HHb levels in conditions 4, 5, and 6 (p < 0.001), whereas older adults had decreases in HHb for all conditions (p < 0.001). Overall, no difference in overall O2Hb and HHb was observed between the findings from the subsample study and main study (see Supporting Information Appendix S1).
3.6. The relationship between changes in O2Hb, HHb and balance performance
Figure 3 shows the relationship between overall change in O 2 Hb and balance performance. For sensory conditions 1 and 2, there was no association between the change in overall change in O2Hb levels and equilibrium scores (Table 3). For sensory conditions 3 through to 6, a greater increase in O2Hb levels was associated with higher equilibrium scores for both younger and adults, with no differences in this relationship between groups. Additional adjustment for education status and MMSE score did not materially affect these results. Further, no associations between HHb and equilibrium scores (Table 4) were observed in all six sensory conditions.
Table 3.
Condition | β (95%CI) | R 2 | p | β (95%CI) | R 2 | p | Fisher's r to z | p |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Young | Old | |||||||
1 | 10.16 (−7.49 to 27.80) | 0.08 | 0.24 | 9.55 (−7.68 to 26.77) | 0.08 | 0.26 | −0.02 | 0.98 |
2 | 10.40 (−1.02 to 21.81) | 0.17 | 0.07 | 6.31 (−2.05 to 14.67) | 0.14 | 0.13 | 0.13 | 0.90 |
3 | 9.55 (2.22 to 16.89) | 0.29 | 0.01 | 9.61 (1.73 to 17.49) | 0.29 | 0.02 | 0.91 | 0.36 |
4 | 6.37 (1.66 to 11.09) | 0.31 | 0.01 | 10.48 (4.76 to 16.19) | 0.49 | 0.001 | −0.66 | 0.51 |
5 | 8.85 (4.72 to 12.99) | 0.53 | <0.001 | 10.79 (6.41 to 15.16) | 0.63 | <0.001 | −0.45 | 0.65 |
6 | 7.30 (3.91 to 10.69) | 0.53 | <0.001 | 10.84 (5.77 to 15.92) | 0.56 | <0.001 | −0.13 | 0.90 |
Crude model.
Results adjusted for education and MMSE were not materially different.
Values in boldface denote significance p < 0.05.
Table 4.
Condition | β (95%CI) | R 2 | p | β (95%CI) | R 2 | p |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Young | Old | |||||
1 | −2.45 (−34.05 to 29.16) | 0.002 | 0.873 | −9.41 (−52.67 to 33.83) | 0.013 | 0.651 |
2 | −2.22 (−26.3 to 21.87) | 0.002 | 0.849 | 19.08 (−19.86 to 58.02) | 0.063 | 0.314 |
3 | 8.85 (−14.05 to 31.76) | 0.035 | 0.427 | 23.21 (−18.04 to 64.47) | 0.082 | 0.250 |
4 | −5.21 (−28.26 to 17.85) | 0.012 | 0.641 | −2.41 (−21.49 to 16.66) | 0.005 | 0.792 |
5 | −19.03 (−68.18 to 30.11) | 0.036 | 0.426 | −3.64 (−20.34 to 13.05) | 0.013 | 0.650 |
6 | 6.28 (−26.69 to 40.34) | 0.010 | 0.674 | 18.34 (−2.16 to 38.38) | 0.184 | 0.076 |
Crude model.
Results adjusted for education and MMSE were not materially different.
4. DISCUSSION
This study compared the age‐related changes in hemodynamic response of the bilateral DLPFC during the SOT. Our main findings showed that in both age‐groups, an increase in bilateral DLPFC activation was associated with better balance performance as the level of sensory complexity increased. This was characterized by an increase in bilateral DLPFC O2Hb in both age‐groups for all eyes closed conditions and those using the sway‐referenced surface, which was greater in older compared to younger adults. Furthermore, under sensory conditions with increasing complexity, older adults exhibited poorer balance compared to the younger age‐group. Additionally when comparing the left and right DLPFC in both groups, older adults showed greater bilateral activation of the DLPFC during more complex sensory tasks compared to younger adults. This may indicate a greater allocation of cognitive resources in older adults that is compensatory in nature, to maintain balance performance.
In line with previous studies, age‐related increases in postural sway have been reported under conditions that disrupt visual or somatosensory cues (Faraldo‐Garcia, Santos‐Perez, Crujeiras, & Soto‐Varela, 2016; Liaw, Chen, Pei, Leong, & Lau, 2009). In particular, a study by Camicioli et al. (1997) showed that older adults were more impaired with the presence of inaccurate proprioceptive information (while maintaining accurate visual cues), suggesting an increased dependence on proprioceptive cues over visual cues with age. Their results were supported by a recent study by Wiesmeier, Dalin, and Maurer (2015), which suggested that the greater reliance of proprioceptive cues with age may represent some form of compensation strategy in older adults. In our study, we showed in condition 1 (eyes open) that older adults had lower scores compared to young adults; however, in condition 2 (eyes closed), no difference in balance performance were observed. This paradoxical finding was indeed surprising as we initially hypothesized a reduction in balance performance with increasing sensory complexity. However, we further observed that the increase in bilateral DLPFC O2Hb was significantly greater during condition 2 compared to condition 1 in older adults, and this increase in DLPFC activity, perhaps as a compensation strategy, may in part explain the attenuation in balance performance declines in condition 2 for older adults. However, in all other conditions (3–6) where either inaccurate proprioceptive and/or vestibular cues were presented, balance performance was significantly impaired in older adults. Indeed, a recent systematic review by de Dieuleveult et al. (2017) indicated that older adults relied more on multisensory information in order to accurately perform activities of daily living. However, in tasks that require increased cognitive loading (i.e., dual‐tasking or distractions), or when inaccurate information was presented, task performance in older adults was greatly impacted. Taken in the context of a balance task, poorer balance performance with increasing complexity of sensory disruption is likely to be indicative of an age‐related deficit in central integration of multiple sensory inputs (Camicioli et al., 1997).
The main finding from this study demonstrated that compared to younger adults, older adults had greater bilateral DLPFC activation (increase in O2Hb and decrease in HHb) particularly during more complex balance tasks (see Figure 2a,b,d), while younger adults showed greater lateralization to the RDLPFC with increased sensory demands. These findings are in general agreement with previous fNIRS studies (Karim et al., 2013; Lin et al., 2017; Mihara et al., 2008) demonstrating that the PFC and temporal‐parietal regions were engaged during active balancing, which was thought to be involved with allocation of attentional demands in standing postural control. While our data showed that level of bilateral DLPFC activation increased with sensory complexity, it was also observed that the magnitude of bilateral DLPFC activation was greater for the eyes closed conditions and those using the sway‐referenced surface in older adults. In other neuroimaging studies, increased cortical activity with aging was commonly reported. For example, a study by Zwergal et al. (2012) using fMRI showed an age‐related increase in cortical activity of the vestibular and visual cortices during imaginary gait. Similarly, Lin et al. (2017) recently demonstrated an age‐related increase in the frontal–lateral and occipital regions during an abbreviated version of the SOT in middle‐aged and older adults; however, they reported no differences in balance performance between age‐groups. In our study, we observed a greater increase in bilateral DLPFC activation for older adults, which was concomitant with the complexity of the balance task in conditions 3–6. In condition 2, the equilibrium score in both young and older adults was similar; however, the increase in bilateral DLPFC activation was significantly greater in older adults. From a motor control perspective, the DLPFC has been implicated in planning of action motor sequences and allocating attentional resources (Kaller, Rahm, Spreer, Weiller, & Unterrainer, 2011; Unterrainer & Owen, 2006). Therefore, the greater increase in bilateral DLPFC activation with greater sensory demands during the balance tasks in older compared to younger adults may be indicative of greater reliance on executive function, indirect locomotor pathways (Hamacher, Herold, Wiegel, Hamacher, & Schega, 2015; Herold, Wiegel et al., 2017; Wittenberg et al., 2017), and reduced automaticity in balance control (Paul, Ada, & Canning, 2013). The indirect locomotor pathway forms part of the corticobasal ganglia network that is involved with preventing unwanted muscle contractions from competing with voluntary movements (Nambu, 2004).
While we observed a greater increase in bilateral DLPFC activation in older adults that was associated with increased sensory demands, the greater right lateralized effect of DLPFC activation in younger adults suggests a task‐specific reliance of the RDLPFC during postural control. In young adults, the inhibition of unwanted movements may have involved a greater involvement of right‐hemisphere brain regions such as the right inferior and superior frontal gyri that are functionally responsible for response inhibition, attention control, and spatial self‐awareness (Goldberg, Herel, & Malach, 2006; Hampshire, Chamberlain, Monti, Duncan, & Owen, 2010). Conversely in older adults, we showed a lower right lateralized effect and by an overall greater increase in bilateral activation of the DLPFC. This reduction in hemispheric asymmetry has previously been proposed by Cabeza (2002) in the hemispheric asymmetry reduction in older adult (HAROLD) model, which suggests that the shift from a lateralized to more symmetrical pattern of DLPFC activation may be compensatory in nature, where greater allocation of resources is required to perform a cognitive task as we age (Grady, McIntosh, & Craik, 2005; Rajah, Languay, & Valiquette, 2010). Additionally, there are further suggestions that the right hemisphere, as depicted by the right hemi‐aging model (Dolcos, Rice, & Cabeza, 2002), may be more susceptible to age‐related declines in cognitive processing, which may help to explain this shift toward an increase in bilateral DLPFC activation in older adults. Taken together, our results in older adults suggest a pattern of overactivity of the bilateral DLPFC that may be a result of greater attentional and resource allocation requirements, and inefficiencies in RDLPFC processing of sensory information.
While the magnitude of bilateral DLPFC activation was associated with balance performance in both young and older adults, several limitations have to be acknowledged. We were unable to measure hemodynamic changes in other brain regions such as the occipital, parietal, and temporal areas that have previously been suggested to be implicated in postural control (Karim et al., 2013; Lin et al., 2017). Certainly, increased activation in sensory brain regions during active balance could potentially result in increases in PFC activation to consolidate information from sensory regions. Compared to previous fNIRS studies (Karim et al., 2013; Lin et al., 2017), our study also used a lower number of fNIRS channels. However, we chose to specifically focus all channels solely on the DLPFC and were able to detect significant age‐related changes in hemodynamic responses during the SOT. A further limitation of this study was the omission of short‐separation channels to account for extracerebral blood flow or to exclude any systemic changes that may have occurred. However, since O2Hb signals are more affected by extracerebral/systemic physiological changes than HHb (Kirilina et al., 2012), and since O2Hb changes were corroborated by HHb changes, we are confident that the differences in DLPFC activation derived from both O2Hb and HHb changes are a true representation of cortical activation.
Our study provided evidence for a strong link between the functioning of the DLPFC and postural control, particularly during complex sensory conditions. Our results also showed that bilateral DLPFC activation during postural control increased as a function of aging, which is likely to be indicative of increased attentional resources being used to maintain balance. Taken together, increased bilateral DLPFC activation may act to compensate for balance performance decrements that occur due to both aging and task difficulty. It remains to be seen whether targeted interventions such as exercise or neurocognitive training may lead to improved DLPFC functioning and whether such changes may elicit performance gains in balance performance in older adults.
CONFLICT OF INTERESTS
The authors declare no conflict of interests.
Supporting information
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
The authors would like to thank Ms Jessica Freeman for her assistance in data collection and Dr Jorn Horschig and Ms Saskia Kruitwagen for their assistance with HOMER2. Dr Severine Lamon is thanked for her financial contributions to the publication fees.
Teo W‐P, Goodwill AM, Hendy AM, Muthalib M, Macpherson H. Sensory manipulation results in increased dorsolateral prefrontal cortex activation during static postural balance in sedentary older adults: An fNIRS study. Brain Behav. 2018;8:e01109 10.1002/brb3.1109
Funding information
WPT was supported by an Alfred Deakin Postdoctoral Research Fellowship during the conduct of this study. MM is support by Silverline Research Services. HM is supported by an NHMRC‐ARC Dementia Fellowship.
REFERENCES
- Allali, G. , van der Meulen, M. , Beauchet, O. , Rieger, S. W. , Vuilleumier, P. , & Assal, F. (2014). The neural basis of age‐related changes in motor imagery of gait: An fMRI study. Journals of Gerontology. Series A, Biological Sciences and Medical Sciences, 69(11), 1389–1398. 10.1093/gerona/glt207 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Blumle, A. , Maurer, C. , Schweigart, G. , & Mergner, T. (2006). A cognitive intersensory interaction mechanism in human postural control. Experimental Brain Research, 173(3), 357–363. 10.1007/s00221-006-0384-z [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Boas, D. A. , Dale, A. M. , & Franceschini, M. A. (2004). Diffuse optical imaging of brain activation: Approaches to optimizing image sensitivity, resolution, and accuracy. NeuroImage, 23(Suppl 1), S275–S288. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.07.011 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brigadoi, S. , Ceccherini, L. , Cutini, S. , Scarpa, F. , Scatturin, P. , Selb, J. , & Cooper, R. J. (2014). Motion artifacts in functional near‐infrared spectroscopy: A comparison of motion correction techniques applied to real cognitive data. NeuroImage, 85(Pt 1), 181–191. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.04.082 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cabeza, R. (2002). Hemispheric asymmetry reduction in older adults: The HAROLD model. Psychology and Aging, 17(1), 85–100. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Camicioli, R. , Panzer, V. P. , & Kaye, J. (1997). Balance in the healthy elderly: Posturography and clinical assessment. Archives of Neurology, 54(8), 976–981. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Clark, D. J. , Patten, C. , Reid, K. F. , Carabello, R. J. , Phillips, E. M. , & Fielding, R. A. (2010). Impaired voluntary neuromuscular activation limits muscle power in mobility‐limited older adults. Journals of Gerontology. Series A, Biological Sciences and Medical Sciences, 65(5), 495–502. 10.1093/gerona/glq012 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cohen, H. , Heaton, L. G. , Congdon, S. L. , & Jenkins, H. A. (1996). Changes in sensory organization test scores with age. Age and Ageing, 25(1), 39–44. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- de Dieuleveult, A. L. , Siemonsma, P. C. , van Erp, J. B. , & Brouwer, A. M. (2017). Effects of aging in multisensory integration: A systematic review. Frontiers in Aging Neuroscience, 9, 80 10.3389/fnagi.2017.00080 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Delpy, D. T. , Cope, M. , Vanderzee, P. , Arridge, S. , Wray, S. , & Wyatt, J. (1988). Estimation of Optical Pathlength through Tissue from Direct Time of Flight Measurement. Physics in Medicine and Biology, 33(12), 1433–1442. https://doi.org/Doi/10.1088/0031-9155/33/12/008 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dolcos, F. , Rice, H. J. , & Cabeza, R. (2002). Hemispheric asymmetry and aging: Right hemisphere decline or asymmetry reduction. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 26(7), 819–825. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Faraldo‐Garcia, A. , Santos‐Perez, S. , Crujeiras, R. , & Soto‐Varela, A. (2016). Postural changes associated with ageing on the sensory organization test and the limits of stability in healthy subjects. Auris, Nasus, Larynx, 43(2), 149–154. 10.1016/j.anl.2015.07.001 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ferrari, M. , & Quaresima, V. (2012). A brief review on the history of human functional near‐infrared spectroscopy (fNIRS) development and fields of application. NeuroImage, 63(2), 921–935. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.03.049 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fujita, H. , Kasubuchi, K. , Wakata, S. , Hiyamizu, M. , & Morioka, S. (2016). Role of the frontal cortex in standing postural sway tasks while dual‐tasking: A functional near‐infrared spectroscopy study examining working memory capacity. Biomed Research International, 2016, 7053867 10.1155/2016/7053867 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Goldberg, I. , Herel, M. , & Malach, R. (2006). When the brain loses its self: Prefrontal inactivation during sensorimotor processing. Neuron, 50(2), 329–339. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gomes, M. M. , Reis, J. G. , Neves, T. M. , Petrella, M. , & de Abreu, D. C. C. (2013). Impact of aging on balance and pattern of muscle activation in elderly women from different age groups. International Journal of Gerontology, 7(2), 106–111. 10.1016/j.ijge.2012.11.013 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Grady, C. L. , McIntosh, A. R. , & Craik, F. I. (2005). Task‐related activity in prefrontal cortex and its relation to recognition memory performance in young and old adults. Neuropsychologia, 43(10), 1466–1481. 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2004.12.016 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hamacher, D. , Herold, F. , Wiegel, P. , Hamacher, D. , & Schega, L. (2015). Brain activity during walking: A systematic review. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 57, 310–327. 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2015.08.002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hampshire, A. , Chamberlain, S. R. , Monti, M. M. , Duncan, J. , & Owen, A. M. (2010). The role of the right inferior frontal gyrus: Inhibition and attentional control. NeuroImage, 50(3), 1313–1319. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.12.109 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Herold, F. , Orlowski, K. , Bormel, S. , & Muller, N. G. (2017). Cortical activation during balancing on a balance board. Human Movement Science, 51, 51–58. 10.1016/j.humov.2016.11.002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Herold, F. , Wiegel, P. , Scholkmann, F. , Thiers, A. , Hamacher, D. , & Schega, L. (2017). Functional near‐infrared spectroscopy in movement science: A systematic review on cortical activity in postural and walking tasks. Neurophotonics, 4(4), 041403 10.1117/1.NPh.4.4.041403 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Horak, F. B. (2006). Postural orientation and equilibrium: What do we need to know about neural control of balance to prevent falls? Age and Ageing, 35, i7–ii11. 10.1093/ageing/afl077 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Huo, C. C. , Zhang, M. , Bu, L. G. , Xu, G. C. , Liu, Y. , Li, Z. Y. , & Sun, L. L. (2018). Effective connectivity in response to posture changes in elderly subjects as assessed using functional near‐infrared spectroscopy. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 12, 98 10.3389/fnhum.2018.00098 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Huppert, T. J. , Diamond, S. G. , Franceschini, M. A. , & Boas, D. A. (2009). HomER: A review of time‐series analysis methods for near‐infrared spectroscopy of the brain. Applied Optics, 48(10), D280–D298. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Isac, C. , & Matthes, K. (1938). Analysis of the transportation of oxygen in human blood. I Memorandum – On the oxygen transportation in physical exercise. Naunyn‐Schmiedebergs Archiv Fur Experimentelle Pathologie Und Pharmakologie, 189, 606–614. 10.1007/Bf01860278 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Isac, C. , Matthes, K. , & Yamanaka, T. (1938). Analysis of the transportation of oxygen in human blood. 2 Memorandum ‐ On the oxygen transportation by blood in different illnesses. Naunyn‐Schmiedebergs Archiv Fur Experimentelle Pathologie Und Pharmakologie, 189, 615–627. 10.1007/Bf01860279 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Kaller, C. P. , Rahm, B. , Spreer, J. , Weiller, C. , & Unterrainer, J. M. (2011). Dissociable contributions of left and right dorsolateral prefrontal cortex in planning. Cerebral Cortex, 21(2), 307–317. 10.1093/cercor/bhq096 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Karim, H. , Fuhrman, S. I. , Sparto, P. , Furman, J. , & Huppert, T. (2013). Functional brain imaging of multi‐sensory vestibular processing during computerized dynamic posturography using near‐infrared spectroscopy. NeuroImage, 74, 318–325. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.02.010 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kirilina, E. , Jelzow, A. , Heine, A. , Niessing, M. , Wabnitz, H. , Bruhl, R. , & Tachtsidis, I. (2012). The physiological origin of task‐evoked systemic artefacts in functional near infrared spectroscopy. NeuroImage, 61(1), 70–81. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.02.074 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Liaw, M. Y. , Chen, C. L. , Pei, Y. C. , Leong, C. P. , & Lau, Y. C. (2009). Comparison of the static and dynamic balance performance in young, middle‐aged, and elderly healthy people. Chang Gung Medical Journal, 32(3), 297–304. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lin, C. C. , Barker, J. W. , Sparto, P. J. , Furman, J. M. , & Huppert, T. J. (2017). Functional near‐infrared spectroscopy (fNIRS) brain imaging of multi‐sensory integration during computerized dynamic posturography in middle‐aged and older adults. Experimental Brain Research, 235(4), 1247–1256. 10.1007/s00221-017-4893-8 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Matthes, K. (1938). On the uneven composition of the alveolar air. (Remarks on the work of the same title by M N J Dirken). Naunyn‐Schmiedebergs Archiv Fur Experimentelle Pathologie Und Pharmakologie, 189, 22–24. 10.1007/Bf01861807 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Mihara, M. , Miyai, I. , Hatakenaka, M. , Kubota, K. , & Sakoda, S. (2008). Role of the prefrontal cortex in human balance control. NeuroImage, 43(2), 329–336. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.07.029 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nambu, A. (2004). A new dynamic model of the cortico‐basal ganglia loop. Progress in Brain Research, 143, 461–466. 10.1016/S0079-6123(03)43043-4 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Paul, S. S. , Ada, L. , & Canning, C. G. (2013). Automaticity of walking – implications for physiotherapy practice. Physical Therapy Reviews, 10(1), 15–23. 10.1179/108331905X43463 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Rajah, M. N. , Languay, R. , & Valiquette, L. (2010). Age‐related changes in prefrontal cortex activity are associated with behavioural deficits in both temporal and spatial context memory retrieval in older adults. Cortex, 46(4), 535–549. 10.1016/j.cortex.2009.07.006 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rorden, C. , & Brett, M. (2000). Stereotaxic display of brain lesions. Behavioural Neurology, 12(4), 191–200. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Scholkmann, F. , & Wolf, M. (2013). General equation for the differential pathlength factor of the frontal human head depending on wavelength and age. Journal of Biomedical Optics, 18(10), 105004 10.1117/1.jbo.18.10.105004 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Seghier, M. L. (2008). Laterality index in functional MRI: Methodological issues. Magnetic Resonance Imaging, 26(5), 594–601. 10.1016/j.mri.2007.10.010 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Severinghaus, J. W. (2007). Takuo Aoyagi: Discovery of pulse oximetry. Anesthesia and Analgesia, 105(6 Suppl), S1–S4, tables of contents. 10.1213/01.ane.0000269514.31660.09 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Takacs, J. , Carpenter, M. G. , Garland, S. J. , & Hunt, M. A. (2013). The role of neuromuscular changes in aging and knee osteoarthritis on dynamic postural control. Aging and Disease, 4(2), 84–99. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Unterrainer, J. M. , & Owen, A. M. (2006). Planning and problem solving: From neuropsychology to functional neuroimaging. Journal of Physiology ‐ Paris, 99(4–6), 308–317. 10.1016/j.jphysparis.2006.03.014 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wang, B. T. , Zhang, M. , Bu, L. G. , Xu, L. W. , Wang, W. , & Li, Z. Y. (2016). Posture‐related changes in brain functional connectivity as assessed by wavelet phase coherence of NIRS signals in elderly subjects. Behavioural Brain Research, 312, 238–245. 10.1016/j.bbr.2016.06.037 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wiesmeier, I. K. , Dalin, D. , & Maurer, C. (2015). Elderly use proprioception rather than visual and vestibular cues for postural motor control. Frontiers in Aging Neuroscience, 7, 97 10.3389/fnagi.2015.00097 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wittenberg, E. , Thompson, J. , Nam, C. S. , & Franz, J. R. (2017). Neuroimaging of human balance control: A systematic review. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 11, 170 10.3389/fnhum.2017.00170 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Woollacott, M. , & Shumway‐Cook, A. (2002). Attention and the control of posture and gait: A review of an emerging area of research. Gait & Posture, 16(1), 1–14. 10.1016/s0966-6362(01)00156-4 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zimeo Morais, G. A. , Balardin, J. B. , & Sato, J. R. (2018). fNIRS Optodes’ Location Decider (fOLD): A toolbox for probe arrangement guided by brain regions‐of‐interest. Scientific Reports, 8(1), 3341 10.1038/s41598-018-21716-z [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zwergal, A. , Linn, J. , Xiong, G. M. , Brandt, T. , Strupp, M. , & Jahn, K. (2012). Aging of human supraspinal locomotor and postural control in fMRI. Neurobiology of Aging, 33(6), 1073–1084. 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2010.09.022 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.