Abstract
Background
Reducing meat consumption can help prevent non-communicable diseases and protect the environment. Interventions targeting conscious determinants of human behaviour are generally acceptable approaches to promote dietary change, but little is known about their effectiveness to reduce the demand for meat.
Objective
To evaluate the effectiveness of interventions targeting conscious determinants of human behaviour to reduce the demand for meat.
Methods
We searched six electronic databases on the 31st of August 2017 with a predefined algorithm, screened publicly accessible resources, contacted authors, and conducted forward and backward reference searches. Eligible studies employed experimental designs to evaluate interventions targeting conscious determinants of human behaviour to reduce the consumption, purchase, or selection of meat in comparison to a control condition, a baseline period, or relative to other eligible interventions. We synthesised results narratively and conducted an exploratory crisp-set Qualitative Comparative Analysis to identify combinations of intervention characteristics associated with significant reductions in the demand for meat.
Results
We included 24 papers reporting on 59 interventions and 25,477 observations. Self-monitoring interventions and individual lifestyle counselling led to, or were associated with reduced meat consumption. Providing information about the health or environmental consequences of eating meat was associated with reduced intentions to consume and select meat in virtual environments, but there was no evidence to suggest this approach influenced actual behaviour. Education about the animal welfare consequences of eating meat was associated with reduced intentions to consume meat, while interventions implicitly highlighting animal suffering were not. Education on multiple consequences of eating meat led to mixed results. Tailored education was not found to reduce actual or intended meat consumption, though few studies assessed this approach.
Conclusion
Some interventions targeting conscious determinants of human behaviour have the potential to reduce the demand for meat. In particular, self-monitoring interventions and individual lifestyle counselling can help to reduce meat consumption. There was evidence of effectiveness of some educational messages in reducing intended consumption and selection of meat in virtual environments.
Protocol registration
Electronic supplementary material
The online version of this article (10.1186/s12966-018-0729-6) contains supplementary material, which is available to authorized users.
Keywords: Meat consumption, Dietary change, Behaviour change, Interventions, Education, Motivation, Systematic review
Background
Red and processed meat consumption is associated with an increased risk of developing some non-communicable chronic conditions, including cardiovascular disease [1–3], type-2-diabetes [3–5], and some forms of cancer [6–8]. Additionally, livestock negatively affects the natural environment and advances anthropogenic climate change [9–11]. These environmental changes might in turn affect human health by contributing – among other things – to the pollution of air and drinking water, the rise in antimicrobial resistance, and the spread of vector-borne diseases [12–14]. While the potential health and environmental benefits of reducing meat consumption are well established, concerns about a consumer backlash and the poor understanding of how to promote this behaviour change have contributed to a general state of inaction [15–17]. Interventions targeting conscious determinants of human behaviour, such as those providing information, are generally perceived to be acceptable approaches to promote health behaviours by the public in developed countries [18, 19] and might therefore help to overcome this state of inaction. Furthermore, interventions targeting conscious determinants of human behaviour might, over time, enhance the public’s support for more structural interventions aiming at reducing the demand for meat [15, 16, 20]. Accordingly, identifying effective interventions targeting conscious determinants of human behaviour to reduce the demand for meat is an important step towards promoting healthier and more environmentally sustainable diets. The aim of this systematic review is to synthesise the evidence from studies evaluating the effectiveness of interventions targeting conscious determinants of human behaviour to reduce the actual or intended consumption, purchase, and selection of meat and to identify combinations of intervention characteristics that effectively promote this behaviour change.
Methods
Protocol registration and eligibility criteria
A protocol for this systematic review was published on PROSPERO [21]. This review includes studies evaluating interventions targeting conscious determinants of human behaviour to reduce the consumption, purchase, or selection of meat, and that fulfilled the eligibility criteria outlined in Table 1.
Table 1.
Inclusion criteria | Exclusion criteria | |
---|---|---|
Population | All are eligible except those listed in the exclusion criteria. | People diagnosed with clinical condition(s) for which it is required to consume specific amounts of meat. |
Intervention | Interventions targeting conscious determinants of human behaviour to reduce the demand for meat, including information provision, motivational interviewing, and interventions aiding self-regulatory processes. | Dietary interventions aiming to promote a general dietary pattern (e.g. interventions promoting the Mediterranean dietary pattern) and interventions restructuring elements of the physical microenvironment (e.g. pricing, positioning, or portion size). |
Comparator | In order of preference (1) no- or minimal-intervention controls, (2) pre-intervention baseline, or (3) other eligible intervention(s). | Interventions not fulfilling the eligibility criteria. |
Outcome | Objective or self-reported measures of demand for meat, defined as actual or intended consumption, purchase, or selection of meat in real or virtual environments. We extracted data pertaining to the follow-up closest to the intervention completion and to the longest available follow-up, with the former representing our primary outcome. | N/A |
We extracted data on participants’ demand for meat, defined as the actual or intended consumption, purchase, or selection of meat. We refer to meat purchases when the selection of meat involves a real or virtual monetary transaction, while we refer to meat selection when no form of monetary transaction is involved. Where reported, we also extracted data pertaining to attitudes, subjective social norms, and perceived behavioural control of eating, purchasing, or selecting meat, and on body weight, blood pressure, blood glucose, and blood lipids. When an outcome was assessed in multiple ways we only extracted data for the most granular measure (e.g. food diary > self-reported score of change) referring to the longest time-span (e.g. consumption over a month > consumption over a week). There were no exclusion criteria pertaining to the length of follow-up, publication status, publication year, or language.
Search strategy and data extraction
We searched six electronic databases on the 31st of August 2017 using a pre-specified search algorithm (Additional file 1: Table S1). We searched for further eligible records contacting researchers and experts, manually conducting forward and backward reference searches, and screening publicly accessible online resources following the methodology described by Stansfield et al. ( [22], Additional file 1: Table S2). Two members of the research team independently assessed the eligibility of the studies, extracted data from eligible records using a pre-piloted data extraction form, and evaluated the methodological quality of all eligible studies using the Quality Assessment Tool for Quantitative Studies [23]. Where additional information was required, we contacted authors and/or reviewed study protocols. Disagreements were resolved by discussion and by referral to a third member of the research team.
Data synthesis
We synthesised results narratively grouping interventions in five categories: (1) individual lifestyle counselling, (2) goal-setting and self-monitoring interventions, (3) non-tailored education (about meat consumption and one or more of health, the environment, animal welfare, socio-economic issues), (4) tailored education, and (5) interventions implicitly highlighting animal suffering. We augmented our narrative synthesis with an exploratory crisp-set Qualitative Comparative Analysis (QCA). In our QCA we used a binary coding system to categorise each intervention as presenting or not presenting specific behavioural strategies or implementation features to identify configurations of intervention characteristics associated with (and those not associated with) statistically significant (p < 0.05) reductions in the demand for meat at the follow-up closest to the intervention completion. Where studies reported results for multiple eligible outcomes we only considered the outcome closest to ‘actual behaviour’ (actual behaviour > behaviour in virtual environments > intention). The intervention characteristics included in QCA pertained to whether an intervention provided information about the (a) health, (b) environmental, (c) animal welfare, (d) socio-economic, (e) and multiple consequences of meat consumption, and whether it (e) provided tailored information, (f) employed self-monitoring strategies, (g) employed goal-setting strategies, (h) implicitly highlighted animal suffering, (i) implemented individual lifestyle counselling sessions, (j) targeted people with, or at increased risk of ill-health (k) provided information on ‘how to’ eat less meat, and whether (l) the outcome was actual as opposed to intentions or behaviour in virtual environments. In QCA we included comparisons between interventions and no-intervention controls or pre-intervention baselines, while comparisons between multiple interventions were excluded. A more detailed account on QCA in systematic reviews is outlined by Thomas et al. [24].
Results
Study selection
We screened 10,733 titles/abstracts, assessed the full-text of 60 papers, and included 24 papers reporting on 29 studies and 59 intervention conditions (Fig. 1). The majority of papers (54%) were published or written between 2015 and 2017.
Study characteristics
This review includes 25,477 observations (i.e. individuals or individual meal purchases) at the follow-up closest to the intervention completion and 305 observations at the longest follow-up for the four studies reporting on multiple end-points. Where reported, mean age ranged from 19 to 74 years (median = 36), the proportion of female participants ranged from 49 to 100% (median = 61%), the proportion of white participants ranged from 58 to 83% (median = 75%), and participants’ education and/or household income were high in most studies. Forty-five percent of studies only included individuals who ate meat. Studies recruited individuals (N = 26) or food providers (N = 3) through active recruitment (N = 15), advertisement and passive recruitment (N = 8), or through online panels (N = 6). Studies were conducted in Europe (N = 12), the USA (N = 9), China (N = 2), New Zealand (N = 2), and Brazil (N = 1), and were implemented in various settings, including experimental settings (N = 18), canteens or food services (N = 3), healthcare settings (N = 2), and among free-living individuals (N = 6). Fourteen studies employed a randomised controlled trial (RCT), 5 employed a non-randomised controlled trial (CT), 1 study employed a crossover design, 7 employed a pre-post design, and 2 studies retrospectively evaluated the intervention effectiveness. Retention at the shortest follow-up ranged from 59 to 100% (median = 95%). The overall methodological quality was ‘strong’ for 6 studies, ‘medium’ for 10 studies, and ‘weak’ for 13 studies. Table 2 summarises the characteristics of all studies included.
Table 2.
Reference and country | Eligibility criteria | Recruitment strategy | Attrition and sample size (a, b) | Availability | EPHPP QATQS score (c) |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Randomised Controlled Trials (RCT) | |||||
Arndt, 2016, study 1, USA [43] | Exclusion: Individuals who seldom or never ate meat, did not eat meat weekly, consumed no meat containing meal in the past 3 days, ate no serving of meat on an average day, less than 10% of what they ate on an average day is meat, believed that eating meat is bad, disliked eating meat, identified as vegetarians or vegans. | Individual recruitment through Amazon Mechanical Turk. | T1: 0% (179 to 179) | Unpublished, available online | Low |
Arndt, 2016, study 2, USA [43] | See Arndt 2016, study 1. | See Arndt 2016, study 1. | T1: 0% (296 to 296) | Unpublished, available online | Low |
Carfora et al., 2017, Italy [32] | Exclusion: Individuals following specific diets (such as vegan, vegetarian, protein, slimming and/or fattening diets). | E-mails were sent to a convenience sample of Italian undergraduates. | T1: 9.68% (124 to112) | Peer reviewed publication | Medium |
Carfora et al., 2017, Italy [31] | Participants had to have a mobile phone supporting SMS. Exclusion: Individuals following specific diets or who participated to the other study by Carfora. | See Carfora et al., 2017 above. | T1: 4.2% (238 to 228) | Peer reviewed publication | Medium |
Emmons et al., 2005, USA [25] | Participants had to be between 40 and 75 years, have an adenomatous colon polyp removed within 4 weeks of recruitment, have no personal history of CRC, be competent in English, be capable of informed consent, and be reachable by phone. | Eligible individuals were sent a letter describing the study and were later contacted by phone unless they opted out. | T1: 12.59% (1247 to 1090) | Peer reviewed publication | Strong |
Emmons et al., 2005, USA [26] | Participants had to be 18 to 75 years old, have a visit scheduled with a participating healthcare provider, be competent in English or Spanish, and come from an eligible working-class neighbourhood. Exclusion: Individuals who had cancer at enrolment, or who were employed by the participating health centres or at a worksite participating in the companion study (Emmons et al., 2005 (a)). | See Emmons et al., 2005 above. | T1: 12% (2219 to 1954) | Peer reviewed publication | Medium |
Fehrenbach, 2013, USA [33] | Exclusion: Individuals who were vegan, vegetarian, or pescetarian, self-reported insufficient attention to the message, previously completed part or all of the survey, had incomplete data, received the wrong intervention, and international or non-undergraduate students. | Individuals were sampled from communication classes at a large university in Arizona in exchange for extra credits. | T1: 10.1% (208 to 187) | Unpublished, not available online | Low |
Fehrenbach, 2015, USA [37] | Participants had to be U.S. resident, 25–44 years of age and consume meat 7+ times/week. Exclusion: Individuals who took the survey on mobile devices, failed an attention filter, or completed the survey too quickly or without viewing the video. | Individual recruitment from a national panel using Qualtrics. | T1: 1.61% (373 to 367) | Unpublished, available online | Low |
T2: 58.98% (373 to 153) | |||||
Graham et al., 2017, New Zealand [39] | Participants had to reside in New Zealand and pass an attention filter. | Individual recruitment through convenience and snowball techniques on a university campus, and advertisement outside the university campus. | T1: 0% | Peer reviewed publication | Medium |
Klöckner et al., 2017, study 1, Netherlands [44] | Participants had to be adult Norwegians. | Individuals were randomly selected from the population registry and sent an invitation letter. | T1: 17.1% (1047 to 868) | Peer reviewed publication | Low |
Klöckner et al., 2017, study 2, Netherlands [44] | Participants had to be adults. | Individuals were recruited from the professional online panel TNS Gallup. | T1: 8.63% (3895 to 3559) | Peer reviewed publication | Medium |
Tian et al., 2016, study 1, France and China [49] | Exclusion: Individuals who identified as vegetarians. | Individuals were recruited using social media and internal university advertisement. | T1: 41.47% (885 to 518) | Peer reviewed publication | Low |
Tian et al., 2016, study 2, France and China [49] | See Tian et al., 2016, study 1. | See Tian et al., 2016, study 1. | T1: 14.52% (606 to 518) | Peer reviewed publication | Low |
Vibhuti, 2016, USA [40] | Participants had to be adults and reside in the US. | Individual recruitment through Amazon Mechanical Turk. | T1: 0,97% (412 to 408) | Unpublished, available online | Low |
Non-randomised Controlled Trials (CT) | |||||
Allen et al., 2012, Australia [42] | N/A | The survey was sent to a random sample of individuals drawn from the telephone directory. | T1: 1.82% (220 to 216) | Peer reviewed publication | Low |
T2: 55.91% (220 to 97) | |||||
Berndsen et al., 2005, study 1, Netherlands [34] | Participants had to be meat eaters. | Individual recruitment through internal university advertisement | T1: 0% (141 to 141) | Peer reviewed publication | Low |
T2: 0% (141 to 141) | |||||
Berndsen et al., 2005, study 2, Netherlands [34] | See Berndsen et al., 2005, study 1. | See Berndsen et al., 2005, study 1. | T1: 0% (92 to 92) | Peer reviewed publication | Low |
T2: 0% (92 to 92) | |||||
Bertolotti et al., 2016, Italy [38] | Participants had to be over 60 years old, had to volunteer to participate, and complete sufficient sections of the questionnaire. | Active recruitment of individuals from socio-recreational centres for the elderly in Milan, Italy. | T1: 19.17% (120 to 97) | Peer reviewed publication | Strong |
Schiavon et al., 2015, Brazil [27] | All patients admitted for surgical treatment of suspected malignant breast tumors in the Maternidade Carmela Dutra Hospital. Exclusion: Individuals who had a history of cancer or a surgical procedure in the previous year; were pregnant or breastfeeding at the time of diagnosis; had positive results for HIV; had neoadjuvant cancer treatment, or a neurological disease. | Active recruitment of all aforementioned patients. | T1: 9.71% (103 to 93) | Peer reviewed publication | Strong |
Crossover design (CO) | |||||
Scrimgeour, 2012, New Zealand [35] | N/A | Individuals were recruited using the University Psychology and Geography mailing lists and snowballing techniques | T1: 18.66% (434 to 353) | Unpublished, available online | Medium |
Single group pre-post design | |||||
Cordts et al., 2014, Germany [36] | Participants had to be meat eaters. | Individual recruitment through a professional panel provider with the aim of obtaining a representative sample of the German population. | T1: 5.76% (590 to 556) | Peer reviewed publication | Strong |
Godfrey, 2014, Canada [41] | N/A | Food stations were recruited from the University Dining Centre at the University of Calgary. | T1: N/A (16,786 meal purchases) | Unpublished, available online | Medium |
Grimmet et al., 2016, UK [30] | Participants had to be over 18 years, have completed treatment for non-metastatic CRC within the last 6 months, be competent in English, have adequate mobility and no contraindications for unsupervised physical activity. | Consultants in 3 London hospitals referred patients to the researchers and research-nurses recruited participants from 5 London hospitals. | T1: 20.69% (29 to 23) | Peer reviewed publication | Medium |
Hawkes et al., 2009, Australia [28] | Participants had to be 20–80 years old, approximately 6 months post-CRC diagnosis; competent in English; and have no hearing, speech, or cognitive disabilities preventing them from completing telephone interviews. | Invitation and consent packages were sent to individuals who had undergone treatment in 3 the practices of three practitioners in Brisbane. | T1: 0% (20 to 20) | Peer reviewed publication | Strong |
Hawkes et al., 2012, Australia [29] | Participants had to be able to understand and give informed consent in English; have no current or previous diagnosis of CRC or medical conditions limiting adherence to an unsupervised lifestyle program; own a phone; and have one or more poor health behaviour(s) among: not achieving ≥150 min of physical activity/week; eating > 4 servings of red meat/week or < 2 serves of fruit/day, or < 5 servings of vegetables/day; consuming > 2 drinks/day; or if they had a BMI ≥25. Participants had to have a first degree relative with CRC. | Social media, printed material, radio and online advertisement. | T1: 0% (22 to 22) | Peer reviewed publication | Medium |
Loy et al., 2016, Germany [45] | Participants had to be non-vegetarians and proficient in German. | Individuals were recruited through internal university advertisement. | T1: 3,33% (60 to 58) | Peer reviewed publication | Medium |
T2: 8.33% (60 to 55) | |||||
Marette et al., 2016, France [46] | Participants had to eat ground beef, at least occasionally. | Individuals were recruited via phone to randomly select a sample representative of the age groups and socio-economic status of the population in Dijon, France. | T1: 3,23% (124 to 120) | Report, available online | Strong |
Retrospective intervention evaluation | |||||
Leidig, 2012, study 1, USA [47] | All healthcare accounts of Sodexo’s food service in the USA were eligible. | The survey was distributed to the managers of all USA healthcare accounts. | T1: N/A (119 account managers) | Report, available online | Low |
Leidig, 2012, study 2, USA [47] | All corporate and governmental accounts of Sodexo’s food service in the USA were eligible. | The survey was distributed to the managers of all USA corporate and government accounts. | T1: N/A (126 account managers) | Report, available online | Low |
Legend: (a) T1 and T2 respectively refer to the follow up closest to the intervention completion and to the longest available follow-up. (b) Studies reporting no attrition may have reported data of completers only throughout the paper. (c) The EPHPP QATQS (Effective Public Health Practice Project Quality Assessment Tool For Quantitative Studies) score indicates the study’s methodological quality and is based on: (1) study design, (2) selection bias, (3) confounders, (4) blinding, (5) data collection method, (6) withdrawal and dropouts. Studies with ≥2 weak ratings in the aforementioned dimensions are assigned a ‘low’ score, studies with 1 weak rating are assigned a ‘medium’ score, and studies with no weak rating are assigned a ‘strong’ score
Interventions and outcomes
We included 37 interventions providing non-tailored information about meat consumption and health (N = 11), the environment (N = 8), animal welfare (N = 2), socio-economic issues (N = 2), or a combination thereof (N = 14). Ten interventions provided tailored education, four implicitly highlighted animal suffering, six delivered individual lifestyle counselling, and two delivered self-monitoring interventions. Of all interventions included, two interventions targeted food suppliers while the rest targeted consumers. Fifteen studies reported on participants’ actual meat consumption, 6 on meat purchase or selection, and 15 on intended consumption. Fifteen studies additionally reported on other pre-specified secondary psychosocial outcomes or biomarkers of health risk. Table 3 summarises each intervention and its impact on, or association with participants’ demand for meat at the shortest follow-up. The results for the longest follow-up and for other secondary psychosocial and biological outcomes are summarized in Additional file 1: Tables S3, S4, and S5.
Table 3.
Paper | Sample characteristics and comparison (a) | Intervention | Outcome | Results (b) | Direction of outcome |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Individual lifestyle counselling | |||||
Emmons et al., 2005, [26]. | Sample size: IG: N = 1088, CG: N = 1131 Age: M = 49 Female: 66% Comparison: IG vs CG, RCT |
The intervention targeted consumption of red meat, fruit and vegetable, multivitamin, and physical activity. It comprised endorsement and tailored prescription to prompt behavior change by participants’ clinician, 1 × 20′ in-person and 4 × 10′ telephone counseling sessions with a health advisor, and tailored supporting material including information on barriers to change. The intervention focused on social determinants of behavior. CG: Usual care. |
The proportion of participants reporting consuming ≤3 servings/w of red meat over the past 4 weeks was assessed at the baseline and 8 months later with a semi-quantitative Food Frequency Questionnaire (FFQ). | In the IG the proportion of participants consuming ≤3 servings/w rose by 11.8%, while in the CG it decreased by 0.2%. The changes over time between the two conditions were significantly different (p < 0.001). | Desired direction |
Emmons et al. (a), 2005, [25]. | Sample size: IG: N = 591, CG: N = 656 Age: Median: within 60–75 Female: 42% Comparison: IG vs CG, RCT |
IG: The intervention targeted consumption of red meat, fruit and vegetable, alcohol, multivitamin, physical activity, and smoking. It comprised 1 motivational and goal-setting telephone session and 4 telephone counselling sessions delivered at monthly intervals by health advisors, and tailored supporting materials. CG: Usual care, gastroenterologist endorsement of the study’s behavioural targets, and CRC prevention leaflet. |
The proportion of participants reporting consuming on average ≤ 3 portions/w of red meat was assessed at the baseline and 8 months later with a semi-quantitative FFQ. | Compared to the CG (12%) more participants in the IG reduced their meat intakes to < 3 portions/w (18%, p = 0.002). | Desired direction |
Schiavon et al., 2015, [27]. | Sample size: IG: N = 18, CG: N = 75 Age: M = 51 Female: 100% Comparison: IG vs CG, CT |
IG: The 12 month intervention targeted consumption of red and processed meat and fruit and vegetables. It provided information bi-weekly phone calls, bimonthly 24-h dietary recalls followed by researchers’ feedback, and supporting material. CG: Basic healthy lifestyle guidelines at the baseline and follow-up. |
Red and processed meat consumption (in g/d) was assessed with an FFQ for Brazilian diets, directly post-intervention. | There was a significant difference in red and processed meat consumption between the groups in unadjusted analyses (B(exp) = 0.5, p < 0.05) and in analyses adjusting for post-intervention energy intake and baseline red and processed meat consumption (B(exp) = 0.6, p < 0.05). This effect was not detected when also adjusting for baseline saturated and monounsaturated fat, and carbonyl protein and reduced glutathione (B(exp) = 0.6, p > 0.05). | Desired direction |
Grimmet et al., 2016, [30]. | Sample size: N = 29 Age: M = 65 Female: 62% Comparison: Pre-post |
IG: The 12 week intervention targeted consumption of red and processed meat, fruit and vegetables, and physical activity. It comprised 2 weekly telephone calls from the researcher and supporting materials including recipes. The intervention focused on goal setting, review of goals, self-monitoring, and feedback on performance. | Consumption of red (in g/w) and processed meat (in portions/w) was assessed with an FFQ before and directly post-intervention. | Red and processed meat consumption decreased from pre- to post-intervention (mean reduction for red meat: 147.4, p = 0.013; mean reduction for processed meat: 0.83, p = 0.002). | Desired direction |
Hawkes et al., 2009, [28]. | Sample size: N = 20 Age: Median: 66 Female: 50% Comparison: Pre-post |
IG: The 6 week intervention targeted consumption of red and processed meat, fruit and vegetable, alcohol, weight management, physical activity, and smoking. It comprised 6 weekly 45′ telephone counselling sessions from a trained health coach, and supporting material. The intervention included lifestyle support, health risks information, behaviour change strategies, self-efficacy, and outcome expectations. | Consumption of red and processed meat (in servings/w) was assessed via phone, before and directly post-intervention. | There was a significant decrease in the intake of processed meat servings/w from baseline (Median = 1) to post intervention (Median = 0, p = 0.01). The proportion of participants eating ≤3–4 servings/w of red meat did not change from pre- (85%) to post-intervention (85%, p = 1). | Desired direction (processed meat) No association (red meat) |
Hawkes et al., 2012, [29]. | Sample size: N = 22 Age: M = 47 Female: 82% Comparison: Pre-post |
IG: The 6 week intervention targeted consumption of red and processed meat, fruit and vegetable, alcohol, weight management, physical activity, and smoking. It comprised 6 × 1-h telephone-coaching sessions with a trained health coach, focussing on motivation, expectations, values, mindfulness, action planning, goal-setting, and self-monitoring, and supporting material. | Consumption of red and processed meat (in servings/w) was assessed via phone before and directly post-intervention. | Processed meat consumption declined from pre- to post-intervention (mean change, 95%CI = − 1.2, − 1.8 to − 0.5, p < 0.01). Red meat consumption did not change from pre- to post-intervention (mean change, 95%CI = 0.02, − 0.6 to 0.6, p = 0.93). | Desired direction (processed meat) Undesired direction (red meat) |
Self-monitoring and goal setting interventions | |||||
Carfora et al., 2017, [32], | Sample size: IG: N = 57, CG: N = 55 Age: M = 19 Female: 56% Comparison: IG vs CG, RCT |
IG: Daily text messages for a week, encouraging participants to self-monitor their consumption of processed meat and to ‘think about the regret they could experience’ if they were to exceed the recommended levels of processed meat consumption (50 g/d). CG: No intervention. |
Consumption of processed meat (in servings) was assessed using a 7-day food diary during the week preceding and the week concomitant to the intervention. Intention to eat ≤50 g of processed meat over the upcoming week was assessed with three items on a scale from 1 (strongly disagree) to 7 (strongly agree) before and directly post-intervention. | During the intervention the IG ate significantly fewer servings of processed meat (M = 1.74) than the CG (M = 3.29, p < 0.001, d = 0.7). At post- intervention, the IG reported higher intentions to eat ≤1 serving of processed meat in the upcoming week (M = 4.47) compared to the CG (M = 3.60, p < 0.008, d = 0.51). | Desired direction |
Carfora et al., 2017(a), [31]. | Sample size: IG: N = 116, CG: N = 112 Age: M = 19 Female: 72% Comparison: IG vs CG, RCT |
IG: Daily text messages for a week, encouraging participants to self-monitor their consumption of red meat to not exceed a recommended maximum of two medium servings per week. CG: No intervention. |
Consumption of red meat (in servings) was assessed using a 7-day food diary during the week preceding and the week concomitant to the intervention. Intention to eat < 2 portions of red meat over the upcoming week was assessed with three items on a scale from 1 (strongly disagree) to 7 (strongly agree) before and directly post-intervention. | During the intervention the IG ate significantly fewer servings of red meat (M = 1.62) than the CG (M = 3.03, p < 0.001, d = 0.74). At post- intervention, the IG reported higher intentions to eat < 2 servings of red meat over the upcoming week (M = 4.80) compared to the CG (M = 4.07, p < 0.01, d = 0.41). | Desired direction |
Non-tailored information about meat consumption and health | |||||
Fehren-bach, 2013, [33]. | Sample size: N = 187 (total study) Age: Median: within 18–25 (total study) Female: 57% (total study) Comparison: IG vs CG, RCT |
IG: A webpage on the health impact of eating meat, recommending practical strategies to eat less meat. CG: A webpage about the Rolling Stones. |
Intention to reduce meat consumption was measured directly post-intervention, with three items on a scale from 1 (strongly disagree) to 5 (strongly agree). | Post-intervention intention to eat less meat was higher in the IG (M = 3.90) than in the CG (M = 2.69, p < 0.001). | Desired direction |
Fehren-bach, 2015, [37]. | Sample size: IG: N = 124, CG: N = 124 Age: 57 (total study) Female: 57 (total study) Comparison: IG vs CG, RCT |
IG: A 4′ video about the health impact of eating meat, highlighting participants’ susceptibility to these outcomes. CG: No intervention. |
At the baseline and one week post-intervention, participants reported how many meat-containing meals they ate in the past 7 days. Intention to eat less meat in the upcoming 7 days was assessed with three 5-points scales, directly post-intervention. | Meat intakes did not differ between the IG (M = − 3.16) and the CG (M = − 1.92) or any other study groups (p = 0.31, d = 0.29). Intention to eat less meat was higher in the IG (M = 3.46) than in the CG (M = 2.57, p < 0.001). | Desired direction |
Fehren-bach, 2015, [37]. | Sample size: IG: N = 124, CG: N = 124 Age: 57 (total study) Female: 57 (total study) Comparison: IG vs CG, RCT |
IG: A 7′ video about the negative health outcomes of eating meat, highlighting participants’ susceptibility to these outcomes, the health benefits of low meat diets, and strategies to eat less meat. CG: No intervention. |
At the baseline and one week post-intervention, participants reported how many meat-containing meals they ate in the past 7 days. Intention to eat less meat in the upcoming 7 days was measured with three 5-points scales, directly post-intervention. | Meat intakes did not differ between the IG (M = − 2.11) and the CG (M = − 1.92), or any other study groups (p = 0.31, d = 0.29). Intention to eat less meat was higher in the IG (M = 3.69) than in the CG (M = 2.57, p < 0.001). | Desired direction |
Berndsen et al., 2005, study 1, [34]. | Sample size: IG: N = 50, CG: N = 38 Age: M = 20 (total study) Female: 59% (total study) Comparison: IG vs. CG, CT |
IG: Cognitively framed paragraph on the health consequences of eating meat. CG: No intervention. |
Three weeks post-intervention, participants reported whether they ate less meat in the past 3 weeks. Directly post-intervention, participants reported if they intended to eat less meat over the upcoming 3 weeks. The scales ranged from 1 (fully disagree) to 9 (fully agree). | Self-reported change in meat consumption did not differ between the IG (M = 2.78) and CG (M = 3.16, p > 0.05). Intention to eat less meat did not differ between the IG (M = 2.38) and the CG (M = 3.08, p > 0.05). | Undesired direction |
Scrim-geour, 2012, [35]. | Sample size: N = 363 Age: M = 28 Female: 68% Comparison: Pre-post, Crossover |
IG: Information paragraph on the health impact of eating less meat. | Whether participants intended to eat less, the same, or more meat in the future was assessed pre- and post-intervention. | Compared to the baseline (M ≈ 2.18), participants’ intention to eat less meat was higher after the intervention (M ≈ 2.26, p < 0.001, d = 0.59). (c) | Desired direction |
Cordts et al., 2014, [36]. | Sample size: N = 136 Age: Median: within 40–59 (total study) Female: 45% Comparison: Pre-post |
IG: Article on the health impact of eating meat. | The number of participants intending to eat less meat was assessed pre- and post-intervention by asking whether they would eat less, the same, or more meat in the future. | The percentage of participants intending to reduce meat consumption increased from pre- (13.1%, N = 137) to post-intervention (23.5%, N = 136, p < 0.001). | Desired direction |
Berndsen et al., 2005, study 1, [34]. | Sample size: IG1: N = 53, IG2: N = 50 Age: M = 20 (total study) Female: 59% (for total study) Comparison: IG1 vs. IG2, CT |
IG1: Affectively framed paragraph on the health impact of eating less meat. IG2: Cognitively framed paragraph on the health impact of eating less meat. |
Three weeks post-intervention, participants reported whether they ate less meat in the past 3 weeks. Directly post-intervention, participants reported if they intended to eat less meat over the upcoming 3 weeks. The scales ranged from 1(fully disagree) to 9(fully agree). | Self-reported change in meat consumption did not differ between IG1 (M = 3.75) and IG2 (M = 2.78, p = 0.07). Directly post-intervention, IG1 (M = 3.48) had higher intentions to eat less meat over the upcoming 3 weeks than IG2 (M = 2.38, p < 0.05). (d) | N/A |
Bertolotti et al. 2016, [38]. | Sample size: IG1: N = 25, IG2: N = 23, IG3: N = 24, IG4: N = 25 Age: M = 74 (total study) Female: 73% (total study) Comparison: IG1 vs. IG2, IG3 vs. IG4, IG1 vs IG4, CT |
IG1: Factually framed paragraph on the health impact of eating less meat. IG2: Pre-factually framed paragraph on the health impact of eating less meat. IG3: Factually framed paragraph on the well-being impact of eating less meat. IG4: Pre-factually framed paragraph on the well-being impact of eating less meat. |
Selection of meat dishes was assessed in a simulated food choice task directly post-intervention. Intention to eat red and processed meat in the upcoming month was assessed on a scale from 1 (“much less than before”) to 7 (“much more than before”) at post-intervention. |
At post-intervention, IG1 chose fewer meat dishes than IG2 (p = 0.015) but had not significantly lower intentions to eat red or processed meat (p > 0.07). IG4 did not choose fewer meat dishes than IG3 (p = 0.089) but had lower intention to eat red (p = 0.046) and processed meat (p = 0.035). Intention to eat red and processed meat did not differ significantly between IG1 and IG4 (p > 0.28). There was no main effect of content (health vs well-being) or frame (pre-factual vs factual) on any outcome. | N/A |
Information about meat and the environment | |||||
Fehren-bach, 2013, [33]. | Sample size: N = 187 (total study) Age: Median: within 18–25 (total study) Female: 57% (total study) Comparison: IG vs CG, RCT |
IG: A webpage on the environmental impact of eating meat, recommending practical strategies to eat less meat. CG: Control web-site on the Rolling Stones. |
Intention to reduce meat consumption was measured directly post-intervention, with three items on a scale from 1 (strongly disagree) to 5 (strongly agree). | Intention to eat less meat was higher in the IG (M = 3.71) than in the CG (M = 2.69, p < 0.001). | Desired direction |
Graham, 2017, [39]. | Sample size: IG: N = 264, CG: N = 317 Age: Median: within 21–30 (total study) Female: 69% (total study) Comparison: IG vs CG, RCT |
IG: A self-transcendent framed paragraph on the livestock related GHG emissions in NZ and the mitigation potential of reduced consumption. | Intention to eat meat in the upcoming month was assessed with three items on a scale from 1 (low intention) to 7 (high intention), directly post-intervention. | Intention to eat meat was lower in the IG (M = 3.9) than in the CG (M = 4.2, p < 0.05). | Desired direction |
Graham, 2017, [39]. | Sample size: IG: N = 267, CG: N = 317 Age: Median: within 21–30 (total study) Female: 69% (total study) Comparison: IG vs CG, RCT |
IG: A self-enhancement framed paragraph on the livestock related GHG emissions in NZ and the mitigation potential of reduced consumption. | Intention to eat meat in the upcoming month was assessed with three items on a scale from 1 (low intention) to 7 (high intention), directly post-intervention. | Intention to eat meat was lower in the IG (M = 4.0) than in the CG (M = 4.2, p < 0.05). | Desired direction |
Vibhuti, 2016, [40]. | Sample size: IG: N = 183, CG: N = 225 Age: M = 36 Female: 54% Comparison: IG vs CG, RCT |
IG: An essay on the environmental impact of the meat consumption and production. CG: No intervention. |
Directly post-intervention participants completed six virtual food choices, selecting between a meat-based food and a comparable meat-free alternative. | The proportion of selected meat and meat-free products differed significantly between the CG (meat≈61%) and the IG (meat≈55%, p = 0.003, V = 0.06). | Desired direction |
Scrim-geour, 2012, [35]. | Sample size: N = 363 Age: M = 28 Female: 68% Comparison: Pre-post, Crossover |
IG: Information paragraph on the environmental impact of eating meat and strategies to reduce consumption. | Whether participants intended to eat less, the same, or more meat in the future was assessed pre- and post-intervention. | Intention to eat less meat was higher at post-intervention (M ≈ 2.27) than at the baseline (M ≈ 2.18, p < 0.001, d = 0.27). | Desired direction |
Cordts et al., 2014, [36]. | Sample size: N = 128 Age: Median: within 40–59 (total study) Female: 45% Comparison: Pre-post |
IG: An article on the environmental impact of eating meat. | The number of participants intending to eat less meat was assessed pre- and post-intervention by asking whether they would eat less, the same, or more meat in the future. | The percentage of participants intending to eat less meat increased from pre- (13.4%, N = 127) to post- intervention (18.8%, N = 128, p < 0.001). | Desired direction |
Godfrey, 2014, [41]. | Sample size: N = 6758 purchases during the intervention period, N = 4426 purchases during the control period. Comparison: Pre-post |
IG: A poster on the water footprint of meals containing vegetables (300 L/meal), pork, chicken or fish (590 L/meal), and beef (1350 L/meal) was displayed over 2 weeks in a university canteen. | Meat purchases were assessed using production reports of the dining centres indicating how many servings of each main course were made daily. | There was no difference in the proportion of meat dishes purchased in the control period (87.19%) and in the intervention period (87.82%, p > 0.05). | Undesired direction |
Godfrey, 2014, [41]. | Sample size: N = 1176 purchases during the intervention period, N = 4426 purchases during the control period. Comparison: Pre-post |
IG: A poster on the water footprint of meals containing vegetables (300 L/meal), pork, chicken or fish (590 L/meal), and beef (1350 L/meal) and mentioning the daily water-friendly option was displayed for 4 days in a university canteen. | Meat purchases were assessed using production reports of the dining centres indicating how many servings of each main course were made daily. | There was no difference in proportion of meat dishes purchased in the control period (87.19%) and in the intervention period (91.58%, p > 0.05). | Undesired direction |
Information on meat and animal welfare | |||||
Scrim-geour, 2012, [35]. | Sample size: N = 363 Age: M = 28 Female: 68% Comparison: Pre-post, Crossover |
IG: Information paragraph on the animal welfare implications of eating meat and strategies to eat less. | Whether participants intended to eat less, the same, or more meat in the future was assessed pre- and post-intervention. | Intention to eat less meat was higher at post-intervention (M ≈ 2.3) than at the baseline (M ≈ 2.18, p < 0.001, d = 0.65). | Desired direction |
Cordts et al., 2014, [36]. | Sample size: N = 150 Age: Median: within 40–59 (total study) Female: 55% Comparison: Pre-post |
IG: An article on the animal welfare implications of eating meat. | The number of participants intending to eat less meat was assessed pre- and post-intervention by asking whether they would eat less, the same, or more meat in the future. | The percentage of participants intending to eat less meat increased from pre- (15.6%, N = 147) to post- intervention (28%, N = 150, p < 0.001). | Desired direction |
Information about the socio-political consequences of meat consumption | |||||
Allen et al., 2002, [42]. | Sample Size: IG: N = 103, CG: N = 113 Age: Median within: 45–65 Female: 59% Comparison: IG vs CG, CT |
IG: Participants were informed that people higher in social dominance orientation (SDO) eat more meat and fewer vegetables, while people lower in social dominance do the opposite. CG: No intervention. |
Intended consumption of red and white meat servings in the upcoming 3 days was assessed with a single item directly at post-intervention. Three weeks post-interventions participants reported their consumption of meat servings over the past 7 days. | Intended meat consumption did not differ between IG (High SDO = 1.45, Low SDO = 1.45) and CG (High SDO = 1.56, Low SDO = 1.37). Actual meat consumption remained unchanged in both groups from pre- (IG = 2.88, CG = 2.66) to post-intervention (IG = 2.87, CG = 2.61, p > 0.05). | Desired direction (High SDO) Undesired direction (Low SDO) |
Cordts et al., 2014, [36]. | Sample size: N = 149 Age: Median within: 40–59 (total study) Female: 48% Comparison: Pre-post |
IG: An article on the social consequences of eating meat. | The number of participants intending to eat less meat was assessed pre- and post-intervention by asking whether they would eat less, the same, or more meat in the future. | The percentage of participants intending to eat less meat increased from pre- (9%, N = 145) to post-intervention (12.1%, N = 149, p < 0.001). | Desired direction |
Information about multiple consequences of eating meat | |||||
Arndt, 2016, study 1, [43]. | Sample size: IG: N = 29, CG: N = 40 Age: M = 37 (total study) Female: 64% (total study) Comparison: IG vs CG, RCT |
IG: Paragraph on the impact of meat consumption of an average American on health, and personal finances, and animal welfare, and the environment, and personal appearance. CG: No intervention. |
Intended average daily meat consumption (in servings) was assessed with a single open question directly post-intervention. | Adjusted for baseline meat intake, intended meat consumption did not differ between IG (M = 2.21) and CG (M = 2.63), or among any other study groups (p = 0.19). | Desired direction |
Arndt, 2016, study 2, [43]. | Sample size: IG: N = 37, CG: N = 40 Age: 37 (total study) Female: 62% (total study) Comparison: IG vs CG, RCT |
IG: Paragraph on the impact of meat consumption on health, and personal finances, and animal welfare, and the environment, also stating that eating less meat can help fulfil one’s altruistic duty. CG: No intervention. |
Intended average daily meat consumption (in servings) was assessed with a single open question directly post-intervention. | Adjusted for baseline meat intake, intended meat consumption did not differ between IG (M = 2.24) and CG (M = 2.75), or among any other study groups (p = 0.45). | Desired direction |
Arndt, 2016, study 2, [43]. | Sample size: IG: N = 38, CG: N = 40 Age: 37 (total study) Female: 62% (total study) Comparison: IG vs CG, RCT |
IG: Paragraph on the impact of meat consumption on health, and personal finances, and animal welfare, and the environment, also stating that eating less meat could help fulfil one’s personal (egoistic) objectives. CG: No intervention. |
Intended average daily meat consumption (in servings) was assessed with a single open question directly post-intervention. | Adjusted for baseline meat intake, intended meat consumption did not differ between IG (M = 1.92) and CG (M = 2.75), or among any other study groups (p = 0.45). | Desired direction |
Arndt, 2016, study 2, [43]. | Sample size: IG: N = 30, CG: N = 40 Age: 37 (total study) Female: 62% (total study) Comparison: IG vs CG, RCT |
IG: Paragraph on the impact of meat consumption on health, and personal finances, and animal welfare, and the environment. CG: No intervention. |
Intended average daily meat consumption (in servings) was assessed with a single open question directly post-intervention. | Adjusted for baseline meat intake, intended meat consumption did not differ between IG (M = 2.57) and CG (M = 2.75), or among any other study groups (p = 0.45). | Desired direction |
Klöckner et al., 2017, study 1, [44]. | Sample size: IG: N = 246, CG: N = 235 Age: M = 40 (total study) Female: 49% (total study) Comparison: IG vs CG, RCT |
IG: Access to one of three subsections of a web-page (selected at random) outlining (a) why to eat less beef, or (b) how to eat less beef, or (c) how to master challenges associated with eating less beef. The webpages included health, and environmental, and social reasons for eating less beef, practical strategies, statements triggering personal values, links to scientific sources, and videos of people’s stories. CG: No intervention. |
Change in beef consumption from pre- to 8 weeks post-intervention, was measured with a retrospective food diary. | Adjusted for baseline consumption, there was no difference in the changes in beef consumption between the IG (M = 54.42) and the CG (M = −37.09, simple contrast: p = 0.3). | Undesired direction |
Klöckner et al., 2017, study 1, [44]. | Sample size: IG: N = 273, CG: N = 235 Age: M = 40(for total study) Female: 49% (for total study) Comparison: IG vs CG, RCT |
IG: Access to a web-page outlining (a) why to eat less beef, and (b) how to eat less beef, and (c) how to master challenges associated with eating less beef. The webpages included health, and environmental, and social reasons for eating less beef, practical strategies, statements triggering personal values, links to scientific sources, and videos of people’s stories. CG: No intervention. |
Change in beef consumption from pre- to 8 weeks post-intervention, was measured with a retrospective food diary. | Adjusted for baseline consumption, there was no difference in the changes in beef consumption between the IG (M = 38.17) and the CG (M = −37.09, simple contrast: p = 0.79). | Undesired direction |
Klöckner et al., 2017, study 2, [44]. | Sample size: IG: N = 975, CG: N = 970 Age: M = 43 (for total study) Female: 47% (for total study) Comparison: IG vs CG, RCT |
IG: Access to one of three subsections of a web-page (selected at random) outlining (a) why to eat less beef, or (b) how to eat less beef, or (c) how to master challenges associated with eating less beef. The webpages included health, environmental, and social reasons for eating less beef, practical strategies, statements triggering personal values, links to scientific sources, and videos of people’s stories. CG: No intervention. |
Change in beef consumption from pre- to 8 weeks post-intervention, was measured with a retrospective food diary. | Adjusted for baseline consumption, there was no difference in the changes in beef consumption between the IG (M = 28.5) and the CG (M = −66.38, simple contrast: p = 0.79). | Undesired direction |
Klöckner et al., 2017, study 2, [44]. | Sample size: IG: N = 974, CG: N = 970 Age: M = 43 (for total study) Female: 47% (for total study) Comparison: IG vs CG, RCT |
IG: Access to a web-page outlining (a) why to eat less beef, and (b) how to eat less beef, and (c) how to master challenges associated with eating less beef. The webpages included health, environmental, and social reasons for eating less beef, practical strategies, statements triggering personal values, links to scientific sources, and videos of people’s stories. CG: No intervention. |
Change in beef consumption from pre- to 8 weeks post-intervention, was measured with a retrospective food diary. | Adjusted for baseline consumption, there was a significant difference in the changes in beef consumption between the IG (M = 13) and the CG (M = −66.38, simple contrast: p < 0.001) but these changes went in the undesired direction. | Undesired direction |
Berndsen et al., 2005, study 2, [34]. | Sample size: IG: N = 45, CG: N = 47 Age: M = 20.6 Female: 58% Comparison: IG vs. CG, CT |
IG: Paragraph on animal welfare, and health, and environmental impact of eating meat. CG: No intervention control. |
Three weeks post-intervention, participants reported whether they ate less meat in the past 3 weeks. Directly post-intervention, participants reported if they intended to eat less meat over the upcoming 3 weeks. The scales ranged from 1(fully disagree) to 9 (fully agree). | There was no significant main effect of condition (p < 0.10). Interaction and post-hoc tests were not reported. | N/A |
Loy et al., 2016, [45]. | Sample size: IG: N = 28 Age: M = 22 Female: 82% Comparison: Pre-post |
IG: A paragraph on the environmental, and ethical, and health, and socio-economic consequences of eating meat and written instructions for mental contrasting and intention implementation. | Meat consumption in g/d was assessed with a 7-day diary the week pre- and the week directly post-intervention. | Meat consumption decreased significantly from pre- to post-intervention (average reduction: 45.2 g/d, p < 0.001, d = 1.09). | Desired direction |
Loy et al., 2016, [45]. | Sample size: IG: N = 30 Age: M = 23 Female: 75% Comparison: Pre-post |
IG: A paragraph on the environmental, and ethical, and health, and socio-economic consequences of eating meat. | Meat consumption in g/d was assessed with a 7-day diary the week pre- and the week directly post-intervention. | Meat consumption decreased significantly from pre- to post-intervention (average reduction: 26.4 g/day, p = 0.001, d = 0.63). | Desired direction |
Marette et al., 2016, [46]. | Sample size: 124 (recruited) Age: Median: within 40–49 Female: 54% Comparison: Pre-post |
IG: Four paragraphs outlining the health and environmental impact of red meat consumption and the respective benefits of alternative soy-based products. | Before and directly post-intervention, participants conducted a virtual food choice task in which they selected 5 items from either beef or soy burgers. | The selection of beef items declined from pre- (M = 3.52) to post-intervention (M = 2.69, p < 0.01). | Desired direction |
Leidig, 2012, study 1, [47]. | Sample size: IG: N = 119 Comparison: Retrospective evaluation |
IG: The Meatless Monday campaign toolkit was sent to all healthcare accounts in the US and it was posted on Sodexo’s intranet. The toolkit included information about various benefits of eating less meat, and practical suggestions for implementing a Meatless Monday campaign. | Three months post-intervention, the general managers of Sodexo’s healthcare accounts retrospectively assessed the change in meat purchases on a single scale with 5% increments ranging from 1 (10% + decrease) to 7 (10% + increase), with a score of 4 indicating no changes. | Three months post-intervention 8% of accounts reported increases, 35% reported declines, and the rest reported no changes in meat sales. Using the scale’s mid-points as the average changes in sales, an overall decline was observed M = −0.75%, p < 0.001). | Desired direction |
Leidig, 2012, study 2, [47]. | Sample size: IG: N = 126 Comparison: Retrospective evaluation |
IG: The Meatless Monday campaign toolkit was posted on Sodexo’s intranet for the corporate and governmental accounts to retrieve. The toolkit included information about various benefits of eating less meat, and practical suggestions for implementing a Meatless Monday campaign. | Three months post-intervention, the general managers of Sodexo’s healthcare accounts retrospectively assessed the change in meat purchases on a single scale with 5% increments ranging from 1 (10% + decrease) to 7 (10% + increase), with a score of 4 indicating no changes. | Three months post-intervention 14% of accounts reported increases, 20% reported declines, and the rest reported no changes in meat sales. There was no overall reduction in sales, when using the scale’s mid-points as average changes (M = − 0.33%, p = 0.23). | Desired direction |
Tailored information provision | |||||
Arndt, 2016, study 1, [43]. | Sample size: IG: N = 37, CG: N = 40 Age: M = 37 (total study) Female: 64% (total study) Comparison: IG vs CG, RCT |
IG: Tailored paragraph on how strongly a participant’s personal levels of meat consumption affect their health, or personal finances, or animal welfare, or environment, or personal appearance depending on which consequence participants valued. CG: No intervention. |
Intended average daily meat consumption (in servings) was assessed with a single open question directly post-intervention. | Adjusted for baseline meat intake, intended meat consumption did not differ between IG (M = 1.70) and the CG (M = 2.63), or among any other study groups (p = 0.19). | Desired direction |
Arndt, 2016, study 1, [43]. | Sample size: IG: N = 37, CG: N = 40 Age: M = 37 (total study) Female: 64% (total study) Comparison: IG vs CG, RCT |
IG: Tailored paragraph on how strongly a participant’s personal levels of meat consumption affect their health, and personal finances, and animal welfare, and the environment, and personal appearance. CG: No intervention. |
Intended average daily meat consumption (in servings) was assessed with a single open question directly post-intervention. | Adjusted for baseline meat intake, intended meat consumption did not differ between IG (M = 2.57) and the CG (M = 2.63), or among any other study groups (p = 0.19). | Desired direction |
Arndt, 2016, study 1, [43]. | Sample size: IG: N = 36, CG: N = 40 Age: M = 37 (total study) Female: 64% (total study) Comparison: IG vs CG, RCT |
IG: Tailored paragraph on the consequences of an average American’s meat consumption on health, or personal finances, or animal welfare, or the environment, or personal appearance, depending on which consequence participants valued. CG: No intervention. |
Intended average daily meat consumption (in servings) was assessed with a single open question directly post-intervention. | Adjusted for baseline meat intake, intended meat consumption did not differ between IG (M = 2.69) and the CG (M = 2.63), or among any other study groups (p = 0.19). | Undesired direction |
Arndt, 2016, study 2, [43]. | Sample size: IG: N = 42, CG: N = 40 Age: 37 (total study) Female: 62% (total study) Comparison: IG vs CG, RCT |
IG: Tailored paragraph on the consequences of meat consumption on health, or personal finances, or animal welfare, or the environment, depending on which consequence participants valued. The message stated that eating less meat is congruent with being responsible, or adventurous, or logical, or compassionate depending on participants’ self-schema. CG: No intervention. |
Intended average daily meat consumption (in servings) was assessed with a single open question directly post-intervention. | Adjusted for baseline meat intake, intended meat consumption did not differ between IG (M = 2.73) and the CG (M = 2.75), or among any other study groups (p = 0.45). | Desired direction |
Arndt, 2016, study 2, [43]. | Sample size: IG: N = 30, CG: N = 40 Age: 37 (total study) Female: 62% (total study) Comparison: IG vs CG, RCT |
IG: Tailored paragraph on the consequences of meat consumption on health, and personal finances, and animal welfare, and the environment. The message stated that eating less meat is congruent with being responsible, or adventurous, or logical, or compassionate depending on participants’ self-schema. CG: No intervention. |
Intended average daily meat consumption (in servings) was assessed with a single open question directly post-intervention. | Adjusted for baseline meat intake, intended meat consumption did not differ between IG (M = 2.13) and the CG (M = 2.75), or among any other study groups (p = 0.45). | Desired direction |
Arndt, 2016, study 2, [43]. | Sample size: IG: N = 33, CG: N = 40 Age: 37 (total study) Female: 62% (total study) Comparison: IG vs CG, RCT |
IG: Tailored paragraph on the consequences of meat consumption on health, or personal finances, or animal welfare, or the environment, depending on which consequence participants valued. The message stated that eating less meat can help fulfil one’s altruistic duty. CG: No intervention. |
Intended average daily meat consumption (in servings) was assessed with a single open question directly post-intervention. | Adjusted for baseline meat intake, intended meat consumption did not differ between IG (M = 1.97) and the CG (M = 2.75), or among any other study groups (p = 0.45). | Desired direction |
Arndt, 2016, study 2, [43]. | Sample size: IG: N = 29, CG: N = 40 Age: 37 (total study) Female: 62% (total study) Comparison: IG vs CG, RCT |
IG: Tailored paragraph on the consequences of meat consumption on health, or personal finances, or animal welfare, or the environment, depending on which consequence participants valued. The message stated that eating less meat can help fulfil one’s personal (egoistic) objectives. CG: No intervention. |
Intended average daily meat consumption (in servings) was assessed with a single open question directly post-intervention. | Adjusted for baseline meat intake, intended meat consumption did not differ between IG (M = 1.55) and the CG (M = 2.75), or among any other study groups (p = 0.45). | Desired direction |
Arndt, 2016, study 2, [43]. | Sample size: IG: N = 29, CG: N = 40 Age: 37 (total study) Female: 62% (total study) Comparison: IG vs CG, RCT |
IG: Tailored paragraph on the consequences of meat consumption on health, or personal finances, or animal welfare, or the environment, depending on which consequence participants valued. | Intended average daily meat consumption (in servings) was assessed with a single open question directly post-intervention. | Adjusted for baseline meat intake, intended meat consumption did not differ between IG (M = 2.86) and the CG (M = 2.75), or among any other study groups (p = 0.45). | Undesired direction |
Klöckner et al., 2017, study 1, [44]. | Participants had to be adults Sample size: IG: N = 275, CG: N = 235 Age: M = 40 (total study) Female: 49% (total study) Comparison: IG vs CG, RCT |
IG: Depending on participants stage of change for eating less beef, they were given access to the ‘stage matched’ subsections of a web-page about: (a) why to eat less beef, or (b) how to eat less beef, or (c) how to master challenges associated with eating less beef. The webpages included health, environmental, and social reasons for eating less beef, practical strategies, statements triggering personal values, links to scientific sources, and videos of people’s stories. CG: No intervention. |
Change in beef consumption from pre- to 8 weeks post-intervention, was measured with a retrospective food diary. | Adjusted for baseline consumption, there was no difference in the changes in beef consumption between the IG (M = 81.12) and the CG (M = −37.09, simple contrast: p = 0.37). | Undesired direction |
Klöckner et al., 2017, study 2, [44]. | Sample size: IG: N = 976, CG: N = 970 Age: M = 43 (for total study) Female: 47% (for total study) Comparison: IG vs CG, RCT |
IG: See Klöckner et al. (2017) tailored intervention above. CG: No intervention. |
Change in beef consumption from pre- to 8 weeks post-intervention, was measured with a retrospective food diary. | Adjusted for baseline consumption, there was no difference in the changes in beef consumption between the IG (M = 23.79) and the CG (M = −66.38, simple contrast: p = 0.65). | Undesired direction |
Implicitly highlighting animal suffering | |||||
Tian, 2016, study 1, [49]. | Sample size: IG: N = 105, CG: N = 166 Age: M = 23 (total study) Female: 79% (total study) Comparison: IG vs CG, RCT |
IG: Participants viewed a diagram displaying the meat products from various parts of a cow’s image and were asked to write a paragraph describing the figure. (e) CG: No intervention. |
Intention to eat beef was measured with 2 items ranging from 1 (low intention to eat beef) to 7 (high intention to eat beef), directly post-intervention | Intended beef consumption did not differ between CG and IG (Mean difference = −0.16, 95%CI = −0.6 to 0.27, p = 0.77). | Undesired direction |
Tian, 2016, study 1, [49]. | Sample size: IG: N = 123, CG: N = 166 Age: M = 23 (total study) Female: 79% (total study) Comparison: IG vs CG, RCT |
IG: Participants viewed a picture of a cow with a statement that the cow will be sent to another pasture the next day and were asked to write what would happen to the cow. CG: No intervention. |
Intention to eat beef was measured with 2 items ranging from 1 (low intention to eat beef) to 7 (high intention to eat beef), directly post-intervention | Intended beef consumption did not differ between CG and IG (mean difference = 0.11, 95%CI = −0.31 to 0.52, p = 0.91). | Desired direction |
Tian, 2016, study 1, [49]. | Sample size: IG: N = 124, CG: N = 166 Age: M = 23 (total study) Female: 79% (total study) Comparison: IG vs CG, RCT |
IG: Participants viewed a picture of a cow with a statement that the cow will be sent to the abattoir the next day and were asked to write what would happen to the cow. CG: No intervention. |
Intention to eat beef was measured with 2 items ranging from 1 (low intention to eat beef) to 7 (high intention to eat beef), directly post-intervention | Intended beef consumption did not differ between CG and IG (mean difference = 0.32, 95%CI = − 0.09 to 0.73, p = 0.19). | Desired direction |
Tian 2016, study 2, [49]. | Sample size: IG: N = 129, CG: N = 120 Age: M = 32 (total study) Female: 67% (total study) Comparison: IG vs CG, RCT |
IG: Participants were given a description of a beef dish together with an image of a cow to highlight its animal origin. CG: No intervention. |
Intention to eat beef was measured with 2 items ranging from 1 (low intention to eat beef) to 7 (high intention to eat beef), directly post-intervention | Intended beef consumption did not differ between CG and IG (Mean difference: − 0.05, 95%CI = − 0.47 to 0.37, p = 0.99). | Undesired direction |
(a) Throughout this paper IG and CG respectively refer to Intervention Group and Control Group. (b) Where possible, effect sizes were converted to Cohen’s d using an online tool. (c) Throughout the paper ‘≈’ indicates results that were read from figures or graphs. (d) No inference could be made for the comparison between IG1 and the CG. (e) This intervention was not described in the original paper as being developed to reduce meat consumption, but was included as it highlights the animal origin of meat products
Interventions’ impact on the demand for meat
Individual lifestyle counselling
Six studies (N = 2 RCT, N = 1 CT, N = 3 pre-post) evaluated the effectiveness of six lifestyle counselling interventions to reduce red and/or processed meat consumption [25–30]. All found evidence that lifestyle counselling led to [25, 26] or was associated with reduced meat consumption [27–30]. However, two studies measuring red and processed meat separately, only found significant reductions in the consumption of the latter [28, 29]. Lifestyle counselling interventions were delivered individually by a trained health professional through multiple face-to-face and/or phone sessions and lasted from 6 weeks [28, 29] to 1 year. All counselling interventions additionally comprised written supporting material, with two interventions tailoring this material to individuals [25, 26]. Lifestyle counselling promoted behavioural changes other than meat consumption, including one or more of fruit and vegetable consumption, multivitamin supplement usage, smoking cessation, physical activity, weight loss, and reduction in alcohol consumption. Most counselling interventions (N = 5) targeted individuals affected by, or at increased risk of chronic diseases [25, 27–30], and only one such intervention targeted healthy working-class individuals [26].
Self-monitoring interventions
Evidence from two RCTs suggested that two self-monitoring interventions reduced red [31] and processed meat consumption [32] during the intervention period and increased intentions to not exceed recommended levels of meat consumption over the week following the intervention. Both interventions lasted 1 week and comprised daily text-messages encouraging participants to self-monitor their red or processed meat consumption with the goal of not exceeding pre-specified recommendations. The intervention focussing on processed meat additionally encouraged participants to think about ‘the regret they could experience’ if they exceeded the recommendations [32].
Education on meat consumption and health
Five studies (N = 2 RCT, N = 1 CT, N = 1 CO, N = 1 Pre-post) evaluated seven interventions providing written information [33–36] or informational videos [37] about the health consequences of eating meat. Of these, five interventions led to [33, 37], or were associated with intended reductions in meat consumption directly post-intervention [35, 36]. Among these studies, one RCT found this effect to be sustained 1 week after the intervention [37]. Conversely, neither a cognitively framed nor an affectively framed message about the health consequences of eating meat were found to be associated with intended reductions in meat consumption directly post-intervention and/or 3 weeks later [34]. Two studies (N = 1 RCT, N = 1 CT) assessed the impact of two health focussed educational interventions on actual meat consumption but neither found evidence of an effect [34, 37]. Finally, one controlled trial including elderly people compared the effect of four interventions on the selection of meat in a virtual environment and found that messages on meat consumption and health more effectively reduced the selection of meat products when framed factually (i.e. describing the causal link between meat consumption and its consequences) rather than pre-factually (i.e. outlining hypothetical consequences of hypothetical present meat consumption) [38]. Conversely, messages on meat consumption and well-being more effectively reduced intended meat consumption when framed pre-factually rather than factually [38].
Education on meat consumption and the natural environment
Six studies (N = 3 RCT, N = 1 CO, N = 2 Pre-post) evaluated eight interventions providing written information about the environmental consequences of meat consumption [33, 35, 36, 39–41]. Six interventions led to [33, 39, 40] or were associated with increased intentions to eat less meat [35, 36] or with fewer meat products being selected in a simulated food choice experiment [40]. These interventions provided information in the form of paragraphs [35, 39], brief articles [36, 40], or websites [33] and two interventions also outlined practical strategies to aid reductions in meat intake [33, 35]. Conversely, one pre-post study found no evidence to suggest that two informational posters on livestock’s water footprint reduced meat purchases in a university canteen [41].
Education on meat consumption and animal welfare
Two studies (N = 1 crossover, N = 1 pre-post) evaluated two interventions providing written information about the animal welfare implications of consuming meat [35, 36]. Both were associated with significant reductions in intended meat consumption, and showed more promise than comparable messages on the impact of meat consumption on health or the environment. There was no study assessing the impact of such interventions on actual consumption, purchase, or selection of meat.
Education on meat consumption and social issues
Two studies (N = 1 CT, N = 1 pre-post) evaluated two interventions focussing on the social consequences or antecedents of eating meat [36, 42]. Providing information about the association between pursuing high meat diets and holding social dominance values was not found to be associated with reductions in intended consumption directly post-intervention or with actual meat consumption 3 weeks later [42]. Conversely, reading an article on the adverse social consequences of high meat diets was associated with increased intentions to eat less meat, though it offered less promise than similar articles on meat consumption and health, the environment, or animal welfare [36].
Education on multiple consequences of meat consumption
Nine studies (N = 4 RCT, N = 1 CT, N = 2 Pre-post, N = 2 retrospective evaluations) assessed 14 interventions providing written information about multiple consequences of meat consumption [34, 43–47]. These interventions provided printed or online information about two or more of health, environmental, animal welfare, social, personal appearance, and economic consequences of eating meat. The impact on actual meat consumption was evaluated in seven interventions: two were associated with lower meat intakes directly and 4 weeks post-intervention in pre-post studies [45], four were not found to effectively reduce meat intakes in RCTs [44], and there was insufficient data to make inferences about the effectiveness of the last intervention [34]. There was no evidence that any of five interventions reduced intended consumption directly post-intervention [43] or 3 weeks later [34], while a pre-post study suggested that providing information about the health and environmental consequences of meat consumption was associated with reduced meat selection in a virtual food choice experiment [46]. Actual food purchases were measured following two interventions providing the Meatless Monday toolkit to the US government/corporate accounts or the US healthcare accounts of a large food service company. Only the latter intervention was associated with significant declines in meat purchases [47]. In summary, four of 14 interventions providing written information about multiple consequences of eating meat were associated with significant reductions in meat consumption, purchases or selection, though none of these interventions was assessed in a RCT.
Tailored education
Four RCTs evaluated ten tailored educational interventions, none of which was found to effectively reduce actual [44] or intended meat consumption [43]. Specifically, providing information that was matched to participants’ stage of change (for an account on the stages of change model refer to [48]) for eating less beef was not found to reduce beef consumption in two RCTs, and there was no robust evidence that this tailored intervention outperformed its stage-mismatched equivalent [44]. Two RCTs found no evidence that messages tailored to participants’ most valued consequence of meat consumption, and/or to participants’ self-schema (being responsible, or adventurous, or compassionate, or logical), and/or participants’ personal levels of meat intakes reduced their intended meat consumption [43].
Interventions implicitly highlighting animal suffering
Two RCTs found no evidence that any of four interventions implicitly highlighting animal suffering reduced intended meat consumption [49]. All such interventions employed a combination of visual and written material leading recipients to reflect upon the animal suffering involved in the production of meat [49]. For example, participants were shown a picture of a cow accompanied by the statement that the cow will be send to a slaughterhouse, and were asked to think about what would happen to the animal. None of the aforementioned studies assessed actual meat consumption or purchases.
Qualitative comparative analysis
Fifty-five comparisons were included in QCA. Four configurations of intervention characteristics were consistently associated with reductions in actual or intended meat consumption in real or virtual environments among two or more intervention evaluations (Table 4), while six configurations were consistently not found to be associated with these outcomes (Table 5). QCA supported the findings of the narrative synthesis suggesting that the approaches that were consistently associated with reductions in actual meat consumption were self-monitoring and lifestyle counselling interventions. Non-tailored information provision about the detrimental health or environmental consequences of eating meat was consistently associated with reductions in intended consumption or purchases/selection of meat in virtual environments but was not found to be associated with changes in actual behaviour. Tailored and non-tailored interventions elaborating on several consequences of eating less meat and interventions implicitly highlighting animal suffering were not found to be associated with actual or intended consumption, purchase, or selection of meat in real or virtual environment.
Table 4.
Non-tailored environmental information Raw coverage: 25%, Internal consistency: 100% | |
Outcome: | (1) Reduction in intended consumption or purchase/selection of meat in virtual environments |
In the presence of: | (2) Non-tailored (3) information about environmental issues (4) targeting healthy individuals |
In the absence of: | Information about (5) health, (6) socio-economic, (7) animal welfare issues, (8) multiple consequences of eating meat, and (9) implicitly highlighting animal suffering, (10) self-monitoring, (11) goal-setting, and (12) lifestyle counselling |
Regardless of: | (13) Provision of practical strategies to eat less meat |
Non-tailored health information with practical strategies to eat less meat Raw coverage: 8%, Internal consistency: 100% | |
Outcome: | (1) Reduction in intended consumption or purchase/selection of meat in virtual environments |
In the presence of: | (2) Non-tailored (3) information about health issues (4) with practical strategies to eat less meat (5) targeting healthy individuals |
In the absence of: | Information about (6) environmental, (7) socio-economic, (8) animal welfare issues, (9) multiple consequences of eating meat, and (10) implicitly highlighting animal suffering, (11) self-monitoring, (12) goal-setting, and (13) lifestyle counselling |
Self-monitoring and goal-setting interventions Raw coverage: 8%, Internal consistency: 100% | |
Outcome: | (1) Reduction in actual meat consumption, purchase, or selection |
In the presence of: | (2) Non-tailored (3) self-monitoring and (4) goal-setting interventions (5) targeting healthy individuals |
In the absence of: | Information about (6) health (7) environmental, (8) socio-economic, (9) animal welfare issues, (10) multiple consequences of eating meat, and (11) implicitly highlighting animal suffering, (12) practical strategies to eat less meat, and (13) lifestyle counselling |
Lifestyle-counselling for people with, or at increased risk of ill-health Raw coverage: 17%, Internal consistency: 100% | |
Outcome: | (1) Reduction in actual meat consumption, purchase, or selection |
In the presence of: | (2) Tailored (3) lifestyle counselling (4) targeting people with ill health or at increased risk thereof, and including (5) information on health, (6) self-monitoring, (7) goal-setting, and (8) practical strategies to eat less meat |
In the absence of: | Information about (9) environmental, (10) animal welfare, (11) socio-economic issues, (12) multiple consequences of eating meat, and (13) implicitly highlighting animal suffering |
Configurations of intervention components associated with reductions in meat consumption, purchase, or selection. The overall solution covers 58% of the 24 interventions included in QCA and associated with reductions in meat consumption, purchase, or selection in all comparisons in which these configurations were evaluated. Raw coverage refers to the percentage of interventions associated with reductions in meat consumption, purchase, or selection covered by an intervention configuration. Raw consistency refers to the percentage of interventions within a configuration being associated with the aforementioned outcomes
Table 5.
Tailored information provision Raw coverage: 26%, Internal consistency: 100% | |
Outcome: | (1) Reduction in intended consumption or purchase/selection of meat in virtual environments |
In the presence of: | (2) Tailored information about (3) one or more of (4) environmental, (5) health, (6) animal welfare, or (7) socio-economic issues, (8) targeting healthy individuals |
In the absence of: | (9) Implicitly highlighting animal suffering, (10) self-monitoring, (11) goal-setting, (12) practical strategies to eat less meat, and (13) lifestyle counselling |
Information about multiple issues Raw coverage: 19%, Internal consistency: 100% | |
Outcome: | (1) Reduction in intended consumption or purchase/selection of meat in virtual environments |
In the presence of: | Information about (2) two or more of (3) health, (4) environmental, (5) animal welfare, and (6) socio-economic issues (7) targeting healthy individuals |
In the absence of: | (8) Practical strategies to eat less meat, (9) implicitly highlighting animal suffering, (10) self-monitoring, (11) goal-setting, and (12) lifestyle counselling |
Regardless of: | (13) Tailoring |
Information about multiple issues and practical strategies Raw coverage: 19%, Internal consistency: 100% | |
Outcome: | (1) Reduction in actual meat consumption, purchase, or selection |
In the presence of: | Information about (2) two or more of (3) health, (4) environmental, and (5) socio-economic issues (6) targeting healthy individuals, and (7) practical strategies to eat less meat |
In the absence of: | Information about (8) animal welfare, (9) implicitly highlighting animal suffering, (10) self-monitoring, (11) goal-setting, and (12) lifestyle counselling |
Regardless of: | (13) Tailoring |
Interventions implicitly highlighting animal suffering Raw coverage: 13%, Internal consistency: 100% | |
Outcome: | (1) Reduction in intended consumption or purchase/selection of meat in virtual environments |
In the presence of: | (2) Non-tailored interventions (3) implicitly highlighting animal suffering, (4) among healthy individuals |
In the absence of: | Information about (5) environmental, (6) health, (7) socio-economic, (8) animal welfare issues, (9) multiple consequences of eating meat, as well as (10) self-monitoring, (11) goal-setting, (12) practical strategies to eat less meat, and (13) lifestyle counselling. |
Non-tailored education on the environment, when actual behaviour is the outcome Raw coverage: 6%, Internal consistency: 100% | |
Outcome: | (1) Reduction in actual consumption, purchase, or selection of meat |
In the presence of: | (2) Non-tailored (3) information about environmental issues (4) targeting healthy individuals |
In the absence of: | Information about (5) health, (6) socio-economic, (7) animal welfare, (8) or multiple issues, as well as (9) implicitly highlighting animal suffering, (10) self-monitoring, (11) goal-setting, (12) practical strategies to eat less meat, and (13) lifestyle counselling. |
Non-tailored education on health, when actual behaviour is the outcome Raw coverage: 6%, Internal consistency: 100% | |
Outcome: | (1) Reduction in actual consumption, purchase, or selection of meat |
In the presence of: | (2) Non-tailored (3) information about health issues (4) targeting healthy individuals |
In the absence of: | Information about (5) environmental, (6) socio-economic, (7) animal welfare, (8) or multiple issues, as well as (9) implicitly highlighting animal suffering, (10) self-monitoring, (11) goal-setting, (12) practical strategies to eat less meat, and (13) lifestyle counselling. |
Configurations of intervention components consistently not found to be associated with reductions in meat consumption, purchase, or selection. The overall solution covers 84% of the 31 interventions included in QCA and not found to be associated with reductions in meat consumption, purchase, or selection in all comparisons in which these configurations were evaluated. Raw coverage refers to the percentage of interventions not found to be associated with reductions in meat consumption, purchase, or selection covered by an intervention configuration. Raw consistency refers to the percentage of interventions within a configuration not found to be associated with the aforementioned outcomes
Secondary psychosocial outcomes
Self-monitoring interventions led to more favourable instrumental attitudes (beliefs about the impact of eating meat) but not affective attitudes (feeling about the hedonic aspects of eating meat) towards eating less meat [31, 32]. One self-monitoring intervention additionally enhanced perceived behavioural control [31], while there was no evidence that either influenced subjective social norms [31, 32]. In all five interventions evaluating the impact of providing information about the health consequences of eating meat and measuring attitudes, there was evidence that the intervention led to or was associated with more favourable attitudes towards eating less meat [33–35, 37]. Two out of four interventions providing information on the environmental impact of meat consumption led to or were associated with more favourable attitudes towards eating less meat [33, 35, 39]. One non-tailored educational intervention focussing on animal welfare [35] and another focussing on social antecedents of meat consumption were associated with less favourable attitudes towards meat consumption [42]. Conversely, none of the eight tailored educational interventions or five non-tailored interventions focussing on multiple consequences of meat consumption were found to influence attitudes [34, 43].
Secondary biological outcomes
The only biomarker of health on which studies reported was weight. Of four studies (N = 1 CT, N = 3 pre-post) evaluating the impact of lifestyle counselling interventions on weight [27–30] only one intervention, which explicitly focussed on weight management, was associated with a significant reduction in BMI in a pre-post comparison [29].
Discussion
Evidence from experimental intervention studies suggests that some interventions targeting conscious determinants of human behaviour could contribute towards reducing the demand for meat. Six lifestyle counselling interventions designed to reduce red and processed meat consumption led to [25, 26] or were associated with reduced meat consumption [27–30]. Evidence from RCTs suggested that two self-monitoring interventions reduced red [31] or processed meat consumption [32] during the intervention period and enhanced intentions to not exceed recommended levels of consumption. Providing non-tailored information on the health, or environmental, or socio-political, or animal welfare consequences of eating meat was associated with reduced intended consumption and virtual selection of meat in 16 out of 17 interventions [33–37, 39–42]. However, there was no evidence that any of six such interventions influenced actual behaviour [34, 37, 41, 42]. Only four of 14 interventions providing information about multiple consequences of eating meat were associated with reductions in meat consumption, purchases, or selection [34, 43–47]. None of the ten tailored educational interventions or of the four interventions implicitly highlighting animal welfare issues was found to reduce actual [44] or intended meat consumption [43, 49]. Participants’ attitudes towards eating (less) meat generally mirrored their intentions to do so. There was no robust evidence pertaining to any of the other secondary psychosocial outcomes or biomarkers of health risk.
Strengths and limitations
In this systematic review we employed gold standard methods striving to achieve robust and unbiased results. The scope of our review was kept broad to allow capturing a wide range of intervention approaches, which is important to inform this novel field of research. However, the results should be interpreted in the context of limitations of this review and of the studies included, most of which were of medium or low methodological quality. Having employed extensive methods to identify literature beyond the databases searches, over 40% of the papers included were unpublished records. This potentially prevented publication bias affecting the review unduly, but it also increased the proportion of studies with methodological limitations. Given this field is developing, we elected to include non-randomised studies, which increases the risk of bias in the findings. Additionally, the main outcome measures of this review presented challenges, with self-reported measures of consumption being prone to bias [50], food selection in virtual settings potentially lacking external validity [51, 52], and behavioural intentions being only moderately related to future behaviour [53]. Most studies only measured outcomes during or shortly after the intervention, so conclusions cannot be drawn on lasting effects. Many interventions were either underpowered, making potentially effective interventions difficult to detect, or were evaluated in non-randomised designs, precluding direct causal inference of effectiveness. Future studies should employ well-powered RCTs with longer follow-up to allow for causal inferences to be drawn on the interventions’ effectiveness and to better understand their longer term impact. The studies reviewed included predominantly white and well-educated volunteers, limiting the generalizability of the data to other population groups. Particular caution should be exercised when interpreting results of lifestyle counselling, as our search algorithm might have failed to identify all such interventions. Titles and abstracts of these studies often referred to ‘multiple health behaviours’ as the intervention target, and it is possible that papers specifically mentioning ‘meat consumption’, and therefore identified by our search algorithm, were more likely to be those finding significant results for this outcome. None of the interventions directly targeted gender-related barriers to reduce the demand for meat. Considering the importance of gender as a determinant of meat consumption, future studies should explore whether interventions targeting gender-related barriers can effectively reduce the demand for meat. In the absence of a taxonomy to classify different interventions targeting conscious determinants of human behaviour we grouped interventions according to some of their key behavioural strategies and implementation features. Our synthesis was partly based on an exploratory crisp-set QCA, which represents a sophisticated technique to descriptively identify patterns within data, but not to infer causality of effects. In particular, combinations associated with no evidence of effectiveness should not be thought as ‘ineffective’, as ‘absence of evidence does not equal evidence of absence’ [54]. Similarly, since we did not exclusively include RCTs, combinations associated with effectiveness do not necessarily support an underlying causal mechanism. It must also be considered that QCA did not differentiate between studies of different quality and size. Finally, while the quality assessment tool employed in this review allowed assessing different study designs, caution must be exercised when comparing quality scores between different study designs.
Findings in the context of existing evidence
The results of this review were consistent with past research on behaviour change interventions targeting other health behaviours. Lifestyle counselling and self-monitoring interventions have previously emerged as promising approaches to change different eating behaviours [55–57]. However, the resources needed to implement lifestyle counselling pose a barrier to its scalability. Moreover, existing evidence on lifestyle counselling targeting the general population rather than people suffering from ill-health is less clear [55], suggesting that our findings on lifestyle counselling may not apply to the general population. Educational interventions providing rational reasons for eating less meat may have underpinned the observed changes in conscious intentions. Nevertheless, unconscious psychosocial and environmental cues that influence behaviour in real-life settings [20, 58] may have prevented these effects from translating into actual behaviour outside experimental settings. Interventions targeting unconscious determinants of human behaviour [20, 58, 59] may therefore play an important role to help overcome this intention-behaviour gap. We plan to review the effectiveness of these approaches elsewhere [60]. The non-significant findings on tailored education are in contrast with the literature highlighting the importance of tailoring information [61, 62]. Nevertheless our findings were based on only two papers that found no evidence of effectiveness for any of the interventions they tested, regardless of tailoring [43, 44]. It is therefore possible, that intervention characteristics other than tailoring or methodological limitations of the aforementioned studies contributed to these non-significant results. However, it is also possible that individuals do not substantially benefit from receiving information about issues that they already value as important consequences of meat consumption, as these arguments might have naturally exhausted their potential for prompting behavioural change. Finally, while providing information was not found to directly influence behaviour, future research should explore whether this approach might contribute towards reducing population-wide demand for meat in other ways. For example, providing information on the benefits of eating less meat might increase the public’s acceptability for more structural interventions to reduce meat consumption.
Conclusion
This review is the first systematic synthesis of evidence about the effectiveness of interventions targeting conscious determinants of human behaviour to reduce the demand for meat. Some interventions targeting conscious determinants of human behaviour have the potential to reduce the demand for meat. In particular, self-monitoring and individual lifestyle counselling interventions showed promise in reducing actual consumption of meat. Education about health, environmental, socio-political, or animal welfare consequences of eating meat can reduce intended meat consumption and selection of meat in virtual environments, but there was little evidence on whether this approach influenced actual behaviour and the few studies examining this found no evidence that it did. Interventions providing information on several consequences of meat consumption, those implicitly highlighting animal suffering in the context of meat production or consumption, and those providing tailored information offered less promise. While the impact of interventions targeting conscious determinants of human behaviour was modest, if delivered at scale these interventions could contribute towards reducing the demand for meat at population-level.
Additional file
Acknowledgements
This research is part of the Wellcome Trust, Our Planet Our Health programme (Livestock, Environment and People - LEAP), award number 205212/Z/16/Z. We thank Nia Roberts for helping with designing and conducting the searches. We thank Brian Cook for providing useful feedback on previous versions of this paper. We thank the authors who sent us further information about their studies.
Funding
FB’s time on this project is funded by the Medical Research Council (MRC), Green Templeton College Oxford, and the National Institute for Health Research (NIHR) School for Primary Care Research (SPCR). CD’s time on this project is funded by the Wellcome Trust (LEAP – Livestock Environment And People). EG’s time on this project is funded by the Natural Environment Research Council (NERC). PA and SJ are supported by the NIHR Oxford Biomedical Research Centre (BRC) and the Collaboration for Leadership in Applied Health Research and Care Oxford at Oxford Health NHS Foundation Trust (CLAHRC).
Availability of data and materials
The datasets used and/or analysed during the current study are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.
Abbreviations
- BMI
Body mass index
- CT
Controlled trial
- QCA
Qualitative Comparative Analysis
- RCT
Randomised controlled trial
Authors’ contributions
All authors designed research; FB, CD, and EG conducted research; FB, SAJ, and PA analysed data; FB led the writing of the paper; FB had primary responsibility for the final content. All authors read, edited and approved the final manuscript.
Ethics approval and consent to participate
N/A.
Consent for publication
N/A.
Competing interests
The authors declare that they have no competing interests.
Publisher’s Note
Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Contributor Information
Filippo Bianchi, Phone: 00447507866672, Email: filippo.bianchi@phc.ox.ac.uk.
Claudia Dorsel, Email: claudia.dorsel@yahoo.de.
Emma Garnett, Email: eg334@cam.ac.uk.
Paul Aveyard, Email: paul.aveyard@phc.ox.ac.uk.
Susan A Jebb, Email: susan.jebb@phc.ox.ac.uk.
References
- 1.Bechthold A, Boeing H, Schwedhelm C, Hoffmann G, Knüppel S, Iqbal K, et al. Food groups and risk of coronary heart disease, stroke and heart failure: a systematic review and dose-response meta-analysis of prospective studies. Crit Rev Food Sci Nutr. 2017:1–20 Available from: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/29039970. Cited 9 Nov 2017. [DOI] [PubMed]
- 2.Micha R, Wallace SK, Mozaffarian D. Red and processed meat consumption and risk of incident coronary heart disease, stroke, and diabetes mellitus: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Circulation. 2010;121(21):2271–2283. doi: 10.1161/CIRCULATIONAHA.109.924977. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Micha R, Michas G, Mozaffarian D. Unprocessed red and processed meats and risk of coronary artery disease and type 2 diabetes – an updated review of the evidence. Curr Atheroscler Rep. 2012;14(6):515–524. doi: 10.1007/s11883-012-0282-8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Feskens EJM, Sluik D, van Woudenbergh GJ. Meat consumption, diabetes, and its complications. Curr Diab Rep. 2013;13(2):298–306. doi: 10.1007/s11892-013-0365-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Barnard N, Levin S, Trapp C. Meat consumption as a risk factor for type 2 diabetes. Nutrients. 2014;6(2):897–910. doi: 10.3390/nu6020897. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Bouvard V, Loomis D, Guyton KZ, Grosse Y, El Ghissassi F, Benbrahim-Tallaa L, et al. Carcinogenicity of consumption of red and processed meat. Lancet Oncol. 2015;16(16):1599–1600. doi: 10.1016/S1470-2045(15)00444-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Chan DSM, Lau R, Aune D, Vieira R, Greenwood DC, Kampman E, et al. Red and processed meat and colorectal cancer incidence: meta-analysis of prospective studies. PLoS One. 2011;6(6):e20456. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0020456. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Parkin DM, Boyd L, Walker LC. 16. The fraction of cancer attributable to lifestyle and environmental factors in the UK in 2010. Br J Cancer. 2011;105(Suppl 2):S77–S81. doi: 10.1038/bjc.2011.489. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Steinfeld H, Gerber P, Wassenaar T, Castel V, Rosales M, De Haan C. Livestock’s long shadow. FAO, Rome. 2006;2006. http://www.fao.org/3/a-a0701e.pdf.
- 10.Tilman David, Clark Michael. Global diets link environmental sustainability and human health. Nature. 2014;515(7528):518–522. doi: 10.1038/nature13959. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Pimentel D, Pimentel M. Sustainability of meat-based and plant-based diets and the environment. Am J Clin Nutr. 2003;78(3 Suppl):660S–663S. doi: 10.1093/ajcn/78.3.660S. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Watts N, Amann M, Ayeb-Karlsson S, Belesova K, Bouley T, Boykoff M, et al. The Lancet Countdown on health and climate change: from 25 years of inaction to a global transformation for public health. Lancet. 2017; Elsevier. Available from: https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0140673617324649?_rdoc=1&_fmt=high&_origin=gateway&_docanchor=&md5=b8429449ccfc9c30159a5f9aeaa92ffb. Cited 25 Jan 2018. [DOI] [PubMed]
- 13.Mcmichael AJ, Campbell-Lendrum DH, Corvalán CF, Ebi KL, Githeko AK, Scheraga JD, et al. Climate change and human health. Risks and responses. 2003. [Google Scholar]
- 14.Economou V, Gousia P. Agriculture and food animals as a source of antimicrobial-resistant bacteria. Infect Drug Resist. 2015;8:49–61. doi: 10.2147/IDR.S55778. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Garnett T, Mathewson S, Angelides P, Borthwick F. Policies and actions to shift eating patterns: what works? 2015. [Google Scholar]
- 16.Bailey R, Froggatt A, Wellesley L. Livestock–climate change’s forgotten sector. 2014. Available from: https://www.chathamhouse.org/publication/livestock-climate-change-forgotten-sector-global-public-opinion-meat-and-dairy.
- 17.Wellesley L, Happer C, Froggatt A. Chatham house report changing climate, changing diets pathways to lower meat consumption. 2015 Available from: https://www.chathamhouse.org/publication/changing-climate-changing-diets.
- 18.Diepeveen S, Ling T, Suhrcke M, Roland M, Marteau TM. Public acceptability of government intervention to change health-related behaviours: a systematic review and narrative synthesis. BMC Public Health. 2013;13(1):756. doi: 10.1186/1471-2458-13-756. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Jung JY, Mellers BA. American attitudes toward nudges. Judgm Decis Mak. 2016;11(1):62–75 Society for Judgment and Decision Making. Available from: http://journal.sjdm.org/15/15824a/jdm15824a.pdf.
- 20.Marteau TM. Towards environmentally sustainable human behaviour: targeting non-conscious and conscious processes for effective and acceptable policies. Philos Trans R Soc A Math Eng Sci. 2017;375(2095):20160371. doi: 10.1098/rsta.2016.0371. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Bianchi F, Garnett E, Dorsel C, Paul A, Jebb S. Effectiveness of educational and motivational interventions to reduce the consumption, purchase, or selection of meat products: protocol for a systematic review with narrative synthesis. 2017. [Google Scholar]
- 22.Stansfield C, Dickson K, Bangpan M. Exploring issues in the conduct of website searching and other online sources for systematic reviews: how can we be systematic? Syst Rev. 2016;5(1):191. doi: 10.1186/s13643-016-0371-9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Armijo-Olivo S, Stiles CR, Hagen NA, Biondo PD, Cummings GG. Assessment of study quality for systematic reviews: a comparison of the Cochrane Collaboration Risk of Bias Tool and the Effective Public Health Practice Project Quality Assessment Tool: methodological research. J Eval Clin Pract. 2012;18(1):12–8 Available from: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/20698919. Cited 25 Feb 2015. [DOI] [PubMed]
- 24.Thomas J, O’Mara-Eves A, Brunton G. Using qualitative comparative analysis (QCA) in systematic reviews of complex interventions: a worked example. Syst Rev. 2014;3(1):67. doi: 10.1186/2046-4053-3-67. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Emmons KM, McBride CM, Puleo E, Pollak KI, Clipp E, Kuntz K, et al. Project PREVENT: a randomized trial to reduce multiple behavioral risk factors for colon cancer. Cancer Epidemiol Biomarkers Prev. 2005;14(6):1453–1459. doi: 10.1158/1055-9965.EPI-04-0620. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Emmons KM, Stoddard AM, Fletcher R, Gutheil C, Suarez EG, Lobb R, et al. Cancer prevention among working class, multiethnic adults: results of the healthy directions–health centers study. Am J Public Health. 2005;95(7):1200–1205. doi: 10.2105/AJPH.2004.038695. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Schiavon CC, Vieira FGK, Ceccatto V, de Liz S, Cardoso AL, Sabel C, et al. Nutrition education intervention for women with breast cancer: effect on nutritional factors and oxidative stress. J Nutr Educ Behav. 2015;47(1):2–9. doi: 10.1016/j.jneb.2014.09.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Hawkes AL, Gollschewski S, Lynch BM, Chambers S. A telephone-delivered lifestyle intervention for colorectal cancer survivors ‘CanChange’: a pilot study. Psychooncology. 2009;18(4):449–455. doi: 10.1002/pon.1527. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Hawkes AL, Patrao TA, Green A, Aitken JF. CanPrevent: a telephone-delivered intervention to reduce multiple behavioural risk factors for colorectal cancer. BMC Cancer. 2012;12(1):560. doi: 10.1186/1471-2407-12-560. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Grimmett C, Simon A, Lawson V, Wardle J. Diet and physical activity intervention in colorectal cancer survivors: a feasibility study. Eur J Oncol Nurs. 2015;19(1):1–6. doi: 10.1016/j.ejon.2014.08.006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Carfora V, Caso D, Conner M. Correlational study and randomised controlled trial for understanding and changing red meat consumption: the role of eating identities. Soc Sci Med. 2017;175:244–252. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2017.01.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Carfora V, Caso D, Conner M. Randomised controlled trial of a text messaging intervention for reducing processed meat consumption: the mediating roles of anticipated regret and intention. Appetite. 2017;117:152–160. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2017.06.025. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Fehrenbach KS. The health and environmental impacts of meat consumption: using the extended parallel process model to persuade college students to eat less meat. 2013. [Google Scholar]
- 34.Berndsen M, van der Pligt J. Risks of meat: the relative impact of cognitive, affective and moral concerns. Appetite. 2005;44(2):195–205. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2004.10.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Scrimgeour LR. Digital Persuasion: Effects of web-based information and beliefs on meat consumption attitudes and intentions. Psychology. 2012; University of Canterbury. Available from: https://ir.canterbury.ac.nz/handle/10092/7427. Cited 28 Nov 2017.
- 36.Cordts A, Nitzko S, Spiller A, Cordts A, Nitzko S, Spiller A. Consumer response to negative information on meat consumption in Germany. Int Food Agribus Manag Rev. 2014;17(A) International Food and Agribusiness Management Association (IFAMA). Available from: https://www.ifama.org/resources/Documents/v17ia/Cordts-Spiller-Nitzko.pdf.
- 37.Fehrenbach KS. Designing messages to reduce meat consumption: a test of the extended parallel process model: Arizona State University; 2015. Available from: https://repository.asu.edu/items/34852. Cited 28 Nov 2017
- 38.Bertolotti M, Chirchiglia G, Catellani P. Promoting change in meat consumption among the elderly: factual and prefactual framing of health and well-being. Appetite. 2016;106:37–47. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2016.02.150. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Graham T, Abrahamse W. Communicating the climate impacts of meat consumption: the effect of values and message framing. Glob Environ Chang. 2017;44:98–108. doi: 10.1016/j.gloenvcha.2017.03.004. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Vibhuti P. Rainforest, reef, and our appetite for beef: communication for sustainable behaviour change. University of Otago; 2017. Available from: https://ourarchive.otago.ac.nz/handle/10523/7581. Cited 28 Nov 2017.
- 41.Godfrey DM. Communicating sustainable food: connecting scientific information to consumer action: University of Calgary; 2014. Available from: http://theses.ucalgary.ca/handle/11023/1598. Cited 28 Nov 2017
- 42.Allen MW, Baines S. Manipulating the symbolic meaning of meat to encourage greater acceptance of fruits and vegetables and less proclivity for red and white meat. Appetite. 2002;38(2):118–130. doi: 10.1006/appe.2001.0474. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Schnabelrauch Arndt CA. Tailoring feedback and messages to encourage meat consumption reduction: Kansas State University; 2016. Available from: http://krex.k-state.edu/dspace/handle/2097/32159. Cited 28 Nov 2017
- 44.Klöckner CA, Ofstad SP. Tailored information helps people progress towards reducing their beef consumption. J Environ Psychol. 2017;50:24–36. doi: 10.1016/j.jenvp.2017.01.006. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Loy LS, Wieber F, Gollwitzer PM, Oettingen G. Supporting sustainable food consumption: mental contrasting with implementation intentions (MCII) aligns intentions and behavior. Front Psychol. 2016;7:607. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00607. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Marette S, Millet G. Can information about health and environment beef up the demand for meat alternatives? 2016. [Google Scholar]
- 47.Leidig R. Sodexo meatless monday survey results. 2012. [Google Scholar]
- 48.Prochaska JO, Velicer WF. The transtheoretical model of health behavior change. Am J Heal Promot. 1997;12(1):38–48. doi: 10.4278/0890-1171-12.1.38. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Tian Q, Hilton D, Becker M. Confronting the meat paradox in different cultural contexts: reactions among Chinese and French participants. Appetite. 2016;96:187–194. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2015.09.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Schoeller DA, Thomas D, Archer E, Heymsfield SB, Blair SN, Goran MI, et al. Self-report–based estimates of energy intake offer an inadequate basis for scientific conclusions. Am J Clin Nutr. 2013;97(6):1413–1415. doi: 10.3945/ajcn.113.062125. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Forwood SE, Ahern AL, Marteau TM, Jebb SA. Offering within-category food swaps to reduce energy density of food purchases: a study using an experimental online supermarket. 2015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Hartmann-Boyce Jamie, Bianchi Filippo, Piernas Carmen, Riches Sarah Payne, Frie Kerstin, Nourse Rebecca, Jebb Susan A. Grocery store interventions to change food purchasing behaviors: a systematic review of randomized controlled trials. The American Journal of Clinical Nutrition. 2018;107(6):1004–1016. doi: 10.1093/ajcn/nqy045. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.French DP, Cooke R, Mclean N, Williams M, Sutton S. What Do People Think about When They Answer Theory of Planned Behaviour Questionnaires? J Health Psychol. 2007;12(4):672–687. doi: 10.1177/1359105307078174. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Jaykaran, Saxena D, Yadav P, Kantharia ND. Nonsignificant P values cannot prove null hypothesis: absence of evidence is not evidence of absence. J Pharm Bioallied Sci. 2011;3(3):465–466. doi: 10.4103/0975-7406.84470. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Pomerleau J, Lock K, Knai C, McKee M. Interventions designed to increase adult fruit and vegetable intake can be effective: a systematic review of the literature. J Nutr. 2005;135(10):2486–2495. doi: 10.1093/jn/135.10.2486. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56.Semper HM, Povey R, Clark-Carter D. A systematic review of the effectiveness of smartphone applications that encourage dietary self-regulatory strategies for weight loss in overweight and obese adults. Obes Rev. 2016;17(9):895–906. doi: 10.1111/obr.12428. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57.Flores Mateo G, Granado-Font E, Ferré-Grau C, Montaña-Carreras X. Mobile phone apps to promote weight loss and increase physical activity: a systematic review and meta-analysis. J Med Internet Res. 2015;17(11):e253. doi: 10.2196/jmir.4836. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58.Hollands GJ, Marteau TM, Fletcher PC. Non-conscious processes in changing health-related behaviour: a conceptual analysis and framework. Health Psychol Rev. 2016;10(4):381–394. doi: 10.1080/17437199.2015.1138093. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59.Marteau TM, Hollands GJ, Fletcher PC. Changing human behavior to prevent disease: the importance of targeting automatic processes. Science. 2012;337(6101):1492–1495. doi: 10.1126/science.1226918. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 60.Bianchi Filippo, Garnett Emma, Dorsel Claudia, Aveyard Paul, Jebb Susan A. Restructuring physical micro-environments to reduce the demand for meat: a systematic review and qualitative comparative analysis. The Lancet Planetary Health. 2018;2(9):e384–e397. doi: 10.1016/S2542-5196(18)30188-8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 61.Noar SM, Benac CN, Harris MS. Does tailoring matter? Meta-analytic review of tailored print health behavior change interventions. Psychol Bull. 2007;133(4):673–693. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.133.4.673. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 62.Whatnall MC, Patterson AJ, Ashton LM, Hutchesson MJ. Effectiveness of brief nutrition interventions on dietary behaviours in adults: a systematic review. Appetite. 2018;120:335–347. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2017.09.017. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Supplementary Materials
Data Availability Statement
The datasets used and/or analysed during the current study are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.