Skip to main content
Scientific Reports logoLink to Scientific Reports
. 2018 Oct 19;8:15525. doi: 10.1038/s41598-018-33942-6

Internal cueing improves gait more than external cueing in healthy adults and people with Parkinson disease

Elinor C Harrison 1, Adam P Horin 1, Gammon M Earhart 1,2,3,
PMCID: PMC6195608  PMID: 30341367

Abstract

Walking can be challenging for aging individuals and people with neurological disorders such as Parkinson disease (PD). Gait impairment characterized by reduced speed and higher variability destabilizes gait and increases the risk of falls. External auditory cueing provides an effective strategy to improve gait, as matching footfalls to rhythms typically increases gait speed and elicits larger steps, but the need to synchronize to an outside source often has a detrimental effect on gait variability. Internal cueing in the form of singing may provide an alternative to conventional gait therapy. In the present study, we compare the effects of internal and external cueing techniques on forward and backward walking for both people with PD and healthy controls. Results indicate that internal cueing was associated with improvements in gait velocity, cadence, and stride length in the backward direction, and reduced variability in both forward and backward walking. In comparison, external cueing was associated with minimal improvement in gait characteristics and a decline in gait stability. People with gait impairment due to aging or neurological decline may benefit more from internal cueing techniques such as singing as compared to external cueing techniques.

Introduction

Age-related gait disorders affect a third of the population over 70 years of age1 and cause people to walk slower with less stability. Reduced gait speed in older adults is a sensitive marker of overall health and can predict adverse events, such as falls, and future disability2,3. Two-thirds of gait disorders are related to neurological decline4 and are exacerbated in movement disorders such as Parkinson disease (PD). PD is characterized by bradykinesia, rigidity, and postural instability, all of which contribute to walking difficulty5. Compared to age-matched controls, people with PD experience accelerated gait decline as evidenced by reductions in speed, step frequency, and step length. In addition to these basic gait deficits, people with PD exhibit substantial increases in gait variability6 which may reflect diminished balance control7 and a disruption of internal timing mechanisms within the brain. Gait variability is a strong indicator of overall stability810, worsens with disease severity, and may lead to a loss of mobility and independence11,12. When moving in the backward direction, as is common in everyday life, gait impairment is more pronounced and more likely to contribute to fall risk1315. Hence, a major focus of gait therapy is to reduce gait variability in order to stabilize walking and reduce the risk of falls.

External auditory cueing through music is widely established as an effective tool to normalize gait disturbance1618. For people with PD, matching one’s footfalls to the beat of a song can restore gait to levels closer to those of healthy controls16,17,19,20. Rhythmic cues allow predictable mapping of motor output onto stable auditory templates via a process called “entrainment” that enables people to anticipate the next beat and step on it. Musical cues are superior to other types of cues at increasing velocity and stride length19 though they are more effective after a period of training21 and for those with more severe gait impairment22.

In spite of evidence supporting the efficacy of rhythmic auditory cues for improving certain gait characteristics17,2326, recent research suggests that synchronizing footfalls to external rhythmic cues detrimentally effects gait variability27. External cues require adjusting every step in order to synchronize, and this increased cognitive load may have the undesirable effect of increasing gait variability, particularly for older adults or neurological patients who are more likely to experience cognitive decline28. Internal cueing through singing, on the other hand, eliminates the need to entrain to an external source. Instead, a rhythm generated and produced via the vocal system is then adopted by the locomotor system to produce rhythmic motion of the legs. This method may allow for greater coupling between systems, potentially reducing attentional load and enhancing stability.

Singing is already used for vocal rehabilitation in PD because, in spite of speech degradation, singing ability is preserved2931. Evidence also suggests that the benefits of singing may extend beyond speech to improvements in motor control32 as singing may engage a vocal sensorimotor loop involving both perceptual and motor planning components33. For example, self-generated vocal cues enhance upper extremity movement in people with PD, resulting in faster and smoother reaching movements34. Vocalizations are also likely to enhance lower body movement, as people with PD report using singing to aid with gait initiation and maintenance, particularly during challenging gait situations such as moving backwards and turning35. Despite abundant evidence supporting the use of singing to improve walking in aging and neurologic populations, previous research is mostly limited to the use of external cueing for gait rehabilitation.

In this study, we examined the effects of internal cueing, in the form of singing, versus external cueing, in the form of listening to music, on gait in people with and without PD. We addressed both forward walking, which engages automatic locomotor circuits, and backward walking, which represents a more challenging gait situation. We hypothesized that both external and internal musical cueing would improve backward walking more than forward walking in all our participants, and that internal cueing would be more effective at reducing gait variability over external cueing. We also expected to see the greatest benefit from cueing in people with PD, followed by older adults and finally younger adults.

Results

Gait characteristics

Differences between conditions

In forward walking, there was an overall effect of condition (F(1, 87) = 6.978, p < 0.001) with univariate tests showing a significant increase in cadence for SING versus MUSIC (F(1, 87) = 15.121, p < 0.001). (Fig. 1, Supplementary Table 1).

Figure 1.

Figure 1

Gait characteristics shown as a percent change from Uncued walking compared across groups for forward and backward walking. All bars represent means ± SEM. Horizontal significance bars indicate an overall effect of condition, whereas vertical significance bars indicate an overall effect of group. *Indicates p < 0.01. #Indicates p < 0.05.

In backward walking, there was an overall effect of condition (F(1, 87) = 8.396, p < 0.001) with univariate tests showing that participants walked faster (F(1, 87) = 10.868, p = 0.001) with higher cadence (F(1, 87) = 22.523, p < 0.001) in SING as compared to MUSIC.

Differences between groups

There were no significant differences between groups in forward walking gait characteristics.

In backward walking, there was a significant between-subject effect of group for velocity (F(2, 87) = 3.552, p = 0.033) and stride length (F(2, 87) = 5.744, p = 0.005). Regardless of condition, pairwise comparisons indicated that the PD group showed a more robust response to cueing than the YC group as evidenced by their greater percent change in velocity (p = 0.010) and their greater percent change in stride length (p = 0.001). The OC group also showed a greater percent change in stride length as compared to the YC group (p = 0.028). There were no significant interactions, indicating that all groups responded similarly to cueing.

Gait variability

Differences between conditions

In forward walking, all participants walked with less variability in SING than in MUSIC, as evidenced by a significant main effect of condition (F(1, 87) = 14.564, p < 0.001) (Fig. 2). This was significant for CVs of stride length (F(1, 87) = 20.039, p < 0.001), stride time (F(1, 87) = 27.623, p < 0.001), and single support time (F(1, 87) = 10.673, p = 0.002).

Figure 2.

Figure 2

Coefficients of variation compared across groups for forward and backward walking. All bars represent means ± SEM. Horizontal significance bars indicate an overall effect of condition, whereas vertical significance bars indicate an overall effect of group. *Indicates p < 0.01. #Indicates p < 0.05.

For backward walking, participants walked with less variability in SING than in MUSIC, as there was a main effect of condition (F(1, 87) = 3.035, p = 0.034). This was significant for CVs of stride length (F(1, 87) = 5.498, p = 0.021), stride time (F(1, 87) = 5.793, p = 0.018), and single support time (F(1, 87) = 6.825, p = 0.011).

Differences between groups

There were no significant differences between groups in forward walking variability.

In backward walking, there was a significant main effect of group for stride time (F(2, 87) = 4.525, p = 0.014). Pairwise comparisons revealed that the OC group (p = 0.004) and the PD group (p = 0.05) had significantly less variability regardless of condition than the YC group. There were no significant interactions.

Discussion

In this study, we examined the effects of internal versus external cues on forward and backward walking in three groups of people: healthy young, healthy older, and people with PD. The results support our hypotheses, that internal cueing in the form of singing may be more beneficial to gait than external cueing. The results also confirm that people with PD exhibit greater improvement than their healthy counterparts36 and may stand to gain the most benefit from internal cueing techniques, particularly in challenging gait situations such as moving in the backward direction.

One of our primary results was that singing increased cadence in both walking directions. In backward walking, this increase in cadence led to higher velocity as well. External cues, in contrast, did not have a significant effect on gait speed, cadence, or stride length on forward walking and had a lesser effect than internal cues on backward walking. This is in accordance with previous studies of forward walking showing only small effects of external cues at preferred walking tempos37,38 and with a recent review revealing generalized small effects on velocity and cadence in cueing without training20. During MUSIC, the cadence was set by the cue, and as we explicitly told participants to synchronize to it, they did not stray far from baseline. In SING, by contrast, with no outside source dictating the song tempo, participants tended to increase their cadence as they sang.

One possible explanation for this is that active music-making (such as singing) may confer greater motor benefits than passive music listening39 by tapping into reward circuitry and affecting movement “vigor,” both of which are compromised in PD. Endorphin and oxytocin release during singing has positive effects on motivation and may translate into higher motor output3942. Singing is also known to activate motor regions in the brain including the primary motor cortex, the basal ganglia, thalamus, and cerebellum43,44, which may additively combine with motor activation during locomotion. While synchronizing movement to music may induce an arousal effect that makes movement faster, larger, and more vigorous45, synchronizing movement to one’s own voice may lead to even greater overall motor network activation and, hence, higher cadence46.

We also noted that, in relation to baseline, external cues had a detrimental effect on forward-walking variability. This supports previous work showing that, for healthy young adults with low baseline variability, external cues tend to perturb normally-functioning internal cueing mechanisms and interfere with gait stability4750. Similarly, older adults do not benefit when constrained by external cues, as gait variability is either unaffected19 or increased with cues at preferred cadence48,50,51. Cues at tempos below52 or above36 preferred cadence also increase gait variability24,48.

For people with PD, preferred cadence cues have shown no effect24,51 or increases in variability52, even after training22. Reductions in variability have been reported, but only for faster tempos and after a brief period of training53. The sum of these studies shows that isochronous external cues lend only a minor benefit to gait characteristics and may come at the price of sacrificing temporal stability, particularly for those with more impaired baseline gait.

In contrast, singing did not negatively affect gait variability. In forward walking, internal cues did not cause gait decrement, and in backward walking, internal cues elicited greater reductions in variability than external cues. The effectiveness of internal cues over external cues in decreasing gait variability may be partially explained through several speculations detailed below.

While external rhythms rely on auditory-motor coupling within the brain to perceive sensory stimuli and match body movement to them, internal rhythms utilize what we will refer to as vocal-motor coupling. As humans are capable of entrainment within both the vocal and motor systems, it is possible that matching one system’s output to that of another through self-generated cues allows for greater stability. Entrainment of one system to another within the same body may reduce attentional load and facilitate motor synchronization. Additively combining motor output from two effectors within one individual may reduce variability in a central timing process that results in lower movement variability. For instance, a bimanual advantage makes tapping with two hands less variable than tapping with only one54.

A similar mechanism may be at play when a motor effector matches a vocal effector. Skills in motor synchronization and singing are strongly linked, as the neuronal networks that support sensorimotor translation in both partly overlap55. Aligning speech to movement enhances verbal processing and facilitates temporal predictions, as information at expected times is processed more efficiently56. Furthermore, concurrent rhythmic vocalizations can reduce variability of whole-body movement, which suggests that moving and vocalizing as a coordinative structure causes mutual stabilization between systems57. As seen through the lens of an internal model, feedforward control during singing masks auditory feedback and allows singers to continuously phonate without processing each note before continuing. By canceling out reafferent signals to the auditory cortex, singing may reduce reliance on real-time auditory feedback that is necessary with external cues, thereby increasing predictability and decreasing motor variability58.

Better synchronization when singing may also be related to our bias for hearing the human voice, or a “vocal advantage”. This postulates that it is easier to match stimuli to personal motor representations that are recognized as biologically possible. The voice is a highly salient stimulus that causes enhanced arousal59, greater pupil dilation60, and greater activation in the sensorimotor cortex61 in listeners as compared to non-vocal melody perception. The dorsal auditory stream, which connects the auditory and motor cortices, has stronger connectivity when participants listen to singing-voice versus non-vocal music, facilitating matching between perceived sounds and motor representations62, and sung melodies are better encoded than instrumental melodies, resulting in faster auditory processing63. Faster processing and stronger dorsal stream connectivity may enable motor improvement during vocally-produced sounds as well.

Notably, the PD group exhibited the largest response from internal cueing. This implies that, in spite of basal ganglia degeneration linked to internal timing deficiencies6467, people with PD were not only capable of internally generating rhythms through singing but were also able to match their movement to it. Beat impairment in PD is thought to impact movement as specific motor network regions, such as the basal ganglia, cerebellum, premotor cortex, and supplementary motor areas, are also responsible for rhythm processing16,68. Neurodegeneration of these motor regions may disrupt the internal regulation of movement amplitude and timing in PD and lead to an inability to control automatic locomotor rhythm67. For people with PD, for whom disease-related decreases in striatal dopamine affect excitatory input to the putamen, external cues are thought to reduce reliance on putamen activity by compensating for impaired internal timing mechanisms69. Singing may achieve the same end by rerouting temporal sequencing from the impaired basal-ganglia-thalamocortical network to other brain areas, such as the spared cerebellar-thalamocortical network, which regulates perceptual and motor timing, or the premotor cortex (PMC), an area known to upregulate its activity during explicit cues to synchronize to a beat70,71.

Furthermore, the same features of singing that underscore its therapeutic benefit to dysarthric speech may also explain the motor benefit we witnessed. In continuous voicing that occurs when singing, increases in phonation time and syllable lengthening lead to greater connectedness between words. This fluency-enhancing effect on speech may translate to motor impairments as well. As people with PD who experience vocal softness, hoarseness, and slurring when they speak are able to maintain tempo and interval variability when they sing72, increased vocal fluency during singing may similarly encourage motor fluidity and reduce movement variability31.

One limitation of this study is that we only tested one version of one song, and other musical choices might affect gait parameters differently45,49. Our participants had only mild-moderate disease severity, and, as external cues tend to improve gait variability for patients with greater disease progression51 or freezing of gait73, our technique should be tested on a broader spectrum of individuals. Another limitation is that all walking trials were tested on a short walkway, and some research suggests that older adults require several steps to attune to acoustic stimuli74 and choose different speed strategies over longer distances75. Although habitual walking tends to occur in short spurts, future work should explore this technique over longer distances. Lastly, as participants were never required to begin singing without hearing the song first, we do not know how this technique would translate to everyday life in which people would self-initiate their own singing. Future work should address internal cueing techniques using both beat-continuation and beat-initiation paradigms.

This study is the first to our knowledge to compare internal and external cues on walking performance in healthy adults and people with PD and to explore the effects of cueing on backward walking. While effective in laboratory settings1618, external cueing has limitations that reduce its applicability to the real world. Carry-over effects are limited, so a device is required to provide constant stimulation17,76. Fixed-tempo rhythmic cues do not readily adapt to ever-changing environmental surroundings and are less effective than variable cues that oscillate in accordance with human gait7779. Perhaps most importantly, people with PD do not report using external cues in their daily lives35.

Our results indicate that internal cueing through singing may be more useful than external cueing techniques for people who experience gait dysfunction from aging or neurological decline. Future work should examine different cue rates to potentially elicit stronger responses and explore rhythmic ability and musical training to elucidate who best responds to this technique. Mental singing, or singing in one’s head, should also be tested to discover if it is necessary to produce sound in order to gain benefit from singing as a cue. As external cueing is useful to a wide range of people with health conditions, from Alzheimer’s to multiple sclerosis to cerebral palsy, internal cueing may also hold benefit for myriad populations. Ultimately, a singing intervention study should be undertaken to begin to transfer this technique into a clinical setting to make it accessible to patients and carry-over effects should be tested to explore whether vocalizations enhance motor memory61.

Methods

Participants

A total of 90 participants, thirty (15 male) in each group (young control (YC), older control (OC), and Parkinson disease (PD)) took part in this study (Table 1). PD participants were recruited from the Movement Disorders Center at Washington University School of Medicine. Healthy controls were recruited via emails, social media, and flyers in and around the Washington University School of Medicine campus as well as through the Research Participant Registry through the Volunteers for Health database managed by Washington University School of Medicine. Age criteria for young controls were 18–35 whereas older controls were ≥50. PD participants were ≥50 years of age and had a neurological diagnosis of “definite PD”, as previously described80 and based upon established criteria81.

Table 1.

Participant Demographics.

Young control (YC) Older control (OC) Parkinson disease (PD)
N (male) 30 (15) 30 (15) 30 (15)
Age, yrs 25.8 (±2.8) 64.9 (±7.2) 65.8 (±6.5)
MDS-UPDRS-III 24.9 (±10.27)
MMSE, median (range) 30 (28, 30) 30 (27, 30) 29 (24, 30)
LEDD, mg 933 (±658)
Years since dx 5.77 (±3.79)
Musical experience, yrs 4.43 (±3.39) 4.42 (±6.02) 7.77 (±11.45)

Values represent mean ± SD, except where noted.

MDS-UPDRS, Movement Disorder Society Unified Parkinson Disease Rating Scale. MMSE, Mini Mental Status Examination. LEDD, Levodopa Equivalent Daily Dose.

All participants had vision corrected to 20/40 or better, were able to stand independently for at least 30 minutes, and had no evidence of dementia (MMSE ≥ 26). Participants were excluded for any history of neurological deficit (aside from PD), orthostatic hypotension, or prior deep brain stimulation surgery. One participant in the OC group was excluded for cognition as evidenced by an MMSE score of below 24 and an additional participant was recruited as a replacement.

Participants provided informed consent before participating and were compensated for their time. The protocol was approved by the Human Research Protection Office at Washington University School of Medicine, and the methods were carried out in accordance with the approved guidelines. Prior to testing, participants were assessed via the following questionnaires: the New Freezing of Gait Questionnaire (nFOGq), the Fall History questionnaire, and the Betts’ Questionnaire upon Mental Imagery (BQMI). The Movement Disorders Society Unified Parkinson’s Disease Rating Scale (MDS-UPDRS) was used to assess disease severity. Sub-sections I (non-motor symptoms), II (motor aspects of daily living), and III (motor sign severity) were administered and scored by trained staff.

Experimental Protocol

Participants in the PD group were tested in the “on” state (i.e., they had taken their anti-Parkinson medication within the previous 2 hours) to maximize relevance to everyday walking26 and to optimize gait performance82. All walking trials were performed on a 5 m instrumented, computerized GAITRite Walkway (CIR Systems, Inc., Franklin, NJ). Three baseline trials (UNCUED) were collected in both forward and backward walking to capture each participant’s comfortable walking features. Participants then completed three walking trials in each of the conditions below in both forward and backward directions. Condition order and walking direction within each condition were randomized and counterbalanced to eliminate any training effects. In order to control cadence across conditions, participants always heard the music immediately prior to walking.

  1. MUSIC: Participants listened to one verse of the song and then began walking to the beat of the song while the song looped for the duration of the walking trial. This condition is similar to a beat-synchronization paradigm and replicates traditional external cueing techniques.

  2. SING: Participants listened to one verse of the song, but then the music stopped and they began singing aloud and walking to the beat of their singing. In this condition, no external source provided a cue while they walked, so participants had to generate the cue themselves.

For all cued conditions (both MUSIC and SING), we used an instrumental version of “Row, row, row your boat” that was designed with a salient beat that participants could readily detect. All participants were familiar with the melody and lyrics and sang the song without difficulty. The musical cue was administered from a laptop connected to speakers no farther than 10 m from the participant during walking and at an audible volume. Song tempo was adjusted maintaining key consistency via Audacity open source audio editing software (The Audacity Team, audacity.sourceforge.net/) to match preferred cadence in each direction, as determined from the baseline trials. Cue rate was set to 100% of preferred cadence of each direction so as not to complicate task demands, particularly for backward walking.

Data analysis

Statistical analyses were done using IBM SPSS Statistics 24. For each participant, data were averaged across the three trials of each condition. Gait characteristics (velocity, cadence, and stride length) and variability (coefficients of variation for stride length, stride time, and single support time) were compared in two separate analyses, one for each walking direction. Normalized velocities were calculated as velocity/average leg length (cm/s/leg length) and coefficients of variation (CV) were calculated as the ((standard deviation/mean) × 100) for each person in each condition. As we were only interested in how cueing affected these measures, we ran analyses on each variable as it compared to the UNCUED condition. Hence, gait characteristics were expressed as a percent change from UNCUED and gait variabilities were expressed as a change in CV from UNCUED. Mixed model repeated measures ANOVAs with between-subject factor of group and within-subject factor of condition were used to assess differences, and Tukey-corrected post-hoc pairwise comparisons were used as appropriate. Statistical significance was set at α = 0.05.

The datasets generated and analyzed during the current study are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.

Electronic supplementary material

Supplementary Table 1 (180.2KB, pdf)

Acknowledgements

This work was supported by a grant from the GRAMMY Museum Grant Program. The authors gratefully acknowledge Martha Hessler and Richard Nagel for assistance with data collection.

Author Contributions

E.H. and G.E. designed the experimental protocol, E.H. and A.H. collected data, and E.H. analyzed the data and wrote the main manuscript text while A.H. and G.E. provided feedback.

Competing Interests

The authors declare no competing interests.

Footnotes

Publisher’s note: Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Electronic supplementary material

Supplementary information accompanies this paper at 10.1038/s41598-018-33942-6.

References

  • 1.Verghese J. Cognitive and mobility profile of older social dancers. J Am Geriatr Soc. 2006;54:1241–1244. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-5415.2006.00808.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Cesari M, et al. Prognostic value of usual gait speed in well-functioning older people–results from the Health, Aging and Body Composition Study. J Am Geriatr Soc. 2005;53:1675–1680. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-5415.2005.53501.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Montero-Odasso M, et al. Gait velocity as a single predictor of adverse events in healthy seniors aged 75 years and older. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci. 2005;60:1304–1309. doi: 10.1093/gerona/60.10.1304. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Pirker W, Katzenschlager R. Gait disorders in adults and the elderly: A clinical guide. Wien Klin Wochenschr. 2017;129:81–95. doi: 10.1007/s00508-016-1096-4. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Kemoun G, Defebvre L. Gait disorders in Parkinson disease. Clinical description, analysis of posture, initiation of stabilized gait. Presse Med. 2001;30:452–459. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Moon Y, Sung J, An R, Hernandez ME, Sosnoff JJ. Gait variability in people with neurological disorders: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Human movement science. 2016;47:197–208. doi: 10.1016/j.humov.2016.03.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Brach JS, Studenski S, Perera S, VanSwearingen JM, Newman AB. Stance time and step width variability have unique contributing impairments in older persons. Gait & posture. 2008;27:431–439. doi: 10.1016/j.gaitpost.2007.05.016. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Hausdorff JM, et al. Etiology and modification of gait instability in older adults: a randomized controlled trial of exercise. J Appl Physiol (1985) 2001;90:2117–2129. doi: 10.1152/jappl.2001.90.6.2117. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Salzman B. Gait and balance disorders in older adults. Am Fam Physician. 2010;82:61–68. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Aboutorabi A, Arazpour M, Bahramizadeh M, Hutchins SW, Fadayevatan R. The effect of aging on gait parameters in able-bodied older subjects: a literature review. Aging Clin Exp Res. 2016;28:393–405. doi: 10.1007/s40520-015-0420-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Paker N, et al. Gait speed and related factors in Parkinson’s disease. Journal of physical therapy science. 2015;27:3675–3679. doi: 10.1589/jpts.27.3675. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Hausdorff JM, Cudkowicz ME, Firtion R, Wei JY, Goldberger AL. Gait variability and basal ganglia disorders: stride-to-stride variations of gait cycle timing in Parkinson’s disease and Huntington’s disease. Movement disorders: official journal of the Movement Disorder Society. 1998;13:428–437. doi: 10.1002/mds.870130310. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Bryant MS, Rintala DH, Hou JG, Lai EC, Protas EJ. Effects of levodopa on forward and backward gait patterns in persons with Parkinson’s disease. NeuroRehabilitation. 2011;29:247–252. doi: 10.3233/NRE-2011-0700. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Hackney ME, Earhart GM. Backward walking in Parkinson’s disease. Movement disorders: official journal of the Movement Disorder Society. 2009;24:218–223. doi: 10.1002/mds.22330. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Fritz NE, et al. Backward walking measures are sensitive to age-related changes in mobility and balance. Gait & posture. 2013;37:593–597. doi: 10.1016/j.gaitpost.2012.09.022. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Nombela C, Hughes LE, Owen AM, Grahn JA. Into the groove: can rhythm influence Parkinson’s disease? Neuroscience and biobehavioral reviews. 2013;37:2564–2570. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2013.08.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Thaut MH, et al. Rhythmic auditory stimulation in gait training for Parkinson’s disease patients. Movement disorders: official journal of the Movement Disorder Society. 1996;11:193–200. doi: 10.1002/mds.870110213. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Thaut MH, McIntosh KW, McIntosh GC, Hoemberg V. Auditory rhythmicity enhances movement and speech motor control in patients with Parkinson’s disease. Functional neurology. 2001;16:163–172. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Wittwer JE, Webster KE, Hill K. Music and metronome cues produce different effects on gait spatiotemporal measures but not gait variability in healthy older adults. Gait & posture. 2013;37:219–222. doi: 10.1016/j.gaitpost.2012.07.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Ghai S, Ghai I, Effenberg AO. Effect of rhythmic auditory cueing on gait in cerebral palsy: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Neuropsychiatr Dis Treat. 2018;14:43–59. doi: 10.2147/NDT.S148053. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Rochester L, et al. Evidence for motor learning in Parkinson’s disease: acquisition, automaticity and retention of cued gait performance after training with external rhythmical cues. Brain Res. 2010;1319:103–111. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2010.01.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Dalla Bella, S. et al. Gait improvement via rhythmic stimulation in Parkinson’s disease is linked to rhythmic skills. Scientific reports7, 10.1038/srep42005 (2017). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • 23.Rochester L, et al. The effect of external rhythmic cues (auditory and visual) on walking during a functional task in homes of people with Parkinson’s disease. Archives of physical medicine and rehabilitation. 2005;86:999–1006. doi: 10.1016/j.apmr.2004.10.040. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Hausdorff JM, et al. Rhythmic auditory stimulation modulates gait variability in Parkinson’s disease. The European journal of neuroscience. 2007;26:2369–2375. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2007.05810.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Nieuwboer A, et al. Cueing training in the home improves gait-related mobility in Parkinson’s disease: the RESCUE trial. Journal of neurology, neurosurgery, and psychiatry. 2007;78:134–140. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.200X.097923. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Spaulding SJ, et al. Cueing and gait improvement among people with Parkinson’s disease: a meta-analysis. Archives of physical medicine and rehabilitation. 2013;94:562–570. doi: 10.1016/j.apmr.2012.10.026. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Harrison EC, McNeely ME, Earhart GM. The feasibility of singing to improve gait in Parkinson disease. Gait & posture. 2017;53:224–229. doi: 10.1016/j.gaitpost.2017.02.008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Yogev G, et al. Dual tasking, gait rhythmicity, and Parkinson’s disease: which aspects of gait are attention demanding? The European journal of neuroscience. 2005;22:1248–1256. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2005.04298.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Haneishi E. Effects of a music therapy voice protocol on speech intelligibility, vocal acoustic measures, and mood of individuals with Parkinson’s disease. Journal of music therapy. 2001;38:273–290. doi: 10.1093/jmt/38.4.273. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Narayana S, et al. Neural correlates of efficacy of voice therapy in Parkinson’s disease identified by performance-correlation analysis. Human brain mapping. 2010;31:222–236. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20859. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Wan CY, Ruber T, Hohmann A, Schlaug G. The Therapeutic Effects of Singing in Neurological Disorders. Music perception. 2010;27:287–295. doi: 10.1525/mp.2010.27.4.287. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Barnish J, Atkinson RA, Barran SM, Barnish MS. Potential Benefit of Singing for People with Parkinson’s Disease: A Systematic Review. Journal of Parkinson’s disease. 2016;6:473–484. doi: 10.3233/JPD-160837. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Berkowska M, Dalla Bella S. Acquired and congenital disorders of sung performance: A review. Advances in cognitive psychology. 2009;5:69–83. doi: 10.2478/v10053-008-0068-2. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Maitra KK. Enhancement of reaching performance via self-speech in people with Parkinson’s disease. Clinical rehabilitation. 2007;21:418–424. doi: 10.1177/0269215507074058. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Jones D, et al. Everyday walking with Parkinson’s disease: Understanding personal challenges and strategies. Disability and rehabilitation. 2008;30:1213–1221. doi: 10.1080/09638280701828955. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Cochen De Cock V, et al. Rhythmic abilities and musical training in Parkinson’s disease: do they help? npj Parkinson’s Disease. 2018;4:8. doi: 10.1038/s41531-018-0043-7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Howe TE, Lovgreen B, Cody FW, Ashton VJ, Oldham JA. Auditory cues can modify the gait of persons with early-stage Parkinson’s disease: a method for enhancing parkinsonian walking performance? Clinical rehabilitation. 2003;17:363–367. doi: 10.1191/0269215503cr621oa. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Willems AM, et al. The use of rhythmic auditory cues to influence gait in patients with Parkinson’s disease, the differential effect for freezers and non-freezers, an explorative study. Disability and rehabilitation. 2006;28:721–728. doi: 10.1080/09638280500386569. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Pacchetti C, et al. Active music therapy in Parkinson’s disease: an integrative method for motor and emotional rehabilitation. Psychosomatic medicine. 2000;62:386–393. doi: 10.1097/00006842-200005000-00012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Dunbar RI, Kaskatis K, MacDonald I, Barra V. Performance of music elevates pain threshold and positive affect: implications for the evolutionary function of music. Evol Psychol. 2012;10:688–702. doi: 10.1177/147470491201000403. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Fogg-Rogers L, et al. Choral singing therapy following stroke or Parkinson’s disease: an exploration of participants’ experiences. Disability and rehabilitation. 2016;38:952–962. doi: 10.3109/09638288.2015.1068875. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Abell RV, Baird AD, Chalmers KA. Group singing and health-related quality of life in Parkinson’s disease. Health Psychol. 2017;36:55–64. doi: 10.1037/hea0000412. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Jeffries KJ, Fritz JB, Braun AR. Words in melody: an H(2)15O PET study of brain activation during singing and speaking. Neuroreport. 2003;14:749–754. doi: 10.1097/01.wnr.0000066198.94941.a4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Formby C, Thomas RG, Halsey JH., Jr. Regional cerebral blood flow for singers and nonsingers while speaking, singing, and humming a rote passage. Brain and language. 1989;36:690–698. doi: 10.1016/0093-934X(89)90094-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Leman M, et al. Activating and relaxing music entrains the speed of beat synchronized walking. PloS one. 2013;8:e67932. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0067932. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Schaefer RS, Morcom AM, Roberts N, Overy K. Moving to music: effects of heard and imagined musical cues on movement-related brain activity. Frontiers in human neuroscience. 2014;8:774. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2014.00774. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Leow LA, Rinchon C, Grahn J. Familiarity with music increases walking speed in rhythmic auditory cuing. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences. 2015;1337:53–61. doi: 10.1111/nyas.12658. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Hamacher D, Hamacher D, Herold F, Schega L. Effect of dual tasks on gait variability in walking to auditory cues in older and young individuals. Experimental brain research. 2016;234:3555–3563. doi: 10.1007/s00221-016-4754-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Leow LA, Parrott T, Grahn JA. Individual differences in beat perception affect gait responses to low- and high-groove music. Frontiers in human neuroscience. 2014;8:811. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2014.00811. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Brodie MA, et al. Symmetry matched auditory cues improve gait steadiness in most people with Parkinson’s disease but not in healthy older people. Journal of Parkinson’s disease. 2015;5:105–116. doi: 10.3233/JPD-140430. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Arias P, Cudeiro J. Effects of rhythmic sensory stimulation (auditory, visual) on gait in Parkinson’s disease patients. Experimental brain research. 2008;186:589–601. doi: 10.1007/s00221-007-1263-y. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Ebersbach G, et al. Interference of rhythmic constraint on gait in healthy subjects and patients with early Parkinson’s disease: evidence for impaired locomotor pattern generation in early Parkinson’s disease. Movement disorders: official journal of the Movement Disorder Society. 1999;14:619–625. doi: 10.1002/1531-8257(199907)14:4&#x0003c;619::AID-MDS1011&#x0003e;3.0.CO;2-X. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Hausdorff JM. Gait variability: methods, modeling and meaning. Journal of neuroengineering and rehabilitation. 2005;2:19. doi: 10.1186/1743-0003-2-19. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Helmuth LL, Ivry RB. When two hands are better than one: Reduced timing variability during bimanual movements. J Exp Psychol Human. 1996;22:278–293. doi: 10.1037/0096-1523.22.2.278. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Dalla Bella S, Berkowska M, Sowinski J. Moving to the Beat and Singing are Linked in Humans. Frontiers in human neuroscience. 2015;9:663. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2015.00663. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56.Falk S, Dalla Bella S. It is better when expected: aligning speech and motor rhythms enhances verbal processing. Lang Cogn Neurosci. 2016;31:699–708. doi: 10.1080/23273798.2016.1144892. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Miyata K, Kudo K. Mutual stabilization of rhythmic vocalization and whole-body movement. PloS one. 2014;9:e115495. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0115495. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 58.Selleck MA, Sataloff RT. The impact of the auditory system on phonation: a review. J Voice. 2014;28:688–693. doi: 10.1016/j.jvoice.2014.03.018. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 59.Loui P, Bachorik JP, Li HC, Schlaug G. Effects of voice on emotional arousal. Frontiers in psychology. 2013;4:675. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00675. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 60.Weiss MW, Trehub SE, Schellenberg EG, Habashi P. Pupils dilate for vocal or familiar music. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform. 2016;42:1061–1065. doi: 10.1037/xhp0000226. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 61.Weiss MW, Trehub SE, Schellenberg EG. Something in the way she sings: enhanced memory for vocal melodies. Psychol Sci. 2012;23:1074–1078. doi: 10.1177/0956797612442552. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 62.Hickok G, Houde J, Rong F. Sensorimotor integration in speech processing: computational basis and neural organization. Neuron. 2011;69:407–422. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2011.01.019. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 63.Agus TR, Thorpe SJ, Pressnitzer D. Rapid formation of robust auditory memories: insights from noise. Neuron. 2010;66:610–618. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2010.04.014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 64.Harrington DL, Haaland KY, Knight RT. Cortical networks underlying mechanisms of time perception. The Journal of neuroscience: the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience. 1998;18:1085–1095. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.18-03-01085.1998. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 65.Grahn JA. The role of the basal ganglia in beat perception: neuroimaging and neuropsychological investigations. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences. 2009;1169:35–45. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.2009.04553.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 66.O’Boyle DJ, Freeman JS, Cody FW. The accuracy and precision of timing of self-paced, repetitive movements in subjects with Parkinson’s disease. Brain: a journal of neurology. 1996;119(Pt 1):51–70. doi: 10.1093/brain/119.1.51. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 67.Cameron DJ, Pickett KA, Earhart GM, Grahn JA. The Effect of Dopaminergic Medication on Beat-Based Auditory Timing in Parkinson’s Disease. Frontiers in neurology. 2016;7:19. doi: 10.3389/fneur.2016.00019. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 68.Thaut MH. Neural basis of rhythmic timing networks in the human brain. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences. 2003;999:364–373. doi: 10.1196/annals.1284.044. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 69.Grahn JA, Rowe JB. Feeling the beat: premotor and striatal interactions in musicians and nonmusicians during beat perception. The Journal of neuroscience: the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience. 2009;29:7540–7548. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2018-08.2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 70.Schubotz RI. Prediction of external events with our motor system: towards a new framework. Trends Cogn Sci. 2007;11:211–218. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2007.02.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 71.Grahn JA, Rowe JB. Finding and feeling the musical beat: striatal dissociations between detection and prediction of regularity. Cerebral cortex. 2013;23:913–921. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhs083. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 72.Harris R, Leenders KL, de Jong BM. Speech dysprosody but no music ‘dysprosody’ in Parkinson’s disease. Brain and language. 2016;163:1–9. doi: 10.1016/j.bandl.2016.08.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 73.Young WR, Shreve L, Quinn EJ, Craig C, Bronte-Stewart H. Auditory cueing in Parkinson’s patients with freezing of gait. What matters most: Action-relevance or cue-continuity? Neuropsychologia. 2016;87:54–62. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2016.04.034. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 74.Roerdink M, Bank PJ, Peper CL, Beek PJ. Walking to the beat of different drums: practical implications for the use of acoustic rhythms in gait rehabilitation. Gait & posture. 2011;33:690–694. doi: 10.1016/j.gaitpost.2011.03.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 75.Najafi B, Helbostad JL, Moe-Nilssen R, Zijlstra W, Aminian K. Does walking strategy in older people change as a function of walking distance? Gait & posture. 2009;29:261–266. doi: 10.1016/j.gaitpost.2008.09.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 76.Nieuwboer A, et al. The short-term effects of different cueing modalities on turn speed in people with Parkinson’s disease. Neurorehabilitation and neural repair. 2009;23:831–836. doi: 10.1177/1545968309337136. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 77.Hove MJ, Keller PE. Impaired movement timing in neurological disorders: rehabilitation and treatment strategies. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences. 2015;1337:111–117. doi: 10.1111/nyas.12615. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 78.Hove MJ, Suzuki K, Uchitomi H, Orimo S, Miyake Y. Interactive rhythmic auditory stimulation reinstates natural 1/f timing in gait of Parkinson’s patients. PloS one. 2012;7:e32600. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0032600. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 79.Dotov DG, et al. Biologically-variable rhythmic auditory cues are superior to isochronous cues in fostering natural gait variability in Parkinson’s disease. Gait & posture. 2017;51:64–69. doi: 10.1016/j.gaitpost.2016.09.020. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 80.Racette BA, Rundle M, Parsian A, Perlmutter JS. Evaluation of a screening questionnaire for genetic studies of Parkinson’s disease. American journal of medical genetics. 1999;88:539–543. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1096-8628(19991015)88:5&#x0003c;539::AID-AJMG19&#x0003e;3.0.CO;2-S. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 81.Calne DB, Snow BJ, Lee C. Criteria for diagnosing Parkinson’s disease. Annals of neurology. 1992;32(Suppl):S125–127. doi: 10.1002/ana.410320721. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 82.McIntosh GC, Brown SH, Rice RR, Thaut MH. Rhythmic auditory-motor facilitation of gait patterns in patients with Parkinson’s disease. Journal of neurology, neurosurgery, and psychiatry. 1997;62:22–26. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.62.1.22. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

Supplementary Table 1 (180.2KB, pdf)

Articles from Scientific Reports are provided here courtesy of Nature Publishing Group

RESOURCES