Abstract
Middle childhood is a transitional period for episodic memory (EM) performance, as a result of improvements in strategies that are used to encode and retrieve memories. EM is also a skill continually assessed for testing in the school setting. The purpose of this study was to examine EM performance during middle childhood and its relation to individual differences in attentional abilities and in neurophysiological functioning. We examined self-reports of attention at 6, 7 and 8-years of age as well as parietal EEG recorded during baseline, memory task encoding, and memory task retrieval. Results indicate that child self-reports of attention predicted EM performance. Additionally, the difference from baseline to retrieval-related EEG activation contributed variance to EM performance. Results replicate other middle childhood studies showing a positive association between EM performance and attention while also suggesting that parietal EEG yields critical information regarding memory performance.
Keywords: episodic memory, attention, EEG, middle childhood
Episodic memory (EM) involves conscious awareness of detailed memory creation (encoding) and retrieval, and undergoes rapid development throughout early to middle childhood (Ghetti & Bunge, 2012). EM is often measured using visual recognition tasks (e.g., Ritchey, Montchal, Yonelinas, & Ranganath, 2015). A typical recognition memory task involves the presentation of a set of visual stimuli followed by a test of the ability to accurately classify items in the next group of stimuli as “old” when seen in the earlier set or “new” when not previously seen (e.g., Marshall, Drummey, Fox & Newcombe, 2002). Performance on these types of tasks shows steady improvement with development, as adolescents and adults exhibit better accuracy and faster reaction times than children (Cycowicz, Friedman, Snodgrass, & Duff, 2001). Given that EM is associated with academic abilities, it is critical to understand potential contributors to individual differences in EM performance in school-age children (Blankenship, O’Neill, Ross, & Bell, 2015; Mirandola, Del Prete, Ghetti & Cornoldi, 2011). In our study we focused on two potential sources of individual differences in EM performance: attention and brain electrical activity.
Middle childhood is a transitional period for EM performance, with increases observed throughout (Ghetti & Bunge, 2012). These developmental changes are assumed to be a result of improvements in strategies that are used to encode and retrieve memories. The use of effective strategies requires attentional processes (Riggins, 2014). Sophisticated attentional processes begin to develop in early childhood and continue to improve as children enter middle childhood. Specifically, attentional skills allow children to engage in planning, establish goals, and monitor progress on simple and complex tasks (e.g., Ridderinkhof, van der Molen, Band, & Bashore, 1997). With development, children show increasing flexibility and focus of attention allowing them to concentrate for longer durations on a wider variety of targets (Archibald, Levee, & Olino, 2015) and to adapt attention to contextual demands and goals (Pezzica, Pinto, Bigozzi, & Vezzani, 2015).
Object recognition involves the ability to selectively deploy attentional processes and correctly parse an object from its environmental context (Coldren & Haaf, 1999; Haaf, Lundy, & Coldren, 1996). Memory processes require a certain amount of attentional resources. Indeed, without attention, memory would not exist (Chun & Turk-Browne, 2007). The very process of encoding relevant information into long-term memory requires attentional resources (Chun & Turk-Browne, 2007), which later impact retrieval of such memories (Warrington & Ackroyd, 1975). For example, when visual cues are added to memory tasks, performance significantly improves (Makovsik & Jiang, 2007; Makovsik, Sussman, & Jiang, 2008). This suggests that visual cues allow focused attention to be allocated to relevant information and irrelevant information to be ignored. Given the well-established relation between attention and memory, we expected associations between attention and EM performance in our study.
In addition to attentional skills, research on brain activity during EM may provide insight on the existence of individual differences in EM. One common method used to examine neural activity during EM encoding and retrieval, especially during childhood, is the electroencephalogram (EEG; Riggins et al., 2009). Specifically, event-related potential (ERP) studies associate correct identification of items (i.e., as “old” or “new” stimuli) with an old/new effect that typically comprises of an early mid-frontal and a later parietal component in adults (Rugg, Schloerscheidt, & Mark, 1998). However, children’s ERPs during recognition demonstrate a parietal old/new effect but with longer latencies compared to young adults (Mecklinger et al., 2011). These findings suggest children may rely partially on parietal regions during recognition, due to underdeveloped mechanisms of prefrontal control (Czernochowski et al., 2004).
Recently studies also suggest that in addition to ERP, the ongoing electrical activity of the EEG may be used to distinguish correct from incorrect EM responses and to assess developmental changes in relation to EM performance in children (Blankenship & Bell, 2015; Rajan & Bell, 2015). In a sample of 9–12-year-olds, differential EEG connectivity during encoding and retrieval contributed to EM performance. Moreover, event-related increases in EEG power values at parietal scalp locations during recall was associated with memory performance in an EM task with 6- and 8-year-old children (Rajan et al., 2014). This coincides with neuroimaging studies in adults that report the involvement of the posterior parietal cortex (PPC; Wagner, Shannon, Kahn, & Buckner, 2005; Cabeza, Ciaramelli, Olson, & Moscovitch, 2008; Vilberg & Rugg, 2008) during tasks requiring memory retrieval, suggesting a role for PPC in forming and maintaining a bound representation of the recognized item and task-relevant contextual elements (Johansson & Mecklinger, 2003). Evidence also suggests that parietal cortex activation is involved in directing attention toward relevant source features (Vilberg & Rugg, 2008). This effect is thought to be less consistently present and/or to exhibit a different topography in early development as a result of refinements of networks underlying the PPC (Cycowicz, Friedman & Duff, 2003). To our knowledge, only one study investigated and found evidence for a relation between recognition memory and the parietal area in school age children (Cycowicz et. al., 2003). Because limited empirical evidence exists supporting the reliance on the parietal region during middle childhood, we examined EEG activity during middle childhood in an attempt to validate reliance on parietal regions during a recognition memory task.
We used ongoing EEG in our study because of its ability to yield different types of information relative to ERP. For example, task-related EEG have been associated with performance on both working memory and source recall tasks during early and middle childhood (Rajan & Bell, 2015; Wolfe & Bell, 2007). Previous research has also shown an association between task performance and changes in EEG activation from a resting state to a cognitively demanding state (Wolfe & Bell, 2004). Although individual scalp locations reveal critical information regarding neural networks in various regions (Bell & Cuevas, 2012), EEG is context-related; thus, the EEG generated during baseline or rest is believed to be quantitatively different than that generated during cognitive processing (Deater-Deckard & Bell, 2017). Recent research has provided evidence that greater change in EEG power from baseline-to-task is associated with more attention and cognitive processing in both infancy and early childhood (Perry, Swingler, Calkins, & Bell, 2016; Watson & Bell 2013). However, EEG research on brain activity associated with middle childhood memory development and performance is limited. Nevertheless, we know that during adulthood, individual differences in memory encoding and recall are associated with the ongoing EEG (Klimesch, 1999). The lack of EEG studies during middle childhood is surprising as theories of memory allow specific hypotheses to be made about changes in EEG activity (Burgess & Gruzelier, 2000).
We analyzed EEG power within the lower alpha power band (8–10 Hz). Alpha is thought to reflect top-down widespread cortical control of lower level internal processing of information (Benedek, Bergner, Könen, Fink, & Neubauer, 2011). Lower alpha was used in our analyses due to the established relation between this frequency band and attentional processes and memory in both adults and children (Burgess & Gruzelier, 2000; Klimesch, 1996; 2012). In this study, we were interested in whether the EEG recorded during baseline, familiarization (encoding), or recognition (retrieval) proved to be similar predictors of recognition task performance during middle childhood. Specifically, we examined changes in EEG reactivity (operationalized as the power difference from baseline resting state to effortful task performance state) to investigate whether EEG was associated with variance in EM performance. If baseline-to-familiarization EEG predicted EM, then the change in brain activity associated with encoding would be critical for individual differences in task performance. Likewise, if baseline-to-recognition EEG predicted EM, then the change in brain activity associated with retrieval would be critical. These are not mutually exclusive processes; thus, it may be that EEG recorded during all conditions is critical for identifying individual differences in EM performance.
Current Study
We focused on individual differences in EM in middle childhood. We chose to study 6-, 7-, and 8-year-old children because of our overarching interests in cognitive processes associated with successful school performance. Recognition memory processes are typically assessed for testing in the school setting (Nouwens, Groen, & Verhoeven, 2016). We predicted that child age, attention, and changes in brain electrophysiology (measured via EEG) would each contribute unique variance to EM performance.
We administered a questionnaire in order to get a sense of day-to-day attentional behaviors exhibited in many different contexts by children. We asked children to report on their own attentional processes because children are able to do so by age six (Simonds & Rothbart, 2004). Additionally, we used ongoing electroencephalogram (EEG) as our measure of brain activity associated with EM, rather than measures of ERP traditionally used in EM research. We focused on EEG because it provides a continuous measurement of electrophysiological activity during the course of recognition memory processes (encoding and retrieval). Moreover, baseline-to-task change scores were utilized to measure individual differences in EEG change during cognitive processing relative to a resting state. We focused on EEG activity at parietal locations and hypothesized that both familiarization and recognition EEG would predict EM performance.
Method
Participants
Participants included 105 six (M = 73.4 months, SD = 3.16; 12 boys, 22 girls), seven (M = age 85 months, SD 2.24; 14 boys, 21 girls), and eight (M = 96.6 months, SD =4.31; 20 boys, 16 girls) year olds (97 Caucasian, 4 African American, 2 Asian American, 2 Hispanic). All children were born to parents with a high school diploma; college degrees or higher were held by 90% of the mothers and 82% of the fathers. Children were full term, experienced no prenatal or birth complications, had no developmental delays or cognitive disabilities, and were healthy at the time of the lab visit. Children were seen in the research lab within four months after their birth date.
Procedures
Mother and child were greeted by a research assistant who explained the study procedures and obtained signed consent from the mother and assent from the child. After a brief warm-up period, an EEG Electro-cap was situated on the child’s head. Electrodes remained on the scalp during the entire procedure. Children sat in front of a table where a baseline movie clip showing slowly turning circles and colors played on a television screen 1.5m away from the child for 1 minute (Diaz & Bell, 2012; Rajan & Bell, 2015). After the baseline video, a rollaway desk with a touch screen computer replaced the table. The child sat at the desk while familiarization and test phases of the EM task were administered. EEG recordings were event marked (baseline, familiarization, test) by a research assistant in an adjacent room. At the end of the EM task, children were asked to self-report on their attention.
Episodic Memory Task
Based on an old/new recognition task designed by Marshall and colleagues (2002), children viewed color photographs of common objects on a touch screen monitor; objects were perceptually simple and easy to label. Prior to the task, children were told that they were about to see several pictures on the screen and that each picture would go by rather quickly. They were asked to sit quietly and pay attention to each picture. The children were also told to try to remember the pictures as they would later be asked whether they had been shown a particular picture or whether it was a new picture. The pictures belonged to familiar categories such as furniture (e.g., sofa, chair), kitchen objects (e.g., frying pan, can opener), and musical instruments (e.g., piano, guitar). The familiarization phase (i.e., memory encoding) consisted of 37 pictures. Each image was displayed for 2-seconds with an interstimulus interval of 2-seconds. After a 5 minute delay, the test phase (i.e., memory retrieval) began.
The retrieval phase was comprised of 74 pictures, including the 37 pictures displayed at the familiarization phase and an additional set of 37 new pictures, randomly mixed. Practice trials were given to teach the child the rules of the game on the touch screen computer monitor. For both practice and test trials, children were told that the object of the game was to indicate whether they had seen the image on the computer monitor (touch the word "old" to the left of the picture on the screen) or whether they had not seen the image on the monitor (touch the word "new" to the right of the picture on the screen) and to do so as quickly as possible. Children began each trial of the test phase with their hands in their lap and then returned their hands to their lap during the intertrial interval. Table 1 provides summary scores for each age. To ensure that the participants would understand the procedure, the practice phase included both study and test blocks images and asked children to demonstrate their response to a presentation of “new” and “old” pictures. Thirteen children did not proceed to the test phase for not demonstrating understanding of instructions or refusal to continue. Additionally, three computer malfunctions occurred during the test phase of the task. Thus, 16 children did not contribute retrieval memory behavioral data. Chi Square and t-tests were performed in order to determine whether there were differences between children who passed and did not passed the test trials on the variables of age, sex, cultural identification, mother-rated and self-rated attention. Results indicated no statistical differences between the two groups on any variable (p's > .08).
Table 1.
M | SD | Range | |
---|---|---|---|
6-year-olds | |||
VRM Number Correct | 46.90 | 6.62 | 35–62 |
Child Reported Attention | 2.22 | .74 | .57–4.00 |
7-year-olds | |||
VRM Number Correct | 48.74 | 6.28 | 36–61 |
Child Reported Attention | 2.30 | .82 | .57–3.71 |
8-year-olds | |||
VRM Number Correct | 50.30 | 6.49 | 37–62 |
Child Reported Attention | 2.25 | .88 | .29–3.86 |
Attention Measure
Child report of attention
The Temperament in Middle Childhood Questionnaire Self- Report (TMCQ-Self) was utilized to measure self-reported observations of attention (Simonds & Rothbart, 2004). This 157-item questionnaire measures 17 domains of child temperament. The focus of this study however was on self-rated attentional focusing (α = .63) and children responded only to items associated with that scale. Children were told they would be asked a few things about themselves and it was really important that they answer as truthfully as possible. Children used stampers with brightly colored ink to mark their answers on a sheet that contained a 5-point likert scale (none, a little, sometimes, mostly, a lot). The experimenter would read out a statement; for instance, I like candy. Then follow with, do you not like candy? Do you like candy a little bit? Do you like candy sometimes? Do you like candy most of the time? Or do you like candy a lot? Then children were told to stamp their answers on the sheet in front of them with whichever stamp they liked. All statements were asked in this matter. Three practice statements were given in order to assess if the child comprehended the instructions. Two children did not demonstrate comprehension of the task and began to draw on the form and thus contributed no self-report attention data. The children reported relatively low levels of self-rated attention (M = 2.22, SE = .09, range = .30 – 4.00). The sample range, however, shows that there was variability in children’s responses to their own attention focusing behaviors (see Table 1).
EEG
The EEG electrodes were placed on the child’s head using an EEG cap (Electro-Cap International, Inc., Eaton, OH). Recordings were made from 24 left and right scalp sites [frontal pole (Fp1, Fp2), medial frontal (F3, F4), lateral frontal (F7, F8), central (C3, C4), central frontal (FC1, FC2, FC5 FC6), temporal (T7, T8), lateral parietal (P7, P8), medial parietal (P3, P4), central parietal (CP1, CP2, CP5, CP6), and occipital (O1, O2)]. All electrode sites were referenced to Cz during recordings.
After the EEG cap was placed on the head, a small amount of abrasive gel was placed into each recording site and the scalp gently rubbed. Next, conductive gel was placed in each site and the scalp gently rubbed. Electrode impedances were measured and accepted if they were below 10K Ohms. The electrical activity from each lead was amplified using separate James Long Company Bioamps (Caroga Lake, NY) and bandpassed from .1 to 100 Hz. Activity for each lead was displayed on the monitor of an acquisition computer. The EEG signal was digitized at 512 Hz for each channel so that the data would not be affected by aliasing. The acquisition software was Snapshot-Snapstream (HEM Data Corp., Southfield, MI) and the raw EEG data were stored for later analyses.
EEG data were examined and analyzed using EEG Analysis System software developed by James Long Company (Caroga Lake, NY). Data were re-referenced via software to an average reference configuration and then artifact scored for eye movements using a peak-to-peak criterion of 100 uV or greater. Artifact associated with gross motor movements over 200 uV peak-to-peak were also scored. These artifact-scored epochs were eliminated from all subsequent analyses. Finally, some children had excessive artifact specific to the electrodes of interest in this study. The data were then analyzed with a discrete Fourier transform (DFT) using a Hanning window of 1-second width and 50% overlap. Power was computed for the 8 to 10 Hz (lower alpha) frequency band and expressed as mean square microvolts. Data were transformed using the natural log (ln) to normalize the distribution. Nine children were missing EEG data (1 refused to wear the EEG cap, 1 removed the cap during the task, 2 had excessive gross motor artifact throughout the protocol, and 5 had excessive artifact at parietal electrode sites). Child EEG data were included if the number of artifact-free DFT windows for each phase of processing (baseline, familiarization, test) exceeded 10 (Watson & Bell, 2013). Two children did not meet this minimum requirement and did not contribute physiological data. Because task-related changes in EEG power (in comparison with baseline) are hypothesized to represent activation of brain areas underlying specific scalp electrodes (Cuevas & Bell, 2011), we derived a measure of neural activity at parietal scalp locations by subtracting baseline EEG power from EEG power during familiarization (i.e., encoding) and by subtracting baseline EEG power from EEG power during recognition (i.e., retrieval) at parietal electrode sites (CP1, CP2, CP5, and CP6).
Results
Age was not correlated with attention (p > .77). The correlation between age and EM performance approached significance, with older children performing better than younger children (r = .19, p > .07). Children’s reports of their own attention positively correlated with their EM performance (r = .23, p = .03). EEG baseline-to-task change scores (i.e., task EEG power - baseline EEG power) were computed for encoding and retrieval trails for CP1, CP2, CP5, and CP6. Decreases in task-related EEG power, relative to baseline, were observed for both encoding and retrieval at electrodes CP1 (encoding, M = −.02, SD = .28; retrieval, M = −.07, SD = .34) and CP2 (encoding, M = −.02, SD = .29; retrieval, M = −.03, SD = .39). Increases in EEG power were observed at CP5 (encoding, M = .03, SD = .28; retrieval, M = −.01, SD = .30) and CP6 (encoding, M = .05, SD = .31; retrieval, M = .06, SD = .44), with the exception of a decrease at CP5 during retrieval. A MANOVA suggested no age-related differences in EEG change scores, (F (16, 148) = 1.60, p = .08).
Regression analyses were conducted to examine contributions of child age, attention, and EEG power changes from baseline-to-task on EM task performance (i.e., mean number correct). Child age and self-ratings of attention were entered in Step 1. EEG change scores at individual electrode sites were entered in Step 2. Our hypotheses were associated with posterior parietal (we used central parietal for posterior parietal) areas and we performed separate regression analyses to examine baseline-to-familiarization and baseline-to-test EEG change scores. Children’s electrophysiology may provide additional variance to children’s EM score above and beyond child age and self-reports of attention.
Results of regression analyses using baseline-to-familiarization EEG (i.e., during encoding) are presented in Table 2. Before children’s posterior parietal EEG was entered into model, children’s attention significantly predicted their performance F(2, 75) = 4.01, p < .03 However, the model was not significant after accounting for baseline-to-familiarity EEG (F(6, 75) = 1.81, p = .11).
Table 2.
EM Number Correct (N= 76) | b | SE b | β | t | p | sR2 |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Step 1 | ||||||
Age | .002 | .001 | .17 | 1.53 | .13 | .03 |
Child Reported Attention | .3 | .01 | .27 | 2.42 | .02 | .07 |
Step 2 | ||||||
Age | .001 | .001 | .14 | 1.17 | .25 | .02 |
Child Reported Attention | .03 | .01 | .26 | 2.27 | .03 | .06 |
Baseline-to-task change CP1 | .07 | .06 | .19 | 1.19 | .24 | .02 |
Baseline-to-task change CP2 | −.07 | .05 | −.20 | −1.30 | .20 | .02 |
Baseline-to-task change CP5 | −.05 | .05 | −.16 | −1.02 | .31 | .01 |
Baseline-to-task change CP6 | .05 | .05 | .15 | .95 | .35 | .01 |
Note. Step 1 R2 < .10, p >.02; Step 2 ∆R2 < .04, p >.57
Results of regression analyses using baseline-to-recognition EEG (i.e., during retrieval) are presented in Table 3. When the final model included age, child reports of attention, and posterior parietal EEG recorded during the test portion of the EM task, posterior parietal changes at CP1 and CP5, as well as self-reported attention, significantly contributed to EM performance accounting for 15% of the variance, F(6, 73) = 3.09, p = .01.
Table 3.
EM Number Correct (N= 75) | b | SE b | β | t | p | sR2 |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Step 1 | ||||||
Age | .002 | .001 | .17 | 1.49 | .14 | .03 |
Child Reported Attention | .03 | .01 | .27 | 2.36 | .02 | .07 |
Step 2 | ||||||
Age | .002 | .001 | .17 | 1.10 | .17 | .02 |
Child Reported Attention | .03 | .01 | .24 | 2.20 | .03 | .06 |
Baseline-to-task change CP1 | .10 | .05 | .37 | 2.20 | .03 | .06 |
Baseline-to-task change CP2 | −.06 | .05 | −.20 | −1.17 | .25 | .02 |
Baseline-to-task change CP5 | −.14 | .05 | −.45 | −2.81 | .007 | .09 |
Baseline-to-task change CP6 | .08 | .05 | .24 | 1.52 | .13 | .03 |
Note. Step 1 R2 < .10, p >.03; Step 2 ∆R2 < .12, p <.05
Discussion
With respect to the regression analyses performed, there were three main findings. First, only the model containing recognition-related (i.e., retrieval) posterior parietal EEG change scores contributed variance to EM scores in middle childhood. Second, child self-reports of attention predicted EM performance. Lastly, child age was not a significant contributor of variance to EM performance.
Results suggest that the change in EEG activation from baseline to task predicts children’s EM performance. We know that EEG continues to develop during middle childhood (see Bell, 1998, for a review of EEG development), and our data show that changes in parietal activation accounted for significant variance in EM task performance. Parietal areas are thought to be associated with the subjective experience of vividness and confidence in memories as well as with helping to hold the qualitative content of memories for retrieval, and thus make them accessible to decision-making processes (Ciaramelli, Grady & Moscovitch, 2008) including EM. That parietal EEG was predictive of EM only when examining the baseline-to-recognition change compared to baseline-to-familiarization EEG change, suggests that the cognitively demanding aspect of retrieving information associated with recognition EEG yields critical information for the subjective nature of memory performance. This is also true in EEG work with adults (Deater-Deckard & Bell 2016). Indeed, the PPC is thought to be involved in allocating attentional resources (Cabeza et al., 2008), specifically item-context associations (Mecklinger, 2010), which in turn may enhance mechanisms involved in EM retrieval.
Our data also captured one specific aspect of EEG activity during retrieval that was associated with individual differences in EM performance, and that was changes in EEG activity at two posterior parietal electrodes (CP1 & CP5) of the left hemisphere. We did not have specific hypotheses regarding differences between encoding and retrieval EEG; however, it is not surprising that parietal EEG activity during retrieval contributed to memory performance, while encoding did not. Indeed, previous research suggests that parietal regions are active during retrieval, but not encoding (Cabeza et al., 1997; Wagner et al., 2005). This activation pattern may be attributed to the need to direct attention to relevant features during retrieval (Vilberg & Rugg, 2008), which would be required when engaging in old/new judgments. Our hypotheses were also not specific to hemisphere; although because our stimuli were photographs of familiar objects, finding effects for only left hemisphere EEG should not be surprising. Perhaps children utilized their left hemisphere to help them discriminate local feature differences between objects during the recognition task as the left hemisphere has been linked with various cognitive abilities including attending to local cues and visual-spatial analyses on local features (Vallortigara & Rogers, 2005). Alternately, perhaps children were naming the objects as they viewed them and utilized left hemisphere language areas in the process (Lochy, van Reybroeck, & Rossion, 2016).
The mean values of our EEG baseline-to-task change scores suggested that alpha power decreased from baseline to retrieval (desynchronized) at CP1 and increased (synchronized) at CP5. Further, CP5 desynchronization positively and CP1 synchronization negatively predicted memory performance. In adults, alpha synchronization (i.e., increased power) is associated with decreased cortical activity and resting cognition, while desynchronization is associated with increased cortical activity and cognitive control (Pfurtscheller, Stancak, & Neuper, 1996; Sauseng, Klimesch, Doppelmayr, Pecherstorfer, Freunberger, & Hanslmayr, 2005). Our results suggest that children between 6–8 years display EEG patterns that are comparable to adults.
In support of our prediction, attention did significantly predict EM performance. The TMCQ self-report utilized in this study is believed to capture children’s attentional focusing (Simonds & Rothbart, 2004). Results suggest that attentional focusing may be important for recognition ability during middle childhood. These data also suggest that children may contribute critical self-report of cognitive information (Simonds & Rothbart, 2004).
The development of EM has been characterized as a continuous increase in the quantity and quality of memory accuracy (Ofen et al., 2007). Developmental studies of EM have shown consistent improvement across development, with mature performance occurring during adolescence (Cycowicz et al., 2001; Mandler & Robinson, 1978). Age-related changes in EM performance are thought to be due to different memory functions developing at different rates (e.g., Ghetti & Angelini, 2008). Moreover, due to the established relation between attention and memory, we expected attentional skills to contribute unique variance to memory performance. However, this study utilized children’s physiology to further understand individual differences in EM performance in addition to maturation and attentional control abilities in middle childhood. Our results suggest that child age did not contribute unique variance in children’s EM performance when examining baseline-to-task EEG; it may be the case that with older children, child age no longer contributes unique variance when also examining encoding/familiarization or retrieval/test EEG. This would be an intriguing developmental pattern and would inform regarding the effects of age relative to the effects of developing EEG activity. Moreover, although examining individual electrode sites and hemispheric specialization is informative, future studies should also examine EEG composite scores and obtain information about the temporal relationships and functional connectivity of EM in this population.
Our study was not without challenges. We had more children with unusable EEG data than is typical for EEG research studies during middle childhood (e.g., Rajan & Bell, 2015); nine children had excessive artifact in their EEG recordings and two did not tolerate the EEG electrodes. Additionally, thirteen children did not pass the practice trials for our EM task and two children were not able to complete the self-report attention measure. Larger sample sizes and longitudinal research is critically needed to further elucidate these relations.
In sum, the findings from our study provide evidence for an association between EM and children’s neurophysiology. Parietal baseline-to-retrieval EEG and self-reports of attention were linked to EM performance, where baseline-to-familiarization EEG and child age was not. These findings suggest that more developmental studies combining psychological and neurophysiological measures are required to reveal critical information regarding normative EM development during middle childhood.
Highlights.
Examined children’s episodic memory (EM), attention, & neurophysiological functioning.
Parietal baseline-to-task changes in EEG and attention linked to EM performance.
Parietal EEG may yield critical information about EM in middle childhood.
Acknowledgments
We are grateful to the families for their participation in our research. We thank Vinaya Rajan, Amanda Watson Joyce, Trisha Breslauer, April Medina, and Emma Schmitt for their assistance with data collection and coding.
Funding
This research was supported by the Eunice Kennedy Shriver National Institute of Child Health and Human Development (NICHD) grant [HD049878] awarded to Martha Ann Bell. The content of this manuscript is solely the responsibility of the authors and does not necessarily represent the official views of the NICHD or the National Institutes of Health.
Footnotes
Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final citable form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.
References
- Archibald LM, Levee T, Olino T. Attention allocation: Relationships to general working memory or specific language processing. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology. 2015;139:83–98. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2015.06.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bell MA. The ontogeny of the EEG during infancy and childhood: Implications for cognitive development. In: Garreau B, editor. Neuroimaging in child neuropsychiatric disorders. Berlin: Springer-Verlag; 1998. pp. 97–111. [Google Scholar]
- Bell MA, Cuevas K. Using EEG to study cognitive development: Issues and practices. Journal of Cognition and Development. 2012;13:281–294. doi: 10.1080/15248372.2012.691143. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Blankenship TL, Bell MA. Frontal-temporal coherence and executive functions contribute to active and passive processing memory performance in middle childhood. Developmental Neuropsychology. 2015;40:430–444. doi: 10.1080/87565641.2016.1153099. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Blankenship TL, O'Neill M, Ross A, Bell MA. Working memory and recollection contribute to academic achievement. Learning and Individual Differences. 2015;43:164–169. doi: 10.1016/j.lindif.2015.08.020. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Burgess AP, Gruzelier JH. Short duration power changes in the EEG during recognition memory for words and faces. Psychophysiology. 2000;37:596–606. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cabeza R, Ciaramelli E, Olson IR, Moscovitch M. The parietal cortex and episodic memory: An attentional account. Nature Reviews Neuroscience. 2008;9:613–625. doi: 10.1038/nrn2459. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cabeza R, Grady CL, Nyberg L, McIntosh AR, Tulving E, Kapur S, Craik FI. Age-related differences in neural activity during memory encoding and retrieval: a positron emission tomography study. Journal of Neuroscience. 1997;17:391–400. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.17-01-00391.1997. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chun MM, Turk-Browne NB. Interactions between attention and memory. Current Opinion in Neurobiology. 2007:17177–184. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2007.03.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ciaramellia E, Gradya CL, Moscovitch M. Top-down and bottom-up attention to memory: A hypothesis (AtoM) on the role of the posterior parietal cortex in memory retrieval. Neuropsychologia. 2008;6:1828–1851. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2008.03.022. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cuevas K, Bell MA. EEG and ECG from 5 to 10 months of age: Developmental changes in baseline activation and cognitive processing during a working memory task. International Journal of Psychophysiology. 2011;80:119–128. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2011.02.009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cycowicz YM, Friedman D, Duff M. Pictures and their colors: What do children remember? Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 2003;15:759–768. doi: 10.1162/089892903322307465. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cycowicz YM, Friedman D, Snodgrass JG, Duff M. Recognition and source memory for pictures in children and adults. Neuropsychologia. 2001;39:255–267. doi: 10.1016/S0028-3932(00)00108-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Czernochowski D, Brinkmann M, Mecklinger A, Johansson M. When binding matters: An ERP analysis of the development of recollection and familiarity. In: Mecklinger A, Zimmer H, Lindenberger U, editors. Bound in Memory: Insights from Behavioral and Neuropsychological Studies. Aachen, Germany; Shaker Verlag: 2004. [Google Scholar]
- Deater-Deckard K, Bell MA. Maternal executive function, heart rate, and EEG alpha reactivity interact in the prediction of harsh parenting. Journal of Family Psychology. 2017;31:41–50. doi: 10.1037/fam0000286. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Diaz A, Bell MA. Information processing efficiency and regulation at five months. Infant Behavior and Development. 2011;34:239–247. doi: 10.1016/j.infbeh.2010.12.011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ghetti S, Angelini L. The development of recollection and familiarity in childhood and adolescence: Evidence from the dual-process signal detection model. Child Development. 2008;79:339–358. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2007.01129.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ghetti S, Bunge SA. Neural changes underlying the development of episodic memory during middle childhood. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience. 2012;2:381–395. doi: 10.1016/j.dcn.2012.05.002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Holmbeck GN. Post-hoc probing of significant moderational and mediational effects in studies of pediatric populations. Journal of Pediatric Psychology. 2002;27:87–96. doi: 10.1093/jpepsy/27.1.87. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Johansson M, Mecklinger A. The late posterior negativity in ERP studies of episodic memory retrieval: Action monitoring and retrieval of attribute conjunctions. Biological Psychology. 2003;64:91–117. doi: 10.1016/S0301-0511(03)00104-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ofen N, Kao YC, Sokol-Hessner P, Kim H, Whitfield-Gabrieli S, Gabrieli JD. Development of the declarative memory system in the human brain. NatureNeuroscience. 2007;10:1198–1205. doi: 10.1038/nn1950. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Klimesch W. Alpha-band oscillations, attention, and controlled access to stored information. Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 2012;16:606–617. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2012.10.007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Klimesch W. EEG alpha and theta oscillations reflect cognitive and memory performance: a review and analysis. Brain Research Review. 1999;29:169–195. doi: 10.1016/S0165-0173(98)00056-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lochy A, van Reybroeck M, Rossion B. Loft cortical specialization for visual letter strings predicts rudimentary knowledge of letter-sound association in preschoolers. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 2016;113:8544–8549. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1520366113. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Makovsik T, Jiang YV. Distributing versus focusing attention in visual short-term memory. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review. 2007:141072–1078. doi: 10.3758/bf03193093. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Makovsik T, Sussman R, Jiang YV. Orienting attention in visual working memory reduces interference from memory probes. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition. 2008;34:369–380. doi: 10.1037/0278-7393.34.2.369. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mandler JM, Robinson CA. Developmental changes in picture recognition. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology. 1978;26:122–136. doi: 10.1016/0022-0965(78)90114-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Marshall DH, Drummery AB, Fox NA, Newcombe NS. An event-related potential study of item recognition memory in children and adults. Journal of Cognition and Development. 2002;3:201–224. doi: 10.1207/S15327647JCD0302_4. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Mecklinger A. The control of long-term memory: Brain systems and cognitive processes. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews. 2010;34:1055–1065. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2009.11.020. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mecklinger A, Brunnemann N, Kipp K. Two processes for recognition memory in children of early school age: An event-related potential study. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 2011;23:435–446. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2010.21455. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mirandola C, Del Prete F, Ghetti S, Cornoldi C. Recollection but not familiarity differentiates memory for text in students with and without learning difficulties. Learning and Individual Differences. 2011;21:206–209. doi: 10.1016/j.lindif.2010.12.001. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Nouwens S, Groen MA, Verhoeven L. How storage and executive functions contribute to children’s reading comprehension. Learning and Individual Differences. 2016;47:96–102. doi: 10.1016/j.lindif.2015.12.008. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Perry NB, Swingler MM, Calkins SD, Bell MA. Neurophysiological correlates of attention behavior in early infancy: Implications for emotion regulation during early childhood. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology. 2016;142:245–261. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2015.08.007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pezzica S, Pinto G, Bigozzi L, Vezzani C. Where is my attention? Children’s metaknowledge expressed through drawings. Educational Psychology. 2015;36:616–637. doi: 10.1080/01443410.2014.1003035. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Pfurtscheller G, Stancák A, Jr, Neuper C. Event-related synchronization (ERS) in the alpha band—an electrophysiological correlate of cortical idling: A review. International Journal of Psychophysiology. 1996:2439–46. doi: 10.1016/S0167-8760(96)00066-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rajan V, Bell MA. Developmental changes in fact and source recall: Contributions from executive function and brain electrical activity. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience. 2015;12:1–11. doi: 10.1016/j.dcn.2014.10.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ridderinkhof KR, van der Molen MW, Band GP, Bashore TR. Sources of interference from irrelevant information: A developmental study. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology. 1997;65:315–341. doi: 10.1006/jecp.1997.2367. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Riggins T. Longitudinal investigation of source memory reveals different developmental trajectories for item memory and binding. Developmental Psychology. 2014;50:449–459. doi: 10.1037/a0033622. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Riggins T, Miller NC, Bauer PJ, Georgieff MK, Nelson CA. Electrophysiological indices of memory for temporal order in early childhood: implications for the development of recollection. Developmental Science. 2009;12:209–219. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2008.00757.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ritchey M, Montchal ME, Yonelinas AP, Ranganath C. Delay-dependent contributions of medial temporal lobe regions to episodic memory retrieval. Elife. 2015;4:1–19. doi: 10.7554/eLife.05025. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rugg MD, Schloerscheidt AM, Mark RE. An electrophysiological comparison of two indices of recollection. Journal of Memory and Language. 1998;39:47–69. doi: 10.1006/jmla.1997.2555. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Sauseng P, Klimesch W, Doppelmayr M, Pecherstorfer T, Freunberger R, Hanslmayr S. EEG alpha synchronization and functional coupling during top-down processing in a working memory task. Human Brain Mapping. 2005;26:148–155. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20150. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Simonds J, Rothbart MK. The Temperament in Middle Childhood Questionnaire (TMCQ): A computerized self-report measure of temperament for ages 7–10; Poster session presented at the Occasional Temperament Conference; Athens, GA. 2004. Oct, [Google Scholar]
- Vallortigara G, Rogers LJ. Survival with an asymmetrical brain: Advantages and disadvantages of cerebral lateralization. Behavioral and Brain Sciences. 2005;28:575–589. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X05000105. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vilberg KL, Rugg MD. Functional significance of retrieval-related activity in lateral parietal cortex: Evidence from fMRI and ERPs. Human Brain Mapping. 2009;30:1490–1501. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20618. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vilberg KL, Rugg MD. Memory retrieval and the parietal cortex: A review of evidence from a dual-process perspective. Neuropsychologia. 2008;46:1787–1799. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2008.01.004. doi:1016/j.neuropsychologia.2008.01.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wagner AD, Shannon BJ, Kahn I, Buckner RL. Parietal lobe contributions to episodic memory retrieval. Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 2005;9:445–453. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2005.07.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Warrington EK, Ackroyd C. The effect of orienting tasks on recognition memory. Memory & Cognition. 1975;3:140–142. doi: 10.3758/BF03212890. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Watson AJ, Bell MA. Individual differences in inhibitory control skills at three years of age. Developmental Neuropsychology. 2013;38:1–21. doi: 10.1080/87565641.2012.718818. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Welsh MC, Pennington BF, Groisser DB. A normative-development study of executive function: A window on prefrontal function in children. Developmental Neuropsychology. 1991;7:131–149. doi: 10.1080/87565649109540483. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Wolfe CD, Bell MA. Working memory and inhibitory control in early childhood: Contributions from physiology, temperament, and language. Developmental Psychobiology. 2004;44:68–83. doi: 10.1002/dev.10152. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wolfe CD, Bell MA. Sources of variability in working memory in early childhood: A consideration of age, temperament, language, and brain electrical activity. Cognitive Development. 2007;22:431–455. doi: 10.1016/j.cogdev.2007.08.007. [DOI] [Google Scholar]