Abstract
Music has been identified as a strength in people with Autism Spectrum Disorder; however, there is currently no neuroscientific evidence supporting its benefits. Given its universal appeal, intrinsic reward value and ability to modify brain and behaviour, music may be a potential therapeutic aid in autism. Here we evaluated the neurobehavioural outcomes of a music intervention, compared to a non-music control intervention, on social communication and brain connectivity in school-age children (ISRCTN26821793). Fifty-one children aged 6–12 years with autism were randomized to receive 8–12 weeks of music (n = 26) or non-music intervention (n = 25). The music intervention involved use of improvisational approaches through song and rhythm to target social communication. The non-music control was a structurally matched behavioural intervention implemented in a non-musical context. Groups were assessed before and after intervention on social communication and resting-state functional connectivity of fronto-temporal brain networks. Communication scores were higher in the music group post-intervention (difference score = 4.84, P = .01). Associated post-intervention resting-state brain functional connectivity was greater in music vs. non-music groups between auditory and subcortical regions (z = 3.94, P < .0001) and auditory and fronto-motor regions (z = 3.16, P < .0001). Post-intervention brain connectivity was lower between auditory and visual regions in the music compared to the non-music groups, known to be over-connected in autism (z = 4.01, P < .00001). Post-intervention brain connectivity in the music group was related to communication improvement (z = 3.57, P < .0001). This study provides the first evidence that 8–12 weeks of individual music intervention can indeed improve social communication and functional brain connectivity, lending support to further investigations of neurobiologically motivated models of music interventions in autism.
Introduction
Autism Spectrum Disorder (ASD) is a neurodevelopmental condition characterized by social communication difficulties and restricted and repetitive behaviours among strengths in varied domains1. ASD is highly prevalent but there is considerable heterogeneity in its aetiology, clinical presentation and underlying brain connectivity1,2. Consequently, a variety of behavioural and psychosocial treatments are sought by families3. However, there is little consensus on which treatments are most effective4. Thus, a diagnosis of ASD is associated with substantial costs to the individual, the family and the community5.
ASD is a lifelong condition with a median age of diagnosis >4 years6, although most current intervention strategies target children <6 years to promote early behavioural change3. Individuals with ASD and their families face significant challenges during developmental transitions7. School-age children in particular often remain unengaged in social settings, reducing opportunities for socio-communicative development8,9. This has led to investigations of alternate and creative means of expression such as music that might improve social communication, increasing prospects for meaningful relationships. Furthermore, cross-culturally applicable music-based interventions hold potential for scalability at home, school and global community settings10.
Previous randomized controlled trials (RCTs) of music interventions for ASD have reported positive effects of music on emotional engagement, social interaction, communication and parent–child relationships11,12, suggesting that musical activities in a therapeutic context can promote measurable behavioural changes in children with ASD. Strengths in music processing have been noted since the first description of ASD13 and many studies have reported intact or enhanced musical skills such as absolute pitch, enhanced melodic memory and contour-processing in children with ASD14–16. Greater brain responses to song versus speech in fronto-temporal brain regions17,18 and intact emotional responsiveness to music have also been demonstrated19. Supporting anecdotal reports from parents and caregivers have described the profound effects music has had on children with ASD20.
The positive impact of music on social skills has been demonstrated beyond ASD21,22. Typically developing children are more likely to play with another following a shared musical experience23 and joint musical interactions can enhance emotional empathy, prosociality and bonding in children24–26. More recently, neuroimaging studies have shown that participating in musical activities engages a multimodal network of brain regions involved in hearing, movement, emotion, pleasure and memory27–31, thus allowing transfer of music-related therapeutic effects to non-musical domains32 through structural and functional brain changes33,34. However, a direct link between effects of music interventions and changes in the brain is yet to be demonstrated in autism35,36 and was our aim here.
Altered intrinsic brain connectivity is a hallmark of ASD. Both over-connectivity and under-connectivity have been reported, in particular, under-connectivity of fronto-temporal and cortico-subcortical networks and over-connectivity of sensory networks may be considered potential treatment targets37–40 given their associations with verbal and social communication skills in autism18,41. Resting-state functional magnetic resonance imaging (rsfMRI) allows measurement of intrinsic brain connectivity by computing temporal correlations of spontaneous blood-oxygen-level-dependent (BOLD) signals among spatially distributed brain regions and may be a promising target of music-induced neuroplasticity40,42. The use of resting-state functional connectivity (RSFC) as an outcome measure for intervention studies, particularly for clinical populations, has been recommended since it affords the advantage of being task-independent, has high test–retest reliability, limited practice effects and can provide reliable estimates of functional brain connectivity corresponding to underlying anatomy43.
Currently, evidence for effectiveness of music interventions is limited and there is no neuroscientific basis for its use in ASD. However, given the impact of music on social functioning and brain connectivity, alongside atypicalities in these areas in ASD, music-based activities may restore altered brain connectivity and social difficulties in ASD. Synthesizing findings from previous research, two possible mechanisms for such music-induced neuroplasticity and its impact on social functioning may be proposed:32,36,44,45 (1) top–down reward-based cortical modulation to reinforce learning of non-musical behaviours such as social interactions through the intrinsic reward value of music, (2) bottom–up sensorimotor integration through sound and auditory–motor entrainment of neural networks through synchronization leading to modulation of atypical sensory processing, which in turn may improve social communication41,46. Our goal was to investigate whether music-based interventions can indeed alter spontaneous rsfMRI signals, leading to improved functioning in ASD based on one of the above hypotheses.
The specific aim of this RCT was to investigate whether 8–12 weeks of a music-based intervention (compared to a non-music control intervention) can improve social communication, family quality of life (FQoL) and functional brain connectivity in school-age children with ASD. This would provide evidence for an effective, inexpensive, easy-to-administer and relatively non-specialized strength-based intervention that may be scaled in varied settings across cultures, addressing the need for globally applicable ASD intervention models10.
Materials and methods
Study design
We report an assessor-blinded, parallel-group RCT47,48 of a music intervention (MT) compared to a non-music control intervention (NM) for improving social communication and fronto-temporal brain connectivity in school-age children with ASD. The trial (isrctn.org: ISRCTN26821793) was conducted between April and December 2016 in Montreal, Canada with ethics approval from the Montreal Neurological Institute (MNI) at McGill University. Written informed consent was obtained from parents/guardians of participants.
Participants
Children aged 6–12 years, meeting Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Fourth edition criteria for ASD49, were screened from January to August 2016 (Fig. 1). Exclusion criteria were (1) individual music therapy within 6 months prior to study, (2) private musical lessons for a cumulative period of 1 year prior to study, (3) group music therapy in school; (4) <35 weeks of gestation, (5) hearing disorders or (6) a medical history of neurological disease. Power analysis using evidence-based effect size estimation11 suggested that for a large effect (d = 0.8) of music therapy on social communication, detectable with 80% power at P < .05, a sample of n = 50 (25 per arm) would be required.
Baseline assessment
Assessment at baseline consisted of two sessions. In the first session, detailed demographics on socioeconomic status50 (SES), handedness51, music experience history and past and current intervention history of the child were obtained. Participant diagnosis was confirmed using a best-estimate diagnosis of ASD supported by an ADOS (Autism Diagnostic Observation Scale52), Autism Diagnostic Interview–Revised53 or Childhood Autism Rating Scale54 and detailed clinical assessment report. Additionally, parent-reported behavioural outcomes on Social Responsiveness Scale (SRS-II55), the Children’s Communication Checklist (CCC-256), the maladaptive behaviour subscale of the Vineland Adaptive Behaviour Scales (VABS-MB57) as well as the Beach Family Quality of Life Scale (FQoL58) were obtained. Children’s cognitive ability was assessed using the Wechsler’s Abbreviated Intelligence Scale (WASI-II59). If the child had completed an intelligence quotient (IQ) test (WASI-I/II/WISC-IV/V) within 2 years of the study, available scores were used. Children’s language ability was assessed using the Sentence Repetition subtest of the Clinical Evaluation of Language Fundamentals (CELF-460,61) and receptive vocabulary was measured using the Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test (PPVT-462). Musical ability was assessed using the Montreal Battery for Evaluation of Musical Abilities63. Detailed baseline characteristics of participants are provided in Table 1.
Table 1.
Measure | Music | Non-music | P value | ||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
n | Mean | SD | n | Mean | SD | ||
Participant characteristics | |||||||
Age (in years) | 26 | 10.30 | 1.91 | 25 | 10.20 | 1.87 | .85 |
Sex (male:female) | 26 | 21:5 | 0.40 | 25 | 22:3 | 0.33 | .75 |
Social Communication Questionnaire | 26 | 21.31 | 5.90 | 25 | 19.68 | 5.50 | .31 |
ADOS totala | 22 | 15.64 | 5.50 | 13 | 14.84 | 4.62 | .65 |
Language impairmentb | 26 | 13/26 | — | 25 | 14/24 | — | .53 |
Parent-reported sentence level speech | 26 | 23/26 | — | 25 | 19/25 | — | .29 |
Verbal IQc | 25 | 94.72 | 21.40 | 23 | 87.30 | 23.47 | .26 |
Nonverbal IQc | 24 | 110.79 | 18.15 | 21 | 102.38 | 18.22 | .13 |
Full-Scale IQc | 25 | 102.00 | 18.82 | 24 | 94.00 | 18.18 | .14 |
MacArthur SES (Ladder)d | 26 | 5.38 | 1.83 | 25 | 5.72 | 2.28 | .57 |
Annual income (in $) | 25 | 39760 | 30847 | 25 | 43300 | 30145 | .68 |
Handedness (augmented laterality index)e | 26 | 71.12 | 51.63 | 25 | 73.28 | 52.74 | .88 |
Musical ability (MBEMA)f | 23 | 0.72 | 0.14 | 22 | 0.69 | 0.14 | .57 |
VABS gross motor skillsk | 26 | 13.5 | 2.2 | 25 | 13.24 | 2.08 | .67 |
VABS fine motor skillsk | 26 | 15.92 | 3.09 | 24 | 15.21 | 2.6 | .38 |
Number of therapy sessions completed | 26 | 10.50 | 1.61 | 25 | 10.16 | 1.70 | .47 |
Outcome measures | |||||||
SRS-II T-scoreg | 26 | 70.15 | 9.62 | 25 | 72.24 | 11.43 | .48 |
CCC-2 general compositeh | 25 | 76.84 | 14.44 | 23 | 77.65 | 13.35 | .46 |
PPVT-4 standard scorei | 26 | 94.58 | 26.18 | 25 | 85.48 | 29.42 | .25 |
Family Quality of Lifej | 26 | 102.38 | 13.62 | 25 | 104.08 | 13.79 | .66 |
VABS maladaptive behavioursk | 25 | 19.80 | 1.50 | 23 | 20.00 | 1.86 | .69 |
P values are calculated using independent samples t tests for continuous variables and chi-square tests for categorical variables between groups
SD standard deviation
aADOS: Autism Diagnostic Observation Scale Total score from ADOS or ADOS-2. Higher scores mean greater symptom severity
bNumber of participants meeting criteria for language impairment based on scaled scores 1 SD or greater below the mean (=10) on the Sentence Repetition subtest of Clinical Evaluation of Language Fundamentals (CELF-4)60,61
cIQ was measured using the Wechsler’s Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence (WASI-II) or the WISC-IV/V when scores were available from the past 2 years. Full-scale scores have a mean of 100 and SD of 15
dSocioeconomic status (SES) was measured using the MacArthur SES Ladder
eHandedness was measured using the augmented 15-item index of the Edinburgh handedness inventory
fMusical ability was measured using the global accuracy score on the Montreal Battery for Evaluation of Musical Abilities (MBEMA)63
gSRS-II: Social Responsiveness Scale. Range: higher scores mean poorer skills
hCCC-2: Children’s Communication Checklist. Details provided in Supplementary text
iPPVT-4: Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test. Details provided in Supplementary text
jFamily Quality of Life was measured using the Beach Questionnaire. Details provided in Supplementary text
kVABS: Vineland Adaptive Behaviour Scales. Estimated v-scale scores with mean of 15 and SD of 3 for the gross motor skills, fine motor skills and maladaptive behaviours subdomains are reported. Scores between 12 and 18 estimate performance in the average range
In the second session, participants completed a 20-minute MRI scan in a 3 Tesla Siemens Magnetom TimTrio scanner with a 32-channel head coil at the MNI. During this scan, participants were asked to fixate on a cross-hair on the screen. Resting-state BOLD echo-planar images were obtained in 38 slices with a 3.5 mm3 voxel resolution, covering the entire brain (TR = 2340 ms, TE = 30 ms, matrix size, 64 × 64; field of view (FOV), 224 mm; flip angle 90°). One hundred and forty volumes were obtained in 5 minutes 32 s. Participants also completed a high-resolution sagittal T1-weighted anatomical scan with a voxel resolution of 1 mm3 and an acceleration factor of 2. Participants with their parents underwent a detailed orientation procedure before the MRI scan to ensure comfort and compliance and to maximize good quality outcomes64. Audio-visual media aids and mock scanner trials were used in most cases to motivate the participants. Participants’ wakefulness and motion during the actual scans was monitored using an MRI-compatible infra-red camera.
Randomization and blinding
Fifty-one participants were randomized to MT (n = 26) or NM (n = 25) using the covariate-adaptive method65 where the first 20 participants were randomized using simple coin toss and remaining 31 by the MinimPy software (http://minimpy.sourceforge.net/) by the first author (M.S.), who was not involved in assessing behavioural outcomes. MinimPy is a free, open-source, desktop program implemented in Python, which allows random allocation of subjects to treatment groups in a clinical trial using a stochastic covariate-adaptive minimization algorithm66. The success of randomization was assessed by comparing baseline similarity of intervention groups. All other assessors and authors were blind to group allocation information. Our attempt to blind parents (who assessed parent-rated outcomes) was only partially successful, with 31 out of the 51 parents reporting awareness of group allocation. Data were independently double entered to ensure accuracy and stored on an electronic server with restricted, password-controlled access.
Interventions and fidelity
Both interventions (Fig. S1) involved 45-minute individual weekly sessions conducted over 8–12 weeks by the same accredited therapist (M.T.) using established approaches. Using a child-centric approach, MT made use of musical instruments, songs and rhythmic cues while targeting communication, turn-taking, sensorimotor integration, social appropriateness and musical interaction47,67–69. NM was designed as a structurally matched “active comparison” play-based intervention to control for non-specific factors, such as positive treatment expectancies, intervention support, therapist attention and emotional engagement. Both interventions were conducted in the same setting and targeted similar outcomes using theoretically motivated approaches70 such as creating a shared experience, building meaningful relationships and emphasizing self-expression71 through the use of varied activities targeting common goal such as verbal and social communication, multisensory integration and emotional regulation (SI Table S1). The primary difference was the use of music as a central component in MT. All sessions were video-recorded to assess treatment fidelity72 (Supplementary Information).
Outcomes
Behavioural outcomes
Primary behavioural outcomes included a social communication battery consisting of the CCC-2 to measure pragmatic communication, SRS-II to measure symptom severity and PPVT-4 to measure receptive vocabulary. Secondary outcomes were FQoL and the maladaptive behaviours subdomain of the VABS. Outcomes were selected to provide both direct and parent-reported evaluations of treatment-related change using measures that have good psychometric properties, limited practice effects and applicability to a wide range of individuals73,74 and were collected at baseline and post-intervention for n = 50 participants (Supplementary Information).
Statistical analysis
Behavioural outcomes were analysed by fitting linear mixed-effects models (LMEMs) with restriction maximum-likelihood estimation to cope with missing data, inhomogeneity of dependent-variable-variance across factor levels and unequal group size. LMEMs with treatment group (MT, NM), timepoint (baseline, post-intervention) and their interaction as well as participant intercept as random effect were estimated for all primary and secondary behavioural outcomes75. Prior to analysis, data were checked for normality. A group×timepoint interaction indicating a change in MT vs. NM post-intervention at P < .016 (Bonferroni-corrected from alpha-level of P = .05 to account for three primary behavioural outcomes) was considered significant. Clinical significance was limited to changes from baseline to post-intervention within MT or significant difference between MT and NM post-intervention as confirmed by post hoc Tukey tests at alpha-level of P = .05. An intention-to-treat analysis was carried out, whereby missing data from any drop-out participants was replaced with data at baseline. Both unstandardized (beta-coefficients and mean difference) scores76 and standardized effect sizes (standardized mean difference, Cohen’s d) are reported since standardized effect sizes are often influenced by study design and complexity of models used. Standardized effects sizes are calculated as the difference in change scores between groups divided by the pooled within- and between-group standard deviation77. The unstandardized measure is a simple effect size (with 95% confidence intervals (CIs)) in terms of mean difference and does not depend on variance estimates78. All statistical analyses were done in R v3.3.479.
Neuroimaging outcomes
Primary neuroimaging outcomes were intrinsic functional brain connectivity of fronto-temporal brain networks measured using rsfMRI at baseline and post-intervention. RSFC methods provide an approach for investigating how musical engagement may alter functional connectivity among several brain regions. RSFC metrics of inter-regional correlations specifically afford the advantage of being task-independent, have high test–retest reliability and provide reliable estimates of brain functional connectivity80. RSFC metrics also have limited practice effects and may provide an objective method to measure response-to-intervention40. Here we tested the extent to which music alters fronto-temporal RSFC in six fronto-temporal seed regions.
Image preprocessing
Resting-state images were first preprocessed using FSL (v. 5.0.9; www.fmrib.ox.ac.uk, FMRIB’s Software Library, FMRIB, Oxford, UK)81,82 via the SeeBARS pipeline developed at the Center for Research on Brain, Language and Music83. Image preprocessing steps consisted of removal of the first five volumes in each scan series as well as removal of non-brain tissue using BET81, slice-time correction, motion correction (using a six-parameter affine transformation implemented in FLIRT, global intensity normalization, spatial smoothing (Gaussian kernel of FWHM = 6 mm), temporal high-pass filtering (100 s) and temporal band-pass filtering (0.01–0.1 Hz). To achieve the transformation between the low-resolution functional data and standard space (MNI152: average T1 brain image constructed from 152 normal subjects), two transformations were performed: (1) T2*-weighted image to T1-weighted structural image (using a 7 degree of freedom (DOF) transformation) and (2) T1-weighted structural image to average standard space (using a 12 DOF linear affine transformation, voxel size = 2 × 2 × 2 mm3). In addition, physiological noise was removed using the method described by Vahdat and colleagues83. The global signal was calculated by averaging the time series over all voxels in the brain. In total, 18 nuisance regressors were used: white matter, cerebrospinal fluid, global signal and their derivatives, and six motion parameters and their derivatives in the first-level analysis84,85. Additional motion scrubbing was done using guidelines in Power et al. (2012)86. Volumes with framewise displacement (FD) = 0.5 mm or DVARS = 50 (the spatial root mean square of the data after temporal differencing) were masked from whole-brain analysis. Participants with >35% volumes censored at either timepoint were excluded from further analysis (n = 6, MT = 2, NM = 4).
Statistical analysis
Seeds were defined as 6 mm spheres around coordinates in the left and right Heschl’s gyrus (HG; ±46 −18 10), left and right inferior frontal gyrus (±50 18 7) and left and right temporal pole (TP; ±38 10 −28; Fig. S3). These seeds are known to anchor fronto-temporal networks involved in language and communication and altered in ASD87. The timeseries for each of the six seeds was used to generate individual participant-level maps using whole-brain general linear models at baseline and post-intervention. The unthresholded participant-level maps were then entered into a group-level analysis. To assess potential differences between groups at baseline, independent sample t tests were computed for maps from all seeds. No baseline differences between groups on any of the six RSFC networks was found (all P > .05). To compare groups post-intervention, we used adjusted analysis of covariance (ANCOVA) with post-intervention RSFC as the dependent variable and intervention group, mean-centred baseline RSFC, age, IQ and mean FD86 as covariates. Using covariate-adjusted ANCOVA models are more powerful as they can account for baseline imbalance and correlation between baseline and post-intervention measures, increase statistical power and minimize biases88–92. Z-scores of parameter estimates were used to measure connectivity strength. In RSFC maps where a difference between groups was observed, we evaluated whether post-intervention RSFC was related to improvement in behavioural outcomes (measured by difference scores) in a whole-brain analysis. Z-statistics were extracted for each participant from the post-intervention RSFC maps and used in a linear regression model to evaluate strength of the association between RSFC and behavioural improvement. To account for multiple comparisons, random-field theory using a cluster-forming threshold of P < .001 was applied93. To account for six seeds, a Bonferroni correction was used and a final alpha-level of P = .00016 was used for significance testing. All locations are described in MNI coordinates.
Results
Participants
One hundred and eleven children meeting diagnosis for ASD were screened in the community, of which 60 did not meet study criteria or declined to participate (further details are provided in Fig. 1). Fifty-one participants in the age range 6–12 years (mean age = 10.25 years, 8 females) were assessed at baseline and randomly assigned to music (MT; n = 26) or non-music (NM; n = 25) intervention groups (Fig. 1,S1). Assessment at baseline consisted of two sessions: (1) first, detailed demographics, diagnostic reports and baseline measurements of behavioural outcomes were obtained (Table 1). (2) In the second session, anatomical and rsfMRI brain images were obtained on a 3 Tesla MRI scanner. Participants did not differ at baseline on age, sex, language, motor skills, IQ, SES or musical ability and completed an average of 10.3 therapy sessions (n = 5 participants had <10 sessions) during the study (Table 1). Fifty participants completed follow-up assessments with one drop-out whose baseline data was used for analysis.
Treatment fidelity
Treatment fidelity72 of delivery of both interventions was assessed using 103 out of the 527 video-recorded intervention sessions by two raters blind to session order, not involved in the trial and demonstrating high inter-rater reliability (intraclass correlation coefficient = 0.91, P < .001). There was high adherence to treatment protocols, process fidelity (80–100% with no difference between groups; P = .24) and content fidelity (>75% with no difference between groups; P = .16) of delivered intervention with no difference in implementation fidelity across MT and NM (Supplementary Information).
Behavioural outcomes
Using LMEMs, we found that MT, relative to NM, showed improvements in communication on the CCC-2, indicated by a significant group×timepoint interaction corrected for multiple outcomes (β = −1.35, P = .01; MTPost-intervention–MTBaseline: post-hoc Tukey test, t = 1.43, P = .024, Fig. 2a). The simple effect size calculated as a mean difference between MT and NM scores from baseline to post-intervention was 4.84 (95% CI: 0.76–8.92) with a larger proportion in the MT group (15/26) compared to the NM group (5/24) showing an improvement. An exploratory analysis of scaled subtests of the CCC-2 (not subjected to correction for multiple comparisons) revealed that these differences stemmed from tests of structural language, Speech (P = .01) and Semantics (P = .046); a pragmatics subtest, Inappropriate Initiations (P = .006); and two autism-relevant subtests (not included in the CCC-2 composite): Social Relations (P = .048) and Interests (P = .02). There was no group×timepoint interaction on the SRS-II55 t-scores (β = −0.04, P = .92; mean difference = 0.65, 95% CI:−3.25 to 4.1), or PPVT-462 standard scores (β = 0.15, P = .78; mean difference = 0.03, 95% CI:−4.32 to 4.38; Fig. 2b, c). There was, however, a significant group×timepoint interaction on parent-reported FQoL (β = −1.9, P = .01, Fig. 2d) with mean difference = 7.06 favouring MT (95% CI: 0.79 to 13.33) even though no post hoc tests were significant. Additionally, both groups showed reduction in maladaptive behaviours on the VABS post-intervention (β = 0.22, P = .01, Fig. 2e, Table 2, SI Table S2).
Table 2.
Outcomes | Observed values | Effect size | |||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Music | Non-music | Mean difference | ±95% CI | Standardized effect size (d) | |||||
n | Mean | ±95% CI | n | Mean | ±95% CI | ||||
Primary outcomes | |||||||||
CCC-2 | 4.84 | 4.08 | 0.34 | ||||||
Baseline | 25 | 76.84 | 5.64 | 23 | 77.65 | 5.45 | |||
Post-intervention | 24 | 80.46 | 6.43 | 23 | 76.43 | 5.02 | |||
Changes from baseline | 25 | 3.62 | 0.78 | 23 | −1.22 | −0.43 | |||
SRS-II | 0.65 | 3.45 | 0.06 | ||||||
Baseline | 26 | 70.15 | 3.68 | 25 | 72.24 | 4.47 | |||
Post-intervention | 26 | 69.36 | 4.39 | 25 | 70.8 | 3.98 | |||
Changes from baseline | 26 | −0.79 | 0.71 | 25 | −1.44 | −0.49 | |||
PPVT-4 | 0.03 | 4.35 | 0.00 | ||||||
Baseline | 26 | 94.57 | 10.05 | 25 | 85.48 | 11.52 | |||
Post-intervention | 26 | 95.04 | 10.66 | 25 | 85.92 | 12.11 | |||
Changes from baseline | 26 | 0.47 | 0.61 | 25 | 0.44 | 0.59 | |||
Secondary outcomes | |||||||||
FQoL | 7.06 | 6.27 | 0.57 | ||||||
Baseline | 26 | 102.42 | 5.25 | 25 | 104.08 | 5.39 | |||
Post-intervention | 26 | 105.36 | 3.86 | 25 | 99.96 | 4.65 | |||
Changes from baseline | 26 | 2.94 | −1.39 | 25 | −4.12 | −0.74 | |||
VABS-MBa | 0.08 | 0.65 | 0.04 | ||||||
Baseline | 26 | 19.8 | 0.59 | 24 | 20 | 0.74 | |||
Post-intervention | 26 | 19.42 | 0.71 | 24 | 19.54 | 0.86 | |||
Changes from baseline | 26 | −0.38 | 0.12 | 24 | −0.46 | 0.12 |
CCC-2 Children’s Communication Checklist Composite score, SRS-II Social Responsiveness Scale T-Score, PPVT-4 Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test Standard score, FQoL Family Quality of Life total score measured using the Beach Centre Scale, CI confidence interval
aVABS-MB Vineland Adaptive Behaviour Scales–Maladaptive behaviour subdomain v-scale score. Scores <18 are average, scores of 18–20 are elevated and scores 21–24 are clinically significant
Brain connectivity outcomes
There were no baseline differences between groups on any of the six RSFC networks (all P > .05). Using covariate-adjusted ANCOVA models, we found greater RSFC post-intervention in the MT group compared with NM between auditory seeds (left and right HG) and striatal and motor regions (right HG: z = 3.94, P = .000019, left HG: z = 3.79, P = .00009, Fig. 3a, b, SI Table S3) and reduced RSFC in MT between auditory seeds (left HG and right TP) and visual regions (left HG: z = 3.39, P < .00001, right TP: z = 4.01, P < .00001, Fig. 3d, e, SI Table S3).
To evaluate whether changes in RSFC were related to improvements in behavioural outcome, we tested whole-brain models with CCC-2 improvement (CCC-2Post-Intervention−CCC-2Baseline) as covariate of interest for the three seeds (left HG, right HG and right TP) where significant differences between groups was found. Greater RSFC post-intervention between left HG and subcortical thalamic and striatal regions was related to greater improvement on CCC-2 scores (z = 3.57, P < .0001, Fig. 3c). Lower post-intervention RSFC between right HG and visual areas was related to greater improvement in CCC-2 scores (z = 3.64, P < .001, Fig. 3f).
Discussion
Individuals with ASD have a unique profile of strengths amid limitations, which can be harnessed to design treatment paradigms that improve functional outcomes94. Given their universal appeal, intrinsic reward value and ability to modify brain and behaviour, musical activities have been proposed as a potential strength-based rehabilitation tool for ASD22,36,95. In the current trial, we demonstrate that 8–12 weeks of music intervention can indeed alter intrinsic brain connectivity and improve parent-reported outcomes in social communication and FQoL in school-age children with ASD.
Improvements in social communication were found on the CCC-2 from baseline to post-intervention in MT vs. NM, with a medium-sized positive effect (d = 0.34). Improvements were specific to pragmatics, reduction of inappropriate initiations and better social relations and interests. These findings are consistent with the idea that music employs a structured approach to social communication, which may otherwise be hindered by sensory and social difficulties12,36. Despite being modestly sized, these effects are highly specific to MT given the comparable structure of the control intervention and may have promising clinical and policy implications96. No MT-specific improvements were found on SRS-II or PPVT-4. Despite convergence between the SRS-II and CCC-2 and similar susceptibility to assessor-blinding biases, the SRS-II is a measure of ASD symptom severity, whereas the CCC-2 is a pragmatic communication measure, indicating limited effects of MT on reducing ASD symptom severity or improving receptive vocabulary.
We also found a positive effect of MT on FQoL (d = 0.57). The family is the primary support system for individuals with ASD throughout their lifespan. However, parents of children with ASD experience high levels of stress that can negatively impact well-being97, making FQoL a critical component in evaluating treatment outcomes. Although both groups received a form of interventional support, only parents of children in MT reported increases in FQoL, particularly on items on family interaction, cohesion and coping and benefits of disability-related supports.
Recently, Bieleninik and colleagues98 published a multicentre trial of improvisational music therapy for children aged 4–7 years and found no reductions in ASD symptoms on the ADOS Social Affect domain after 5 months of therapy, compared to standard care. The authors suggest that this could be due to variability across therapists, clinical assessors99 and the choice of ADOS as outcome measure, particularly because no previous well-controlled and blinded intervention studies3 have found treatment effects on the ADOS. While this trial demonstrates the global feasibility of implementing music therapy in large-scale international settings, it also indicates the importance of choosing appropriate outcome measures for psychosocial interventions in heterogeneous neurodevelopmental populations. The focus should not only be on symptom reduction but also on overall quality of life and functional improvements. In turn, outcomes that are malleable through intervention can inform future targets of research.
To complement behavioural improvements, we present the first evidence that music intervention alters functional brain activity in ASD leading to functional communication gains. Specifically, MT, relative to NM, increased functional connectivity between bilateral primary auditory cortex and subcortical and motor regions (often reduced in ASD)100 and reduced over-connectivity between auditory and visual-association areas38. Importantly, changes in brain connectivity were related to improvements in children’s communication skills after MT (Fig. 3c–f).
Brain connectivity in ASD has often been conceptualized as a trade-off between bottom–up and top–down processing. However, it is still not clear whether an increased reliance on bottom–up sensory processing and hence, sensory over-connectivity, is a cause or consequence of atypical top–down cortical modulation41,46. As a result, social communication impairments may result from alterations not just in the brain’s “social” network” but in domain-general disconnections in sensorimotor and cognitive functions, which are building blocks of later social skills46. In line with this idea, we find that engaging in musical activities can directly influence auditory–motor connections in the brains of children with ASD similar to effects of musical training in neurotypical populations27,30,31. Previous studies have reported that early motor difficulties are often predictive of later social communication impairments in ASD101. Thus interventions targeting motor skills may impact later social outcomes. It is important to note that our participants did not exhibit significant motor deficits and that the two intervention groups did not differ in their range of motor skills (Table 1). Thus any gains observed in auditory–motor connectivity in the MT group are not driven by group differences in motor skills and are specific to the music intervention. Furthermore, our findings show that music might play a modulating role in reducing the over-connectivity between sensory cortices, subsequently improving communication processes102,103. In light of mechanisms of music-induced neuroplasticity introduced earlier, our findings support bottom–up integration of sensorimotor brain networks leading to improved social functioning rather than top–down music-based reward36. Music interventions may thus have a positive influence on social functioning, possibly though modulation of domain-general sensory and cognitive processes, which are often atypical ASD41,46. Future research should focus on better understanding the neural mechanisms underlying music-related changes in brain connectivity and its impact on social behaviour.
Evidence-based behavioural and psychosocial interventions for school-age children have received limited attention3. Neuroscience-informed support for such interventions offers the opportunity to integrate brain development with behavioural approaches, allowing development of individualized treatment paradigms104. A strength of the current study is the use of neuroimaging to support improvements in behavioural outcomes resulting from MT. Consequently, the sample size (n = 51) is quite modest. Future work should focus on identifying individuals whose profiles may benefit most from music and integrate neuroimaging in multisite trials of such interventions. Inclusion of more direct observation-based outcomes and the role of mediators and moderators (e.g. quality of therapeutic relationship, cognitive, language and motor profiles, symptom level and musical interest of the participant) on short- and long-term outcomes will also be crucial to further the evidence base for music-based interventions.
In conclusion, the present study demonstrates that 8–12 weeks of music intervention (relative to non-music behavioural intervention) can improve parent-reported social communication, FQoL and intrinsic brain connectivity in school-age children, thus supporting the use of music as a therapeutic tool for individuals with ASD.
Electronic supplementary material
Acknowledgements
The authors would like to thank all parents and children who took part in the study. We would also like to thank Esther Germain, Ana Tryfon, Maya Kunigis, Samantha Chung and Ruveneko Ferdinand for assistance with data collection; Alison Usher-Jones for allowing access to Westmount Music Therapy and all MR technicians at the Montreal Neurological Institute. This work was supported by funds from the Canadian Institutes of Health Research to K.H. and a pilot grant from the Quebec Bioimaging Network to K.H. and M.S.
Conflict of interest
M.T. was a contractual employee of Westmount Music Therapy during the course of trial. The other authors declare that they have no conflict of interest.
Footnotes
Publisher’s note: Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
These authors contributed equally: Aparna Nadig, Krista Hyde
Electronic supplementary material
Supplementary Information accompanies this paper at (10.1038/s41398-018-0287-3).
References
- 1.Autism and Developmental Disabilities Monitoring Network Surveillance Year 2008 Principal Investigators, et al. Prevalence of autism spectrum disorders: Autism and Developmental Disabilities Monitoring Network, 14 sites, United States, 2008. MMWR Surveill. Summ. 61, 1–19 (2012). [PubMed]
- 2.Masi A, DeMayo MM, Glozier N, Guastella AJ. An overview of autism spectrum disorder, heterogeneity and treatment options. Neurosci. Bull. 2017;33:183–193. doi: 10.1007/s12264-017-0100-y. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Weitlauf, A. S. et al. Therapies for Children with Autism Spectrum Disorders (Agency for Healthcare Research and Quality, Rockville, MD, 2014). [PubMed]
- 4.Warren, Z. et al. Therapies for Children with Autism Spectrum Disorders (Agency for Healthcare Research and Quality, Rockville, MD, 2011). [PubMed]
- 5.Horlin C, Falkmer M, Parsons R, Albrecht MA, Falkmer T. The cost of autism spectrum disorders. PLoS ONE. 2014;9:e106552. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0106552. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Christensen DL, et al. Prevalence and characteristics of autism spectrum disorder among children aged 8 years — Autism and Developmental Disabilities Monitoring Network, 11 Sites, United States, 2012. MMWR Surveill. Summ. 2016;65:1–23. doi: 10.15585/mmwr.ss6503a1. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Makin Chantelle, Hill Vivian, Pellicano Elizabeth. The primary-to-secondary school transition for children on the autism spectrum: A multi-informant mixed-methods study. Autism & Developmental Language Impairments. 2017;2:239694151668483. doi: 10.1177/2396941516684834. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Coffey A. Relationships: the key to successful transition from primary to secondary school? Improving Schools. 2013;16:261–271. doi: 10.1177/1365480213505181. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Dillon GV, Underwood JDM. Parental perspectives of students with autism spectrum disorders transitioning from primary to secondary school in the United Kingdom. Focus Autism Other Dev. Disabil. 2012;27:111–121. doi: 10.1177/1088357612441827. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Rice CE, Lee LC. Expanding the global reach of research in autism. Autism. 2017;21:515–517. doi: 10.1177/1362361317704603. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Geretsegger M, Elefant C, Mössler KA, Gold C. Music therapy for people with autism spectrum disorder. Cochrane Database Syst. Rev. 2014;6:CD004381. doi: 10.1002/14651858.CD004381.pub3. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.LaGasse AB. Social outcomes in children with autism spectrum disorder: a review of music therapy outcomes. Patient Relat. Outcome Meas. 2017;8:23–32. doi: 10.2147/PROM.S106267. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Kanner, L. Autistic disturbances of affective contact. Nerv. Child2, 217–250 (1943). [PubMed]
- 14.Molnar-Szakacs I, Heaton P. Music: a unique window into the world of autism. Ann. NY Acad. Sci. 2012;1252:318–324. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.2012.06465.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Ouimet T, Foster NEV, Tryfon A, Hyde KL. Auditory-musical processing in autism spectrum disorders: a review of behavioral and brain imaging studies. Ann. NY Acad. Sci. 2012;1252:325–331. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.2012.06453.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Quintin EM, Bhatara A, Poissant H, Fombonne E, Levitin DJ. Processing of musical structure by high-functioning adolescents with autism spectrum disorders. Child Neuropsychol. 2013;19:250–275. doi: 10.1080/09297049.2011.653540. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Lai G, Pantazatos SP, Schneider H, Hirsch J. Neural systems for speech and song in autism. Brain. 2012;135:961–975. doi: 10.1093/brain/awr335. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Sharda M, Midha R, Malik S, Mukerji S, Singh NC. Fronto-temporal connectivity is preserved during sung but not spoken word listening, across the autism spectrum. Autism Res. 2015;8:174–186. doi: 10.1002/aur.1437. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Caria A, Venuti P, de Falco S. Functional and dysfunctional brain circuits underlying emotional processing of music in autism spectrum disorders. Cereb. Cortex. 2011;21:2838–2849. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhr084. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Sacks, O. Musicophilia: Tales of Music and the Brain, New York, N.Y.: Knopf. (2007).
- 21.Miendlarzewska EA, Trost WJ. How musical training affects cognitive development: rhythm, reward and other modulating variables. Front. Neurosci. 2013;7:279. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2013.00279. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Särkämö T, Tervaniemi M, Huotilainen M. Music perception and cognition: development, neural basis, and rehabilitative use of music. Wiley Interdiscip. Rev. Cogn. Sci. 2013;4:441–451. doi: 10.1002/wcs.1237. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Kirschner S, Tomasello M. Joint music making promotes prosocial behavior in 4-year-old children. Evol. Hum. Behav. 2010;31:354–364. doi: 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2010.04.004. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Rabinowitch TC, Meltzoff AN. Joint rhythmic movement increases 4-year-old children’s prosocial sharing and fairness toward peers. Front. Psychol. 2017;8:1050. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01050. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Cirelli LK, Einarson KM, Trainor LJ. Interpersonal synchrony increases prosocial behavior in infants. Dev. Sci. 2014;17:1003–1011. doi: 10.1111/desc.12193. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Schellenberg EG, Corrigall KA, Dys SP, Malti T. Group music training and children’s prosocial skills. PLoS ONE. 2015;10:e0141449. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0141449. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Zatorre RJ, Chen JL, Penhune VB. When the brain plays music: auditory-motor interactions in music perception and production. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2007;8:547–558. doi: 10.1038/nrn2152. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Zatorre RJ, Salimpoor VN. From perception to pleasure: music and its neural substrates. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA. 2013;110(Suppl):10430–10437. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1301228110. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Wollman I, Penhune V, Segado M, Carpentier T, Zatorre RJ. Neural network retuning and neural predictors of learning success associated with cello training. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. 2018;115:E6056–E6064. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1721414115. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Klein C, Liem F, Hänggi J, Elmer S, Jäncke L. The ‘silent’ imprint of musical training. Hum. Brain. Mapp. 2016;37:536–546. doi: 10.1002/hbm.23045. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Li Q, et al. Musical training induces functional and structural auditory-motor network plasticity in young adults. Hum. Brain. Mapp. 2018;39:2098–2110. doi: 10.1002/hbm.23989. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Stegemöller EL. Exploring a neuroplasticity model of music therapy. J. Music Ther. 2014;51:211–227. doi: 10.1093/jmt/thu023. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Hyde KL, et al. Musical training shapes structural brain development. J. Neurosci. 2009;29:3019–3025. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5118-08.2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Habibi, A. et al. Childhood music training induces change in micro and macroscopic brain structure: results from a longitudinal study. Cereb. Cortex 1–12 (2017). [DOI] [PubMed]
- 35.Cheever T, et al. NIH/Kennedy Center Workshop on Music and the Brain: finding harmony. Neuron. 2018;97:1214–1218. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2018.02.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Janzen Thenille Braun, Thaut Michael H. Rethinking the role of music in the neurodevelopment of autism spectrum disorder. Music & Science. 2018;1:205920431876963. doi: 10.1177/2059204318769639. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Murdaugh DL, Maximo JO, Kana RK. Changes in intrinsic connectivity of the brain’s reading network following intervention in children with autism. Hum. Brain. Mapp. 2015;36:2965–2979. doi: 10.1002/hbm.22821. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Rudie JD, Dapretto M. Convergent evidence of brain overconnectivity in children with autism? Cell Rep. 2013;5:565–566. doi: 10.1016/j.celrep.2013.10.043. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Uddin LQ. Idiosyncratic connectivity in autism: developmental and anatomical considerations. Trends Neurosci. 2015;38:261–263. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2015.03.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Jack, A. Neuroimaging in neurodevelopmental disorders: focus on resting-state fMRI analysis of intrinsic functional brain connectivity. Curr. Opin. Neurol.31, 140–148 (2018). [DOI] [PubMed]
- 41.Thye Melissa D., Bednarz Haley M., Herringshaw Abbey J., Sartin Emma B., Kana Rajesh K. The impact of atypical sensory processing on social impairments in autism spectrum disorder. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience. 2018;29:151–167. doi: 10.1016/j.dcn.2017.04.010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Woodward ND, Cascio CJ. Resting-state functional connectivity in psychiatric disorders. JAMA Psychiatry. 2015;72:743. doi: 10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2015.0484. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Fox MD, Greicius M. Clinical applications of resting state functional connectivity. Front. Syst. Neurosci. 2010;4:19. doi: 10.3389/fnsys.2010.00019. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Särkämö T. Editorial: Music, brain, and rehabilitation: emerging therapeutic applications and potential neural mechanisms. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 2016;10:1–5. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2016.00103. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Koelsch S. A neuroscientific perspective on music therapy. Ann. NY Acad. Sci. 2009;1169:374–384. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.2009.04592.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Ronconi L, Molteni M, Casartelli L. Building blocks of others’ understanding: a perspective shift in investigating social-communicative deficit in autism. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 2016;10:144. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2016.00144. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Bradt J. Randomized controlled trials in music therapy: guidelines for design and implementation. J. Music Ther. 2012;49:120–149. doi: 10.1093/jmt/49.2.120. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Turner L, et al. Consolidated standards of reporting trials (CONSORT) and the completeness of reporting of randomised controlled trials (RCTs) published in medical journals. Cochrane Database Syst. Rev. 2012;11:MR000030. doi: 10.1002/14651858.MR000030.pub2. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders: DSM-IV-TR® 4th edn (Washington, DC, 2000) 10.1176/appi.books.9780890423349.
- 50.Singh-Manoux A, Adler NE, Marmot MG. Subjective social status: its determinants and its association with measures of ill-health in the Whitehall II study. Soc. Sci. Med. 2003;56:1321–1333. doi: 10.1016/S0277-9536(02)00131-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Oldfield RC. The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory. Neuropsychologia. 1971;9:97–113. doi: 10.1016/0028-3932(71)90067-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Lord C, et al. The autism diagnostic observation schedule-generic: a standard measure of social and communication deficits associated with the spectrum of autism. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 2000;30:205–223. doi: 10.1023/A:1005592401947. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.De Bildt A, et al. Interrelationship between Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule-Generic (ADOS-G), Autism Diagnostic Interview-Revised (ADI-R), and the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-IV-TR) classification in children and adolescents with mental disorders. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 2004;34:129–137. doi: 10.1023/B:JADD.0000022604.22374.5f. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Schopler, E., Richler, R. & Renner, B. The Childhood Autism Rating Scale (CARS) for Diagnostic Screening and Classification of Autism (Irvington, New York, NY, 1986).
- 55.Constantino JN, Todd RD. Autistic traits in the general population: a twin study. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry. 2003;60:524–530. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.60.5.524. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56.Bishop DVM. Development of the Children’s Communication Checklist (CCC): a method for assessing qualitative aspects of communicative impairment in children. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry. 1998;39:879–891. doi: 10.1017/S0021963098002832. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57.Sparrow S, Balla D, Cicchetti D. Vineland Adaptive Behaviour Scales. Interview. Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service; 1984. [Google Scholar]
- 58.Park J, et al. Toward assessing family outcomes of service delivery: validation of a family quality of life survey. J. Intellect. Disabil. Res. 2003;47:367–384. doi: 10.1046/j.1365-2788.2003.00497.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59.Wechsler, D. & Wechsler, D. Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence (The Psychological Corporation, Harcourt Brace & Company, New York, NY, 1999). Open Access Library. http://www.oalib.com/references/10668025 (accessed 29 Jul 2015).
- 60.Semel, E. M., Wiig, E. H. & Secord, W. CELF: Clinical Evaluation of Language Fundamentals. fourth edition (CELF-4) (The Psychological Corporation, A Harcourt Assessment Company, Toronto, Canada, 2003).
- 61.Conti-Ramsden G, Botting N, Faragher B. Psycholinguistic markers for specific language impairment (SLI) J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry. 2001;42:741–748. doi: 10.1111/1469-7610.00770. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 62.Dunn, L. M. Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test-Revised (PPVT-R). Forms L and M (Circle Pines, Minn., American Guidance Service, 1981).
- 63.Peretz I, et al. A novel tool for evaluating children’s musical abilities across age and culture. Front. Syst. Neurosci. 2013;7:30. doi: 10.3389/fnsys.2013.00030. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 64.Raschle N, et al. Pediatric neuroimaging in early childhood and infancy: challenges and practical guidelines. Ann. NY Acad. Sci. 2012;1252:43–50. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.2012.06457.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 65.Scott NW, McPherson GC, Ramsay CR, Campbell MK. The method of minimization for allocation to clinical trials. Control. Clin. Trials. 2002;23:662–674. doi: 10.1016/S0197-2456(02)00242-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 66.Taves DR. Minimization: a new method of assigning patients to treatment and control groups. Clin. Pharmacol. Ther. 1974;15:443–453. doi: 10.1002/cpt1974155443. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 67.Guerrero N, Turry A, Geller D, Raghavan P. From historic to contemporary: Nordoff-Robbins music therapy in collaborative interdisciplinary rehabilitation. Music Ther. Perspect. 2014;32:38–46. doi: 10.1093/mtp/miu014. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 68.Nordoff, P. & Robbins, C. Creative Music Therapy: A Guide to Fostering Clinical Musicianship (Barcelona Publishers, Gilsum, NH, 2007).
- 69.Mössler, K. et al. The therapeutic relationship as predictor of change in music therapy with young children with autism spectrum disorder. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 10.1007/s10803-017-3306-y (2017). [DOI] [PubMed]
- 70.Sullivan K, Stone WL, Dawson G. Potential neural mechanisms underlying the effectiveness of early intervention for children with autism spectrum disorder. Res. Dev. Disabil. 2014;35:2921–2932. doi: 10.1016/j.ridd.2014.07.027. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 71.Lerner MD, White SW, McPartland JC. Mechanisms of change in psychosocial interventions for autism spectrum disorders. Dialog. Clin. Neurosci. 2012;14:307–318. doi: 10.31887/DCNS.2012.14.3/mlerner. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 72.Wiens Natalie, Gordon Reyna L. The case for treatment fidelity in active music interventions: why and how. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences. 2018;1423(1):219–228. doi: 10.1111/nyas.13639. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 73.Anagnostou E, et al. Measuring social communication behaviors as a treatment endpoint in individuals with autism spectrum disorder. Autism. 2015;19:622–636. doi: 10.1177/1362361314542955. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 74.Dichter GS, Sikich L, Song A, Voyvodic J, Bodfish JW. Functional neuroimaging of treatment effects in psychiatry: methodological challenges and recommendations. Int. J. Neurosci. 2012;122:483–493. doi: 10.3109/00207454.2012.678446. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 75.Tango T. On the repeated measures designs and sample sizes for randomized controlled trials. Biostatistics. 2016;17:334–349. doi: 10.1093/biostatistics/kxv047. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 76.Lakens D. Calculating and reporting effect sizes to facilitate cumulative science: a practical primer for t-tests and ANOVAs. Front. Psychol. 2013;4:863. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00863. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 77.Hedges LV. Distribution theory for glass’s estimator of effect size and related estimators. J. Educ. Stat. 1981;6:107. doi: 10.3102/10769986006002107. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 78.Baguley T. Standardized or simple effect size: what should be reported? Br. J. Psychol. 2009;100:603–617. doi: 10.1348/000712608X377117. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 79.R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing (R Core Team, 2017). https://www.r-project.org.
- 80.Greicius, M. in Genomics, Circui, and Pathways in Clinical Neuropsychiatry Ch. 16, Eds Lehner, T., Miller, B.L. & State, M.W. (Elsevier Academic Press, San Diego, CA, 2016).
- 81.Smith SM, et al. Advances in functional and structural MR image analysis and implementation as FSL. Neuroimage. 2004;23:S208–S219. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.07.051. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 82.Woolrich MW, Jenkinson M, Brady JM, Smith SM. Fully Bayesian Spatio-Temporal Modeling of FMRI Data. IEEE Trans. Med. Imaging. 2004;23:213–231. doi: 10.1109/TMI.2003.823065. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 83.Vahdat S, Darainy M, Milner TE, Ostry DJ. Functionally specific changes in resting-state sensorimotor networks after motor learning. J. Neurosci. 2011;31:16907–16915. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2737-11.2011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 84.Power JD, Laumann TO, Plitt M, Martin A, Petersen SE. On Global fMRI Signals and Simulations. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2017;21:911–913. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2017.09.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 85.Desjardins AE, Kiehl KA, Liddle PF. Removal of confounding effects of global signal in functional MRI analyses. Neuroimage. 2001;13:751–758. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2000.0719. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 86.Power JD, Barnes Ka, Snyder AZ, Schlaggar BL, Petersen SE. Spurious but systematic correlations in functional connectivity MRI networks arise from subject motion. Neuroimage. 2012;59:2142–2154. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.10.018. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 87.Seeley WW, et al. Dissociable intrinsic connectivity networks for salience processing and executive control. J. Neurosci. 2007;27:2349–2356. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5587-06.2007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 88.O'Connell, N. S. et al. Methods for analysis of pre-post data in clinical research: a comparison of five common methods. J. Biom. Biostat.10.4172/2155-6180.1000334 (2017). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 89.Vickers AJ, Altman DG. Statistics notes: analysing controlled trials with baseline and follow up measurements. BMJ. 2001;323:1123–1124. doi: 10.1136/bmj.323.7321.1123. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 90.Senn S. Testing for baseline balance in clinical trials. Stat. Med. 1994;13:1715–1726. doi: 10.1002/sim.4780131703. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 91.Lee PH. Covariate adjustments in randomized controlled trials increased study power and reduced biasedness of effect size estimation. J. Clin. Epidemiol. 2016;76:137–146. doi: 10.1016/j.jclinepi.2016.02.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 92.Hernández AV, Steyerberg EW, Habbema JDF. Covariate adjustment in randomized controlled trials with dichotomous outcomes increases statistical power and reduces sample size requirements. J. Clin. Epidemiol. 2004;57:454–460. doi: 10.1016/j.jclinepi.2003.09.014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 93.Taylor JE, Worsley KJ. Detecting sparse signals in random fields, with an application to brain mapping. J. Am. Stat. Assoc. 2007;102:913–928. doi: 10.1198/016214507000000815. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 94.Mottron L. Should we change targets and methods of early intervention in autism, in favor of a strengths-based education? Eur. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry. 2017;26:815–825. doi: 10.1007/s00787-017-0955-5. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 95.Chenausky K, Norton A, Tager-Flusberg H, Schlaug G. Auditory-motor mapping training: comparing the effects of a novel speech treatment to a control treatment for minimally verbal children with autism. PLoS ONE. 2016;11:e0164930. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0164930. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 96.Cooper H. The search for meaningful ways to express the effects of interventions. Child Dev. Perspect. 2008;2:181–186. doi: 10.1111/j.1750-8606.2008.00063.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 97.McStay RL, Trembath D, Dissanayake C. Maternal stress and family quality of life in response to raising a child with autism: from preschool to adolescence. Res. Dev. Disabil. 2014;35:3119–3130. doi: 10.1016/j.ridd.2014.07.043. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 98.Bieleninik L, et al. Effects of improvisational music therapy vs enhanced standard care on symptom severity among children with autism spectrum disorder. JAMA. 2017;318:525. doi: 10.1001/jama.2017.9478. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 99.Kamp-Becker I., Albertowski K., Becker J., Ghahreman M., Langmann A., Mingebach T., Poustka L., Weber L., Schmidt H., Smidt J., Stehr T., Roessner V., Kucharczyk K., Wolff N., Stroth S. Diagnostic accuracy of the ADOS and ADOS-2 in clinical practice. European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry. 2018;27(9):1193–1207. doi: 10.1007/s00787-018-1143-y. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 100.Nair A, Treiber JM, Shukla DK, Shih P, Müller RA. Impaired thalamocortical connectivity in autism spectrum disorder: a study of functional and anatomical connectivity. Brain. 2013;136:1942–1955. doi: 10.1093/brain/awt079. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 101.Bedford R, Pickles A, Lord C. Early gross motor skills predict the subsequent development of language in children with autism spectrum disorder. Autism Res. 2016;9:993–1001. doi: 10.1002/aur.1587. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 102.Green SA, Hernandez L, Bookheimer SY, Dapretto M. Reduced modulation of thalamocortical connectivity during exposure to sensory stimuli in ASD. Autism Res. 2017;10:801–809. doi: 10.1002/aur.1726. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 103.Linke Annika C., Jao Keehn R. Joanne, Pueschel Ellyn B., Fishman Inna, Müller Ralph-Axel. Children with ASD show links between aberrant sound processing, social symptoms, and atypical auditory interhemispheric and thalamocortical functional connectivity. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience. 2018;29:117–126. doi: 10.1016/j.dcn.2017.01.007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 104.Muhle Rebecca A., Reed Hannah E., Stratigos Katharine A., Veenstra-VanderWeele Jeremy. The Emerging Clinical Neuroscience of Autism Spectrum Disorder. JAMA Psychiatry. 2018;75(5):514. doi: 10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2017.4685. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.