Skip to main content
American Journal of Men's Health logoLink to American Journal of Men's Health
. 2018 Oct 18;12(6):1916–1928. doi: 10.1177/1557988318803744

The Role of Positive Emotion and Ego-Resilience in Determining Men’s Physical Activity Following a Workplace Health Intervention

Cherisse L Seaton 1,, Joan L Bottorff 2, Margaret Jones-Bricker 3, Sonia Lamont 4,5
PMCID: PMC6199438  PMID: 30334492

Abstract

Men are a hard-to-reach group in the promotion of modifiable behaviors such as physical activity. Examining the individual differences among men that might predict positive behavior changes could support customization of health promotion programs. This study examined the role of emotional outlook, positive emotion, and ego-resilience in determining men’s physical activity and health-related quality of life following implementation of a gender-sensitive workplace health intervention. Using a pre–post within-subjects design, computer-assisted telephone interviewing (CATI) was used to collect measures of emotion and ego-resilience along with physical activity and health-related quality of life (using the 12-item short form [SF-12]) at baseline (n = 139) and after 6 months (n = 80) from adult men (Mage = 43.7, SD = 12.5). Baseline emotional outlook and ego-resilience were both positively related to increased physical activity at follow-up among men. Emotional outlook and positive emotion were positively related to ego-resilience, and ego-resilience mediated the relationship between these and the physical component of health-related quality of life. Workplace health interventions that incorporate the promotion of personal resources hold potential for greater impacts.

Keywords: health promotion, gender, physical activity, emotions, resilience, well-being


Achieving adequate levels of physical activity can be challenging for many men. In Canada, 18% of men over 18 years were meeting the recommended levels of physical activity (i.e., 150 minutes or more of moderate intensity physical activity per week) in 2014/2015 (Statistics Canada, 2017). In the promotion of modifiable health behaviors such as physical activity that are known to reduce the risk of chronic disease, men are a hard-to-reach (or “unreached”) population (Pringle et al., 2014). Men represent a low proportion of participants in randomized controlled trials of lifestyle interventions (Pagoto et al., 2012), as well as in workplace health promotion interventions (Robroek, van Lenthe, van Empelen, & Burdorf, 2009).

Health promotion interventions that employ gender-sensitive messaging (i.e., considering men’s needs based on the social constructions of gender roles and identities; World Health Organization, 2007), to specifically target and engage men hold promise for promoting lifestyle changes among men (Bottorff et al., 2015; Robertson, Witty, Zwolinsky, & Day, 2013). For example, U.K.-based programs offered in conjunction with professional soccer clubs have attracted men to increase physical activity (Gray et al., 2013; Pringle et al., 2013). An Australian weight loss program, Preventing Obesity Without Eating Like a Rabbit (POWER), successfully engaged male shift workers to increase physical activity using humor, pedometers, and friendly competition (Morgan et al., 2012).

In addition to the overall demonstrated success of such interventions, examining individual differences among men that may predict success can also advance understanding and could potentially lead to the development of psychosocial strategies to support greater success among program participants (Baruth et al., 2011). For example, individual differences in ego-resilience, or the capacity to modify one’s level of self-control to meet situational demands (Block, 2002), may predict individual likelihood to strive to change targeted health behaviors, such as physical activity, in a health promotion program (Baumeister, Gailliot, DeWall, & Oaten, 2006; Martin et al., 2012). Indeed, ego-resilience was associated with physical activity in African American high school students (Martin et al., 2012). In a sample of U.S. war veterans, resilient men (assessed in this study as being free of psychiatric diagnosis) were more physically active than non-resilient men (Fields, Hoyt, Linnville, & Moore, 2016); however, no research that we are aware of has examined the link between ego-resilience and increases in physical activity among adult men.

Studies have reported a relationship between emotion and health behaviors (e.g., DeSteno, Gross, & Kubzansky, 2013). Emotion and well-being are frequently examined as outcomes; for example, among European adults from 27 countries, men who self-reported meeting recommended levels of physical activity had higher self-rated well-being (Marques et al., 2016). Moderate-intensity physical activity (as measured by accelerometer) was associated with higher psychological well-being among a sample of both women and men in the United States (Panza, Taylor, Thompson, White, & Pescatello, 2017). Emotions may also influence behavior. A recent study identified that higher levels of psychological well-being were related to increases in physical activity over 11 years among both men and women (Kim, Kubzansky, Soo, & Boehm, 2017). The broaden-and-build theory (Fredrickson, 1998) proposes that positive emotions in particular function to broaden thoughts and actions, undo the effects of negative emotion, and build resources over time (Cohn, Fredrickson, Brown, Mikels, & Conway, 2009). Although little research has examined the relationship between individual differences in positive emotion, well-being, and physical activity among men, in a recent review, positive affective states were linked to subsequent physical activity (i.e., within a few hours) in three studies with primarily female samples (Liao, Shonkoff, & Dunton, 2015). Among men, body-related pride has been associated with greater physical activity whereas body-related shame linked to lower levels of physical activity (Castonguay, Pila, Wrosch, & Sabiston, 2015). Interestingly, Baruth et al. (2011) reported that a positive emotional outlook (i.e., happiness) predicted greater physical activity among previously sedentary men but not women, providing further support that gender-related factors may be important considerations in understanding this relationship.

The broaden-and-build theory suggests that positive emotions reinforce an individual’s self-regulatory capacity by building psychological resources, leading to greater well-being over time (Fredrickson, 2013). In previous research, positive emotion predicted increased ego-resilience over time, and increases in ego-resilience in turn mediated the relationship between positive emotion and well-being over time (Cohn et al., 2009; Seaton & Beaumont, 2015). In another cross-sectional study, resilience (measured using the Connor-Davidson Resilience Scale) mediated the relationship between perceptions of emotional support (from supervisors) and occupational health among a sample of firefighters; specifically, emotionally supportive relationships at work were linked to greater resilience, which in turn allowed firefighters to cope effectively with stress, resulting in lower levels of exhaustion and cynicism, and higher vigor and dedication (Bernabe & Botia, 2016). Previous research has not examined the potential mediating role of ego-resilience in the relationship between positive emotion and physical activity. More research is needed on the interplay between positive emotion and ego-resilience in health behaviors such as physical activity, especially among men.

The purpose of the present study was to examine the role of individual differences in emotion and ego-resilience in determining men’s physical activity following a gender-sensitive workplace wellness program (called POWERPLAY). Physical activity was measured by self-report as minutes of moderate and vigorous physical activity (MVPA), and social-cognitive factors related to physical activity (stages of change for physical activity and self-efficacy for physical activity) were also assessed. Although social-cognitive factors such as self-efficacy are well known to influence physical activity (e.g., Keller, Gellert, Knoll, Schneider, & Ernsting, 2016), less well studied are the personal resources that might be associated with these social-cognitive factors. Health-related quality of life, a measure of well-being associated with physical activity (Bize, Johnson, & Plotnikoff, 2007) was included. The POWERPLAY program was implemented in four male-dominated worksites in northern British Columbia, Canada, from fall 2014 to spring 2015 (Caperchione et al., 2015). Although the results of the intervention have been published (Johnson et al., 2016), measures of emotion and ego-resilience were collected along with overall program evaluation measures with the expressed purpose of examining whether individual differences in positive emotion and ego-resilience at baseline predicted increases in physical activity among men following delivery of the POWERPLAY program. It was hypothesized that

  1. Among men, higher levels of emotional outlook/positive emotion and ego-resilience at baseline would be associated with higher levels of physical activity (i.e., MVPA, stage of change, and self-efficacy) and health-related quality of life

  2. Higher levels of emotional outlook/positive emotion and ego-resilience at baseline would be associated with an increase in men’s MVPA following the POWERPLAY intervention, as well as with health-related quality of life at follow-up

  3. Based on the broaden-and-build theory of positive emotion (Fredrickson, 1998), any relationship between baseline emotional outlook/positive emotion and increased MVPA and health-related quality of life would be mediated by ego-resilience at follow-up (see Figure 1)

Figure 1.

Figure 1.

Hypothesized role of ego-resilience in the relationship between emotional outlook/positive emotion and physical activity/health-related quality of life.

Methods

Study Design

This study analyzes data collected to evaluate the workplace health promotion program called POWERPLAY. The study received human ethics approval from the University of British Columbia Research Ethics Board (#H13-02408) and the Northern Health Research Review Committee (RRC-2014-0015). Detailed design and rationale for the study have been previously reported (Caperchione et al., 2015). Briefly, the POWERPLAY workplace program was specifically designed to incorporate men’s preferences and interests (i.e., “gender-sensitive”). For example, the program included friendly competition between employees as well as tools (e.g., resources for tracking progress, pedometers) to assist with self-monitoring of physical activity, and educational resources designed with input from focus groups held with men from Northern British Columbia (Oliffe et al., 2017). Four worksites in Northern British Columbia were offered the POWERPLAY program and they participated in the research. These included a regional municipality with 180 employees (50% men), two trucking companies with 130 and 137 employees (80% and 95% men, respectively), and a shipping terminal with 140 employees (70% men). A pre–post within-subjects design was used to evaluate the POWERPLAY program. Male employees 18 years or older were recruited through on-site launches in September 2014. The men who signed up and provided phone numbers were contacted during September–October 2014 (prior to program implementation) and again at 6-month follow-up, beginning April 2015 (program conclusion), and invited to complete the telephone surveys in exchange for $20 gift cards. Levels of self-reported physical activity increased post-program implementation (Johnson et al., 2016).

Measures

The measures of emotion and ego-resilience described in the following text were collected along with overall program evaluation measures in order to examine whether individual differences in positive emotion and ego-resilience at baseline predicted increases in physical activity among men following delivery of the POWERPLAY program. With the exception of demographics, participants completed all measures both at the baseline (pre-implementation) assessment and again at the 6-month follow-up via CATI surveys.

Sociodemographic characteristics and health limits to physical activity

At the baseline assessment, a series of questions were used to gather information about the sample including age, marital status, education, income, ethnicity/race, as well as height and weight used to calculate body mass index (BMI) as kg/m2. To assess health limits to physical activity, participants were also asked, “In the past month, to what extent was your participation in physical activities limited by a health condition, injury or disability?” to which they responded on a 6-point scale ranging from not at all (1) to completely (6).

Emotional outlook

Participants were asked to “please rate your general emotional outlook on life” on a scale ranging from often very depressed (1) to usually very happy and optimistic (5), with higher scores reflecting a more positive emotional outlook. This single-item measure of positive emotion has been used by others (Baruth et al., 2011; Puett et al., 2014) and was included in order to determine whether the findings of Baruth et al. (2011) could be replicated in a novel context; however, evidence on the reliability and validity of this single-item measure is not available.

Positive emotion

Participants completed the short form Positive and Negative Affect Schedule (PANAS; Thompson, 2007) with instructions to indicate how they normally feel. The short form PANAS includes five positive (alert, inspired, determined, attentive, and active) and five negative (upset, hostile, ashamed, nervous, and afraid) adjectives. Evidence for the validity and reliability of the full PANAS scale has been reported (Watson, Clark, & Tellegen, 1988), and the short form has also demonstrated acceptable psychometric properties, including internal reliability, cross-sample and temporal stability, and convergent and criterion validity (Thompson, 2007). Two additional items (strong, proud) were included for the positive emotion scale from the original 20-item PANAS in order to ensure that a range of male-centric emotions were captured. Participants responded on a scale ranging from never (1) to always (5), and responses to the seven positive adjectives were summed (possible range of scores 7–35) to create the positive emotions scale (Cronbach’s α = .78 for baseline and .84 for follow-up).

Ego-resilience

The ER89 scale (Block & Kremen, 1996) measures the extent to which individuals maintain flexibility in response to changing situational demands. The ER89 includes 14 items (e.g., “I would be willing to describe myself as a pretty ‘strong’ personality”). In previous research the scale has shown internal and test–retest reliability (Block & Kremen, 1996) as well as construct validity (Letzring, Block, & Funder, 2005). Participants responded to these items on a 4-point scale ranging from does not apply at all (1) to applies very strongly (4). Responses to the 14 items were summed (possible range of scores 14–56), with higher scores reflecting higher levels of ego-resilience (Cronbach’s α = .82 for baseline and .80 for follow-up).

Physical activity and related psychosocial measures

Measures of physical activity and social-cognitive factors related to physical activity were included. First, to assess MVPA, the Godin Leisure-Time Exercise Questionnaire (GLTEQ; Godin & Shephard, 1985) was used. The GLTEQ contains three self-report questions that assess both the average frequency and duration (in min) of mild, moderate, and vigorous activities during free time over a typical week. Moderate and vigorous physical activity was summed to obtain MVPA (in min per week) at baseline and follow-up. Increase in physical activity was calculated by subtracting baseline from follow-up MVPA minutes. The GLTEQ has been validated with objective measures (e.g., accelerometer, maximal oxygen uptake [VO2max], body fat percentage), as well as other self-report measures of physical activity (Jacobs, Ainsworth, Hartman, & Leon, 1993). Using a cutoff of z > 3.29 (Tabachnick & Fidell, 2001), one individual was removed from the baseline MVPA scale (6,090 min, z = 9.63) and one individual was removed from the follow-up MVPA scale (1,080 min, z = 3.63). Second, stages of change for physical activity, based on the Transtheoretical Model of behavior change (Prochaska & DiClemente, 1983), was assessed by asking participants a series of four questions pertaining to engaging in “regular physical activity” (defined as at least 150 min of moderate activity each week) with different branching options depending on response (Dumith, Gigante, & Domingues, 2007; Nigg, 2005). Response options categorized participants into one of five groups: pre-contemplation (“No, and I don’t intend to within the next six months”); contemplation (“No, but I intend to within the next six months”); preparation (“No, but I intend to within the next 30 days”); action (“Yes, and I have for less than six months”); and maintenance (“Yes, and I have for more than six months”). These groupings were used as a scale ranging from 1 (pre-contemplation) to 5 (maintenance). Finally, self-efficacy for physical activity was assessed by asking participants, “Over the next six months, how confident are you that you can participate in regular physical activity?” (Plotnikoff, Hotz, Birkett, & Courneya, 2001). Participants responded on a scale ranging from not at all confident (1) to extremely confident (5).

Health-related quality of life

The SF-12 health-related quality of life survey (Ware, Kosinski, & Keller, 1996) includes items (e.g., “How much did pain interfere with your normal work, including both work outside the home and housework?”) that assess both the physical and mental components of health-related well-being. Participants respond in terms of how they felt during the past 4 weeks using several different response scales (e.g., categorical, yes/no, and ordinal scale ranging from not at all to extremely). The items are weighted and SF-12 is scored using an age- and sex-adjusted algorithm to create physical and mental health component scales, each standardized to a mean of 50 (SD = 10).

Analysis

All analyses were conducted in IBM SPSS Statistics 25. Sociodemographic characteristics were compared between individuals who did and did not complete the follow-up data collection using Fisher’s exact analyses for ethnicity and marital status, Mann Whitney U for income, education, and health limits to physical activity, and t-tests for age, height, weight, and BMI. Given the ordinal nature of some scales, Kendall’s tau-B correlations were used to assess the relationship between baseline emotion, ego-resilience, physical activity, and well-being measures. All the variables were measured on an ordinal or continuous scale, and we were interested in testing the monotonic (i.e., as one increases, so does the other) relationship between pairs of variables, so the assumptions of Kendall’s tau-B analyses were deemed to be met. Hierarchical linear regressions were used to assess the relationships between baseline emotional outlook, positive emotion, and ego-resilience (independent variables) and change in MVPA and health-related quality of life (physical and mental components) at follow-up (dependent variables), controlling for age, BMI, and health limits to physical activity (follow-up emotional outlook, positive emotion, and ego-resilience were also controlled in each model). Because the use of four separate worksites could possibly introduce clustering effects, potentially violating the assumption of independence for errors, we ran all regression analyses with dummy variables to represent each of the four workplaces (coded as 0 or 1) entered first as control variables. Although the means were on the high end for the emotional outlook, positive emotion, and ego-resilience scales, an assumption of linear regression is that errors be normally distributed, as opposed to requiring all the variables themselves to be normally distributed (Williams, Gomez Grajales, & Kurkiewicz, 2013). Preliminary screening produced normally distributed standardized residuals for all three regression models. Plots of the standardized predicted values against the standardized residual values indicated homoscedasticity in that the variance of the residuals was evenly distributed across all points of the model for each. The assumption that the relationship between the independent variables and dependent variables is linear was also deemed to be met after examining each independent variable and dependent variable combination individually using scatterplots. Mediation analyses were conducted by examining the significance of the indirect effect of both positive emotion and emotional outlook (X) on physical activity and health-related well-being as outcome variables (Y) through ego-resilience as the proposed mediator, controlling for workplace and using bootstrapping procedures with 10,000 resamples (Preacher & Hayes, 2008). The SPSS macro INDIRECT (available from: http://afhayes.com/spss-sas-and-mplus-macros-and-code.html) was used to conduct these analyses in SPSS (Version 25). A p value of less than .05 (two sided) was considered statistically significant.

Results

Participants

Prior to the program implementation, 203 eligible participants agreed to be contacted by researchers. At baseline, 139 men completed telephone surveys (response rate 68.5%). All participants provided verbal informed consent, which was entered as a “yes” into the computer by the survey interviewer before proceeding. The sociodemographic characteristics of men who completed the baseline survey only versus those who completed both the baseline and follow-up surveys are reported in Table 1.

Table 1.

Comparison of Sociodemographic Characteristics of Participants Who Did and Did Not Complete Follow-Up Surveys.

Participants with only baseline data (n = 59)
Participants with baseline and follow-up data (n = 80)
n % n % Fisher’s exact p
Marital status 9.41 .031
 Single 19 32.2 13 16.3
 Married 35 59.3 64 80.0
 Divorced 5 8.5 2 2.5
 Widowed 0 0.0 1 1.3
Ethnicity 9.16 .052
 Caucasian 50 84.7 66 82.5
 First Nations 4 6.8 2 2.6
 Metis 0 0.0 6 7.5
 Asian 1 1.7 0 0.0
 Other 5 6.8 6 7.5
n % n % U p
Household income 1885.00 .268
 <$19,999 0 0.0 0 0.0
 $20,000–39,999 1 1.8 3 4.0
 $40,000–59,999 4 7.1 3 4.0
 $60,000–79,999 14 25.0 13 17.3
 $80,000–99,999 9 16.1 11 14.7
 >$100,000 28 50.0 45 60.0
 No response 3 5
Education 2030.00 .137
 Less than high school 5 8.6 11 13.8
 Completed high school 15 25.9 30 37.5
 Trades certification/ college degree 29 50.0 28 35.0
 Some university 2 3.4 4 5.0
 University degree 7 12.1 7 8.8
 No response 1 0
Median IQR Median IQR U p
Health limits to PA 1.00 2.00 1.00 1.00 2264.50 .632
Mean SD Mean SD t p
Age 41.86 11.04 45.08 13.40 −1.55 .124
Height (meters) 1.78 .07 1.79 .07 −.81 .421
Weight (kg) 89.87 14.42 93.69 15.85 −1.46 .147
BMI (kg/m2) 28.18 4.03 29.08 4.59 −1.20 .232

Note. IQR = interquartile range; PA = physical activity; BMI = body mass index.

Preliminary Data Screening

The means, standard deviations, and correlations of all scales at baseline are presented in Table 2.

Table 2.

Means, Standard Deviations, Medians, Interquartile Range, and Correlations Between Emotional Outlook, Positive Emotion, and Ego-Resilience and All Scales at Baseline.

Baseline emotional outlook
Baseline positive emotion
Baseline ego-resilience
Baseline measures Mean SD Median IQR r p r p r p
Emotional outlook 4.14 0.73 4.00 1.00
Positive emotion 25.28 4.41 25.00 5.00 .30 <.001
Ego-resilience 44.31 6.25 45.00 9.00 .34 <.001 .38 <.001
MVPA (min per week) 304.84 339.81 210.00 296.25 .22 .001 .10 .094 .07 .228
Stage of change for PA 4.02 1.26 5.00 2.00 .15 .046 .16 .017 .18 .009
Self-efficacy for PA 4.14 .97 4.00 2.00 .15 .043 .13 .057 .03 .634
Health-related quality of life—physical component 51.41 7.36 52.96 8.73 .20 .003 .30 <.001 .22 <.001
Health-related quality of life—mental component 49.83 5.36 51.09 4.45 .04 .597 −.01 .829 −.10 .082

Note. N = 139 for all variables except those with MVPA, where N = 138. Kendal tau-B coefficients are reported. IQR = interquartile range; MVPA = moderate and vigorous physical activity; PA = physical activity.

Compared to baseline, emotional outlook was not significantly higher at follow-up (M = 4.10, SD = .76), t(79) = .51, p = .615, and neither was positive emotion (M = 25.51, SD = 4.30), t(79) = –.48, p = .634. Likewise, ego-resilience was not significantly higher at follow-up (M = 44.75, SD = 5.90), t(79) = –.85, p = .399. Although means were high for MVPA, a good distribution of scores was present, making ceiling effects unlikely. A comparison of baseline and 6-month follow-up physical activity scores has been conducted previously as part of the overall program evaluation (Johnson et al., 2016).

Analyses Related to Hypothesis #1: Correlations at Baseline

Supporting hypothesis 1, the pattern of correlations in Table 2 suggested that men’s emotional outlook at baseline was positively related to MVPA, to stages of change and self-efficacy for physical activity, and to the physical component of health-related quality of life. Men’s positive emotion at baseline was positively related to stages of change and self-efficacy for physical activity and the physical component of health-related quality of life, though not to MVPA. Finally, men’s ego-resilience at baseline was positively related to stages of change for physical activity and the physical component of health-related quality of life.

Analyses Related to Hypothesis #2: Regressions to Assess Associations With Increased MVPA and Health-Related Quality of Life at Follow-Up

To test the second hypothesis that baseline emotional outlook, positive emotion, and ego-resilience would be associated with increases in MVPA from baseline to follow-up and to levels of health-related quality of life at follow-up, three separate hierarchical regressions were conducted. Workplace, BMI, age, and health limits were entered in all the regressions first as covariates (these were unrelated to increases in MVPA). Follow-up emotional outlook, positive emotion, and ego-resilience were then entered in their respective regressions as control variables, so that the unique contribution of baseline emotional outlook, positive emotion, and ego-resilience could be ascertained. Results, presented in Table 3, suggested that both emotional outlook and ego-resilience at baseline were related to increases in MVPA; however, positive emotion was not.1 Although the regressions involving health-related quality of life were both significant overall, none of the predictor variables made significant independent contributions except BMI, and health limits to physical activity were negatively related to the physical component of health-related quality of life.

Table 3.

Relationships Between Baseline Emotional Outlook, Positive Emotion, and Ego-Resilience and Increase in MVPA and Health-Related Quality of Life at Follow-Up.

Control variables
Predictor variables
Age
BMI
Health limits
Baseline emotional outlook
Baseline positive emotion
Baseline ego-resilience
Full model
Outcome variable β 95% CI p β 95% CI p β 95% CI p β 95% CI p β 95% CI p β 95% CI p F p R 2
Increase in MVPA minutes (from baseline to follow-up) −.05 [–5.31, 3.35] .653 −.08 [–16.75, 8.60] .523 −.04 [–60.52, 42.79] .733 .32 [3.90, 221.74] .043 −.24 [–32.30, 5.48] .161 .49 [4.64, 31.28] .009 2.21 .021 .29
Follow-up health-related quality of life—physical component .05 [–.08, .12] .648 −.29 [–.68, –.11] .007 −.32 [–2.82, –.54] .004 −.15 [–4.04, 1.36] .326 .01 [–.46, .47] .971 .00 [–.33, .34] .983 2.14 .025 .28
Follow-up health-related quality of life—mental component .06 [–.09, .14] .639 .06 [–.24, .41] .606 −.16 [–2.19, .42] .182 .12 [–1.67, 3.86] .433 .25 [–.13, .83] .146 −.07 [–.40, .28] .720 2.00 .038 .26

Note. N = 78. Separate regressions were conducted for each of the outcome variables: (a) increases in MVPA on baseline emotional outlook, positive emotion, and ego-resilience controlling for follow-up emotional outlook, positive emotion, and ego-resilience; (b) follow-up health-related quality of life—physical component on baseline emotional outlook, positive emotion, and ego-resilience controlling for follow-up emotional outlook, positive emotion, and ego-resilience; (c) follow-up health-related quality of life—mental component on baseline emotional outlook, positive emotion, and ego-resilience controlling for follow-up emotional outlook, positive emotion, and ego-resilience; all regressions also included workplace, age, BMI, and health limits as control variables. Adjusted standardized beta (β) coefficients are reported for each predictor variable. MVPA = moderate and vigorous physical activity; BMI = body mass index.

Analyses Related to Hypothesis #3: The Role of Ego-Resilience as a Mediator

Finally, the hypothesis that ego-resilience (at follow-up) would mediate the relationship between emotion and increased MVPA as well as health-related quality of life at follow-up was tested by examining indirect effects using bootstrapping (k = 10,000) procedures (Preacher & Hayes, 2008). The relevant paths as well as the bootstrap results for the indirect effects are reported in Table 4. The bias-corrected confidence intervals were considered significant if they did not include zero (Preacher & Hayes, 2008).

Table 4.

Ego-Resilience as a Mediator in the Relationship Between Emotional Outlook/Positive Emotion and Physical Activity as well as Health-Related Quality of Life.

Path c
Path a
Path b
Path c’
Indirect effect (a × b)
Outcome variable Predictor variable R 2 β (SE) p β (SE) p β (SE) p β (SE) p 95% CI
Increase in MVPA minutes (from baseline to follow-up) Emotional outlook .11 .17 (.09) .065 .40 (.08) <.001 −.15 (.13) .243 .23 (.10) .03 [–.22, .06]
Positive emotion .05 −.00 (.08) .969 .33 (.08) <.001 −.01 (.13) .950 −.00 (.09) .995 [–.10, .11]
Health-related quality of life—physical component Emotional outlook .08 .07 (.09) .457 .40 (.08) <.001 .24 (.13) .075 −.03 (.10) .804 [.02, .20]
Positive emotion .08 .11 (.08) .210 .33 (.07) <.001 .19 (.13) .129 .04 (.09) ..660 [.00, .17]
Health-related quality of life—mental component Emotional outlook .12 .27 (.09) .003 .40 (.08) <.001 .07 (.13) .586 .25 (.10) .018 [–.05, .12]
Positive emotion .13 .26 (.08) .002 .33 (.07) <.001 .08 (.12) .507 .23 (.09) .013 [–.04, .11]

Note. N = 78 for models with MVPA and N = 80 for models with health-related quality of life. The predictor variables (emotional outlook and positive emotion) were standardized (by subtracting the mean and dividing by the standard error) prior to analyses so standardized beta (β) coefficients are reported for each path. Bias-corrected confidence intervals (95%) calculated with 10,000 bootstrap samples were considered significant if they did not include zero. Workplace was included as a statistical control in each model. MVPA = moderate and vigorous physical activity. c’ = The apostrophe refers to the c path AFTER the mediator has been included in the model (see Figure 1).

Moderate and vigorous physical activity

Baseline emotional outlook was significantly related to follow-up ego-resilience, though not to increased MVPA; follow-up ego-resilience was not significantly related to increased MVPA, and the indirect effect coefficient was not significant, suggesting that mediation was not present.

Baseline positive emotion was significantly related to follow-up ego-resilience, though follow-up ego-resilience was not significantly related to increased MVPA and the indirect effect coefficient was not significant, suggesting that mediation was not present.

Health-related quality of life

In the model with emotional outlook, follow-up ego-resilience was not significantly related to the physical component of health-related quality of life at follow-up, although this path was nearly statistically significant (p = .075); however, the indirect effect coefficient was significant, suggesting that mediation was present. In the model with positive emotion, follow-up ego-resilience was not significantly related to the physical component of health-related quality of life at follow-up, and the indirect effect coefficient was not significant, suggesting that mediation was not present.

Follow-up ego-resilience was not significantly related to the mental component of health-related quality of life at follow-up, although both baseline emotional outlook and baseline positive emotion were. Neither indirect effect coefficient was significant, suggesting that mediation was not present.

Discussion

The purpose of this study was to explore whether individual differences in emotional outlook, positive emotion, and ego-resilience were associated with physical activity and health-related quality of life among men following a workplace health intervention. It was hypothesized that emotional outlook/positive emotion and ego-resilience at baseline would be positively associated with physical activity and health-related quality of life as well as increased MVPA among men following the implementation of a workplace health intervention. Furthermore, it was hypothesized that ego-resilience would mediate the relationship between baseline emotional outlook/positive emotion and increased MVPA and follow-up health-related quality of life. These hypotheses were partially supported.

In this study, a positive emotional outlook on life among men was related to increased MVPA in the context of a novel health promotion program designed for men, consistent with the findings of Baruth et al. (2011). In addition, the single-item measure of emotional outlook was related to stage of change for physical activity, suggesting that men with a more positive emotional outlook were in a higher stage of change for physical activity at baseline and, thus, more ready to adopt a positive change in physical activity. However, adjusting for stage of change in the regression model did not change the relationship between emotional outlook and change in moderate-to-strenuous physical activity. Adapting the PANAS with additional items (speculated to capture male-centric emotions) provided additional support for these findings. At baseline, men with higher positive emotion had a higher stage of change for physical activity. The adaptation of the PANAS measure is novel and is supported by growing recognition that differences in the experience and expression of emotions such as distress among men and women need to be examined (Ridge, Emslie, & White, 2011; White, 2006). Baruth et al. (2011) suggested that interventions targeting physical activity for men may be more successful if they first aim to increase happiness. Although the POWERPLAY intervention was not designed to increase happiness, the present results provide further evidence for their suggestion.

Higher levels of ego-resilience at baseline were related to a higher stage of change for physical activity among men, and higher baseline ego-resilience was related to increased physical activity among men following the intervention. Yet follow-up ego-resilience was unrelated to change in physical activity, and individual differences in ego-resilience at follow-up did not mediate the link between emotion and increased physical activity contrary to predictions. Interestingly, physical activity has been linked to increases in psychosocial resources over time, suggesting emotional and mental benefits of becoming active (Hogan, Catalino, Mata, & Fredrickson, 2015). The present research suggests that both positive emotion and ego-resilience may independently contribute to increasing physical activity following a health intervention for men. It seems possible that positive emotion/psychosocial resources and physical activity may have reciprocal effects on one another.

Since emotional outlook and positive emotions were also positively related to ego-resilience and health-related quality of life at baseline, support was also garnered for Fredrickson’s broaden-and-build theory (Fredrickson, 1998). In previous research, positive emotions have also been reported to reinforce an individual’s self-regulatory capacity by building resources over time, contributing to enduring individual differences such as ego-resilience (Cohn et al., 2009; Seaton & Beaumont, 2015). The correlation and mediation analyses suggested that ego-resilience (not the emotion variables) was related to the physical component of health-related quality of life at follow-up, whereas emotional outlook and positive emotion (not ego-resilience) were related to the mental component of health-related quality of life. Although more research examining interventions designed to increase (as opposed to examining individual differences in) positive emotion is needed, in other research, a loving-kindness meditation program increased both positive emotions and life satisfaction over time (Fredrickson, Cohn, Coffey, Pek, & Finkel, 2008). The present results provide a preliminary indication that both emotion and ego-resilience may be important for health-related quality of life, providing further support that the ability to effectively problem-solve, regulate emotion, and change perspective makes ego-resilience a valuable personal resource worth building (Cohn et al., 2009).

Developing ways to increase the impact of health promotion programs could assist in reaching men and translate into improved health outcomes. The findings presented here form a theoretical basis for a novel approach to the development of gender-sensitive programs. The demonstrated linkage between emotional outlook, ego-resilience, and physical activity can be a driver for future health-based interventions for men, such as integrating mental health and chronic disease components in all-encompassing holistic program development. Workplace wellness programs that promote physical activity have demonstrated financial benefits, with a healthier workforce translating into reduced absenteeism and medical costs (van Dongen et al., 2011). Employee mental wellness has been identified to be among the largest contributors to work-related costs such as those attributed to presenteeism (Collins et al., 2005), suggesting that a comprehensive program that includes the promotion of employee well-being has the potential to not only impact physical activity but also directly reduce such costs.

Limitations and Strengths

This research represents an initial attempt to assess the role of individual differences in positive emotion and ego-resilience in promoting men’s physical activity and health-related quality of life. Three limitations deserve mention. First, the gender-sensitive workplace wellness intervention was targeted at improving physical activity, not emotion or resilience; thus, the causal role of emotional outlook and ego-resilience in increasing physical activity needs to be confirmed in future studies. Despite this limitation, the results are based on 6-month follow-up study, with hypotheses that were generated in advance of data collection. Future research should continue to explore the potential mediating role of resilience and consider determining directional causality of emotional outlook so that the impact of promoting these individual resources on specific health and well-being outcomes could be better understood. Second, male-dominated industries were targeted for recruitment in the POWERPLAY program and this research was based on a sample of largely Caucasian men with middle/high incomes participating in a workplace wellness intervention. More research is needed with men in other contexts, such as those with lower income jobs and those working in other industries. Work conditions, supervisor–employee relations, and other factors linked to employee well-being may influence workers differentially in different workplaces and should be explored in future research. Similarly, these findings may not be generalizable to men outside the workplace (e.g., unemployed or retired). Yet, engaging men in the workplace was also a strength of the present program. Other research has suggested that men experience gender role strain, prioritizing their role as providers above being more physically active (Griffith, Gunter, & Allen, 2011), so support to increase physical activity in the workplace works with, rather than against, the gender role as provider. Third, and finally, although both physical activity and socio-cognitive factors related to physical activity were assessed, the data were collected via self-report and may have been subject to recall or response bias. Objective measures of physical activity (e.g., accelerometers) and health data (e.g., blood pressure) could be considered in the future.

Conclusions

In conclusion, emotional outlook and ego-resilience were both related to increases in physical activity among men following a workplace wellness program, suggesting that the effectiveness of interventions promoting physical activity may be enhanced if a positive emotional outlook is also supported. Although ego-resilience at follow-up did not mediate the link between emotion and change in physical activity, it was related to health-related quality of life, so it may still be an important personal resource to consider building in health promotion interventions. Evidence is accumulating in support of the notion that positive emotions are important for both building resources as well as well-being over time (Cohn et al., 2009). The results of the present research provide an important addition to the literature in this area.

1.

We examined one additional model in which stage of change for physical activity was included along with the control variables; however, stage of change was unrelated to change in moderate-to-strenuous physical activity at follow-up in the regression model. The coefficients for the predictor variables (emotional outlook, positive emotion, and ego-resilience) remained unchanged in interpretation, and the overall R2 for the model was only slightly higher (from .214 to .216).

Declaration of Conflicting Interests: The author(s) declared no potential conflicts of interest with respect to the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article.

Funding: The author(s) disclosed receipt of the following financial support for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article: This work was supported by the Canadian Cancer Society Research Institute (Grant number 701259-02).

ORCID iDs: Cherisse L. Seaton Inline graphic https://orcid.org/0000-0003-4646-9894

Joan L. Bottorff Inline graphic https://orcid.org/0000-0001-9724-5351

References

  1. Baruth M., Lee D., Sui X., Church T. S., Marcus B. H., Wilcox S., Blair S. N. (2011). Emotional outlook on life predicts increases in physical activity among initially inactive men. Health Education and Behavior, 38(2), 150–158. doi: 10.1177/1090198110376352 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Baumeister R. F., Gailliot M., DeWall C. N., Oaten M. (2006). Self-regulation and personality: How interventions increase regulatory success, and how depletion moderates the effects of traits on behavior. Journal of Personality, 74(6), 1773–1801. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-6494.2006.00428.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Bernabe M., Botia J. M. (2016). Resilience as a mediator in emotional social support’s relationship with occupational psychology health in firefighters. Journal of Health Psychology, 21(8), 1778–1786. doi: 10.1177/1359105314566258 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Bize R., Johnson J. A., Plotnikoff R. C. (2007). Physical activity level and health-related quality of life in the general adult population: A systematic review. Preventive Medicine, 45(6), 401–415. doi: 10.1016/j.ypmed.2007.07.017 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Block J. (2002). Personality as an affect-processing system: Toward an integrative theory. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. [Google Scholar]
  6. Block J., Kremen A. M. (1996). IQ and ego-resiliency: Conceptual and empirical connections and separateness. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 70(2), 349–361. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.70.2.349 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Bottorff J. L., Seaton C. L., Johnson S. T., Caperchione C. M., Oliffe J. L., More K., … Tillotson S. M. (2015). An updated review of interventions that include promotion of physical activity for adult men. Sports Medicine, 45(6), 775–800. doi: 10.1007/s40279-014-0286-3 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Caperchione C. M., Sharp P., Bottorff J. L., Stolp S., Oliffe J. L., Johnson S. T., … Lamont S. (2015). The POWERPLAY workplace physical activity and nutrition intervention for men: Study protocol and baseline characteristics. Contemporary Clinical Trials, 44, 42–47. doi: 10.1016/j.cct.2015.07.013 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Castonguay A. L., Pila E., Wrosch C., Sabiston C. M. (2015). Body-related self-conscious emotions relate to physical activity motivation and behavior in men. American Journal of Men’s Health, 9(3), 209–221. doi: 10.1177/1557988314537517 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Cohn M. A., Fredrickson B. L., Brown S. L., Mikels J. A., Conway A. M. (2009). Happiness unpacked: Positive emotions increase life satisfaction by building resilience. Emotion, 9(3), 361–368. doi: 10.1037/a0015952 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Collins J. J., Baase C. M., Sharda C. E., Ozminkowski R. J., Nicholson S., Billotti G. M., … Berger M. L. (2005). The assessment of chronic health conditions on work performance, absence, and total economic impact for employers. Journal of Occupational and Environmental Medicine, 47(6), 547–557. doi: 10.1097/01.jom.0000166864.58664.29 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. DeSteno D., Gross J. J., Kubzansky L. (2013). Affective science and health: The importance of emotion and emotion regulation. Health Psychology, 32(5), 474–486. doi: 10.1037/a0030259 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Dumith S. C., Gigante D. P., Domingues M. R. (2007). Stages of change for physical activity in adults from Southern Brazil: A population-based survey. The International Journal of Behavioral Nutrition and Physical Activity, 4(25), 1–10. doi: 10.1186/1479-5868-4-25 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Fields A. J., Hoyt R. E., Linnville S. E., Moore J. L. (2016). Physical activity, sleep, and C-reactive protein as markers of positive health in resilient older men. Journal of Health Psychology, 21(9), 1928–1938. doi: 10.1177/1359105314568578 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Fredrickson B. L. (1998). What good are positive emotions? Review of General Psychology, 2(3), 300–319. doi: 10.1037/1089-2680.2.3.300 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Fredrickson B. L. (2013). Positive emotions broaden and build. In Devine P., Plant A. (Eds.), Advances in experimental social psychology (pp. 1–53). Burlington: Academic Press. [Google Scholar]
  17. Fredrickson B. L., Cohn M. A., Coffey K. A., Pek J., Finkel S. M. (2008). Open hearts build lives: Positive emotions, induced through loving-kindness meditation, build consequential personal resources. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 95(5), 1045–1062. doi: 10.1037/a0013262 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Godin G., Shephard R. J. (1985). A simple method to assess exercise behavior in the community. Canadian Journal of Applied Sport Sciences, 10(3), 141–146. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Gray M. C., Hunt K., Mutrie N., Anderson S. A., Treweek S., Wyke S. (2013). Weight management for overweight and obese men delivered through professional football clubs: A pilot randomized trial. International Journal of Behavioral Nutrition and Physical Activity, 10(121), 1–17. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Griffith D. M., Gunter K., Allen J. O. (2011). Male gender role strain as a barrier to African American men’s physical activity. Health Education and Behavior, 38(5), 482–491. doi: 10.1177/1090198110383660 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Hogan C. L., Catalino L. I., Mata J., Fredrickson B. L. (2015). Beyond emotional benefits: Physical activity and sedentary behaviour affect psychosocial resources through emotions. Psychology and Health, 30(3), 354–369. doi: 10.1080/08870446.2014.973410 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Jacobs D. R., Ainsworth B. E., Hartman T. J., Leon A. S. (1993). A simultaneous evaluation of 10 commonly used physical activity questionnaires. Medicine and Science in Sports and Exercise, 25(1), 81–91. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Johnson S. T. Stolp S. Seaton C. Sharp P. Caperchione C. M. Bottorff J. L. …Healy T. (2016). A men’s workplace health intervention: Results of the POWERPLAY program pilot study. Journal of Occupational and Environmental Medicine, 58(8), 765–769. doi: 10.1097/JOM.0000000000000793 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Keller J., Gellert P., Knoll N., Schneider M., Ernsting A. (2016). Self-efficacy and planning as predictors of physical activity in the context of workplace health promotion. Applied Psychology: Health and Well-Being, 8(3), 301–321. doi: 10.1111/aphw.12073 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Kim E. S., Kubzansky L. D., Soo J., Boehm J. K. (2017). Maintaining healthy behavior: A prospective study of psychological well-being and physical activity. Annals of Behavioral Medicine, 51(3), 337–347. doi: 10.1007/s12160-016-9856-y [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Letzring T. D., Block J., Funder D. C. (2005). Ego-control and ego-resiliency: Generalization of self-report scales based on personality descriptions from acquaintances, clinicians, and the self. Journal of Research in Personality, 39(4), 395–422. doi: 10.1016/j.jrp.2004.06.003 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  27. Liao Y., Shonkoff E. T., Dunton G. F. (2015). The acute relationships between affect, physical feeling states, and physical activity in daily life: A review of current evidence. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 1–7. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01975 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Marques A., Peralta M., Martins J., Catunda R., de Matos M. G., Nunes L. S. (2016). Associations between physical activity and self-rated wellbeing in European adults: A population-based, cross-sectional study. Preventive Medicine, 91, 18–23. doi: 10.1016/j.ypmed.2016.07.021 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Martin J., McCaughtry N., Shen B., Fahlman M., Garn A., Ferry M. (2012). Resiliency, control, enjoyment and physical activity in African American high school students. Sport Science Review, 20(5–6), 53–71. doi: 10.2478/v10237-011-0064-1 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  30. Morgan P. J., Collins C. E., Plotnikoff R. C., Cook A. T., Berthon B., Mitchell S., Callister R. (2012). The impact of a workplace-based weight loss program on work-related outcomes in overweight male shift workers. Journal of Occupational and Environmental Medicine, 54(2), 122–127. doi: 10.1097/JOM.0b013e31824329ab; 10.1097/JOM.0b013e31824329ab [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Nigg C. R. (2005). There is more to stages of exercise than just exercise. Exercise and Sport Sciences Reviews, 33(1), 32–35. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Oliffe J. L., Bottorff J. L., Sharp P., Caperchione C. M., Johnson S. T., Healy T., … Errey S. (2017). Healthy eating and active living: Rural-based working men’s perspectives. American Journal of Men’s Health, 11(6), 1664–1672. doi: 10.1177/1557988315619372 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Pagoto S. L., Schneider K. L., Oleski J. L., Luciani J. M., Bodenlos J. S., Whited M. C. (2012). Male inclusion in randomized controlled trials of lifestyle weight loss interventions. Obesity, 20(6), 1234–1239. doi: 10.1038/oby.2011.140 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Panza G. A., Taylor B. A., Thompson P. D., White C. M., Pescatello L. S. (2017). Physical activity intensity and subjective well-being in healthy adults. Journal of Health Psychology. Advance online publication. doi: 10.1177/1359105317691589 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Plotnikoff R. C., Hotz S. B., Birkett N. J., Courneya K. S. (2001). Exercise and the transtheoretical model: A longitudinal test of a population sample. Preventive Medicine, 33(5), 441–452. doi: 10.1006/pmed.2001.0914 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Preacher K. J., Hayes A. F. (2008). Asymptotic and resampling strategies for assessing and comparing indirect effects in multiple mediator models. Behavior Research Methods, 40(3), 879–891. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Pringle A., Zwolinsky S., McKenna J., Daly-Smith A., Robertson S., White A. (2013). Effect of a national programme of men’s health delivered in English premier league football clubs. Public Health, 127(1), 18–26. doi: 10.1016/j.puhe.2012.10.012 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Pringle A., Zwolinsky S., McKenna J., Robertson S., Daly-Smith A., White A. (2014). Health improvement for men and hard-to-engage-men delivered in English premier league football clubs. Health Education Research, 29(3), 503–520. doi: 10.1093/her/cyu009 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Prochaska J. O., DiClemente C. C. (1983). Stages and processes of self-change of smoking: Toward an integrative model of change. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 51(3), 390–395. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Puett R., Teas J., Espana-Romero V., Artero E. G., Lee D. C., Baruth M., … Blair S. N. (2014). Physical activity: Does environment make a difference for tension, stress, emotional outlook, and perceptions of health status? Journal of Physical Activity and Health, 11(8), 1503–1511. doi: 10.1123/jpah.2012-0375 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Ridge D., Emslie C., White A. (2011). Understanding how men experience, express and cope with mental distress: Where next? Sociology of Health and Illness, 33(1), 145–159. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9566.2010.01266.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Robertson S., Witty K., Zwolinsky S., Day R. (2013). Men’s health promotion interventions: What have we learned from previous programmes. Community Practitioner, 86(11), 38–41. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Robroek S. J., van Lenthe F. J., van Empelen P., Burdorf A. (2009). Determinants of participation in worksite health promotion programmes: A systematic review. The International Journal of Behavioral Nutrition and Physical Activity, 6(1), 26. doi: 10.1186/1479-5868-6-26 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Seaton C. L., Beaumont S. L. (2015). Pursuing the good life: A short-term follow-up study of the role of positive/negative emotions and ego-resilience in personal goal striving and eudaimonic well-being. Motivation and Emotion, 39(5), 813–826. doi: 10.1007/s11031-015-9493-y [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  45. Statistics Canada. (2017). Canadian health measures survey: Activity monitor data, 2014 to 2015. Retrieved from http://www.statcan.gc.ca/daily-quotidien/170419/dq170419e-eng.htm
  46. Tabachnick B. G., Fidell L. S. (2001). Using multivariate statistics (3rd ed.). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. [Google Scholar]
  47. Thompson E. R. (2007). Development and validation of an internationally reliable short-form of the Positive and Negative Affect Schedule (PANAS). Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 38(2), 227–242. doi: 10.1177/0022022106297301 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  48. van Dongen J. M., Proper K. I., van Wier M. F., van der Beek A J, Bongers P. M., van Mechelen W., van Tulder M. W. (2011). Systematic review on the financial return of worksite health promotion programmes aimed at improving nutrition and/or increasing physical activity. Obesity Reviews, 12(12), 1031–1049. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-789X.2011.00925.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Ware J. E., Kosinski M., Keller S. D. (1996). A 12-item short-form health survey: Construction of scales and preliminary tests of reliability and validity. Medical Care, 34(3), 220–233. doi: 10.1097/00005650-199603000-00003 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Watson D., Clark L. A., Tellegen A. (1988). Development and validation of brief measures of positive and negative affect: The PANAS scales. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54(6), 1063–1070. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. White A. (2006). Men and mental wellbeing – encouraging gender sensitivity. Mental Health Review Journal, 11(4), 3–6. [Google Scholar]
  52. Williams M. N., Gomez Grajales C. A., Kurkiewicz D. (2013). Assumptions of multiple regression: Correcting two misconceptions. Practical Assessment, Research & Evaluation, 18(11), 1–14. [Google Scholar]
  53. World Health Organization. (2007). Engaging men and boys in changing gender-based inequity in health: Evidence from programme interventions. Geneva: World Health Organization. [Google Scholar]

Articles from American Journal of Men's Health are provided here courtesy of SAGE Publications

RESOURCES