Skip to main content
F1000Research logoLink to F1000Research
. 2018 Aug 31;7:1325. Originally published 2018 Aug 21. [Version 2] doi: 10.12688/f1000research.15956.2

Central sensitization and pain hypersensitivity: Some critical considerations.

Emanuel N van den Broeke 1,a
PMCID: PMC6206600  PMID: 30416715

Version Changes

Revised. Amendments from Version 1

We have listed the “European Research Council ‘starting’ grant (PROBING PAIN 336130)” in the “Grant information” section of this version 2, as this was missed out in our previous version. We have also made a small correction in the first Introduction paragraph.

Abstract

Since its discovery, central sensitization has gained enormous popularity. It is widely used to explain pain hypersensitivity in a wide range of clinical pain conditions. However, at present there is no general consensus on the definition of central sensitization. Moreover, the use of the term central sensitization in the clinical domain has been criticized. The aim of this paper is to foster the discussion on the definition of central sensitization and its use.

Keywords: Central sensitization, definition, pain, nociception, secondary hyperalgesia.

Introduction

  • “Many subjects, but by no means all, become conscious of soreness of skin surrounding a small area of injury”

With these words Sir Thomas Lewis starts one of the chapters in his book “Pain” 1 (p. 68). The sentence refers to what is now known as “secondary hyperalgesia”, which has intrigued pain neuroscientists for almost a century. Lewis was probably the first that systematically studied this phenomenon. He hypothesized that secondary hyperalgesia was due to a peripheral mechanism (“nocifensor axon reflex”). Impulses generated by nerves at the site of injury travel antidromically via branches to their endings, where there is a release of substances that excite neighboring nerves 1.

However, by performing a series of psychophysical experiments Hardy et al. 2 came to another conclusion. Contrary to Lewis who suggested that secondary hyperalgesia resulted from a spreading of excitation in the skin, Hardy et al. hypothesized that secondary hyperalgesia resulted from a “central excitatory state” 2 (p. 139).

Similar to the idea of Lewis of a network of interconnected nerves, Hardy et al. hypothesized that in the spinal cord there is a pool of neurons consisting of primary and secondary neurons that make synaptic connections to a network of “internuncial” neurons. The function of these internuncial neurons would be to establish and maintain an excitatory state within the neuron pool. In the case of tissue injury, the barrage of noxious impulses originating from the site of injury enters the spinal cord where they excite the network of internuncial neurons, leading to an excitation of connected neurons 2.

  • “If now the skin is pricked in the area of secondary hyperalgesia, a burst of impulses passes into the spinal cord and when reaching the tertiary neuron it is facilitated giving rise to more intense sensation than usual” 2 (p.135).

Woolf 3 was the first that provided evidence for such a “central excitatory state”. He showed that in rats the motor reflex threshold elicited by mechanical punctate stimuli delivered adjacent to a burn injury was reduced for many hours 3. In subsequent studies Woolf and co-workers further showed that the induction of this “central excitatory state” does not require tissue injury, but that it can also be induced after electrical stimulation of C-fiber nociceptors 4. Based on these findings, Woolf and co-workers 5 introduced the term “central sensitization” (CS):

  • “This is the phenomenon of aberrant convergence; the generation of pain by activating sensory fibres that normally only produce innocuous sensations i.e. the large myelinated low threshold afferents. Aberrant convergence arises as a consequence of changes induced within the spinal cord by activity in unmyelinated afferent fibres – a process called central sensitization” (p. 256).

Actually, Woolf et al. describe here what is now called allodynia : “pain in response to a non-nociceptive stimulus” 6.

Since 2008, the task force for taxonomy of the International Association for the Study of Pain (IASP) 6 proposes the following definition of CS:

  • “Increased responsiveness of nociceptive neurons in the central nervous system to their normal or subthreshold afferent input”.

The task force for taxonomy 6 defines a nociceptive neuron as:

  • “A peripheral or central neuron of the somatosensory system that is able to encode a noxious stimulus”.

But what is meant by encoding? And which neurons can be considered part of the somatosensory system and which not?

Nowadays the term CS is very popular and is associated with many more conditions than secondary hyperalgesia. The concept of CS is used by both basic scientists and clinicians; however its use in the clinical domain has been criticized 7. The aim of this paper is to foster the discussion on the definition of CS and its use.

Is CS defined too broadly?

If a definition becomes too broad it will be used non-selectively and it will lose its value. On the other hand, if a definition becomes too specific it may miss important phenomena. The IASP proposal for the definition of CS clearly describes a phenomenon. However, in the literature CS is often presented as mechanism, for example, Vardeh et al. 8 (p. T56). More importantly, the definition does not mention a functional meaning. If the purpose of the term CS was and/or is to explain pain hypersensitivity then this should be included in the definition. Furthermore, the term “nociceptive neurons” may then not be specific enough. As pointed out by Sandkühler 9:

  • “Nociceptive neurons comprise a heterogeneous cell group with putatively many different and sometimes opposing functions, including a large group of inhibitory interneurons. Thus enhanced responsiveness of some of these neurons could contribute to hyperalgesia. On the other hand, enhanced responsiveness of inhibitory nociceptive neurons may well lead to stronger feedback inhibition and analgesia, while still other neurons may not contribute to the experiences of pain but rather to altered motor or vegetative responses to a noxious stimulus” (p. 708).

Woolf 10 proposed an alternative definition of CS which links CS directly to pain hypersensitivity:

  • “An amplification of neural signaling within the CNS that elicits pain hypersensitivity” (p. S5).

However, establishing a causal relationship between CS and pain hypersensitivity is particularly difficult. Indeed, it is possible to measure the activity of nociceptive neurons in the CNS in animal preparations but obviously, we cannot measure pain perception. Conversely, we can measure pain perception in humans but we cannot directly measure the activity of nociceptive neurons 11.

In addition, because we cannot record directly from nociceptive neurons in humans and we have to rely on changes in pain perception or thresholds, the risk is to end up in a circulus in probando 12. For example, patient X shows CS because she/he suffers from pain hypersensitivity and pain hypersensitivity is a manifestation of CS. The described evidence for the conclusion is not different from the conclusion itself.

Taken together, depending on the purpose of the term CS, it may be necessary to reconsider the IASP definition.

Is secondary hyperalgesia the only example of CS?

In a related note, the task force for taxonomy of the IASP 6 further states about the term sensitization:

  • “This is a neurophysiological term that can only be applied when both input and output of the neural system under study is known, e.g. by controlling the stimulus and measuring the neural event”.

According to Treede 13 the phenomenon of secondary hyperalgesia induced by intradermal capsaicin injection

  • “… is currently the only example where both input and output of spinal neurons have been documented in the same model and, hence, the IASP definition of CS is fulfilled” (p. 1200).

This would imply that, for the moment, the term CS, as provided by the IASP, may only be used for this particular condition.

When injected into the skin capsaicin activates TRPV1 expressing nociceptors and elicits a burning sensation 14. A consequence is the development of increased pinprick sensitivity in a large part of the skin surrounding the injection site 14, a phenomenon reminiscent of secondary hyperalgesia after tissue injury. By recording the activity of nociceptive neurons in the primate spinal cord before and after capsaicin injection, Simone et al. 15 showed that both wide-dynamic-range (WDR) and high-threshold (HT) neurons respond more strongly to pinprick stimuli when these stimuli were delivered after the injection to the skin surrounding the injection site (output). The same group also recorded the activity of peripheral A-fiber and C-fiber nociceptors in this area (input) but their activity was unchanged 16. Because these sensitized spinal neurons project via the spinothalamic pathway to the brain, they may contribute to the increase in pinprick perception in humans.

However, it remains puzzling why secondary hyperalgesia is characterized by an increase in the perception for mechanical pinprick stimuli, but not heat stimuli 1719. Should a sensitization of WDR neurons, which are polymodal, not also lead to an increase in perception for other modalities like touch or heat?

Nociceptive input (and increases thereof) does not necessarily elicits pain

An important function of nociception in normal conditions is to warn for tissue damage. Therefore it would make sense that nociceptors are activated before there is any tissue damage. Compatible with this idea are the observations that nociceptors in humans are activated by stimulus intensities that are not perceived as painful 20.

Indeed, in normal conditions (i.e. without sensitization) mechanical pinprick stimuli typically elicit a sharp pricking sensation, which is not perceived as painful in the majority of people. However, studies using microneurography have clearly demonstrated that such mechanical pinprick probes activate mechanosensitive nociceptors in the skin 2123. Moreover, a study comparing the perceptual pain thresholds in human volunteers with the thresholds for nociceptors in animals using the same pinprick probes, suggests that the non-painful sharp pricking sensation is mediated by mechanosensitive nociceptors 24.

Pinprick stimuli delivered after sensitization to the skin surrounding the site at which sensitization was induced clearly elicit an increase in intensity of perception but this is not always perceived as painful. Importantly, the perception elicited by tactile stimuli is not increased 25 (and unpublished observations), indicating that the increase in the pricking sensation elicited by pinprick stimuli after sensitization is mediated by mechano-sensitive nociceptors instead of low-threshold mechanoreceptors.

Likewise, we recently showed that heat perception elicited by tiny laser stimuli selectively activating C-fiber nociceptors in the skin was greater when these stimuli were delivered to the area of secondary hyperalgesia 26. However, despite the fact that our heat stimuli selectively activated C-fiber nociceptors, the perception elicited by these stimuli was not qualified as painful neither at baseline (before inducing sensitization) nor after the induction of sensitization. Importantly, the greater heat sensitivity elicited by these stimuli is probably a perceptual correlate of CS. Indeed, Kronschläger et al. 27 recently showed in rats that strong peripheral nociceptive input activates glial cells (which include microglial and astrocytes) leading to the release of cytokines and chemokines which excites remote C-fiber synapses.

Taken together, both examples (increased pinprick sensitivity and greater heat sensitivity) suggest that CS does not necessarily result in pain hypersensitivity. This would plead for a mechanism-based approach of CS rather than focusing on changes in pain perception only. Indeed, according to the definitions provided by the IASP 6 one cannot label the increases in pinprick and heat perception as “hyperalgesia” because it is not an increase in pain sensitivity. They cannot be labeled as “allodynia” either, because the stimulus is a nociceptive one and is not always perceived as painful after sensitization.

Acknowledgements

The author would like to thank Diana Torta, Omer Van den Bergh, Leon Plaghki, and André Mouraux for the fruitful discussions.

Funding Statement

ENvdB is supported by the Fonds de Recherche Clinique (FRC) of the Université catholique de Louvain, Brussels, Belgium, and the European Research Council "starting" grant (PROBING PAIN 336130).

The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.

[version 2; referees: 2 approved]

References

  • 1. Lewis T: Nocifensor tenderness. In: Pain The Macmillan Company, New-York,1942; 68–83. 10.1037/13987-006 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 2. Hardy JD, Wolff HG, Goodell H: Experimental evidence on the nature of cutaneous hyperalgesia. J Clin Invest. 1950;29(1):115–40. 10.1172/JCI102227 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3. Woolf CJ: Evidence for a central component of post-injury pain hypersensitivity. Nature. 1983;306(5944):686–688. 10.1038/306686a0 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4. Woolf CJ, Wall PD: Relative effectiveness of C primary afferent fibers of different origins in evoking a prolonged facilitation of the flexor reflex in the rat. J Neurosci. 1986;6(5):1433–1442. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.06-05-01433.1986 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5. Woolf CJ, Thompson SW, King AE: Prolonged primary afferent induced alterations in dorsal horn neurones, an intracellular analysis in vivo and in vitro. J Physiol (Paris). 1988;83(3):255–266. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6. Loeser JD, Treede RD: The Kyoto protocol of IASP Basic Pain Terminology. Pain. 2008;137(3):473–477. 10.1016/j.pain.2008.04.025 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7. Hansson P: Translational aspects of central sensitization induced by primary afferent activity: what it is and what it is not. Pain. 2014;155(10):1932–1934. 10.1016/j.pain.2014.07.016 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8. Vardeh D, Mannion RJ, Woolf CJ: Toward a Mechanism-Based Approach to Pain Diagnosis. J Pain. 2016;17(9 Suppl):T50–69. 10.1016/j.jpain.2016.03.001 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9. Sandkühler J: Models and mechanisms of hyperalgesia and allodynia. Physiol Rev. 2009;89(2):707–758. 10.1152/physrev.00025.2008 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10. Woolf CJ: Central sensitization: implications for the diagnosis and treatment of pain. Pain. 2011;152(3 Suppl):S2–15. 10.1016/j.pain.2010.09.030 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11. Cervero F: Central sensitization and visceral hypersensitivity: Facts and fictions. Scand J Pain. 2014;5(2):49–50. 10.1016/j.sjpain.2014.02.005 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12. van den Broeke EN, Torta DM, Van den Bergh O: Central sensitization: Explanation or phenomenon? Clin Psychol Sci. 2018; in press. 10.1177/2167702618781804 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 13. Treede RD: Gain control mechanisms in the nociceptive system. Pain. 2016;157(6):1199–204. 10.1097/j.pain.0000000000000499 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14. LaMotte RH, Shain CN, Simone DA, et al. : Neurogenic hyperalgesia: psychophysical studies of underlying mechanisms. J Neurophysiol. 1991;66(1):190–211. 10.1152/jn.1991.66.1.190 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15. Simone DA, Sorkin LS, Oh U, et al. : Neurogenic hyperalgesia: central neural correlates in responses of spinothalamic tract neurons. J Neurophysiol. 1991;66(1):228–246. 10.1152/jn.1991.66.1.228 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16. Baumann TK, Simone DA, Shain CN, et al. : Neurogenic hyperalgesia: the search for the primary cutaneous afferent fibers that contribute to capsaicin-induced pain and hyperalgesia. J Neurophysiol. 1991;66(1):212–227. 10.1152/jn.1991.66.1.212 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17. Ali Z, Meyer RA, Campbell JN: Secondary hyperalgesia to mechanical but not heat stimuli following a capsaicin injection in hairy skin. Pain. 1996;68(2–3):401–411. 10.1016/S0304-3959(96)03199-5 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18. Raja SN, Campbell JN, Meyer RA: Evidence for different mechanisms of primary and secondary hyperalgesia following heat injury to the glabrous skin. Brain. 1984;107(Pt 4):1179–1188. 10.1093/brain/107.4.1179 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19. van den Broeke EN, Lenoir C, Mouraux A: Secondary hyperalgesia is mediated by heat-insensitive A-fibre nociceptors. J Physiol. 2016;594(22):6767–6776. 10.1113/JP272599 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20. Van Hees J, Gybels JM: Pain related to single afferent C fibers from human skin. Brain Res. 1972;48:397–400. 10.1016/0006-8993(72)90198-9 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21. Garell PC, McGillis SL, Greenspan JD: Mechanical response properties of nociceptors innervating feline hairy skin. J Neurophysiol. 1996;75(3):1177–1189. 10.1152/jn.1996.75.3.1177 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22. Slugg RM, Campbell JN, Meyer RA: The population response of A- and C-fiber nociceptors in monkey encodes high-intensity mechanical stimuli. J Neurosci. 2004;24(19):4649–4656. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0701-04.2004 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23. Slugg RM, Meyer RA, Campbell JN: Response of cutaneous A- and C-fiber nociceptors in the monkey to controlled-force stimuli. J Neurophysiol. 2000;83(4):2179–2191. 10.1152/jn.2000.83.4.2179 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24. Greenspan JD, McGillis SL: Stimulus features relevant to the perception of sharpness and mechanically evoked cutaneous pain. Somatosens Mot Res. 1991;8(2):137–147. 10.3109/08990229109144738 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25. van den Broeke EN, Mouraux A: High-frequency electrical stimulation of the human skin induces heterotopical mechanical hyperalgesia, heat hyperalgesia, and enhanced responses to nonnociceptive vibrotactile input. J Neurophysiol. 2014;111(8):1564–1573. 10.1152/jn.00651.2013 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26. Lenoir C, Plaghki L, Mouraux A, et al. : Quickly-responding C-fibre nociceptors contribute to heat hypersensitivity in the area of secondary hyperalgesia. J Physiol. 2018; In press. 10.1113/JP275977 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27. Kronschläger MT, Drdla-Schutting R, Gassner M, et al. : Gliogenic LTP spreads widely in nociceptive pathways. Science. 2016;354(6316):1144–1148. 10.1126/science.aah5715 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
F1000Res. 2018 Oct 23. doi: 10.5256/f1000research.17668.r38593

Referee response for version 2

Philipp Hüllemann 1

Emanuel N van den Broeke provides a very interesting overview on how the term ‘central sensitization’ (CS) was originally characterized, how the use of the term developed in the scientific field and how extensively it may now be overused in basic and clinical research. The author lists several “historic” and recent scientific examples, which shine light on the mechanistic origin of central sensitization. It soon becomes clear that there is no actual consensus on the definition of central sensitization and that scientific evidence is sparse as well as contradictory on some occasions. Newer studies show that the intensity of thermal and mechanical stimuli increases most probably due to central sensitization processes but that this increase of intensity is not necessarily perceived as painful. Therefore, non-painful aspects of central sensitization are lacking in the current definition of CS. The further, we need to think of a more specific definition, which may guide researchers and clinicians in the use of the term.

I have two suggestions:

 

  1. It might have been useful to add some sentences on peripheral sensitization and its possible role in driving, as well as maintaining central sensitization.

  2. A short conclusion/summary including the authors thoughts would also be helpful.

I have read this submission. I believe that I have an appropriate level of expertise to confirm that it is of an acceptable scientific standard.

F1000Res. 2018 Oct 18. doi: 10.5256/f1000research.17668.r38228

Referee response for version 2

Geert Crombez 1

The paper by Emmanuel van den Broeke critically discusses definition and use of the terms central sensitisation and pain hypersensitivity. As stated by the author, the term central sensitisation has become increasingly popular, and seems to become over-used, if not mis-used. At times, it is important to reflect upon the origin of terms, and to track how meaning and use have changed over time. Evidently, we do not have to hold on the past, and definitions and use may change as science advances. The paper of van den Broeke is timely, and provides essential reading for many. It is an ideal paper for scholarly reflection and group discussion.

It nicely traced the origin in meaning, and the various changes in definition. It critically analyses interrelationships with other constructs, and potential disadvantages. Notwithstanding, it does not provide definite answers. Probably, that is not possible, but I would suggest that the authors reflect upon what should be the way forward. What do they recommend to readers and researchers.

Most importantly, seems to be a precise use of the term, and to avoid confusion in meaning. Indeed, as pointed out central sensitisation can be used to describe a phenomenon or to describe a mechanism. This is confusing and may result in circular reasoning: central sensitisation explains central sensitisation; In that respect, I have learned to make a distinction between at least three ways of using scientific terms: (1) as a result, (2) as an explanation and (3) as a procedure. Central sensitisation as a result refers to the phenomenon, most often as the result of a specific procedure. Indeed, there are some experimental procedures that induce the phenomenon. Finally, an scientific endeavour is to provide explanations, often mechanistic explanations, for the phenomenon that results from particular experimental procedures. In times of confusion and overuse, it is useful to come back and reflect upon what exactly is meant by someone.

I have read this submission. I believe that I have an appropriate level of expertise to confirm that it is of an acceptable scientific standard.


Articles from F1000Research are provided here courtesy of F1000 Research Ltd

RESOURCES