Abstract
There is lack of a standardized measure of technical proficiency and skill acquisition for robot-assisted surgery (RAS). Learning surgical skills, in addition to the interaction with the machine and the new surgical environment adds to the complexity of the learning process. Moreover, evaluation of surgeon performance in operating room is required to optimize patient safety. In this study, we investigated the dynamic changes of RAS trainee’s brain functional states by practice. We also developed brain functional state measurements to find the relationship between RAS skill acquisition (especially human-machine interaction skills) and reconfiguration of brain functional states. This relationship may help in providing trainees with helpful, structured feedback regarding skills requiring improvement and will help in tailoring training activities.
Introduction
With the widespread adoption of robot-assisted surgery (RAS), it becomes crucial to provide robots with the capability of collaborating closely with humans in a complex and critical environment like the operating room (OR) [1, 2]. Understanding how learning and skill acquisition occur can open new horizons for surgical teaching, skill assessment, and also set a platform for active interaction with robots. This shared environment encompasses trainees acquiring the appropriate knowledge, learning the necessary surgical skills and how to interact with robots on one side, and robots should be equipped appropriately to learn new collaborative tasks on the other [3, 4].
The main challenge in skill assessment is the dynamic nature of learning, which results in brain neuroplasticity changes, where the brain’s neural organization changes during the development of a motor cognition skill through practice [5]. Learning technical surgical skills matures through 3 stages. First, the “cognitive” stage as one initially learns the skill and–thoughtfully- performs it. With practice, trainees become less thoughtful about the steps and reach “the associative stage” and can operate with fewer disruptions. The final stage is the “autonomous” stage, where the trainee can perform automatically without putting much thought, meanwhile paying more attention towards other aspects of surgery [1, 6, 7].
Several key brain functional features have been proposed for evaluating surgical skills proficiency, including kinematics and motor control. Hand movement kinematics has been the main source of information in various motor skill assessment studies [8–12]. Completion time, distance by hand, total distance traveled, speed, curvature, and relative phase of two hands trajectories as well as tool-based metrics of tool out of view, tools collision, tissue damage, camera control, number of critical errors, and clutch usage have been proposed for RAS skill evaluation [13–15]. However, tool-based metrics evaluate surgical skills based on static measurements and do not represent any dynamic effect and are not able to assess the associated change in brain functioning and brain neuroplasticity. Still, there is controversy about the optimal method for cognitive analysis during acquisition of RAS skills [6, 16–19]. Recording brain neural oscillations of surgeons while interacting with robots, utilizing electroencephalogram (EEG) neurofeedback, has been proposed to analyze functional dynamics of the brain of surgeons working in laparoscopy compared to those working in RAS [19]. EEG cognitive metrics, such as mental workload and level of engagement, have been used to analyze brain activity improvement during RAS learning [11, 19, 20]. However, skill-specific structured feedback to trainees could not be provided. It has been shown that modular training, defined as deconstruction of a procedure into smaller modules, can transfer technically challenging surgical skills in a step-wise fashion under appropriate supervision. Also, a structured feedback would be more constructive, as it allows tailoring of training activities and focusing on individual steps [21, 22].
High performance level in multifaceted tasks depends upon several factors including the learner’s ability to develop perceptual, cognitive, and motor skills [5]. The human brain is a complex system that includes various subsystems which interact with each other and dynamically change over different temporal scales while interacting with changes in the environment. It has been shown that during motor-cognition skill acquisition, there is a change of functional connectivity throughout areas of the brain [23, 24].
In this study, we hypothesized that the brain functional states reconfiguration during RAS skill acquisition can be informative toward monitoring surgeon’s performance progress during training. We analyzed the features extracted from brain functional network during RAS skill acquisition. Additionally, changes of brain states were quantified using dynamic network analysis to develop metrics for evaluation of performance in RAS. The main goal was to investigate dynamic changes of the brain functional states, and to find whether these changes are associated with performance improvement and practice time.
EEG has been previously used in different applications including mental workload measurement [25] gesture classification in computer aided design areas [26], and stress evaluation [18]. Here, EEG data were used to extract the brain functional network of RAS surgeon involved in different surgical tasks.
Results
Architecture of learning
We first sought to address the question: “Are there sets of RAS surgeon’s brain areas that preferentially interact with one another during RAS tasks practice and learning?” To answer this question, we examined the behavior of a module allegiance matrix (MAM) throughout learning (sessions). Values of elements of this matrix (MAMij) indicate the probability that two areas (channels i and j) be assigned to the same community, in the set of functional brain networks constructed from all subjects and recordings. MAM matrices during six sessions and different frequency bands were represented in Fig 1 (Data available in S1 File). These architectures display that channels in motor area (Frontal and Central channels [27]) consistently were assigned to the same community (recruited). The same thing happened to channels in cognitive (Prefrontal and Parietal channels [27]) and visual areas (Occipital channels [27]). This may suggest that areas relevant to task execution (motor and visual) and cognitive control (cognitive) are recruited consistently while other areas might only be transiently recruited.
Dynamic architecture of learning
We then sought to address the question: “Does functional contribution of these consistent communities or their interactions change with practice during RAS learning process?” To answer this question, we used Integration and recruitment quantities (see Methods). All subjects started study without any RAS or human-robot interaction experience. During the study, we considered practice time (seconds) from session to session as practice time or experience of subject for the next session. It should be noted that practice time may not be the only effective variable here, as gap between practice sessions may also affect learning.
We used correlation analysis to be able to detect significant effects of practice time on integration and recruitment quantities and also investigate relationship between these quantities and performance level. Results of this investigation were represented in Table 1 (Data available in S2 File).
Table 1. Relationship between integration and recruitment through brain areas at different frequency ranges, and average performance level and practice time for recordings used for extraction of each module allegiance matrix through subjects and sessions.
Recruitment (P-value) | Integration (P-value) | Frequency | |||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Motor | Cognitive | Visual | Motor-Visual | Motor-Cognitive | Visual-Cognitive | ||
Performance Level | 0.56(0.24) | 0.32(0.52) | -0.48(0.32) | 0.61(0.19) | -0.58(0.22) | -0.66(0.15) | θ |
-0.17(0.73) | -0.36(0.48) | -0.31(0.53) | 0.28(0.57) | -0.47(0.34) | -0.66(0.15) | α | |
0.18(0.73) | 0.12(0.81) | -0.43(0.38) | 0.57(0.22) | -0.84(0.03) | -0.66(0.15) | β | |
0.28(0.59) | 0.53(0.27) | -0.16(0.76) | 0.53(0.27) | -0.56(0.24) | -0.66(0.15) | γ | |
Practice time (second) | -0.17(0.74) | -0.20(0.70) | -0.72(0.10) | -0.79(0.05) | -0.90(0.01) | -0.89(0.01) | θ |
-0.72(0.10) | -0.22(0.68) | -0.41(0.42) | 0.79(0.06) | -0.77(0.06) | -0.89(0.01) | α | |
-0.45 (0.36) | -0.55 (0.25) | -0.73(0.09) | 0.59(0.21) | -0.68(0.13) | -0.89(0.01) | β | |
-0.30(0.55) | 0.34(0.50) | -0.46(0.35) | 0.85(0.03) | -0.81(0.05) | -0.89(0.01) | γ |
Significant correlation between performance level and motor-cognitive integration during the β frequency band may indicate at higher performance levels (expertise improvement) tendency of channels in motor and cognitive modules to be integrated together decreases. It may suggest that autonomy of motor and cognitive modules increases to process information independently from each other, during β frequency band, by performance improvement. Beta waves are characteristics of a strongly engaged mind, able to perform complex mental processing [28]. Hence, improvement of motor and cognitive autonomy at βfrequency band (while brain is strongly engaged), by performance, may be an informative metric for performance evaluation.
Significant correlation between practice time and motor-cognitive integration during θ frequency band (unconscious band [28]), and motor-visual integration during γ frequency band (crucial frequency range for self-awareness and insight, cognition, and coordinating simultaneous processing throughout brain areas [28]), and visual-cognitive integration during all frequency bands indicates that autonomy of these modules increases by practice. These features may also be a good measurement for performance evaluation.
Dynamic architecture after long gap in learning
We also sought to address the question: “Are the change in brain functioning permanent or does it deteriorate again?” Since our recording sessions were not regular and were limited to 6 sessions and also gap between practices was not equal for all sessions, to answer this question, we found the correlation between practice gap and integration and recruitment, represented in Table 2 (Data available in S2 File).
Table 2. Relationship between integration and recruitment of brain areas and average practice gap for recordings which were used for ‘module allegiance matrix’ extraction.
Recruitment (P-value) | Integration (P-value) | Frequency | |||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Motor | Cognitive | Visual | Motor-Visual | Motor-Cognitive | Visual-Cognitive | ||
Practice Gap | -0.53(0.28) | -0.16 (0.78) | -0.43(0.39) | 0.59(0.20) | -0.76(0.07) | -0.72(0.10) | θ |
-0.81(0.04) | 0.39 (0.43) | -0.07(0.89) | 0.75(0.08) | -0.65(0.15) | -0.72(0.10) | α | |
-0.87 (0.02) | -0.65 (0.15) | -0.85(0.02) | 0.2(0.70) | -0.39(0.44) | -0.72(0.10) | β | |
-0.78(0.06) | 0.03(0.94) | -0.06(0.90) | 0.76(0.07) | -0.44(0.38) | -0.72(0.10) | γ |
This result showed that a higher practice gap was associated with lower recruitment of motor (α and β), and visual (β) modules. This results in longer learning process. Although architecture of RAS learning seems to be permanent (Fig 1), dynamic architecture of the brain during learning was not permanent and it was affected by gap in practice. However, it should be noted that more recording sessions both before gap and after gap are needed to be able to answer this question more accurately, and to quantify the rate of deterioration and recovery.
Change of RAS trainee’s brain functional network and cognitive features by task outcome (FSRS metrics) improvement throgh individual subjects and tasks
After investigating the dynamic architecture of learning, we also sought to address the question: “How does cognitive and functional connectivity features change by performance improvement during RAS skill acquisition?” To answer this question, we calculated the correlations between Fundamental Skills of Robotic Surgery (FSRS)metrics and cognitive and functional connectivity features through all subjects and recordings (Table 3; Data available in S3 File).
Table 3. Relationship between FSRS metrics, CT, and extracted EEG features (cognition and network) at different frequency bands.
Clutch Usage (P-value) |
Left Tool Grasp (P-value) |
Left-Tool out of view (P-value) |
# of-Errors (P-value) | Right-Tool Grasp (P-value) | Right-Tool Out-of-View (P-value) |
Tissue Damage (P-value) |
Tool-Tool Collision (P-value) |
|
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
CT |
0.54 (3.35x10-21) |
0.41 (5.03x10-12) |
0.32 (1.76x10-7) |
0.39 (2.7x10-11) |
0.47 (7.79x10-16) |
0.47 (9.8x10-16) |
0.41 (3.72x10-12) |
0.06 (0.03) |
Distraction | 0.16 (0.01) |
-0.02 (0.79) |
0.07 (0.28) |
0.01 (0.91) |
-0.03 (0.65) |
0.04 (0.48) |
0.07 (0.27) |
0.03 (0.65) |
LE | 0.11 (0.07) |
0.11 (0.07) |
0.13 (0.04) |
0.02 (0.73) |
0.15 (0.01) |
0.11 (0.06) |
0.08 (0.22) |
-0.02 (0.76) |
HE | -0.10 (0.11) |
-0.10 (0.13) |
-0.12 (0.05) |
-0.03 (0.66) |
-0.12 (0.04) |
-0.08 (0.20) |
-0.08 (0.17) |
0.02 (0.74) |
MW | -0.15 (0.01) |
0.003 (0.95) |
-0.09 (0.17) |
-0.10 (0.12) |
-0.14 (0.02) |
-0.07 (0.28) |
-0.10 (0.13) |
-0.08 (0.21) |
APA | -0.13 (0.03) |
-0.10 (0.1) |
-0.07 (0.26) |
-0.03 (0.64) |
-0.17 (0.007) |
-0.11 (0.07) |
-0.02 (0.80) |
0.02 (0.78) |
AI | -0.01 (0.92) |
-0.01 (0.92) |
0.01 (0.82) |
-0.20 (0.00) |
-0.04 (0.49) |
0.05 (0.38) |
-0.05 (0.38) |
-0.01 (0.84) |
Strength θ | -0.18 (0.003) |
-0.08 (0.21) |
-0.11 (0.09) |
-0.04 (0.51) |
-0.18 (0.003) |
-0.13 (0.03) |
-0.11 (0.07) |
0.00 (0.97) |
Strength α | -0.19 (0.001) |
-0.11 (0.08) |
-0.13 (0.03) |
-0.07 (0.26) |
-0.22 (4x10-4) |
-0.14 (0.02) |
-0.13 (0.03) |
-0.02 (0.75) |
Strength β |
-0.29 (2.5x10-6) |
-0.15 (0.01) |
-0.18 (0.004) |
-0.15 (0.02) |
-0.25 (4.9x10-5) |
-0.23 (0.002) |
-0.14 (0.02) |
-0.03 (0.60) |
Strength γ |
-0.33 (3.6x10-8) |
-0.14 (0.02) |
-0.17 (0.007) |
-0.15 (0.01) |
-0.24 (1x10-4) |
-0.22 (3x10-4) |
-0.11 (0.07) |
-0.04 (0.55) |
Communication θ | -0.18 (0.003) |
-0.10 (0.10) |
-0.10 (0.10) |
-0.02 (0.71) |
-0.18 (4x10-3) |
-0.13 (0.03) |
-0.10 (0.1) |
0.03 (0.59) |
Communication α | -0.18 (0.003) |
-0.11 (0.07) |
-0.11 (0.08) |
-0.04 (0.51) |
-0.19 (1x10-3) |
-0.13 (0.04) |
-0.10 (0.09) |
0.03 (0.60) |
Communication β |
-0.24 (8.2x10-5) |
-0.14 (0.02) |
-0.13 (0.04) |
-0.12 (0.05) |
-0.22 (3x10-4) |
-0.18 (0.003) |
-0.11 (0.09) |
0.00 (0.94) |
Communication γ |
-0.29 (2.7x10-6) |
-0.14 (0.02) |
-0.12 (0.05) |
-0.16 (0.01) |
-0.24 (1x10-4) |
-0.18 (0.003) |
-0.11 (0.08) |
-0.01 (0.88) |
Positive correlation between CT and most FSRS metrics is in agreement with results in the literature [20] because higher FSRS scores are associated with lower performance level and higher CT indicates lower expertise level and performance, too. Also, we detected significant negative correlations between network features (strength and communication) and FSRS metrics, which may suggest strength and communication as informative features for performance evaluation during RAS learning.
Relationship between difficulty level, complexity level, practice time, and cognitive and brain network features and FSRS metrics
Although all designed tasks were considered for RAS learning, the complexity level (evaluated by expert surgeons through FSRS curriculum) of tasks were different. Also, difficulty level (evaluated by subjects not necessarily expert) calculated by using NASA-TLX scores showed that subjects evaluate tasks very differently. This likely stems from their ability (e.g. intelligence) as subjects are different. We then sought to address the question: “Do difficulty level and complexity level affect the outcome of subjects while performing tasks?” To answer this question, we calculated the correlation between these quantities and FSRS metrics (Table 4; Data available in S3 File). Results showed that performance level decreased (FSRS metrics increased) by increase of difficulty level. Our previous studies showed that NASA-TLX scores given by trainee are not reliable for expertise level assessment as we could not find any significant correlation between individual NASA-TLX metrics and expertise level (assessed by expert RAS surgeons) [20, 29], maybe because trainee doesn’t have enough proficiency to consider all necessary factors in evaluation [6, 20, 29].
Table 4. Correlations between task difficulty level, complexity level, and FSRS features at different frequency bands.
Difficulty level (P-value) |
Complexity level (P-value) |
|
---|---|---|
Clutch usage | 0.20 (1x10-3) | 0.003 (0.96) |
Left Tool Grasp | 0.34 (1.03x10-8) | -0.005 (0.93) |
Left-Tool out of view | 0.19 (2x10-3) | 0.02 (0.74) |
# of Errors | 0.38 (1.31x10-10) | 0.43 (4.47x10-13) |
Right-Tool Grasp | 0.33 (5.74x10-8) | 0.09 (0.14) |
Right-Tool Out-of-View | 0.27 (6.74x10-6) | 0.05 (0.41) |
Tissue Damage | 0.22 (3x10-4) | 0.14 (0.02) |
Tool-Tool Collision | 0.04 (0.49) | -0.08 (0.17) |
The results in Table 4 may suggest that although scores given to individual NASA-TLX are not reliable for expertise level assessment [6, 20, 29], difficulty level feature (calculated by using some of these metrics) can be an informative feature toward performance evaluation during RAS skill acquisition.
Discussion
In this study we addressed the hypothesis that continuous practice during human-robot interaction (RAS skill acquisition) results in reconfiguration of brain functional states. We recorded EEG data from 27 subjects when performing four RAS surgical tasks, selected from FSRS curriculum focusing on human-machine interface skills and one advanced RAS task, on a simulator over one year. We used network analysis algorithms to extract functional states and investigate the change of RAS trainees’ brain network dynamics at different frequency ranges. We found that motor, cognitive, and visual areas formed three separate, functionally cohesive modules whose recruitment did not change by practice but whose integration changed at different frequency bands as performance became more automatic.
Since learning RAS skills is very complicated and improvement of performance and rate of learning depends on several factors including subject intelligence (we didn’t include this factor in our study), higher practice time may not be necessarily associated with higher performance level. Therefore, we investigated change of integration between motor, cognitive, and visual modules by performance improvement and practice time, separately. We observed that motor-cognitive intergration at βband decreased by performance improvement, while motor-visual and also cognitive-visual modules did not display significant changes by performance improvement. This result may introduce the motor-cognitive integration level at βfrequency band as informative feature to objectify RAS performance evaluation and also to determine the rate of learning RAS skills for individual subjects (trainees).
However, integration between all three modules showed significant changes, at different frequency bands, by practice. These results may present autonomy improvement of motor task executive centers (motor and visual) and cognitive control center (cognitive module) by practice, to process information more independently and more efficiently. Also, integration between motor-cognitive (θ), motor-visual (γ), and cognitive-visual (all frequencies) can be proposed as informative features to estimate practice time each individual trainee needs to learn RAS skills.
We also investigated whether long gap in practice would affect brain network dynamic architecture. Results showed that dynamic architecture of brain functioning may not be permanent and be affected by factors such as practice gap. Decrease of motor module recruitment (at α and β) and visual module recruitment (at β) with practice gap may point out that for a shorter RAS learning curve, trainees need to practice regularly and continuously. Hence, the deterioration of brain functioning changes as well as individual recovery rate should be considered in RAS learning curve. Since recordings in this study were limited to 6 sessions, we were not able to investigate the deterioration and recovery rate for individual subjects and also the effective factors on these variables. However, it seems change of integration and recruitment of motor and visual modules can provide useful information in this regard.
In addition to RAS learning architecture and its dynamic, we also explored change of brain functional network and cognitive features by outcome improvements, and the relationship between difficulty level, task complexity level, practice time, and cognitive and brain network features as well as FSRS metrics. Although we didn’t observe any significant relationship between cognitive features and FSRS metrics, detected significant correlation between strength and communication features and several FSRS metrics may suggest strength and communication as informative features to evaluate RAS outcome performance level. Results also showed that higher task complexity level is associated with higher number of erros, indicating lower performance level. Also, we found difficulty level was positively correlated with clutch usage, left tool grasp, number of errors, right tool grasp, right tool out of view, and tissue damage. This result indicates that higher difficulty level is associated with lower performance level. Results retrieved from correlation analysis showed that both task complexity level and difficulty level are effective variables on surgical outcome and performance level.
Our results highlighted several important opportunities to study RAS skill acquisition, cognitive neuroscience of learning, rehabilitation, and human-robot interaction challenges. Existing methods of objective RAS skill evaluation, in brain functioning analysis framework, are limited to cognitive features, extracted by using power spectral density analyses, which are unable to accurately depict detailed dynamic changes of brain functioning during learning. Use of brain functional network provides access to neurophysiological processes that open new insights into understanding learning and skill acquisition.
Methods
Experimental set up and data recording
We designed an experimental set up, where Electroencephalography (EEG) data from 27 subjects was recorded during six sessions throughout one year of practice, performing five RAS tasks (Fig 2). The study was conducted in accordance with relevant guidelines and regulations, and were approved by Roswell Park Cancer Institute Institutional Review Board (IRB: I-241913). Each subject provided written informed consent before participating.
Subjects
Twenty-seven medical students without any experience in robotic surgery and human-robot interaction. Participant characteristics were summarized in Table 5.
Table 5. Demographics.
Participant characteristics | Characteristics options | Number of participants |
---|---|---|
Age, years | <30 | 12 |
30–45 | 15 | |
Dominant Hand | Right | 25 |
Left | 2 | |
Gender | Male | 17 |
Female | 10 | |
Simulator Experience (and gaming) | No experience | 27 |
Surgical tasks
Simulation based robotic curriculum—fundamental skills of robotic surgery (FSRS)—is a virtual reality-based curriculum that contains 4 modules (orientation, motor skills, basic surgical skills, and intermediate surgical skills) and 16 tasks [30]. Five surgical tasks, selected from FSRS curriculum focusing on human-machine interface skills, were included in this study:
Instrument Control Task (Fig 2A). This is task one of module one of FSRS and is designed for subject knowledge improvement. During this task subjects learn how to move the arms, which helps orient the user to the feel of the Robotic Surgery Simulator (RoSS) console [31].
Ball Placement Task (Fig 2B). This task involves subtasks 5 and 6 of module 2 of FSRS curriculum and is designed to teach psychomotor skills. The trainee is presented with five balls and five columns. Subjects pick up the balls from the tray using the robot simulator tools and place them on top of the columns. The task is completed when a ball is placed on top of every column [31]. Competence of the subjects for this task is evaluated using measures like the time taken to complete the task (Completion Time—CT), the number of times tool-tool collision occurs, and the number of times the camera is used and the clutch pedal is pressed [31].
Spatial Control II Task (threading string through a series of hoops) (Fig 2C). This task includes subtasks 7 and 8 of module 2 of FSRS curriculum and is designed to teach psychomotor skills. The subject passes a thread through a series of rings to hone his/her spatial awareness, instrument control and fine motor skills.
Fourth Arm Tissue Retraction (Fig 2D). This task is third task of module one of FSRS curriculum and is in the level of intermediate surgical skills [31]. This task combines the trainee’s previously acquired skills and requires coordinated control of the 4th arm to retract tissue [31].
Hands-on Surgical Training (HoST) Anastomosis (Fig 2E). This task is designed to teach advanced RAS skills, and is completely different from the other four tasks. In HoST, a simulator has been developed using the hand movements of a master surgeon, and the trainee’s hands follow the master’s hand motion. It feels like a master surgeon holds the hands of the trainee and accompanies him/her throughout the performance. When the trainee’s hand movement is not correct, the program pushes his/her hands to the correct path.
FSRS scores were not available for cases that trainee could not finish task during maximum allowed time. In these cases, RoSS system automatically closed the task without reporting the FSRS scores (260 recordings out of 524). Number of recordings for each task and session were reported in Table 6.
Table 6. Number of total recordings and number of recordings with FSRS scores, while 27 RAS trainees performed 5 tasks during 6 sessions with various number of repeatitions.
Task | Session 1 | Session 2 | Session 3 | Session 4 | Session 5 | Session 6 | ||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Total cases | With FSRS | Total cases | With FSRS | Total cases | With FSRS | Total cases | With FSRS | Total cases | With FSRS | Total cases | With FSRS | |
1 | 21 | 15 | 22 | 13 | 22 | 14 | 17 | 16 | 13 | 10 | 7 | 7 |
2 | 24 | 17 | 22 | 12 | 22 | 12 | 17 | 14 | 13 | 10 | 7 | 7 |
3 | 32 | 8 | 27 | 11 | 23 | 9 | 17 | 11 | 12 | 7 | 7 | 3 |
4 | 22 | 17 | 20 | 11 | 15 | 13 | 17 | 13 | 13 | 9 | 7 | 5 |
5 | 122 | 0 | 118 | 0 | 94 | 0 | 85 | 0 | 61 | 0 | 35 | 0 |
Data
Twenty-seven subjects started learning robot-assisted surgery from the pre-novice level, and during one year of practice they learnt required skills to do the basic tasks of RAS on the simulator. EEG data recording was carried out at the initial session and followed up after one week, one month, three months, six months, and one year.
EEG data was recorded by placing the channels sensors of a 20-channel EEG headset on the Frontal (F; cognition and action; F3, Fz, F4, F7, F8 electrode channels), Prefrontal (PF; cognition; Fp1, Fp2 electrode channels), Central (C; action; C3, Cz, C4 electrode channels), Temporal (T; perception; T3, T4, T5, T6 electrode channels), Parietal (Pa; cognition; P3, Pz, P4, Poz electrode channels), and Occipital (O; perception; O1, O2 electrode channels) cortices (Fig 2). All surgical tasks were done on RoSS system.
EEG data preprocessing
The EEG data from the channels was filtered with a band-pass filter (0.5–128 Hz). Eye blinks, muscle activity, and environmental effects were considered as artifacts. Environmental artifacts were removed by applying a 60 Hz notch filter to EEG data [32, 33]. Muscle activity and eye movement were detected using wavelet transform and discriminant function analyses (DFA) as proposed by Berka et. al [33]. Linear discriminant function analysis was applied to raw data to decontaminate it from eye blinks. Wavelet coefficients from FzPOz and CzPOz channels were used to classify each signal data points into eye-blink or non-eye-blink categories [32]. The eye-blink data points were removed from signal.
Brain functional network and community detection
A continuous wavelet transform with the ‘morlet’ wavelet method was used to extract the spontaneous phase of each channel signal for all recordings. The calculated phases of pair signals were used to find phase synchronized time points. These points were used to extract phase synchronization index for pairs of signals [34]. Calculating the phase difference (Δφxy(t)) between two signals x(t) and y(t) by the following equation, the phase synchronized point was defined as time point (t) in which [35].
Transferring the range of phase into boundary of φx ∈ [−π, π], the phase difference for all pairs of channels was normalized by using the range of phase difference ( and ) [35]. The synchronization index Γxy(FB) was calculated through frequency band (FB) using [35, 36]
where, Ps is the number of data-points in the time series. The calculated synchronization index for all pairs of channels forms the symmetric weighted adjacency matrix (Γ) [35]. The single-layer community detection algorithm was used to extract functional communities of recordings for all subjects through learning sessions. Communities were extracted through (4–60 Hz) with frequency bands of θ(4-8Hz), α(8-12Hz), β(12-35Hz), and γ(35-60Hz).
Single-layer community detection algorithm was used to detect communities of each adjacency matrix, which we defined these communities as functional states.
Single- layer community detection
We partitioned each adjacency matrix into communities (functional states) using modularity maximization criteria [37]. Modularity index (Q), as a quality function, was defined as [37].
where, channel ‘i’ is assigned to cluster gi, channel ‘j’ is assigned to cluster gj, and Mij is the expected weight of the link connecting channels i and j. The coefficient γ>0 is the resolution parameter in the multiscale modularity-maximization algorithm. Maximization of Q is equivalent to partitioning the matrix into communities such that the total link weight inside the modules is as large as possible. The generalized Louvain like “greedy” algorithm and ‘Newman-Girvan (NG)’ null network were used for modularity maximization and community detection [37, 38]. Elements of matrix M are defined as for vertices i and j, where m is defined as and is the degree of the vertices. Expression is called the modularity matrix.
To find the optimum consistent modularity and resolution parameter for each adjacency matrix Γ, a range of γ was considered and ‘Q’ was calculated at each γ (using generalized Louvain algorithm).
Finally, the resolution limit that resulted in maximum modularity and acceptable number of communities (≤ 6 in this study) was selected as the optimum resolution limit for each matrix (Fig 3). We selected the number of communities ≤ 6 because the network in this study was small with 20 nodes and we considered 6 brain predefined cognitive systems. Considering more than 6 communities resulted in several singular communities, which do not convey meaningful information toward our purpose.
Module-allegiance matrix, brain functional network measurements
Using the functional community each channel was assigned to, the Module Allegiance Matrix (MAM) was derived. Element (i,j) in the MAM represents the probability that nodes ‘i’ and ‘j’ belong to the same community (Fig 4).
Comparison between module allegiance matrix and predefined brain structural system was used to extract brain state reconfiguration measurements. Considering the structural connectivity of considered channels (represented in Fig 5), the integration and recruitment coefficients were calculated.
Features
Different categories of features were measured (NASA-TLX metrics and FSRS metrics) and calculated (cognitive and functional network features) to find the relationship between subjective assessment metrics (NASA-TLX), real outcome measures (FSRS metrics and completion time (CT)) and analytical features retrieved from brain activity (measured by EEG).
Subjective metrics
The trainee evaluated each procedure by giving scores to NASA-TLX indices, with a scale of 1 to 20. This subjective skill evaluation survey has six indices: Mental Demand, Physical Demand, Temporal Demand, Performance, Effort, and Frustration [39, 40].
Mental Demand (MD): Evaluates the level of mental/perceptual activity demanded to complete the task.
Physical Demand(PD): Level of physical activity required to complete the task.
Temporal Demand(TD): Level of time pressure the subject feels during completing the task.
Performance Score (PS): Quality level of outcome and the level of satisfaction of doing the task (1 means highest quality and 20 means lowest quality).
Effort (E): Evaluates how hard (mentally and physically) should the subject work to complete the task.
Frustration: Level of negative (compared to positive) psychological emotions the subject feels while completing the task.
Task difficulty: We used scores given to these metrics to extract task difficulty (D) level as
We did not use performance scores given by trainee because trainee does not have enough expertise to be able to evaluate outcome of performed task.
Task complexity: FSRS curriculum modules were used to determine the complexity level of tasks in Table 7, with 1 being simplest and 5 most complicated task.
Table 7. Description of tasks designed in this study, and the reason for the associated skill necessity in RAS.
Task name | Instument control | Ball placement | Spatial control II | Fourth arm tissue dissection | Hands-on surgical training |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Task ID | (1) | (2) | (3) | (4) | (5) |
Complexity level | 2 | 4 | 5 | 3 | 1 |
Skill | orientation and control of position | Hand-eye-tool coordination & depth perception & foot control | Hand-eye-tool coordination & depth perception & foot control | orientation and control of position | Motor skills |
Reason | Lack of tactile feedback in RAS requires development of this skill | Remoteness and lack of tactile feedback requires development of this skill | Remoteness and lack of tactile feedback requires development of this skill | Lack of tactile feedback in RAS requires development of this skill | Learn how to have fine hand movements |
FSRS metrics
RoSS scores given by da Vinci simulator technology were used in this study for outcome evaluation. These metrics include: clutch usage, left tool out of view, number of errors, right tool out of view, tissue damage, and tool-tool collision. These metrics were used in correlation analysis, to investigate the relationship between brain state measurements and RoSS scores. These scores were also used to estimate performance level of doing each task by subjects. Performance level was calculated as the average of scores given to all 8 FSRS metrics as
Completion time
By synchronizing the recorded EEG and the associated video of the task, completion time was defined as the total time a trainee was performing a task. The completion time can be defined using the number of total data points in signal (N) and the data recording sampling frequency (fs) as
Cognitive features- power spectral density
Aiming period activity: Brain activity during short and long term decision making affects the performance while operating a procedure [41]. The whole of hand movement is planned and figured out during long-term decision making, which happens during the Aiming Period (AP). This period has revealed important effects on performance and expertise level in different tasks [41–43]. The brain activity during this period is Aiming Period Activity (APA), which is the power reduction in spatially enhanced α(8–12 Hz) and β(13–30 Hz) bands during movement [43]. It has been shown that the APA depends on the expertise level of the subject [41].
Mental Workload: Mental workload (MW) is related to the engaged memory capacity while performing a procedure of interest [44]. The main assumption in MW interpretation is that each person has a relatively fixed cognitive capacity [32]. Commonly, MW refers to the portion of a person’s total mental capacity which is loaded [45]. Mental workload was estimated using the PSD of signals at the C3-C4, Cz-POz, F3-Cz, F3-C4, Fz-C3, and Fz-POz channels [32].
To calculate MW, we used the framework developed by the B-Alert EEG series from Advanced Brain Monitoring (ABM) company, which has been frequently validated in different studies [32, 33, 46]. Briefly, this framework calculates a baseline value of the absolute and relative power spectral variables from the C3-C4, Cz-PO, F3-Cz, Fz-C3, and Fz-PO channels during mental arithmetic, grid location, and digit-span baseline tasks. These baselines have been recorded from 80 healthy subjects, and are available from ABM software. A two-class quadratic logistic discriminant function analysis (DFA) [32] was used to extract the probability of presenting a high mental workload. The quadratic logistic DFA was established once for one mentor based on baseline data collected before surgeries.
Engagement
Engagement reflects the spatial recruitment of the brain regions in processing tasks associated with decision making. These tasks include, but are not limited to, information gathering, visual scanning, audio processing, and attention concentration on one aspect of the environment while ignoring other distractions [32, 33]. As with the calculation of MW, we used the framework developed by the B-Alert EEG series from Advanced Brain Monitoring (ABM) company. However, in this case, the baselines were drawn from 5 minutes of three different tasks (3-choice vigilance task, eyes open, and eyes closed).These baselines were recorded from one mentor at the beginning of the whole research study. Here, the absolute and relative PSD values of the Fz-POz and Cz-POz channels were used in a four-class quadratic logistic discriminant function analysis (DFA) which returned an estimation of the engagement level [32, 33]. The range of this estimation for 1 second epoch is between 0–1 with 0.1 (sleep onset), 0.3 (distraction), 0.6 (low engagement; LE), and 0.9 (high engagement; HE).
Asymmetry index
Negative feelings such as surprise, frustration, fear, and concern have opposing effects on the activity of the right and left lobes of the frontal cortex [47]. For each recording, Asymmetry Index (AI) was defined as the difference between the power spectral density decrease in the left and right frontal lobes in the alpha frequency band, normalized as [47, 48]
The AI was calculated as the average value over the following pairs of electrodes: (F3 and F4), and (F7 and F8). During negative feelings, the right frontal lobe shows more intense activity (associated with lowerα power [48]) compared to the left lobe [48] (α power is inversely related to activation [47]).
Brain functional network features
Strength. The strength of a cognitive system was defined as the average functional connectivity of electrodes within the system. Adjacency matrices were used to calculate this feature.
Communication. Communication, C, between two cognitive systems k1 and k2 can be defined as the average functional connectivity in electrode pairs where one electrode lies within the first system and the second electrode lies within the second system [49]. Adjacency matrices (Γ) were used to calculate this feature as
where, |Sk| is the number of nodes in the cognitive system k, k = 1…6, and k1 ≠ k2. Note that the strength of each cognitive system can be calculated by letting k1 = k2.
Integration coefficient. Integration of each area measures the average probability that this area is in the same network community as areas from other systems. Module Allegiance Matrices were used to calculate integration feature.
Recruitment coefficient. The recruitment coefficient for each area of the network corresponds to the average probability that this area is in the same network community as other areas from its own system. Module Allegiance Matrices were used to calculate recruitment feature.
All features used in this study were summarized in Table 8.
Table 8. Summary of all features used in this study.
Feature | Description | Category | Main extraction method |
---|---|---|---|
Difficulty level (D) | Extracted by using NASA-TLX features, evaluated by the trainee D = MD + PD + TD + E |
Subjective assessment | Subjective assessment |
Complexity level | Indicates the complexity of task based on assessment of expert surgeons considered in FSRS curriculum. This feature is independent from trainee performance and outcome and is based on the properties of the task. | FSRS curriculum | FSRS curriculum |
Practice time | All subjects started learning without any experience of work with simulator, gaming, and robotic surgery experience. However, during learning they practice on simulator and their experience increases from session to session. We measured practice time of each subject from recording to next recording and considered that (seconds of practice) as practice time. | Direct measurement | Direct measurement |
Practice gap | Gap between practice sessions | Direct measurement | Direct measurement |
Clutch usage | Indicates effectiveness and the level of skill in which the task is performed (economy of motion) [50] | Tool besed metric (FSRS) | Real measurement by RoSS |
Left tool out of view | Indicates awareness of operative environment in which tasks are Performed [50] | Tool besed metric (FSRS) | Real measurement by RoSS |
Right tool out of view | Indicates awareness of operative environment in which tasks are performed [50] | Tool besed metric (FSRS) | Real measurement by RoSS |
Number of errors | Indicates effectiveness and the level of skill in which the task is performed (economy of motion) [50] | Tool besed metric (FSRS) | Real measurement by RoSS |
Tissue damage | Collisions causing damage to the underlying tissue [31]. Indicates awareness of operative environment in which tasks are performed [50] |
Tool besed metric (FSRS) | Real measurement by RoSS |
Tool-Tool collision | Indicates awareness of operative environment in which tasks are performed [50] | Tool besed metric (FSRS) | Real measurement by RoSS |
Performance Level | Performance level was calculated as: |
Tool besed metric (FSRS) | Real measurement by RoSS |
Completion time (CT) | Indicates total time task is performed | Direct measurement | |
Aiming period activity (APA) | Indicates brain activity level during aiming period | Cognition | Power Spectral Density (PSD) analysis |
Mental Workload (MW) | Indicates the level of engaged memory capacity while performing task | Cognition | Power Spectral Density (PSD) analysis |
Engagement (E) | Reflects the spatial cooperation of the brain regions in processing tasks | Cognition | Power Spectral Density (PSD) analysis |
Strength | Indicates the level of total functional connectivity within channels in a specific cognitive system | Brain functional network | Pairwise phase synchronization |
Communication | Indicates the level of total functional connectivity between channels from different cognitive systems | Brain functional network | Pairwise phase synchronization |
Integration | Average probability that a brain area is in the same network community as areas from other cognitive systems | Dynamic architecture feature | Network community detection |
Recruitment | Average probability that a brain area is in the same network community as other areas from its own cognitive system | Dynamic architecture feature | Network community detection |
Conclusion
Current analysis frameworks are unable to describe necessary dynamic changes of brain areas to make performance of motor cognitive skills autonomous, because of statistical and mathematical limitations. In this study we used dynamic network neuroscience methods to investigate dynamic reconfiguration of brain modules during RAS skill learning, effective factors on dynamic architecture of learning, and address several questions related to RAS skill acquisition. Results also suggest integration, recruitment, strength, and communication features to be used for objective performance evaluation.
Supporting information
Acknowledgments
This work was funded by the Roswell Park Alliance Foundation.
Data Availability
All relevant data are within the paper and its Supporting Information files.
Funding Statement
Roswell Park Alliance Foundation. The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.
References
- 1.Lallée S, Hamann K, Steinwender J, Warneken F, Martienz U, Barron-Gonzales H, et al., editors. Cooperative human robot interaction systems: IV. Communication of shared plans with Naïve humans using gaze and speech. Intelligent Robots and Systems (IROS), 2013 IEEE/RSJ International Conference on; 2013: IEEE.
- 2.Shafiei SB, Doyle ST, Guru KA, editors. Mentor’s brain functional connectivity network during robotic assisted surgery mentorship. Engineering in Medicine and Biology Society (EMBC), 2016 IEEE 38th Annual International Conference of the; 2016: IEEE. [DOI] [PubMed]
- 3.Lallée S, Lemaignan S, Lenz A, Melhuish C, Natale L, Skachek S, et al., editors. Towards a platform-independent cooperative human-robot interaction system: I. perception. Intelligent Robots and Systems (IROS), 2010 IEEE/RSJ International Conference on; 2010: IEEE.
- 4.Lallée S, Pattacini U, Boucher JD, Lemaignan S, Lenz A, Melhuish C, et al., editors. Towards a platform-independent cooperative human-robot interaction system: Ii. perception, execution and imitation of goal directed actions. Intelligent Robots and Systems (IROS), 2011 IEEE/RSJ International Conference on; 2011: IEEE.
- 5.Williams AM, Hodges NJ. Skill acquisition in sport: Research, theory and practice: Routledge; 2004. [Google Scholar]
- 6.Shafiei SB, Hussein AA, Guru KA. Cognitive learning and its future in urology: surgical skills teaching and assessment. Current opinion in urology. 2017;27(4):342–7. 10.1097/MOU.0000000000000408 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Reznick RK, MacRae H. Teaching surgical skills—changes in the wind. New England Journal of Medicine. 2006;355(25):2664–9. 10.1056/NEJMra054785 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Shafiei SB, Cavuoto L, Guru KA, editors. Motor Skill Evaluation During Robot-Assisted Surgery. ASME 2017 International Design Engineering Technical Conferences and Computers and Information in Engineering Conference; 2017: American Society of Mechanical Engineers.
- 9.Nisky I, Hsieh MH, Okamura AM, editors. A framework for analysis of surgeon arm posture variability in robot-assisted surgery. Robotics and Automation (ICRA), 2013 IEEE International Conference on; 2013: IEEE.
- 10.Teleoperation AVDR. Uncontrolled Manifold Analysis of Arm Joint Angle Variability During Robotic Teleoperation and Freehand Movement of Surgeons and Novices. 2013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 11.Shafiei SB. Investigation of Brain Computer Interface as a New Modality in Human-Surgical Robot Interaction: State University of New York at Buffalo; 2018.
- 12.Shafiei SB, Guru KA, Esfahani ET, editors. Using two-third power law for segmentation of hand movement in robotic assisted surgery. ASME 2015 International Design Engineering Technical Conferences and Computers and Information in Engineering Conference; 2015: American Society of Mechanical Engineers.
- 13.Judkins TN, Oleynikov D, Stergiou N. Objective evaluation of expert and novice performance during robotic surgical training tasks. Surgical endoscopy. 2009;23(3):590 10.1007/s00464-008-9933-9 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Lin HC, Shafran I, Yuh D, Hager GD. Towards automatic skill evaluation: Detection and segmentation of robot-assisted surgical motions. Computer Aided Surgery. 2006;11(5):220–30. 10.3109/10929080600989189 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Verner L, Oleynikov D, Holtmann S, Haider H, Zhukov L. Measurements of the level of surgical expertise using flight path analysis from da Vinci robotic surgical system. Stud Health Technol Inform. 2003;94:373–8. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Shafiei S, Fiorica T, Hussein A, Ahmed Y, Muldoon S, Guru K. PD41-08 Skill acquisition and its retention after simulation-based practice during robot-assisted surgery: Can functional brain states help us forge forward? The Journal of Urology. 2017;197(4):e810. [Google Scholar]
- 17.Shafiei S, Hussein A, Kozlowski J, Ahmed Y, Muldoon S, Guru K. PD46-02 Looking for your own reflection: Assessing brain functional state of surgical mentor during robot-assisted surgery. The Journal of Urology. 2017;197(4):e890. [Google Scholar]
- 18.Shafiei S, Hussein A, Ahmed Y, Kozlowski J, Guru K. MP51-05 DOES TRAINEE PERFORMANCE IMPACT SURGEON’S STRESS DURING ROBOT-ASSISTED SURGERY? The Journal of Urology. 2017;197(4):e695. [Google Scholar]
- 19.Bocci T, Moretto C, Tognazzi S, Briscese L, Naraci M, Leocani L, et al. How does a surgeon’s brain buzz? An EEG coherence study on the interaction between humans and robot. Behavioral and Brain Functions. 2013;9(1):14. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Guru KA, Shafiei SB, Khan A, Hussein AA, Sharif M, Esfahani ET. Understanding cognitive performance during robot-assisted surgery. Urology. 2015;86(4):751–7. 10.1016/j.urology.2015.07.028 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Volpe A, Ahmed K, Dasgupta P, Ficarra V, Novara G, van der Poel H, et al. Pilot validation study of the European Association of Urology robotic training curriculum. European urology. 2015;68(2):292–9. 10.1016/j.eururo.2014.10.025 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Hussein AA, Ghani KR, Peabody J, Sarle R, Abaza R, Eun D, et al. Development and validation of an objective scoring tool for robot-assisted radical prostatectomy: prostatectomy assessment and competency evaluation. The Journal of urology. 2017;197(5):1237–44. 10.1016/j.juro.2016.11.100 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Sosnik R, Hauptmann B, Karni A, Flash T. When practice leads to co-articulation: the evolution of geometrically defined movement primitives. Experimental Brain Research. 2004;156(4):422–38. 10.1007/s00221-003-1799-4 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Schaal S, Ijspeert A, Billard A. Computational approaches to motor learning by imitation. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences. 2003;358(1431):537–47. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Aghajani H, Garbey M, Omurtag A. Measuring Mental Workload with EEG+ fNIRS. Frontiers in human neuroscience. 2017;11:359 10.3389/fnhum.2017.00359 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Shafiei SB, Esfahani ET, editors. Aligning Brain Activity and Sketch in Multi-Modal CAD Interface. ASME 2014 International Design Engineering Technical Conferences and Computers and Information in Engineering Conference; 2014: American Society of Mechanical Engineers.
- 27.Wu C, Liu Y. Queuing network modeling of driver workload and performance. IEEE Transactions on Intelligent Transportation Systems. 2007;8(3):528–37. [Google Scholar]
- 28.Jemmer P. Getting in a (brain-wave) state through entrainment, meditation and hypnosis. Hypnotherapy Journal. 2009;2:24–9. [Google Scholar]
- 29.Hussein AA, Shafiei SB, Sharif M, Esfahani E, Ahmad B, Kozlowski JD, et al. Technical mentorship during robot‐assisted surgery: a cognitive analysis. BJU international. 2016;118(3):429–36. 10.1111/bju.13445 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Stegemann AP, Ahmed K, Syed JR, Rehman S, Ghani K, Autorino R, et al. Fundamental skills of robotic surgery: a multi-institutional randomized controlled trial for validation of a simulation-based curriculum. Urology. 2013;81(4):767–74. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Raza SJ, Froghi S, Chowriappa A, Ahmed K, Field E, Stegemann AP, et al. Construct validation of the key components of Fundamental Skills of Robotic Surgery (FSRS) curriculum—a multi-institution prospective study. Journal of surgical education. 2014;71(3):316–24. 10.1016/j.jsurg.2013.10.006 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Berka C, Levendowski DJ, Lumicao MN, Yau A, Davis G, Zivkovic VT, et al. EEG correlates of task engagement and mental workload in vigilance, learning, and memory tasks. Aviation, space, and environmental medicine. 2007;78(5):B231–B44. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Berka C, Levendowski DJ, Cvetinovic MM, Petrovic MM, Davis G, Lumicao MN, et al. Real-time analysis of EEG indexes of alertness, cognition, and memory acquired with a wireless EEG headset. International Journal of Human-Computer Interaction. 2004;17(2):151–70. [Google Scholar]
- 34.Quiroga RQ, Panzeri S. Principles of neural coding: CRC Press; 2013. [Google Scholar]
- 35.Jamal W, Das S, Maharatna K, Pan I, Kuyucu D. Brain connectivity analysis from EEG signals using stable phase-synchronized states during face perception tasks. Physica A: Statistical Mechanics and its Applications. 2015;434:273–95. [Google Scholar]
- 36.Shafiei SB, Hussein AA, Guru KA. Relationship between Surgeon’s Brain Functional Network Reconfiguration and Performance Level During Robot-assisted Surgery. IEEE Access. 2018.
- 37.Bazzi M, Porter MA, Williams S, McDonald M, Fenn DJ, Howison SD. Community detection in temporal multilayer networks, with an application to correlation networks. Multiscale Modeling & Simulation. 2016;14(1):1–41. [Google Scholar]
- 38.Jutla IS, Jeub LG, Mucha PJ. A generalized Louvain method for community detection implemented in MATLAB. URL http://netwiki.amath.unc.edu/GenLouvain. 2011.
- 39.Hart SG, Staveland LE. Development of NASA-TLX (Task Load Index): Results of empirical and theoretical research Advances in psychology. 52: Elsevier; 1988. p. 139–83. [Google Scholar]
- 40.Rubio S, Díaz E, Martín J, Puente JM. Evaluation of subjective mental workload: A comparison of SWAT, NASA‐TLX, and workload profile methods. Applied Psychology. 2004;53(1):61–86. [Google Scholar]
- 41.Haufler AJ, Spalding TW, Santa Maria D, Hatfield BD. Neuro-cognitive activity during a self-paced visuospatial task: comparative EEG profiles in marksmen and novice shooters. Biological Psychology. 2000;53(2–3):131–60. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Salazar W, Landers DM, Petruzzello SJ, Han M, Crews DJ, Kubitz KA. Hemispheric asymmetry, cardiac response, and performance in elite archers. Research quarterly for exercise and sport. 1990;61(4):351–9. 10.1080/02701367.1990.10607499 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Babiloni C, Del Percio C, Iacoboni M, Infarinato F, Lizio R, Marzano N, et al. Golf putt outcomes are predicted by sensorimotor cerebral EEG rhythms. The Journal of Physiology. 2008;586(1):131–9. 10.1113/jphysiol.2007.141630 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Carswell CM, Clarke D, Seales WB. Assessing mental workload during laparoscopic surgery. Surgical innovation. 2005;12(1):80–90. 10.1177/155335060501200112 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Carswell CM, Lio CH, Grant R, Klein MI, Clarke D, Seales WB, et al. Hands-free administration of subjective workload scales: acceptability in a surgical training environment. Applied ergonomics. 2010;42(1):138–45. 10.1016/j.apergo.2010.06.003 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Stikic M, Johnson RR, Tan V, Berka C. EEG-based classification of positive and negative affective states. Brain-Computer Interfaces. 2014;1(2):99–112. [Google Scholar]
- 47.Tomarken AJ, Davidson RJ, Henriques JB. Resting frontal brain asymmetry predicts affective responses to films. Journal of personality and social psychology. 1990;59(4):791 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Kim M-K, Kim M, Oh E, Kim S-P. A review on the computational methods for emotional state estimation from the human EEG. Computational and mathematical methods in medicine. 2013;2013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Shafiei SB, Hussein AA, Muldoon SF, Guru KA. Functional Brain States Measure Mentor-Trainee Trust during Robot-Assisted Surgery. Scientific reports. 2018;8(1):3667 10.1038/s41598-018-22025-1 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Chowriappa AJ, Shi Y, Raza SJ, Ahmed K, Stegemann A, Wilding G, et al. Development and validation of a composite scoring system for robot-assisted surgical training—the Robotic Skills Assessment Score. journal of surgical research. 2013;185(2):561–9. 10.1016/j.jss.2013.06.054 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Supplementary Materials
Data Availability Statement
All relevant data are within the paper and its Supporting Information files.