Skip to main content
Wiley Open Access Collection logoLink to Wiley Open Access Collection
. 2018 Aug 1;78(11):1026–1048. doi: 10.1002/dneu.22626

Genomic mosaicism in the developing and adult brain

Suzanne Rohrback 1,2,3, Benjamin Siddoway 2, Christine S Liu 1,2, Jerold Chun 2,
PMCID: PMC6214721  NIHMSID: NIHMS975799  PMID: 30027562

ABSTRACT

Since the discovery of DNA, the normal developing and functioning brain has been assumed to be composed of cells with identical genomes, which remains the dominant view even today. However, this pervasive assumption is incorrect, as proven by increasing numbers of reports within the last 20 years that have identified multiple forms of somatically produced genomic mosaicism (GM), wherein brain cells—especially neurons—from a single individual show diverse alterations in DNA, distinct from the germline. Critically, these changes alter the actual DNA nucleotide sequences—in contrast to epigenetic mechanisms—and almost certainly contribute to the remarkably diverse phenotypes of single brain cells, including single‐cell transcriptomic profiles. Here, we review the history of GM within the normal brain, including its major forms, initiating mechanisms, and possible functions. GM forms include aneuploidies and aneusomies, smaller copy number variations (CNVs), long interspersed nuclear element type 1 (LINE1) repeat elements, and single nucleotide variations (SNVs), as well as DNA content variation (DCV) that reflects all forms of GM with greatest coverage of large, brain cell populations. In addition, technical considerations are examined, along with relationships among GM forms and multiple brain diseases. GM affecting genes and loci within the brain contrast with current neural discovery approaches that rely on sequencing nonbrain DNA (e.g., genome‐wide association studies (GWAS)). Increasing knowledge of neural GM has implications for mechanisms of development, diversity, and function, as well as understanding diseases, particularly considering the overwhelming prevalence of sporadic brain diseases that are unlinked to germline mutations. © 2018 The Authors. Developmental Neurobiology Published by Wiley Periodicals, Inc. Develop Neurobiol, 2018

Keywords: genomic mosaicism, somatic mutation, neurodegenerative disease, retrotransposition, reverse transcriptase, DNA damage, plasticity, memory, RNA, GWAS, sporadic

INTRODUCTION

The exquisite organization and complexity of cells within the brain have been recognized since the days of Golgi and Cajal (Cajal, 1901) at the turn of the 20th century, yet molecular mechanisms from which the brain develops and functions are still not completely understood. However, in the 1940s and 1950s, the discovery of DNA as the carrier of genes (Avery et al., 1944; McCarty, 1995, 2003) and its structural implications for genetics (Watson and Crick, 1953a, 1953b) led to a model in which cells of the brain—and all other organs in an individual—arise from a single, genetic blueprint within a constant, diploid genome of unchanging sequence. Each individual has thus been generally thought to be made up of genomically identical cells, arising from a genome first formed at fertilization as a zygote and extending through mitotic cell division to all cells of the body, including the cells of the brain (Do all our body cells have the same DNA? 2012). A corollary of this assumption is that the universe of normal activities of the brain—from development and functional organization to personality and consciousness—arise from a single, immutable genome.

A prominent exception to the view that all cells of an individual have identical genomes emerged in the 1960s from theoretical estimates of antibody diversity vs. available genetic information (Dreyer and Bennett, 1965; Dreyer et al., 1967) wherein estimates based upon genome size indicated that there was insufficient genomic information to encode for the recognized astronomical diversity of antibodies. This realization implicated somatic changes in DNA sequence—occurring post‐zygotically—to allow gene recombination to produce new, combinatorial coding sequences. This process is known today as V(D)J recombination (Hozumi and Tonegawa, 1976; Schatz and Baltimore, 1988; Jung et al., 2006), which affects both immunoglobulin and T cell receptor loci, and further forms of somatic events within adaptive immune cells occur through heavy chain class switch recombination (Xu et al., 2012) and somatic hypermutation (Odegard and Schatz, 2006). Together, these changes underlie the enormous repertoire of antigen receptors that define the mammalian adaptive immune system. One result of somatic recombination and hypermutation is production of immune cells that form a complex mosaic composed of cells having different DNA sequences. This process occurs somatically and is thus not passed on through the germline. The immune system of an individual therefore represents a first and clear example of genomic mosaicism (GM) involving normal cells of the body, with clear functional consequences through the generation of antigen receptors, and selection and survival of immune cells (Surh and Sprent, 1994; Shlomchik and Weisel, 2012; Stritesky et al., 2012).

An operational definition of GM includes the following features: (1) it occurs somatically and therefore does not affect germline DNA sequences, contrasting with “genetic” mutations that enter the germline (hence the term “genomic” rather than “genetic” mosaicism here); (2) it produces nucleotide sequence changes, as differentiated from epigenetics (Wolffe and Matzke, 1999) which does not, thus allowing cells in an individual to be distinguished by their DNA sequence, and (3) it encompasses all forms of DNA sequence changes that include gains, losses, substitutions, and rearrangements, as well as gene recombination. Importantly, GM can clearly have functional consequences, rather than being simply an epiphenomenon, as already noted for the immune system.

Could GM contribute to the vast cell diversity of form and function within the brain? Early speculations in the 1960s used a general analogy of nervous system complexity as revealed in a tissue section from goldfish tectum (Dreyer et al., 1967) and compared it to immunoglobulin diversity; these conjectures were intriguing but not based on experimental evidence. However, unlike successful identification, in subsequent decades, of somatic DNA rearrangements and mutations in the immune system (Hozumi and Tonegawa, 1976), no definitive (or even circumstantial) evidence of GM in the normal nervous system existed in the scientific literature through 1990. The identification of components of the V(D)J cleavage complex (Schatz et al., 1989), and expression within the brain of one of its components, the recombination activating gene 1 (RAG1) (Chun et al., 1991), rekindled interest in the possibility that GM, potentially produced by DNA recombination, could exist within the brain, albeit distinct from V(D)J recombination in that the latter additionally requires RAG2 expression (Chun et al., 1991). However, efforts to identify neural GM by designed recombination reporters within the brain (Matsuoka et al., 1991; Schatz and Chun, 1992) or candidate gene examinations that included olfactory receptors (Chun, 2004; Eggan et al., 2004; Li et al., 2004), protocadherins (Chun, 1999; Wu and Maniatis, 1999), and drosophila DSCAM (Hattori et al., 2008; Jin et al., 2013) did not support recombination or other directed mechanisms that could produce neural GM.

Commencing in the mid‐1990s, distinct approaches to GM characterization emerged from studies of programmed cell death during neurogenesis (Blaschke et al., 1996; Kuida et al., 1996; Voyvodic, 1996; Staley et al., 1997; Blaschke et al., 1998; Kuida et al., 1998; Pompeiano et al., 1998; Pompeiano et al., 2000; McConnell et al., 2009; Yung et al., 2009; Peterson et al., 2012). These approaches revealed extensive DNA fragmentation within single neurons that could precede cell death by days (Pompeiano et al., 1998), reminiscent of myriad apoptotic cells found in regions undergoing immunological gene recombination (Surh and Sprent, 1994; Chun, 2001). The intermixture of cells with varied levels of fragmented DNA amidst those without fragmentation, virtually all of which appeared normal by standard histological stains, supported tolerance of genomically distinct cells, as defined by levels of DNA fragmentation (Blaschke et al., 1996; Staley et al., 1997; Blaschke et al., 1998; Pompeiano et al., 2000, 1998) within a normal, developing brain, although having primary fates leading ultimately to cell death (Fig. 1) (Blaschke et al., 1996; Blaschke et al., 1998; Pompeiano et al., 2000). Interestingly, at least some of the DNA fragments were associated with apoptotic nucleosomal ladders that possessed blunt‐ended, 5′‐phosphorylated ends (Blaschke et al., 1996; Staley et al., 1997), as is also found during V(D)J recombination (Schlissel et al., 1993). Moreover, proteins required to join the ends during V(D)J recombination—nonhomologous end‐joining (NHEJ) proteins (XRCC4, LigIV)—were found to produce a major phenotype when deleted in mice: massive cell death within the cerebral cortex (and other regions), resulting in embryonic lethality (Chun and Schatz, 1999; Gu et al., 2000).

Figure 1.

Figure 1

Double strand break labeling in the developing brain. (A to L) Nuclear DAPI staining and in situ end‐labeling plus (ISEL+) of double strand breaks in the embryonic mouse cortex from embryonic days 10–18 (E10‐E18) and adult (adapted from Blaschke et al., 1996). ISEL+ labeling increases through E14 and subsequently decreases with further development. [Color figure can be viewed at http://wileyonlinelibrary.com]

Possible sequelae of NHEJ loss, including genomic instability and aneuploidy, were documented in cancers (Difilippantonio et al., 2000; Deans et al., 2003; Thacker and Zdzienicka, 2004), which led to a directed search for aneuploid cells during neurogenesis within the embryonic cerebral cortex. This approach identified the first definitive evidence of neural GM—that which occurred among cells of a single brain—through mosaic, complex aneuploidies among mitotic neural progenitor cells (Rehen et al., 2001) (Fig. 2), and also represents a first example of DNA copy number variations (CNVs). In subsequent years, other forms of GM were identified, including LINE1 elements and sub‐chromosomal CNVs, both of which can be captured by DNA content variation (DCV), as well as single nucleotide variations (SNVs), which together reveal the pervasive prevalence of GM throughout the brain (Kingsbury et al., 2006; Westra et al., 2010; Bushman and Chun, 2013; Bushman et al., 2015) (Fig. 3). The varied forms of neural GM in the normal developing and mature brain are reviewed next, followed by a discussion of functions and putative brain disease relationships.

Figure 2.

Figure 2

Spectral karyotyping (SKY) of NPCs in the developing brain. Spectral karyotyping (SKY) of representative mouse embryonic neuroprogenitor metaphase spreads (adapted from Rehen et al., 2001). (A and B) Spectral (top left) and DAPI (top right) images show chromosome spreads and unique spectral colors for each chromosome. (C and D) Karyotypes (bottom) illustrate losses or gains of particular chromosomes across experiments. Euploid mouse cells have two copies of each chromosome for a total of 40. [Color figure can be viewed at http://wileyonlinelibrary.com]

Figure 3.

Figure 3

Timeline of studies identifying forms of genomic mosaicism and reported functions and/or consequences. As higher‐resolution NGS technologies have become available, the ability to identify smaller somatic differences between brain cells has improved. Many forms of GM have significant functional implications in both healthy brain and disease states. [Color figure can be viewed at http://wileyonlinelibrary.com]

Genomic Mosaicism (GM) as Aneuploidy and Aneusomy in the Developing and Mature Brain

“Aneuploidy” is historically defined as the gain and/or loss of chromosomes from a euploid complement (Tackholm, 1922; Santaguida and Amon, 2015). Its study requires analyses of all chromosomes in a single cell, which, until recently, required examination of condensed chromosomes within metaphase spreads. “Aneusomy” refers typically to a copy number change of a single, defined chromosome without complete knowledge of the state of the remaining chromosomes, and it has been historically identified using fluorescence in situ hybridization (FISH) on nonmitotic cells (Cremer et al., 1986; Pinkel et al., 1986). Analyses of aneuploidy date back over 175 years with the discovery of chromosomes (Naegeli, 1844; Schleicher, 1879; Flemming, 1882; Waldeyer, 1888; Paweletz, 2001) and, indeed, human chromosome number was not accurately reported as 46 chromosomes until 1956 (Tjio and Levan, 1956), before which it was misidentified as 48. This error underscores the technical challenges associated with defining chromosomes before using metaphase spreads, the current gold standard, to examine chromosome number.

Using what was a new technology—spectral karyotyping (SKY) (Liyanage et al., 1996; Macville et al., 1997)—to examine aneuploidy in metaphase spreads, mitotic spreads of neural progenitor cells (NPCs) revealed pervasive aneuploidies within the developing mouse cerebral cortex, affecting ∼30% of mitotic NPCs (Fig. 2) (Rehen et al., 2001). Aneusomic postmitotic neurons (and glia) from both mouse and human were subsequently identified using SKY and/or FISH (McConnell et al., 2004; Kingsbury et al., 2005; Rehen et al., 2005; Yurov et al., 2007a; Peterson et al., 2012). In the adult brain, aneusomies constituted a range that averaged ∼18% (Yurov et al., 2005; Iourov et al., 2006, 2009ab; Yurov et al., 2008; Faggioli et al., 2012; Yurov et al., 2014) (Table 1, Human; Table 2, Mouse). That neural aneuploidies exist is supported not only by hybridization‐based techniques like SKY, but also by histological identification and real‐time imaging of lagging chromosomes and supernumerary centrosomes resulting in multipolar cell divisions, as well as by cytological identification of nondisjunction, micronuclei, and multiple nuclei (Yang et al., 2003). More recently, aneuploidies have been identified by next generation sequencing (NGS) methods (McConnell et al., 2013; Cai et al., 2014; Knouse et al., 2014; van den Bos et al., 2016). All chromosomes contribute to aneuploidy, and there does not appear to be a preference for any one chromosome being affected (Rehen et al., 2001; McConnell et al., 2004; Faggioli et al., 2012; Peterson et al., 2012; Yurov et al., 2014).

Table 1.

Estimates of Aneuploidy Rates in the Human Cerebral Cortex

Method Study Age Cell Type Chromosome Aneuploidy Ratea Hypoploidy : Hyperploidy Ratio
FISH Yurov et al., 2007 8–12 gestational weeks NA 1,9,15–18, X/Y 1.5%b (29%) 5 : 2
Rehen et al., 2005 2 years NA 21 3.2% (53%) 3 : 7
Neuron 21 3.4% (55%) 7 : 9
15 years NA 21 3.8% (59%) 5 : 9
35 years NA 21 3.6% (57%) 3 : 7
Neuron 21 2.8% (48%) 1: 1
48 years NA
Neuron
21
21
3.6% (57%)
2.3% (41%)
1 : 3
5 : 7
Iourov et al., 2009b ∼25 years NA 13,18,21,X/Y 0.5%,0.6%,0.4%,0.3% (10%) 8 : 9b
NA 1,11,17,18,X 0.5%,0.7%, 0.5%, 0.8%,0.4% (13%) 7 : 4b
Iourov et al., 2009a ∼25 years NA 1,7–9,11,16–18,X/Y 0.5%,0.7%,0.9%,0.7%,0.7%,0.5%,0.5%,0.8%,0.4% (14%) 4 : 7b
Yurov et al., 2005 Adult NA 1, 13/21, 18, X/Y 0.4%, 0.3%, 0.7%, 0.8% (12%) b 5 : 4c
Yurov et al., 2008 ∼60 years NA 1 0.6% (13%) 1 : 1
∼79 years NA 1,11,17,18,X 0.6%,0.8%,0.8%,1.1%,1.4% (20%) 2 : 1b
Yurov et al., 2014 69–82 years NA 1,7,11,16–18 0.5%,0.7%,0.7%,0.6%,0.6%,0.8%,1.3% (16%) NR
NA X 1.2% (24%) NR
MCB Iourov et al., 2009a ∼25 years NA 1,7,14,21,X 0.3%,0.6%,0.4%,0.5%,1.2% (13%) NR
Iourov et al., 2006 Adult NA 1, 9, 16, 18, X 0.3%, 0.5%, 0.4%, 0.2%, 2.0% (15%) b NR
Iourov et al., 2009b ∼25 years NA 7,14,21,X 0.6%,0.4%,0.9%,1.2% (16%) NR
∼79 years NA 7,14,21,X 1.0%,0.8%,1.3%,1.9% (25%) NR
scWGS McConnell et al., 2013 20–26 years Neuron All 1.8% 1 : 1
Cai et al., 2014 42 years Neuron All 4.9% 4 : 1
Knouse et al., 2014 40–70 years Neuron All 2.2% 1 : 1
van den Bos et al., 2016 69–93 years Neuron All 0.7% 2 : 5

NR, not reported; all tissue samples contain neurons and all sample sizes are greater than 50 cells.

a

Per chromosome frequency for FISH. Total frequency of aneuploid cells—extrapolated for FISH—is in bold.

b

Averaged from multiple chromosome measurements.

c

Estimated from female X chromosome only.

Table 2.

Estimates of Aneuploidy Rates in the Mouse Cerebral Cortex

Method Study Age Cell Type Chromosome Aneuploidy Ratea Hypoploidy : Hyperploidy Ratio
FISH Rehen et al., 2001 E13‐E14 NA X/Y 6.7% (80%) 15 : 2
Peterson et al., 2012 E19 NA 8,16 1.6%,2.1% (31%) NR
McConnell et al., 2004 8–14 weeks NA X/Y 6.2%b (72%) 9 : 5c
Rehen et al., 2001 Adult NA X/Y 1.2% (21%) 5 : 1
Faggioli et al., 2012 4 months NA 1,7,14–16,18,19 Y 1%b (18%) 1 : 1c
15 months NA 18 1.5% (26%) NR
28 months NA 1,7,14–16,18,19 Y 2.3%b (37%) 1 : 1c
Neuron 18 2.1% (35%) NR
Neuron 18 9.8% (87%) NR
scWGS Knouse et al., 2014 Adult Neuron All 1.4% 0 : 1
Spread counts Peterson et al., 2012 E14 NA All 29.0% 11 : 1c
NA All 24.0% 7 : 1c
SKY McConnell et al., 2004 E12.5‐E14.5 NA All 34.0% b 3 : 1c
Rehen et al., 2001 Embryonic NA All 33.2% 6 : 1b, c

NR, not reported; all tissue samples contain neurons and all sample sizes are greater than 50 cells.

a

Per chromosome frequency for FISH. Total frequency of aneuploid cells—extrapolated for FISH—is in bold.

b

Averaged from multiple animals or from multiple chromosomes.

c

Estimated from a figure.

Neural aneuploidy is most commonly seen as hypoploidy rather than hyperploidy (chromosome loss and gain, respectively) (Rehen et al., 2001; Rehen et al., 2005; Yurov et al., 2005; Yurov et al., 2007a; Westra et al., 2008). This is consistent with the preference for segregation defects involving lagging chromosomes and supernumerary centromeres (as opposed to nondisjunction), which favor the production of hypoploidies in the developing brain (Yang et al., 2003). This difference is most dramatic when assessing the population of cycling neural progenitor cells (∼sixfold more hypoploidies than hyperploidies) (Rehen et al., 2001; McConnell et al., 2004; Peterson et al., 2012), which may suggest that there is negative selection pressure during differentiation, ultimately producing interphase, G0 cells with a preference for chromosome loss. An important technical aspect of assessing the prevalence of aneusomic cells in brain tissue sections is the problem of sectioning through a nucleus to render it artifactually hypoploid. For this reason, a number of studies purposefully biased examination to only chromosome gains (hypersomies) since they would not be produced by sectioning artifact, combined with a focus on sex chromosomes that provided positive, internal controls for identifying 1 (single X‐ and Y‐) chromosome in males or 2 (X‐) chromosomes in females in the face of aneuploid numbers of sex chromosome or autosome copies (Rehen et al., 2001; Kingsbury et al., 2005; Rehen et al., 2005).

Aneusomic neurons survive into adulthood (Rehen et al., 2005; Faggioli et al., 2012; McConnell et al., 2013; Cai et al., 2014; Knouse et al., 2014) where they can become integrated as active components of neuronal circuitry (Kingsbury et al., 2005), and thus likely have functionality. As aneuploid cells are known to have altered gene expression patterns (Yang et al., 2003; Sheltzer et al., 2012), this likely contributes to functional diversity (Letourneau et al., 2014). In addition, although the majority of studies have focused on the cerebral cortex, aneuploidy or aneusomy appears to be ubiquitous across the neuraxis, and has been reported within the medulla oblongata (Yurov et al., 2005), cerebellum (Westra et al., 2008; Iourov et al., 2009a; Faggioli et al., 2012), entorhinal cortex (Mosch et al., 2007), and hippocampus (Rehen et al., 2005; Yurov et al., 2014). Although these tissues are less thoroughly characterized, they generally appear to have similar levels of aneuploidy as the cortex, although there is some evidence that the hippocampus may have slightly higher (Rehen et al., 2005; Yurov et al., 2014) and the cerebellum lower (Faggioli et al., 2012) rates. This form of GM extends evolutionarily from humans through at least teleost fish (Rajendran et al., 2007).

GM as Long Interspersed Nuclear Element 1 (LINE1) Retrotransposons

The second identified element capable of producing GM was LINE1 elements. Retrotransposable elements have produced genomic diversification in both evolution and cancer (Cordaux and Batzer, 2009; Lee et al., 2012). In humans, approximately 17% of the genome is composed of LINE1 repetitive elements within the germline (Viollet et al., 2014), where they exist as over 500,000 copies, most of which are thought to be inactive evolutionary remnants, owing, in part, to many stop codons within their ∼6 Kb sequence (Hancks and Kazazian, 2012). The bicistronic LINE1 RNA contains 2 open reading frames—ORF1 and ORF2—that encode proteins. ORF1 is thought to encode a high affinity RNA binding protein and ORF2 a reverse transcriptase and endonuclease, which together can allow LINE1 DNA insertion into a new genomic location (Hancks and Kazazian, 2012). This process is analogous to the integration of retroviral proviruses (Varmus, 1982) except for a lack of long terminal repeat (LTR) flanking sequences. Mosaic LINE1 insertions, like other elements producing GM, have been hypothesized to contribute to neuronal diversity (Muotri et al., 2005) through somatic retrotransposition into the genome in neuronal precursors from rat hippocampus neural stem cells (Muotri et al., 2005). Further studies expanded the characterization of these elements to human neural stem cell lines that also reported higher copies of LINE1 elements in neural cell populations in comparison to other tissues (Muotri et al., 2009). A variety of sequencing approaches have supported the presence of de novo LINE1 insertions, albeit with widely ranging estimates of their prevalence: <0.6 per genome (Evrony et al., 2012), along with more controversial levels of ∼14 per genome, while other repeat elements (ALUs and STRs) have also been reported and debated (Baillie et al., 2011; Upton et al., 2015; Evrony et al., 2016).

GM Produced by DNA Content Variation (DCV)

The third form of neural GM to be reported was termed DCV. This was first detected in human brain by a combination of brain cell nuclei isolation, labeling with fluorescent DNA dyes (with removal of RNA by digestion), and analyses by either flow cytometry or fluorescence activated cell sorting (FACS) (Westra et al., 2010). A marked population of cells with DNA content gain, as well as populations with loss, were observed, suggesting a further example of CNVs in addition to aneuploidies, manifesting as DCV. Importantly, this approach enabled interrogation of orders of magnitude more cells in a single experiment to reveal major population changes in the total genomic DNA of individual cells interrogated by flow cytometry/FACS (Fig. 4). DCV analyses of nuclei from postmortem human (as well as mouse) brain revealed that many, and in some cases a majority of, neurons, particularly within the human prefrontal cortex, contain more DNA than do lymphocyte controls, which contain, on average, nearly 250 Mb. The technical approach was further optimized to use the now common technique of separating neuronal nuclei based upon their NeuN‐immunopositivity by FACS (or FANS: fluorescence activated nuclear sorting). This technique was first developed and reported in earlier studies of GM (Rehen et al., 2005), in which the most prominent DCV gains occurred in neurons (Westra et al., 2010; Bushman et al., 2015). Moreover, DCV varied with neuroanatomical location, being limited in the cerebellum from the same brain, and reduced in some cell types, as seen in NeuN‐negative populations that were more similar to nuclei isolated from lymphocytes. The specific DNA sequences accounting for DCV remain unknown, but it is likely that DCV comprises “large” forms of GM, like the aneuploidies/aneusomies discussed above, as well as smaller variations broadly dispersed throughout the genome and including LINE1 elements and sub‐chromosomal CNVs, which are discussed next.

Figure 4.

Figure 4

DNA content analysis of nondiseased human nuclei from brain and lymphocytes (adapted from Westra et al., 2010). Chicken erythrocyte nuclei (CEN) were included in each sample as an internal control. (A and B) Histograms of lymphocyte and cerebellar samples do not indicate any increase in genomic content. (C and D) Analysis of DNA content in frontal cortex nuclei has a broad “shoulder” to the right of the main peak revealing increased DNA content. (E) This increase can also be visualized in a flow cytometry plot of individual nuclei. [Color figure can be viewed at http://wileyonlinelibrary.com]

GM Produced by Copy Number Variations (CNVs)

GM produced by mosaic aneuploidies/aneusomies and DCV within the brain was proposed to include DNA sequence changes such as mosaic CNVs with neuroanatomical region specific patterns (Westra et al., 2010). The existence of mosaic neural CNVs was further supported by FISH studies using chromosomal point probes that detected only small chromosomal regions, and may, therefore, have reported CNVs in addition to complete gain and/or loss of an entire chromosome. This phenomenon likely explains, in part, why FISH studies often report higher levels of aneusomy than methods which capture information about entire chromosomes (Knouse et al., 2014; van den Bos et al., 2016). Mosaic CNVs became possible to interrogate more comprehensively using the technical development of single‐cell whole genome sequencing (scWGS), an approach that, like many aspects of DNA sequencing, remains a work in progress. The first report of neuronal scWGS (Evrony et al., 2012) in fact did not report CNVs, but was targeted toward identifying novel LINE1 insertions. However, subsequent reports have identified a range of somatic, neural CNVs focused primarily on neurons (Gole et al., 2013; McConnell et al., 2013; Cai et al., 2014; Knouse et al., 2014; Knouse et al., 2016; van den Bos et al., 2016). To date, at least four publications using scWGS have reported the presence of somatically derived mosaic CNVs in human neurons (Table 3). Mosaic CNVs in these studies showed wide variability, reporting between 9 and 100% of neurons as containing CNVs, with most reported alterations of between 2 and 10 Mb in size, and deletions far outnumbering amplifications (Gole et al., 2013; McConnell et al., 2013; Cai et al., 2014; Knouse et al., 2016), as had been observed earlier for more common chromosomal hypoploidies. In contrast, skin or fibroblast cells were estimated to contain 0.2–0.3 CNVs per cell, with fewer than 25% of cells having any, indicating that this form of GM is enriched in the brain (McConnell et al., 2013; Knouse et al., 2016). Current efforts are ongoing to improve the specificity of this characterization and better understand the developmental relevance of neural CNVs (Rohrback et al., submitted). Still to be determined are the functional consequences of CNVs within the brain.

Table 3.

Somatic CNV Characteristics

Study Methods (Amplification, CNV Calling) QC Metrics Age (years) Cells Analyzeda Smallest Detectable CNV CNVs per Cella Deletion : Amplification Ratioa CNV Positive Cell Frequencya
McConnell et al., 2013 WGA4, CBS Reads, MAD, CS 20, 24, 26 110 (50, 19, 41) 3.4 Mb 1.3 (2.1, 0.9, 0.6) 2 : 1 (3:2, 8:1, 5:1) 41% (60%, 32%, 22%)
Gole et al., 2013 MDA, CBS NR NR 6 (4, 2) 1 Mb 6.0 (6.8, 4.5) 2 : 3 (4:5, 1:4) 100%
Cai et al., 2014 WGA4, CBS MAPD 42 19 2.2 Mb 3.4 65 : 1 68%
Knouse et al., 2016 WGA4, CBS & HMM VS 48, 52, 68, 70 80 (17, 22, 21, 20) 5 Mb 0.2 (0, 0.1, 0.1, 0.6) 15 : 0 9% (0%, 9%, 5%, 20%)

NR, not reported; CBS, circular binary segmentation; HMM, hidden Markov model.

a

Parenthetical values indicate measurements from individual brains.

GM Produced by Single Nucleotide Variations (SNVs)

The smallest form of somatic DNA sequence change is a SNV that can be identified by single‐cell whole genome sequencing of vastly amplified genomes combined with median 30X sequencing coverage, which has revealed SNVs between individual neurons at the level of single nucleotides (Lodato et al., 2015; Bae et al., 2018; Lodato et al., 2018). A crucial initial step in these investigations is massive amplification of single‐cell genomic DNA through use of techniques like “multiple displacement amplification” (MDA) that employs phi29 DNA polymerase, followed by high coverage, whole genome sequencing with paired end Illumina reads, which has revealed SNVs in neurons of the brain. In addition to enormous levels of amplification, data processing must informatically take into account the error rate of the utilized phi29 polymerase, chimeric artifacts, amplification bias and errors, and the significant failure rates of single genome amplifications that likely exclude neurons with genomic attributes that interfere with amplification (e.g., strand breaks, large structural variations, chromatin states). Of further note, the unpredictably biased amplification inherent to MDA generates excessive noise which precludes the reliable examination of larger structural variations such as CNVs. Nonetheless, these high depth single neuron genomic sequences have expanded the forms of GM to thousands of SNVs within single neuronal genomes that differ from the germline. High depth sequencing of single neuron SNVs produced during neurogenesis has also been used for lineage mapping of clonal populations in the adult brain (Evrony et al., 2012; Lodato et al., 2015).

An independent methodology for assessing SNVs utilized somatic cell nuclear transfer (SCNT) and mouse cloning techniques involving mitral cells of the olfactory bulb to amplify single neuronal genomes, which also identified hundreds of SNVs within seven single neurons (Hazen et al., 2016). This methodology allows high depth sequencing without artificial template amplification. It does face intrinsic limitations, including low throughput, high failure rates of SCNT, low rates of mitotic growth of the newly created cells, incompatibility with humans in requiring the use of laboratory mice, and in some cases a need to generate cloned mice, a process that likely excludes interrogation of cells with highly altered genomes (e.g., aneuploid neurons). Nevertheless, these results demonstrated that individual mitral neurons contain hundreds of unique SNVs, and considering the relatively shorter lifespan of mice vs. humans, the numbers of SNVs in mice are generally consistent with the thousands observed in older human neurons in which SNVs appear to increase with age (Bae et al., 2018; Lodato et al., 2018), albeit based upon very few neurons assessed with all of these techniques.

GM Technical Challenges

The study of neural GM has been strongly influenced by technical advances. Metaphase spread analyses have been used to detect chromosomal abnormalities for over 100 years and are still in use today. However, it has two considerable shortcomings for in depth analysis of neural GM. First, metaphase spreads require the presence of mitotic cells, which represent a small fraction of brain cells (Blaschke et al., 1996; Blaschke et al., 1998). Second, genomic resolution is limited to large alterations that do not inform on specific DNA sequences. DNA content changes identified by use of fluorescent DNA dyes combined with flow cytometry or FACS have been widely used as a gold standard in studying the cell cycle and in plant biology (Darzynkiewicz et al., 2004; Dolezel et al., 2007) and have provided a high throughput, albeit low resolution, assessment of GM (Westra et al., 2010; Bushman et al., 2015). Flow cytometric assessments are amenable to nonmitotic analyses of DCV and do not require metaphase spreads, allowing interrogation of hundreds of thousands of nuclei from any tissue type in minutes (Westra et al., 2010; Bushman et al., 2015).

FISH methods, including SKY, allow a more targeted investigation of alterations to one or more chromosomes, and can be performed on mitotic, interphase, or nonmitotic cells. However, these studies have provided exceptionally variable estimates of chromosome alteration rates—from 10% to 80% of cells being aneuploid (Rehen et al., 2001; McConnell et al., 2004; Rehen et al., 2005; Yurov et al., 2005; Iourov et al., 2006, 2009ab; Yurov et al., 2007a; Yurov et al., 2008; Faggioli et al., 2012; Peterson et al., 2012; Yurov et al., 2014). A substantial amount of this variability stems from experiments performing FISH and estimating rates based on a single chromosome (Rehen et al., 2001; McConnell et al., 2004; Pack et al., 2005; Yurov et al., 2008; Faggioli et al., 2012; Yurov et al., 2014). Since metaphase segregation defects can affect multiple chromosomes simultaneously (Yang et al., 2003), aneuploidy of different chromosomes is not necessarily an independent occurrence. Thus, extrapolation produces an artificially high aneuploidy rate when an insufficient number of chromosomes are interrogated. This effect may be further compounded by the confounding variable of these methods reporting subchromosomal alterations with the same signal as for a full aneuploidy (Osada et al., 2002; Iourov et al., 2013; Bushman et al., 2015; Evrony et al., 2015; Lodato et al., 2015; Zhang et al., 2015). Peptide nucleic acid FISH (Westra et al., 2010; Bushman et al., 2015) (PNA‐FISH) provides semi‐quantitative data on a targeted locus and has the ability to visualize single genes when combined with appropriate microscopic techniques. This method has been used to identify centromere and gene copy increases (Bushman et al., 2015) without template amplification or other polymerase dependent approaches, and can provide validation for stereotyped CNVs reported by sequencing (Fig. 5). Additional FISH approaches based upon variations of RNAscope used in validating transcriptomic diversity in the human brain (Lake et al., 2016; Lake et al., 2017; Lake et al., 2018) may have further applicability to genomic loci in the future.

Figure 5.

Figure 5

Peptide nucleic acid visualization of APP CNVs. Peptide nucleic acid fluorescent in situ hybridization was imaged using structured illumination microscopy (adapted from Bushman et al., 2015), revealing copy number variations of APP in Alzheimer's disease cortical neurons. [Color figure can be viewed at http://wileyonlinelibrary.com]

Two approaches allow high resolution sequence information to be obtained from special treatment of bulk (multi‐cell) samples. As noted above, the first was achieved by SCNT and clonal expansion of a single neuron—where all derivative cells have identical genomes—which allowed the collection of high resolution, whole genome coverage data, albeit with the previously noted limitations. A second approach involves capture of targeted genomic regions using bulk DNA combined with pulldown “bait” strategies. The smaller size of the genome under interrogation allows ultra high depth sequencing (> 1,000×), which enables detection of somatic variants with lower allele frequency (Sala Frigerio et al., 2015) (∼1% for SNVs, 10% for CNVs). This approach is useful for known genomic targets and semi‐conserved GM alterations, but is not appropriate for de novo discovery or the detection of ultra low frequency somatic events as can occur with neural GM.

scWGS addresses some shortcomings of the bulk approaches, as it directly assesses the cell of interest without requiring totipotency or conservation of GM. One intriguing discrepancy has been brought to light by this method: scWGS reports much lower rates of aneuploidy (Knouse et al., 2014; van den Bos et al., 2016). This may be due to at least two factors. First, these studies have performed scWGS on interphase cells, contrasting with over 100 years of literature that has relied on definitions of aneuploidy based almost solely on metaphase spreads. Since the relative rates of aneuploidy reported in metaphase spread analyses represent a cell population that has not been assessed by scWGS, the reported discrepancies could simply reflect differences in mitotic and interphase brain cells. Second, scWGS can distinguish between full and partial chromosome alterations, while other approaches may report a broader range of aberrations that manifest in metaphase as aneuploidy. This ultimately brings into question definitions of aneuploidy in nonmitotic cells revealed by scWGS. It is notable that the relationship between interphase (nonmitotic) partial aneuploidies and chromosomes seen in condensed metaphase spreads of the brain is not known. However, SCNT approaches to condense postmitotic neuronal DNA have reported rates of aberrant chromosomal spreads of ∼64%, supporting the interpretation that partial aneuploidies/CNVs observed in nonmitotic cells—postmitotic neurons—manifest as chromosomal aneuploidies in metaphase spreads (Osada et al., 2002).

In addition to large DNA alterations, the high coverage data generated by extreme amplification using MDA and high depth sequencing allowed identification of unique SNVs (Lodato et al., 2015; Lodato et al., 2018). However, this form of scWGS has a high failure rate (possibly excluding neurons with unique genomic attributes), is cost prohibitive for more than a few cells, and regional genome bias under the reported conditions using MDA precludes examination of large structural variants such as CNVs. Lower depth sequencing following PCR‐based amplification allows analysis of CNVs, but these studies have reported extremely inconsistent findings, which is likely due to substantial methodological variability: different ages, amplification methods, sample sizes, informatics, stringencies for quality control (QC), and CNV calling, which were applied in each study (Gole et al., 2013; McConnell et al., 2013; Cai et al., 2014; Knouse et al., 2016).

At least seven critical issues associated with all scWGS approaches to assess CNV and SNV GM exist: (1) an absolute requirement to amplify the single‐cell genomic template using polymerases, which itself may introduce a range of artifacts that could create or obfuscate mosaically present CNVs or SNVs; (2) different protocols and amplification techniques—including different DNA polymerases—limit direct comparisons; (3) an inability to replicate the results, since each single cell is destroyed by sequencing; (4) a current inability to assess sub‐megabase CNVs; (5) varied and often opaque technical details that obfuscate both failure rates and bioinformatics pipeline details; (6) limited individual brain interrogation that consists of a few or even just 1 brain; and (7) miniscule sample sizes—typically less than 100 cells. Considering the 1 trillion cells in the human brain, these samples constitute only 10−8% of cells within a single brain.

GM Generation, Functions, and Disease Implications

The relative sizes and forms of GM (Fig. 6) underscore a vast range—on the order of 8 logs—of DNA sequence changes, from SNVs to hundreds of millions of base pairs in a single chromosome.

Figure 6.

Figure 6

Types of GM occurring in neural progenitor populations of the developing brain and in mature neurons of the adult brain. (A) Multiple different forms of GM have been observed to occur in neural progenitor populations, ranging in size from over 500 Mb or more, down to the level of single nucleotide variations. (B) Mature neurons display increases of altered genomic sequences through at least DNA content variation (DCV), copy number variations (CNVs), and single nucleotide variations (SNVs) during postnatal life. Boxes labeled with question marks reference the current state of the field, since a range of additional forms of GM likely will be found as emerging technologies allow for new insight and novel discoveries. [Color figure can be viewed at http://wileyonlinelibrary.com]

Despite extensive characterization of the occurrence of neural GM, there is limited understanding of the mechanisms through which it is generated. Mosaic neural aneuploidies arise from mitotic errors that include lagging chromosomes, micronuclei, supernumerary centrosomes and chromosomal nondisjunction (Yang et al., 2003). These mechanisms are limited to periods of neurogenesis, particularly during brain development (Rehen et al., 2001; Rehen et al., 2005; Yurov et al., 2005; Westra et al., 2008). In addition to roles in generating full aneuploidies, micronuclei could contribute to smaller (megabase) CNVs. Micronuclei are a common byproduct of mitotic segregation defects and have been associated with further degradation of the affected chromosome(s) upon progression through subsequent cell cycles (Zhang et al., 2015). It is tempting to speculate that this could be one mechanism by which such CNVs are created, particularly those that appear to follow a pattern similar to chromothrypsis (McConnell et al., 2013; Cai et al., 2014; Knouse et al., 2016). Another possible source of CNVs may reflect repair of DNA, which is supported by the presence of both documented DNA breaks and nucleotide incorporation (e.g., BrdU or 3H‐thy) (Blaschke et al., 1996; Blaschke et al., 1998). Also proposed to occur during neurogenesis, mosaic LINE1 insertions, as discussed in a previous section, are theoretically capable of generating GM during the cell cycle (Packer et al., 1993; Muotri et al., 2005; Shi et al., 2007; Singer et al., 2010; Viollet et al., 2014; Mita et al., 2018). By contrast, many neural somatic SNVs have been associated with damage due to transcriptional activity (Lodato et al., 2015), consistent with increased SNV rates in aged brains (Bae et al., 2018; Lodato et al., 2018), suggesting this form of GM is generated in postmitotic neurons. It is entirely possible that other mechanisms could contribute to neural GM, including hypothesized gene recombination, which awaits further investigation.

The normal functions of neural GM of any form are incompletely known, yet almost certainly affect both development and adult brain function. Normal, mosaically aneuploid brain cells produce altered transcriptomes (Kaushal et al., 2003) consistent with studies in simpler systems like yeast (Sheltzer et al., 2012). In this vein, a remarkable degree of transcriptomic diversity has emerged from single‐cell transcriptome studies throughout the neuraxis of mouse (Usoskin et al., 2015; Zeisel et al., 2015; La Manno et al., 2016; Poulin et al., 2016; Tasic et al., 2016; Chung et al., 2017; Karlsson and Linnarsson, 2017; Hochgerner et al., 2018; Rosenberg et al., 2018) and the human brain (Lake et al., 2016; Lein et al., 2017; Regev et al., 2017; Sousa et al., 2017), consistent with the enormous neural GM diversity present in both mice and humans. Transcriptomic variation can cover the gamut of cellular functions, which remain to be fully assessed but are clearly part of the normal brain's circuitry, based upon the functional integration of aneusomic neurons within the brain (Kingsbury et al., 2005). During brain development, clear associations between aneuploidies and cell survival have also been documented through analyses of aneuploid neural cells following cell death attenuation by knockout of caspase 3 or caspase 9, or pan‐caspase pharmacological inhibition by Z‐VAD‐fmk (Peterson et al., 2012), which results in maintenance of increased numbers of aneuploid neural cells, including subpopulations with extreme aneuploidy that are not seen in the wildtype brain (Peterson et al., 2012). These data indicate that forms of neural aneuploidy are not neutral, with mild forms preferentially surviving, whereas more extreme forms are eliminated by cell death. For surviving postmitotic neurons (Kingsbury et al., 2005; Rehen et al., 2005), functional consequences could be vast and difficult to predict. However, based on a meta‐analysis in yeast, aneuploidy often triggers expression of stress response genes, and suppresses cell proliferation pathways (Sheltzer et al., 2012) that may relate in some instances to the postmitotic state of neurons. Possible functions of LINE1 retrotranspositions have been centered around behavior and memory (Singer et al., 2010; Bachiller et al., 2017); however germline changes in LINE1 genomic regions (Erwin et al., 2016) distinct from retrotransposition complicate analyses, which may explain major discrepancies in the literature (e.g., LINE1 putative somatic retrotransposition rates of <0.6 per genome (Evrony et al., 2012) vs. ∼14 per genome (Baillie et al., 2011; Upton et al., 2015)). It is certain that the functional consequences of neural GM will be revealed in increasingly greater detail by ongoing research.

Indeed, clear precedence for functional consequences of GM affecting the brain and body exist in data on diseased states. Mosaic variegated aneuploidy (MVA), in which inactivating gene mutations in mitotic proteins causes an increase in forms and frequency of aneuploidy, ultimately results in microcephaly and mental retardation (Warburton et al., 1991; Kajii et al., 1998). Constitutive aneuploidies as found in Down syndrome (Wiseman et al., 2015) have clear effects on brain function, providing support for functional consequences of neural mosaic aneuploidies and GM, while constitutive cases may also themselves be chromosomal mosaics (Modi et al., 2003; Leon et al., 2010; Hulten et al., 2013). Indeed, multiple reports of elevated levels of somatic aneuploidy in patients with ataxia telangiectasia (McConnell et al., 2004; Iourov et al., 2009a, 2009b), schizophrenia (Yurov et al., 2001; Yurov et al., 2008), autism (Yurov et al., 2007b; Iourov et al., 2008), and Alzheimer's disease (Pack et al., 2005; Mosch et al., 2007; Iourov et al., 2009b; Iourov et al., 2011; Yurov et al., 2014) support pathogenic links. Brain disease relationships of LINE1 GM have been reported in rare disorders resulting from mutations in genes that regulate LINE1: Rett Syndrome (MECP2), ataxia telangiectasia (ATM), and Aicardi‐Goutières syndrome (involving multiple different genes, including SAMHD1 that inhibits viral and LINE1 reverse transcriptases). MECP2 is a suppressor of LINE1 transcription (Skene et al., 2010; Muotri et al., 2010), while ATM recognizes LINE1 target priming retrotransposition intermediates as damage (Coufal et al., 2011). Aicardi‐Goutiéres syndrome is characterized by mutations in genes that inhibit reverse transcription (Zhao et al., 2013; Upton et al., 2015). It is currently unclear how LINE1 contributes mechanistically to these diseases, but it may involve disruption of normal gene transcription.

Rare CNVs associated with disease are supported by somatic repeat expansions that have been reported for multiple pathological states, as seen in tissue‐specific CAG repeat expansion profiles in Huntington's Disease (Telenius et al., 1994; La Spada, 1997; Shelbourne et al., 2007; Gonitel et al., 2008; Kraus‐Perrotta and Lagalwar, 2016). The highest levels of repeat length instability are observed in the brain, predominantly in neurons of brain regions most severely affected by the disease (Telenius et al., 1994; Shelbourne et al., 2007; Gonitel et al., 2008). Repeat expansion GM has been additionally implicated in spinocerebellar ataxia (La Spada, 1997; Kraus‐Perrotta and Lagalwar, 2016), frontotemporal dementia, amyotrophic lateral sclerosis (Almeida et al., 2013), and dentatorubral‐pallidoluysian atrophy (Ueno et al., 1995). Interestingly, somatic repeat expansion variation may occur in postmitotic neurons, supporting neural GM disease mechanisms in both mitotic and postmitotic periods (Gonitel et al., 2008; Kraus‐Perrotta and Lagalwar, 2016), and consistent with DCV changes that are most prominent during adult life (Westra et al., 2010; Bushman et al., 2015). GM produced by SNVs has been linked to rare brain diseases like hemimegalencephaly and focal cortical dysplasia involving point mutations in MTOR pathway genes (Evrony et al., 2012; D'Gama et al., 2017).

Beyond rare familial brain disorders, neural GM has been linked to sporadic Alzheimer's disease (AD) through both increased DCV and specific CNV amplification of the pathogenic gene, Amyloid Precursor Protein (APP). DCV increases of ∼200 Mb over the normal 250 Mb within prefrontal cortical neurons indicate significant, subgenomic increases in DNA content that are not explained by cell cycle reentry (Yang et al., 2001; Westra et al., 2009) nor trisomy 21 (Heston and Mastri, 1977; Potter, 1991) in view of more recent reports (Westra et al., 2009; Bushman et al., 2015). Most notably, single neuron qPCR for APP combined with PNA‐FISH for proximal and distal APP exons identified increased APP copy numbers of up to 12 copies, arising somatically and mosaically in sporadic AD neurons (Bushman et al., 2015): CNV increases of just 3 APP copies is pathogenic for AD in Down syndrome (via trisomy 21 on which APP resides) (Wiseman et al., 2015) and rare familial cases of APP locus duplication (Hooli et al., 2012). These results suggest a more general paradigm for neurological and neuropsychiatric sporadic brain disease, whereby known genes from rare, familial cases—such as APP for Down syndrome or familial AD—are somatically and mosaically altered by GM to produce common forms of disease. This same model may play out in other genomic regions whose germline alterations are not compatible with life—and therefore have not been identified in familial disease—but may be altered mosaically to produce sporadic forms of a disease.

CONCLUDING COMMENTS

Over the last 20 years, neural GM has advanced from a theoretical concept to a definitive experimental fact and now represents a vibrant field of active research. The proven forms of GM within single cells of the brain—aneuploidies and aneusomies, other CNVs, and SNVs—are no doubt the “tip of the iceberg” in considering the pervasive presence of DCV throughout the brain that captures virtually all forms of DNA sequence alterations, affecting both mitotic and postmitotic populations. The combination of these alterations contribute to increased GM over time (Fig. 7a–c). As perhaps the most stable and long lasting biological substrate within the brain, DNA changes produced by GM may underlie fundamental brain activities including complex behaviors and long term memory. The presence of DNA fragmentation and double strand breaks amongst developing brain cell populations associated with cell death and differentiation likely involves recurrent breaks in specific genes, as reported from studies of neural progenitor cell populations (Wei et al., 2016), which are again reminiscent of processes in the adaptive immune system (Chun, 2001; Kingsbury et al., 2006; Westra et al., 2010; Bushman and Chun, 2013). It would thus not be surprising to find novel forms of DNA rearrangement within cells of the brain, given the postmitotic state of neurons (distinct from clonally expanded alterations of the immune system) and expression of different genes (e.g., RAG1 but not RAG2 within the brain vs. both in the immune system). All combined, these diverse, nonmutually exclusive and pervasive forms of neural GM could “barcode” each brain cell by creating a unique genome, thus representing a small universe of genome diversity residing within a single brain. Moreover, further evidence that this universe changes over time adds another dimension of complexity, representing a relatively unassessed variable contributing to neural diversity at all levels of brain development and function. This same genomically diverse universe is currently unrecognized by virtually all genetic models of brain disease, particularly those relying on statistical relationships of genes identified from cells outside of the brain, as is common for genome‐wide association studies (GWAS). The overwhelming prevalence of sporadic brain disease unaccounted for by defined familial genes—as observed in Alzheimer's disease—may be more fully explained by mosaic genomic changes that affect both the genes identified in rare familial cases, as well as new genes and nongenic loci (including mutations that may not be compatible with life if present constitutively), particularly within postmitotic neurons. Individual cells altered in sporadic disease could offer a rich, new source for discovery of meaningful disease targets. GM within the brain therefore represents a vast frontier awaiting further exploration and discovery, toward more fully understanding the developing and functioning brain and its diseases.

Figure 7.

Figure 7

Rate of DNA alteration in developing brain and distribution of genomic mosaicism in mature neurons. (A) In embryonic brain development, DNA alteration rates peak during periods corresponding with high levels of neurogenesis, before leveling off. (B) After prenatal development, genomic alterations continue to accumulate during neonatal and childhood neurodevelopmental periods, with smaller alterations accruing thereafter. (C) Taken together, genomic mosaicism accumulates over a lifetime, starting during embryonic brain development, and continuing throughout life and into adulthood and old age, potentially contributing to age related neurological disorders. Importantly, the vast majority of these somatic changes can only be detected when the mosaic nature of individual neurons is considered in experimental design. [Color figure can be viewed at http://wileyonlinelibrary.com]

ACKNOWLEDGMENTS

We thank Dr. Laura Wolszon for editorial assistance. This work was supported by the NIH (NIMH, NINDS, and NIAAA), the Shaffer Family Foundation, the Bruce Ford and Anne Smith Bundy Foundation, and the Hervey Family Fund. “Somatic copy number gains of a‐synuclein (SNCA) in Parkinson's disease and multiple system atrophy brains” that adds further support GM alterations affecting pathogenic genes in brain diseases.

REFERENCES

  1. Do all our body cells have the same DNA? [Online]. Nature Education. Available: https://www.nature.com/scitable/popular-discussion/1706 2012].
  2. Almeida S, Gascon E, Tran H, Chou HJ, Gendron TF, Degroot S, Tapper AR, et al. 2013. Modeling key pathological features of frontotemporal dementia with C9ORF72 repeat expansion in iPSC‐derived human neurons. Acta Neuropathol 126:385–399. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Avery OT, Macleod CM, Mccarty M. 1944. Studies on the chemical nature of the substance inducing transformation of pneumococcal types: Induction of transformation by a desoxyribonucleic acid fraction isolated from pneumococcus Type III. J Exp Med 79:137–158. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Bachiller S, Del‐Pozo‐Martin Y, Carrion AM. 2017. L1 retrotransposition alters the hippocampal genomic landscape enabling memory formation. Brain Behav Immun 64:65–70. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Bae T, Tomasini L, Mariani J, Zhou B, Roychowdhury T, Franjic D, Pletikos M, et al. 2018. Different mutational rates and mechanisms in human cells at pregastrulation and neurogenesis. Science 359:550–555. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Baillie JK, Barnett MW, Upton KR, Gerhardt DJ, Richmond TA, De Sapio F, Brennan PM, et al. 2011. Somatic retrotransposition alters the genetic landscape of the human brain. Nature 479:534–537. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Blaschke AJ, Staley K, Chun J. 1996. Widespread programmed cell death in proliferative and postmitotic regions of the fetal cerebral cortex. Development 122:1165–1174. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Blaschke AJ, Weiner JA, Chun J. 1998. Programmed cell death is a universal feature of embryonic and postnatal neuroproliferative regions throughout the central nervous system. J Comp Neurol 396:39–50. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Bushman DM, Chun J. 2013. The genomically mosaic brain: Aneuploidy and more in neural diversity and disease. Semin Cell Dev Biol 24:357–369. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Bushman DM, Kaeser GE, Siddoway B, Westra JW, Rivera RR, Rehen SK, Yung YC, et al. 2015. Genomic mosaicism with increased amyloid precursor protein (APP) gene copy number in single neurons from sporadic Alzheimer's disease brains. Elife 4. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Cai X, Evrony GD, Lehmann HS, Elhosary PC, Mehta BK, Poduri A, Walsh CA. 2014. Single‐cell, genome‐wide sequencing identifies clonal somatic copy‐number variation in the human brain. Cell Rep 8:1280–1289. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Cajal, SRY. 1901. Studies on the Cerebral Cortex. Madrid: University of Madrid. [Google Scholar]
  13. Chun J. 1999. Developmental neurobiology: A genetic Cheshire cat?. Curr Biol 9:R651–R654. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Chun J. 2001. Selected comparison of immune and nervous system development. Adv Immunol 77:297–322. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Chun J. 2004. Choices, choices, choices. Nat Neurosci 7:323–325. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Chun J, Schatz DG. 1999. Rearranging views on neurogenesis: Neuronal death in the absence of DNA end‐joining proteins. Neuron 22:7–10. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Chun JJ, Schatz DG, Oettinger MA, Jaenisch R, Baltimore D. 1991. The recombination activating gene‐1 (RAG‐1) transcript is present in the murine central nervous system. Cell 64:189–200. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Chung S, Weber F, Zhong P, Tan CL, Nguyen TN, Beier KT, Hormann N, et al. 2017. Identification of preoptic sleep neurons using retrograde labelling and gene profiling. Nature 545:477–481. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Cordaux R, Batzer MA. 2009. The impact of retrotransposons on human genome evolution. Nat Rev Genet 10:691–703. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Coufal NG, Garcia‐Perez JL, Peng GE, Marchetto MC, Muotri AR, Mu Y, Carson CT, et al. 2011. Ataxia telangiectasia mutated (ATM) modulates long interspersed element‐1 (L1) retrotransposition in human neural stem cells. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 108:20382–20387. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Cremer T, Landegent J, Bruckner A, Scholl HP, Schardin M, Hager HD, Devilee P, et al. 1986. Detection of chromosome aberrations in the human interphase nucleus by visualization of specific target DNAs with radioactive and non‐radioactive in situ hybridization techniques: Diagnosis of trisomy 18 with probe L1.84. Hum Genet 74:346–352. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. D'Gama AM, Woodworth MB, Hossain AA, Bizzotto S, Hatem NE, Lacoursiere CM, et al. 2017. Somatic mutations activating the mTOR pathway in dorsal telencephalic progenitors cause a continuum of cortical dysplasias. Cell Rep 21:3754–3766. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Darzynkiewicz Z, Juan G, SROUR EF. 2004. Differential staining of DNA and RNA. Curr Protoc Cytom, Chapter 7, Unit 7 3. [DOI] [PubMed]
  24. Deans B, Griffin CS, O'regan P, Jasin M, Thacker J. 2003. Homologous recombination deficiency leads to profound genetic instability in cells derived from Xrcc2‐knockout mice. Cancer Res 63:8181–8187. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Difilippantonio MJ, Zhu J, Chen HT, Meffre E, Nussenzweig MC, Max EE, et al. 2000. DNA repair protein Ku80 suppresses chromosomal aberrations and malignant transformation. Nature 404:510–514. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Dolezel J, Greilhuber J, Suda J. 2007. Estimation of nuclear DNA content in plants using flow cytometry. Nat Protoc 2:2233–2244. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Dreyer WJ, Bennett JC. 1965. The molecular basis of antibody formation: A paradox. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 54:864–869. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Dreyer WJ, Gray WR, Hood L. 1967. The genetic, molecular and cellular basis of antibody formation: Some facts and a unifying hypothesis. Cold Spring Harbor Symp Quant Biol 32:353–367. [Google Scholar]
  29. Eggan K, Baldwin K, Tackett M, Osborne J, Gogos J, Chess A, Axel R et al. 2004. Mice cloned from olfactory sensory neurons. Nature 428:44–49. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Erwin JA, Paquola AC, Singer T, Gallina I, Novotny M, Quayle C, Bedrosian TA, et al. 2016. L1‐associated genomic regions are deleted in somatic cells of the healthy human brain. Nat Neurosci 19:1583–1591. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Evrony GD, Cai X, Lee E, Hills LB, Elhosary PC, Lehmann HS, Parker JJ, et al. 2012. Single‐neuron sequencing analysis of L1 retrotransposition and somatic mutation in the human brain. Cell 151:483–496. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Evrony GD, Lee E, Mehta BK, Benjamini Y, Johnson RM, Cai X, Yang L, et al. 2015. Cell lineage analysis in human brain using endogenous retroelements. Neuron 85:49–59. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Evrony GD, Lee E, Park PJ, Walsh CA. 2016. Resolving rates of mutation in the brain using single‐neuron genomics. Elife 5: [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Faggioli F, Wang T, Vijg J, Montagna C. 2012. Chromosome‐specific accumulation of aneuploidy in the aging mouse brain. Hum Mol Genet 21:5246–5253. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Flemming W. 1882. Zellsubstanz, Kern, and Zelltheilung. 1st Ed. F. C. Vogel, Leipzig.
  36. Gole J, Gore A, Richards A, Chiu YJ, Fung HL, Bushman D, Chiang HI, et al. 2013. Massively parallel polymerase cloning and genome sequencing of single cells using nanoliter microwells. Nat Biotechnol 31:1126–1132. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Gonitel R, Moffitt H, Sathasivam K, Woodman B, Detloff PJ, Faull RL, Bates GP. 2008. DNA instability in postmitotic neurons. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 105:3467–3472. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Gu Y, Sekiguchi J, Gao Y, Dikkes P, Frank K, Ferguson D, Hasty P, et al. 2000. Defective embryonic neurogenesis in Ku‐deficient but not DNA‐dependent protein kinase catalytic subunit‐deficient mice. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 97:2668–2673. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Hancks DC, Kazazian HH. JR. 2012. Active human retrotransposons: Variation and disease. Curr Opin Genet Dev 22:191–203. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Hattori D, Millard SS, Wojtowicz WM, Zipursky SL. 2008. Dscam‐mediated cell recognition regulates neural circuit formation. Annu Rev Cell Dev Biol 24:597–620. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Hazen JL, Faust GG, Rodriguez AR, Ferguson WC, Shumilina S, Clark RA, Boland MJ, et al. 2016. The complete genome sequences, unique mutational spectra, and developmental potency of adult neurons revealed by cloning. Neuron 89:1223–1236. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Heston LL, Mastri AR. 1977. The genetics of Alzheimer's disease: Associations with hematologic malignancy and Down's syndrome. Arch Gen Psychiatry 34:976–981. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Hochgerner H, Zeisel A, Lonnerberg P, Linnarsson S. 2018. Conserved properties of dentate gyrus neurogenesis across postnatal development revealed by single‐cell RNA sequencing. Nat Neurosci 21:290–299. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Hooli BV, Mohapatra G, Mattheisen M, Parrado AR, Roehr JT, Shen Y, Gusella JF, et al. 2012. Role of common and rare APP DNA sequence variants in Alzheimer disease. Neurology 78:1250–1257. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Hozumi N, Tonegawa S. 1976. Evidence for somatic rearrangements of immunoglobulin genes coding for variable and constant regions. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 73:3628–3632. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Hulten MA, Jonasson J, Iwarsson E, Uppal P, Vorsanova SG, Yurov YB, Iourov IY. 2013. Trisomy 21 mosaicism: We may all have a touch of Down syndrome. Cytogenet Genome Res 139:189–192. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Iourov IY, Liehr T, Vorsanova SG, Kolotii AD, Yurov YB. 2006. Visualization of interphase chromosomes in postmitotic cells of the human brain by multicolour banding (MCB). Chromosome Res 14:223–229. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Iourov Iy, Vorsanova SG, Liehr T, Kolotii AD, Yurov YB. 2009a. Increased chromosome instability dramatically disrupts neural genome integrity and mediates cerebellar degeneration in the ataxia‐telangiectasia brain. Hum Mol Genet 18:2656–2669. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Iourov IY, Vorsanova SG, Liehr T, Yurov YB. 2009b. Aneuploidy in the normal, Alzheimer's disease and ataxia‐telangiectasia brain: Differential expression and pathological meaning. Neurobiol Dis 34:212–220. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Iourov IY, Vorsanova SG, Voinova VY, Kurinnaia OS, Zelenova MA, Demidova IA, Yurov YB. 2013. Xq28 (MECP2) microdeletions are common in mutation‐negative females with Rett syndrome and cause mild subtypes of the disease. Mol Cytogenet 6:53. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Iourov IY, Vorsanova SG, Yurov YB. 2011. Genomic landscape of the Alzheimer's disease brain: Chromosome instability–aneuploidy, but not tetraploidy–mediates neurodegeneration. Neurodegener Dis 8:35–37. discussion 38–40. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Iourov IY, Yurov YB, Vorsanova SG. 2008. Mosaic X chromosome aneuploidy can help to explain the male‐to‐female ratio in autism. Med Hypotheses 70:456. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Jin XK, Li WW, Wu MH, Guo XN, Li S, Yu AQ, Zhu YT, et al. 2013. Immunoglobulin superfamily protein Dscam exhibited molecular diversity by alternative splicing in hemocytes of crustacean, Eriocheir sinensis. Fish Shellfish Immunol 35:900–909. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Jung D, Giallourakis C, Mostoslavsky R, ALT FW. 2006. Mechanism and control of V(D)J recombination at the immunoglobulin heavy chain locus. Annu Rev Immunol 24:541–570. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Kajii T, Kawai T, Takumi T, Misu H, Mabuchi O, Takahashi Y, Tachino M, et al. 1998. Mosaic variegated aneuploidy with multiple congenital abnormalities: Homozygosity for total premature chromatid separation trait. Am J Med Genet 78:245–249. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Karlsson K, Linnarsson S. 2017. Single‐cell mRNA isoform diversity in the mouse brain. BMC Genomics 18:126. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Kaushal D, Contos JJ, Treuner K, Yang AH, Kingsbury MA, Rehen SK, Mcconnell MJ, et al. 2003. Alteration of gene expression by chromosome loss in the postnatal mouse brain. J Neurosci 23:5599–5606. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Kingsbury MA, Friedman B, Mcconnell MJ, Rehen SK, Yang AH, Kaushal D, Chun J. 2005. Aneuploid neurons are functionally active and integrated into brain circuitry. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 102:6143–6147. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Kingsbury MA, Yung YC, Peterson SE, Westra JW, Chun J. 2006. Aneuploidy in the normal and diseased brain. Cell Mol Life Sci 63:2626–2641. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Knouse KA, Wu J, Amon A. 2016. Assessment of megabase‐scale somatic copy number variation using single‐cell sequencing. Genome Res 26:376–384. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Knouse KA, Wu J, Whittaker CA, Amon A. 2014. Single cell sequencing reveals low levels of aneuploidy across mammalian tissues. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 111:13409–13414. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Kraus‐Perrotta C, Lagalwar S. 2016. Expansion, mosaicism and interruption: Mechanisms of the CAG repeat mutation in spinocerebellar ataxia type 1. Cerebellum Ataxias 3:20. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  63. Kuida K, Haydar TF, Kuan CY, Gu Y, Taya C, Karasuyama H, Su MS, et al. 1998. Reduced apoptosis and cytochrome c‐mediated caspase activation in mice lacking caspase 9. Cell 94:325–337. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  64. Kuida K, Zheng TS, Na S, Kuan C, Yang D, Karasuyama H, Rakic P, et al. 1996. Decreased apoptosis in the brain and premature lethality in CPP32‐deficient mice. Nature 384:368–372. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  65. La Manno G, Gyllborg D, Codeluppi S, Nishimura K, Salto C, Zeisel A, Borm Le, et al. 2016. Molecular diversity of midbrain development in mouse, human, and stem cells. Cell 167:566–580 e19. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  66. La Spada AR. 1997. Trinucleotide repeat instability: Genetic features and molecular mechanisms. Brain Pathol 7:943–963. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  67. Lake BB, Ai R, Kaeser GE, Salathia NS, Yung YC, Liu R, Wildberg A, et al. 2016. Neuronal subtypes and diversity revealed by single‐nucleus RNA sequencing of the human brain. Science 352:1586–1590. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  68. Lake BB, Chen S, Sos BC, Fan J, Kaeser GE, Yung YC, Duong TE, et al. 2018. Integrative single‐cell analysis of transcriptional and epigenetic states in the human adult brain. Nat Biotechnol 36:70–80. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  69. Lake BB, Codeluppi S, Yung YC, Gao D, Chun J, Kharchenko PV, Linnarsson S, et al. 2017. A comparative strategy for single‐nucleus and single‐cell transcriptomes confirms accuracy in predicted cell‐type expression from nuclear RNA. Sci Rep 7:6031. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  70. Lee E, Iskow R, Yang L, Gokcumen O, Haseley P, Luquette LJ, 3rd , Lohr JG, et al. 2012. Landscape of somatic retrotransposition in human cancers. Science 337:967–971. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  71. Lein E, Borm LE, Linnarsson S. 2017. The promise of spatial transcriptomics for neuroscience in the era of molecular cell typing. Science 358:64–69. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  72. Leon E, Zou YS, Milunsky JM. 2010. Mosaic Down syndrome in a patient with low‐level mosaicism detected by microarray. Am J Med Genet A 152A:3154–3156. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  73. Letourneau A, Santoni FA, Bonilla X, Sailani MR, Gonzalez D, Kind J, Chevalier C, et al. 2014. Domains of genome‐wide gene expression dysregulation in Down's syndrome. Nature 508:345–350. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  74. Li J, Ishii T, Feinstein P, Mombaerts P. 2004. Odorant receptor gene choice is reset by nuclear transfer from mouse olfactory sensory neurons. Nature 428:393–399. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  75. Liyanage M, Coleman A, Du Manoir S, Veldman T, Mccormack S, Dickson RB, Barlow C, et al. 1996. Multicolour spectral karyotyping of mouse chromosomes. Nat Genet 14:312–315. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  76. Lodato MA, Rodin RE, Bohrson CL, Coulter ME, Barton AR, Kwon M, Sherman MA, et al. 2018. Aging and neurodegeneration are associated with increased mutations in single human neurons. Science 359:555–559. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  77. Lodato MA, Woodworth MB, Lee S, Evrony GD, Mehta BK, Karger A, Lee S, et al. 2015. Somatic mutation in single human neurons tracks developmental and transcriptional history. Science 350:94–98. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  78. Macville M, Veldman T, Padilla‐Nash H, Wangsa D, O'brien P, Schrock E, Ried T. 1997. Spectral karyotyping, a 24‐colour FISH technique for the identification of chromosomal rearrangements. Histochem Cell Biol 108:299–305. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  79. Matsuoka M, Nagawa F, Okazaki K, Kingsbury L, Yoshida K, Muller U, Larue DT, et al. 1991. Detection of somatic DNA recombination in the transgenic mouse brain. Science 254:81–86. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  80. Mccarty M. 1995. A fifty‐year perspective on the genetic role of DNA. Ann N Y Acad Sci 758:48–54. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  81. Mccarty M. 2003. Discovering genes are made of DNA. Nature 421:406. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  82. Mcconnell MJ, Kaushal D, Yang AH, Kingsbury MA, Rehen SK, Treuner K, Helton R, et al. 2004. Failed clearance of aneuploid embryonic neural progenitor cells leads to excess aneuploidy in the Atm‐deficient but not the Trp53‐deficient adult cerebral cortex. J Neurosci 24:8090–8096. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  83. Mcconnell MJ, Lindberg MR, Brennand KJ, Piper JC, Voet T, Cowing‐Zitron C, Shumilina S, et al. 2013. Mosaic copy number variation in human neurons. Science 342:632–637. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  84. Mcconnell MJ, Macmillan HR, Chun J. 2009. Mathematical modeling supports substantial mouse neural progenitor cell death. Neural Dev 4:28. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  85. Mita P, Wudzinska A, Sun X, Andrade J, Nayak S, Kahler DJ, Badri S, et al. 2018. LINE‐1 protein localization and functional dynamics during the cell cycle. Elife 7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  86. Modi D, Berde P, Bhartiya D. 2003. Down syndrome: A study of chromosomal mosaicism. Reprod Biomed Online 6:499–503. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  87. Mosch B, Morawski M, Mittag A, Lenz D, Tarnok A, Arendt T. 2007. Aneuploidy and DNA replication in the normal human brain and Alzheimer's disease. J Neurosci 27:6859–6867. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  88. Muotri AR, Chu VT, Marchetto MC, Deng W, Moran JV, Gage FH. 2005. Somatic mosaicism in neuronal precursor cells mediated by L1 retrotransposition. Nature 435:903–910. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  89. Muotri AR, Marchetto MC, Coufal NG, Oefner R, Yeo G, Nakashima K, Gage FH. 2010. L1 retrotransposition in neurons is modulated by MeCP2. Nature 468:443–446. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  90. Muotri AR, Zhao C, Marchetto MC, Gage FH. 2009. Environmental influence on L1 retrotransposons in the adult hippocampus. Hippocampus 19:1002–1007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  91. Naegeli C, W. 1844. Zellenkern, Zellenbildung und Zellenwachstum bei den Pflanzen. Zeitschrift fur wissenschaftliche Botanik 1:35–133. [Google Scholar]
  92. Odegard VH, Schatz DG. 2006. Targeting of somatic hypermutation. Nat Rev Immunol 6:573–583. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  93. Osada T, Kusakabe H, Akutsu H, Yagi T, Yanagimachi R. 2002. Adult murine neurons: Their chromatin and chromosome changes and failure to support embryonic development as revealed by nuclear transfer. Cytogenet Genome Res 97:7–12. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  94. Pack SD, Weil RJ, Vortmeyer AO, Zeng W, Li J, Okamoto H, Furuta M, et al. 2005. Individual adult human neurons display aneuploidy: Detection by fluorescence in situ hybridization and single neuron PCR. Cell Cycle 4:1758–1760. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  95. Packer AI, Manova K, Bachvarova RF. 1993. A discrete LINE‐1 transcript in mouse blastocysts. Dev Biol 157:281–283. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  96. Paweletz N. 2001. Walther Flemming: Pioneer of mitosis research. Nat Rev Mol Cell Biol 2:72–75. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  97. Peterson SE, Yang AH, Bushman DM, Westra JW, Yung YC, Barral S, et al. 2012. Aneuploid cells are differentially susceptible to caspase‐mediated death during embryonic cerebral cortical development. J Neurosci 32:16213–16222. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  98. Pinkel D, Gray JW, Trask B, Van Den Engh G, Fuscoe J, Van Dekken H. 1986. Cytogenetic analysis by in situ hybridization with fluorescently labeled nucleic acid probes. Cold Spring Harb Symp Quant Biol 51 Pt 1:151–157. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  99. Pompeiano M, Blaschke AJ, Flavell RA, Srinivasan A, Chun J. 2000. Decreased apoptosis in proliferative and postmitotic regions of the Caspase 3‐deficient embryonic central nervous system. J Comp Neurol 423:1–12. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  100. Pompeiano M, Hvala M, Chun J. 1998. Onset of apoptotic DNA fragmentation can precede cell elimination by days in the small intestinal villus. Cell Death Differ 5:702–709. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  101. Potter H. 1991. Review and hypothesis: Alzheimer disease and Down syndrome–chromosome 21 nondisjunction may underlie both disorders. Am J Hum Genet 48:1192–1200. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  102. Poulin JF, Tasic B, Hjerling‐Leffler J, Trimarchi JM, Awatramani R. 2016. Disentangling neural cell diversity using single‐cell transcriptomics. Nat Neurosci 19:1131–1141. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  103. Rajendran RS, Zupanc MM, Losche A, Westra J, Chun J, Zupanc GK. 2007. Numerical chromosome variation and mitotic segregation defects in the adult brain of teleost fish. Dev Neurobiol 67:1334–1347. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  104. Regev A, Teichmann SA, Lander ES, Amit I, Benoist C, Birney E, Bodenmiller B, et al. 2017. The human cell Atlas. Elife 6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  105. Rehen SK, Mcconnell MJ, Kaushal D, Kingsbury MA, Yang AH, Chun J. 2001. Chromosomal variation in neurons of the developing and adult mammalian nervous system. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 98:13361–13366. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  106. Rehen SK, Yung YC, Mccreight MP, Kaushal D, Yang AH, Almeida BS, Kingsbury MA, et al. 2005. Constitutional aneuploidy in the normal human brain. J Neurosci 25:2176–2180. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  107. Rosenberg AB, Roco CM, Muscat RA, Kuchina A, Sample P, Yao Z, Graybuck LT, Peeler DJ, et al. 2018. Single‐cell profiling of the developing mouse brain and spinal cord with split‐pool barcoding. Science 360:176–182. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  108. Sala Frigerio C, Lau P, Troakes C, Deramecourt V, Gele P, Van Loo P, et al. 2015. On the identification of low allele frequency mosaic mutations in the brains of Alzheimer's disease patients. Alzheimers Dement 11:1265–1276. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  109. Santaguida S, Amon A. 2015. Short‐ and long‐term effects of chromosome mis‐segregation and aneuploidy. Nat Rev Mol Cell Biol 16:473–485. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  110. Schatz DG, Baltimore D. 1988. Stable expression of immunoglobulin gene V(D)J receombinase activity by gene transfer into 3T3 fibroblasts. Cell 53:107–115. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  111. Schatz DG, Chun JJ. 1992. V(D)J recombination and the transgenic brain blues. New Biol 4:188–196. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  112. Schatz DG, Oettinger MA, Baltimore D. 1989. The V(D)J recombination activating gene (RAG‐1). Cell 59:1035–1048. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  113. Schleicher W. 1879. Die Knorpelzellteilung: Ein beitrag zur lehre der teilung von gewebszellen. Arch Mikrosk Anat 16:248–300. [Google Scholar]
  114. Schlissel M, Constantinescu A, Morrow T, Baxter M, Peng A. 1993. Double‐strand signal sequence breaks in V(D)J recombination are blunt, 5'‐phosphorylated, RAG‐dependent, and cell cycle regulated. Genes Dev 7:2520–2532. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  115. Shelbourne PF, Keller‐Mcgandy C, Bi WL, Yoon SR, Dubeau L, Veitch NJ, Vonsattel JP, et al. 2007. Triplet repeat mutation length gains correlate with cell‐type specific vulnerability in Huntington disease brain. Hum Mol Genet 16:1133–1142. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  116. Sheltzer JM, Torres EM, Dunham MJ, Amon A. 2012. Transcriptional consequences of aneuploidy. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 109:12644–12649. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  117. Shi X, Seluanov A, Gorbunova V. 2007. Cell divisions are required for L1 retrotransposition. Mol Cell Biol 27:1264–1270. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  118. Shlomchik MJ, Weisel F. 2012. Germinal center selection and the development of memory B and plasma cells. Immunol Rev 247:52–63. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  119. Singer T, Mcconnell MJ, Marchetto MC, Coufal NG, Gage FH. 2010. LINE‐1 retrotransposons: Mediators of somatic variation in neuronal genomes?. Trends Neurosci 33:345–354. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  120. Skene PJ, Illingworth RS, Webb S, Kerr AR, James KD, Turner DJ, Andrews R, et al. 2010. Neuronal MeCP2 is expressed at near histone‐octamer levels and globally alters the chromatin state. Mol Cell 37:457–468. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  121. Sousa AMM, Zhu Y, Raghanti MA, Kitchen RR, Onorati M, Tebbenkamp ATN, Stutz B, et al. 2017. Molecular and cellular reorganization of neural circuits in the human lineage. Science 358:1027–1032. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  122. Staley K, Blaschke AJ, Chun J. 1997. Apoptotic DNA fragmentation is detected by a semi‐quantitative ligation‐mediated PCR of blunt DNA ends. Cell Death Differ 4:66–75. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  123. Stritesky GL, Jameson SC, Hogquist KA. 2012. Selection of self‐reactive T cells in the thymus. Annu Rev Immunol 30:95–114. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  124. Surh CD, Sprent J. 1994. T‐cell apoptosis detected in situ during positive and negative selection in the thymus. Nature 372:100–103. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  125. Tackholm G. 1922. Zytologische Studien uber die Gattung Rosa. Acta Hort. Berg 7:97–381. [Google Scholar]
  126. Tasic B, Menon V, Nguyen TN, Kim TK, Jarsky T, Yao Z, Levi B, et al. 2016. Adult mouse cortical cell taxonomy revealed by single cell transcriptomics. Nat Neurosci 19:335–346. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  127. Telenius H, Kremer B, Goldberg YP, Theilmann J, Andrew SE, Zeisler J, Adam S, et al. 1994. Somatic and gonadal mosaicism of the Huntington disease gene CAG repeat in brain and sperm. Nat Genet 6:409–414. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  128. Thacker J, Zdzienicka MZ. 2004. The XRCC genes: Expanding roles in DNA double‐strand break repair. DNA Repair (Amst) 3:1081–1090. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  129. Tjio J‐H, Levan A. 1956. The chromosome number of man. Hereditas 42:1–6. [Google Scholar]
  130. Ueno S, Kondoh K, Kotani Y, Komure O, Kuno S, Kawai J, Hazama F, et al. 1995. Somatic mosaicism of CAG repeat in dentatorubral‐pallidoluysian atrophy (DRPLA). Hum Mol Genet 4:663–666. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  131. Upton KR, Gerhardt DJ, Jesuadian JS, Richardson SR, Sanchez‐Luque FJ, Bodea Go, et al. 2015. Ubiquitous L1 mosaicism in hippocampal neurons. Cell 161:228–239. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  132. Usoskin D, Furlan A, Islam S, Abdo H, Lonnerberg P, Lou D, Hjerling‐Leffler J, et al. 2015. Unbiased classification of sensory neuron types by large‐scale single‐cell RNA sequencing. Nat Neurosci 18:145–153. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  133. Van Den Bos H, Spierings DC, Taudt AS, Bakker B, Porubsky D, Falconer E, Novoa C, et al. 2016. Single‐cell whole genome sequencing reveals no evidence for common aneuploidy in normal and Alzheimer's disease neurons. Genome Biol 17:116. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  134. Varmus H. 1982. Form and Function of Retroviral Proviruses. Science 216:812–820. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  135. Viollet S, Monot C, Cristofari G. 2014. L1 retrotransposition: The snap‐velcro model and its consequences. Mob Genet Elements 4:e28907. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  136. Voyvodic JT. 1996. Cell death in cortical development: How much? Why? So what?. Neuron 16: [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  137. Waldeyer HW. 1888. Uber Karyokinese und ihre Beziehungen zu den Befruchtungsvorgangen. Arch. Mikrosk Anat 32:1–32. [Google Scholar]
  138. Warburton D, Anyane‐Yeboa K, Taterka P, Yu Cy, Olsen D. 1991. Mosaic variegated aneuploidy with microcephaly: A new human mitotic mutant?. Ann Genet 34:287–292. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  139. Watson JD, Crick FH. 1953a. Genetical implications of the structure of deoxyribonucleic acid. Nature 171:964–967. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  140. Watson JD, Crick FH. 1953b. Molecular structure of nucleic acids; a structure for deoxyribose nucleic acid. Nature 171:737–738. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  141. Wei PC, Chang AN, Kao J, Du Z, Meyers RM, Alt FW, Schwer B. 2016. Long neural genes harbor recurrent DNA break clusters in neural stem/progenitor cells. Cell. 164:644–655. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  142. Westra JW, Barral S, Chun J. 2009. A reevaluation of tetraploidy in the Alzheimer's disease brain. Neurodegener Dis 6:221–229. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  143. Westra JW, Peterson SE, Yung YC, Mutoh T, Barral S, Chun J. 2008. Aneuploid mosaicism in the developing and adult cerebellar cortex. J Comp Neurol 507:1944–1951. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  144. Westra JW, Rivera RR, Bushman DM, Yung YC, Peterson SE, Barral S, Chun J. 2010. Neuronal DNA content variation (DCV) with regional and individual differences in the human brain. J Comp Neurol 518:3981–4000. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  145. Wiseman FK, Al‐Janabi T, Hardy J, Karmiloff‐Smith A, Nizetic D, Tybulewicz VL, Fisher EM, et al. 2015. A genetic cause of Alzheimer disease: Mechanistic insights from Down syndrome. Nat Rev Neurosci 16:564–574. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  146. Wolffe AP, Matzke MA. 1999. Epigenetics: Regulation through repression. Science 286:481–486. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  147. Wu Q, Maniatis T. 1999. A striking organization of a large family of human neural cadherin‐like cell adhesion genes. Cell 97:779–790. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  148. Xu Z, Zan H, Pone EJ, Mai T, Casali P. 2012. Immunoglobulin class‐switch DNA recombination: Induction, targeting and beyond. Nat Rev Immunol 12:517–531. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  149. Yang AH, Kaushal D, Rehen SK, Kriedt K, Kingsbury MA, Mcconnell MJ, Chun J. 2003. Chromosome segregation defects contribute to aneuploidy in normal neural progenitor cells. J Neurosci 23:10454–10462. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  150. Yang Y, Geldmacher DS, Herrup K. 2001. DNA replication precedes neuronal cell death in Alzheimer's disease. J Neurosci 21:2661–2668. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  151. Yung YC, Kennedy G, Chun J. 2009. Identification of neural programmed cell death through the detection of DNA fragmentation in situ and by PCR. Curr Protoc Neurosci Chapter 3, Unit 3 8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  152. Yurov YB, Iourov IY, Monakhov VV, Soloviev IV, Vostrikov VM, Vorsanova SG. 2005. The variation of aneuploidy frequency in the developing and adult human brain revealed by an interphase FISH study. J Histochem Cytochem 53:385–390. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  153. Yurov YB, Iourov IY, Vorsanova SG, Demidova IA, Kravetz VS, Beresheva AK, Kolotii AD, et al. 2008. The schizophrenia brain exhibits low‐level aneuploidy involving chromosome 1. Schizophr Res 98:139–147. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  154. Yurov YB, Iourov IY, Vorsanova SG, Liehr T, Kolotii AD, Kutsev SI, Pellestor F, et al. 2007a. Aneuploidy and confined chromosomal mosaicism in the developing human brain. PLoS One 2:e558. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  155. Yurov YB, Vorsanova SG, Iourov IY, Demidova IA, Beresheva AK, Kravetz VS, Monakhov VV, et al. 2007b. Unexplained autism is frequently associated with low‐level mosaic aneuploidy. J Med Genet 44:521–525. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  156. Yurov YB, Vorsanova SG, Liehr T, Kolotii AD, Iourov IY. 2014. X chromosome aneuploidy in the Alzheimer's disease brain. Mol Cytogenet 7:20. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  157. Yurov YB, Vostrikov VM, Vorsanova SG, Monakhov VV, Iourov IY. 2001. Multicolor fluorescent in situ hybridization on post‐mortem brain in schizophrenia as an approach for identification of low‐level chromosomal aneuploidy in neuropsychiatric diseases. Brain Dev 23(Suppl 1):S186–S190. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  158. Zeisel A, Munoz‐Manchado AB, Codeluppi S, Lonnerberg P, La Manno G, Jureus A, Marques S, et al. 2015. Brain structure. Cell types in the mouse cortex and hippocampus revealed by single‐cell RNA‐seq. Science 347:1138–1142. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  159. Zhang CZ, Spektor A, Cornils H, Francis JM, Jackson EK, Liu S, Meyerson M, et al. 2015. Chromothripsis from DNA damage in micronuclei. Nature 522:179–184. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  160. Zhao K, Du, J , Han, X , Goodier JL, Li, P , Zhou X, Wei W, et al. 2013. Modulation of LINE‐1 and Alu/SVA retrotransposition by Aicardi‐Goutieres syndrome‐related SAMHD1. Cell Rep 4:1108–1115. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Articles from Developmental Neurobiology are provided here courtesy of Wiley

RESOURCES