Abstract
Background: Cognitive and psychosocial dysfunction are prevalent and disabling features of Major Depressive Disorder (MDD). Emerging evidence suggests that poor cognitive functioning mediates the negative effect of MDD on psychosocial function. However, there is a lack of research examining the domain-specific nature of this relationship. The current study evaluated whether the relationship between MDD and specific psychosocial subdomains (e.g., autonomy, occupational functioning) was mediated by domain-specific cognitive deficits.
Methods: Data from 155 participants was obtained from the Cognitive Function and Mood Study (CoFaMS), a cross-sectional analysis of mood, cognition, social cognition, and functioning in individuals with MDD. Cognitive functioning was assessed (Current MDD n = 45, Healthy n = 110), with the Repeatable Battery for the Assessment of Neuropsychological Status (RBANS), the Colorado Assessment Tests (CATs), and the Psychology Experiment Building Language (PEBL). Psychosocial functioning was clinically evaluated with the Functioning Assessment Short Test (FAST).
Results: The results indicated that spatial cognition and executive functioning partially mediated the negative effect of MDD on overall psychosocial functioning, autonomy, and subjective cognition. In contrast, spatial and executive domains showed divergent mediation patterns on interpersonal relationships and leisure time.
Conclusions: The findings suggest that executive and spatial cognition play an important role in the pathology of overall psychosocial functioning, and specific functional issues in MDD. Treatments targeting psychosocial recovery in MDD may be improved by emphasizing executive and spatial cognitive remediation.
Keywords: depression, MDD, cognition, spatial cognition, mediation, executive function, psychosocial functioning
Introduction
While major depressive disorder is characterized by impaired mood, psychosocial deficits are increasingly recognized as a core symptom of the illness (1, 2). Functional deficits have been identified in domains of occupational functioning, daily responsibilities, interpersonal relationships, financial management and self-perceived quality of life (1, 3–6). While individually debilitating, functional deficits also contribute substantially to the social and economic burden of disease (7, 8), with billions of dollars lost annually to functional issues related to MDD (9, 10). In addition, psychosocial deficits are frequently maintained despite improvement and remittance of mood symptoms (4, 11), and are associated with illness relapse and poor long-term recovery (1). The chronicity and clinical impact of psychosocial impairment highlights the need for an improved understanding of psychosocial dysfunction in MDD.
Emerging research suggests that cognitive deficits contribute independently to the development and maintenance of psychosocial dysfunction acutely depressed patients (6, 12–16). Cognitive dysfunction has been identified in domains of executive functioning, attention, working memory, learning, processing speed, and spatial cognition, with deficits in several domains linked to functional outcome (4, 16, 17). Cognitive impairment is typically greater in older MDD patients (18, 19), and in those with more severe depression (20), with recent evidence suggesting that cognitive deficits enhance functional issues in MDD (21). For example, research by Xiang et al. (21) identified that individuals with cognitive dysfunction and MDD reported poorer psychosocial functioning than individuals with cognitive dysfunction or MDD in isolation. Like psychosocial issues, cognitive deficits are frequently retained during the remitted stage of illness (22–24), and are associated with illness relapse (1, 25). The overlapping profile of psychosocial and cognitive dysfunction in MDD provides further support for the link between these dimensions of illness, and highlights the need for further investigation into cognitive dysfunction as a mechanism for functional difficulties in MDD.
A small body of research has identified that cognitive factors mediate the relationship between MDD and psychosocial functioning, suggesting that cognition plays an important role in the pathology of functional deficits (3, 11, 26–28). For example, Buist-Bouwman et al. (3) found that attention partially mediated the negative effect of MDD on role functioning (i.e., self-perceived ability to fulfill one's occupational and daily role) as reported by the International Classification of Functioning, Disability, and Health (ICF) (29). In addition, Kiosses and Alexopoulos (28) found that the relationship between MDD and impairments in daily living [i.e., Instrumental Activities of Daily Living (IADL)] was mediated by deficits in executive functioning. Taken together, these findings suggests that MDD leads to deficits across a number of cognitive domains (e.g., attention, executive functioning), and that these deficits in turn negatively impact psychosocial functioning. While the studied by Buist-Bouwman et al. (3) and Kiosses and Alexopoulos indicate a general mediation effect on overall (i.e., global) psychosocial functioning, there is a lack of research on which specific domains of psychosocial functioning are mediated by cognitive deficits in currently depressed patients. The IADL and ICF scales utilized in the above studies combine daily responsibilities (e.g., food preparation) and occupational functioning in a single composite indication of function, and as such do not differentiate between specific functional issues. Accordingly, it is not possible to conclude which domains of psychosocial functioning are negatively affected by cognitive dysfunction in MDD. Identification of domain-specific relationships in crucial, as adjunctive cognitive treatment should target cognitive domains which impact directly on patient functioning (2, 30).
The present research addresses the above gap in our understanding by evaluating the role of cognitive domains (e.g., executive functioning, attention, immediate memory) as mediators of specific psychosocial issues (e.g., autonomy, financial issues, interpersonal relationships). It was expected that cognitive deficits would mediate the negative effect of current MDD on psychosocial functioning. However, the limited research in this area did not allow domain-specific hypotheses regarding the effect of cognitive domains on specific psychosocial issues. Cognitive and psychosocial functioning was assessed in healthy, as well as acutely depressed patients, enabling detection of the MDD-Cognition-Psychosocial functioning relationship, illustrated in Figure 1. The primary aims of the present study were:
To evaluate whether the negative effect of a current episode of MDD on overall psychosocial functioning was mediated by performance in specific cognitive domains (i.e., executive functioning, attention, immediate memory, delayed memory, spatial cognition, semantic fluency).
To determine whether specific cognitive domains mediate the negative effect of a current episode of MDD on particular psychosocial issues (i.e., autonomy, occupational functioning, interpersonal relationships, financial issues, subjective cognitive dysfunction, leisure time).
Method
Participant data (N = 155) for the current study was obtained from the Cognitive Function and Mood study (CoFaMS) (17); a cross-sectional analysis of cognition, mood, social cognition, and psychosocial functioning in persons with MDD. Participants were included on the basis of a current episode or previous diagnosis of depression following DSM-IV-TR criteria (31). Exclusion criteria were presence of a psychotic disorder, dementia, learning disorder, autism spectrum disorder, or other illnesses which can cause cognitive issues (e.g., brain tumor). Participants were selected from the CoFaMS study on the basis of completing standard assessments of cognitive and psychosocial functioning.
Cognitive assessment
Cognitive assessments included the Repeatable Battery for the Assessment of Neuropsychological status (i.e., RBANS) (32), the Colorado Assessment Tests (i.e., CATs) (33), and cognitive tests in the Psychology Experiment Building Language (i.e., PEBL) (34). Psychosocial functioning was assessed with the Functioning Assessment Short Test (i.e., FAST) (35). Subjects were screened for mental illness with the MINI600 Neuropsychiatric Diagnostic Interview (36), as well a background medical and psychiatric history questionnaire. Participants with acute MDD were included (n = 45), as well as participants free of psychiatric illness (n = 110) [i.e., healthy controls (HCs)]. Individuals were defined as HCs if they had not experienced any of the 17 common illnesses included in the MINI600 (e.g., OCD, Anxiety disorders), and demonstrated HAM-D scores below 7 (37). Of those with current MDD, mean duration of illness prior to the study was 18 years. No age limit was included in CoFaMS, however all participants were at least 18 years of age. The mean age of participants was 36.29 (SD = 16.07), 61% were female, and mean years of education was 13.62 years (SD = 3.11). Student's t-tests and a chi-squared found identified no differences in age, sex, or years of education between the current MDD and HCs (ps > 0.05). Further demographic information stratified by MDD status (current MDD, HCs) is presented in Supplementary Table 1 in the Appendix.
Domain-specific cognitive functioning was obtained with RBANS, CATS, and PEBL test batteries. The RBANS is a neuropsychiatric screening tool designed as a brief assessment of cognitive performance in individuals with mental ilness (32). The current study employed the RBANS digit span, list learning, list recognition, store memory, figure recall, picture naming, figure copy, and line orientation tasks. Cognitive tests from the CATs battery included the n-back, visual span, word recall, tower of london, and Wisconsin card sorting tests (33). The PEBL is a freely available software package which includes many computerized cognitive assessments (e.g., Corsi blocks, mental rotation). The PEBL Sroop task and Wisconsin card sorting task1 were included in the current study. Domain-specific composite z-scores were derived from performance in the RBANS, CATS, and PEBL cognitive assessments, resulting in composite domains of executive functioning, attention, immediate memory, delayed memory, semantic fluency, and spatial cognition (15). The derivation of domain-specific composites by cognitive test scores is illustrated in Table 1 [see also Knight et al. (23)].
Table 1.
Cognitive domain | Cognitive tests | ||
---|---|---|---|
RBANS | CATS | PEBL | |
Executive functioning | Tower of London (excess moves total) Wisconsin card sorting test (perseverative responses) |
Stroop (incongruency errors) Tower of London (Total excess moves) Wisconsin card Sorting test (perseverative responses) |
|
Attention | Digit span Coding |
VSPANa (total average time) NBACK (accuracy) |
Stroop (incongruency errors) |
Immediate memory | List learning Store memory Digit span |
Word Recall (total words recalled) VSPAN (correct trials forward) VSPAN (correct trials backward) |
|
Delayed memory | List recognition List recall Story recall Figure recall |
Word recall (primacy total) | |
Semantic fluency | Picture naming Semantic fluency |
Word recall (total words recalled) | |
Spatial cognition | Figure copy Line orientation |
Tower of London (percentage above optimal) |
Vspan, Verbal memory span.
Assessment of functioning
Psychosocial functioning was assessed with the FAST, a clinician administered interview in which dysfunction is evaluated in six domains (i.e., autonomy, occupational functioning, cognitive functioning, financial issues, leisure time, and interpersonal relationships) (35). Impairment is measured by the interviewer on a four-point scale, with 0 indicating no difficulty and 3 severe impairment. Six composite FAST ratings were derived from the mean score of impairment in each functional subdomain [i.e., autonomy (Range: 0–12), occupational dysfunction (Range: 0–15), subjective cognitive dysfunction (Range: 0–15), financial issues (Range: 0–6), interpersonal relationships (Range: 0–18), and leisure time (Range: 0–6)]. Overall psychosocial functioning (i.e., FAST total score) was indicated by the sum of FAST subdomains (Range: 0–72). FAST total score and FAST subdomain composites were used as dependent variables in separate multiple regression analyses.
Statistical analyses
Analyses were conducted with SPSS for windows, version 24. Regression analyses were performed to test whether the relationship between MDD status (Current MDD, HCs) and overall psychosocial functioning was mediated by domain-specific cognitive performance. Potential cognitive mediators were identified following the procedure established by Baron and Kenny (38), in which three regression equations are used to derive three criteria for mediation. In the first regression equation, the mediator (i.e., domain-specific cognition) was regressed on the IV (i.e., MDD status), in the second equation the DV (i.e., FAST total score) was regressed on the IV, while in the third equation the DV is regressed on the IV and mediator. To establish mediation three criteria must be met; (1) the IV must be significantly associated with the mediator in the first equation, (2) the IV significantly affects the DV in the second equation, and (3) the mediator must influence the DV in the third equation. If these relationships hold in the expected direction2 then the effect of the IV on the DV in model 3 must be reduced relative to model 2. Complete mediation occurs if the effect of the IV on the DV in equation 3 becomes non-significant with the inclusion of the mediator. Partial mediation occurs if the effect of the IV on the DV in equation 3 is reduced but remains statistically significant.
The procedure above for testing mediation was repeated six times, substituting as mediators each of the six composite cognitive domain z-scores (i.e., of executive functioning, attention, immediate memory, delayed memory, semantic fluency, and spatial cognition). This procedure enabled detection of whether specific cognitive domains mediated the effect of MDD status on overall psychosocial function (i.e., FAST total). If criteria (1) and (3) were satisfied for the included cognitive mediator, then this cognitive domain was further employed in subsequent analyses of FAST subdomains (i.e., FAST autonomy, occupational functioning, interpersonal relationships, leisure time, financial issues, subjective cognitive dysfunction). In contrast, those cognitive domains which did not satisfy criteria (1) and (3) did not proceed to psychosocial-subdomain specific mediation tests. Age, sex, and years of education were entered as covariates, as these factors can affect the relationship between cognitive and psychosocial functioning (4, 39).
Results
Mediation criteria–fast total score
Regression analysis revealed that mediation criteria (2) was satisfied for analysis of FAST total score, demonstrating a significant effect of MDD status (current MDD coded 1, HCs coded 0) on FAST total score (p < 0.001). The results indicated that spatial cognition satisfied criteria (1) and (3), with spatial cognition negatively associated with MDD status (p = 0.028) and FAST total score (p = 0.031). Mediation criteria (1) and (3) approached significance for executive functioning, such that executive cognition was associated with MDD status (p = 0.054) and FAST total score (p = 0.063). Mediation analyses for spatial cognition and executive functioning are reported separately below. For all remaining composite cognitive domains (i.e., attention, immediate memory, delayed memory, semantic fluency), either criteria (1) or criteria (3) did not approach significance. Inferential statistics for mediation criteria (1), (2), and (3) are reported in Table 2.
Table 2.
Cognitive domains | Mediation criteria | Change in R2 owing to mediator | |||
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Criteria (1) | Criteria (2) | Criteria (3) | Equation 2 R2 | Equation 3 R2 | |
Executive functioning | 0.054 | <0.001 | 0.063 | 0.240 | 0.225 |
Attention | 0.445 | <0.001 | 0.002 | NA | NA |
Immediate memory | 0.449 | <0.001 | 0.004 | NA | NA |
Delayed memory | 0.465 | <0.001 | 0.004 | NA | NA |
Semantic fluency | 0.065 | <0.001 | 0.116 | NA | NA |
Spatial cognition | 0.028 | <0.001 | 0.031 | 0.240 | 0.229 |
NA, Not applicable (no mediation); Criteria (1), IV associated with mediator; Criteria (2), IV associated with DV; Criteria (3), Mediator associated with DV. Linear regression model adjusted for age, gender, and years of education.
Spatial cognition
Before entering spatial cognition (i.e., equation 2), MDD status explained a significant 24% of variance in FAST total score (R2 = 0.24, p < 0.001). After controlling for the contribution of spatial cognition in the third equation, MDD status explained a lower, but still significant, 22.9% of variance in FAST total score (R2 = 0.229, p < 0.001), indicating that spatial cognition partially mediated the effect of MDD status on FAST total score. Subsequent tests using FAST subdomains as dependent variables, rather than FAST total score, indicated that spatial cognition satisfied criteria (1), (2), and (3) in subdomains of interpersonal relationships, subjective cognitive dysfunction, and autonomy3 (see Table 3). For interpersonal relationships, variance explained by MDD status was reduced from 28 to 26.4% by the inclusion of spatial cognition in equation three. The variance in subjective cognition explained by MDD status was reduced from 24.3 to 23.4% by the inclusion of spatial cognition, while the variance explained in autonomy by MDD status was reduced from 17.9 to 17.1%. In all analyses, the direct effect of MDD status on FAST subdomains remained significant (p < 0.001) after controlling for spatial cognition, indicating partial mediation.
Table 3.
FAST subdomains | Mediation criteria | Change in R2 owing to mediator | |||
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Criteria (1) | Criteria (2) | Criteria (3) | Equation 2 R2 | Equation 3 R2 | |
Autonomy | 0.028 | <0.001 | 0.068 | 0.179 | 0.171 |
Occupational functioning | 0.028 | <0.001 | 0.200 | NA | NA |
Subjective cognition | 0.028 | <0.001 | 0.028 | 0.243 | 0.234 |
Leisure time | 0.028 | <0.001 | 0.103 | NA | NA |
Financial issues | 0.028 | 0.319 | 0.103 | NA | NA |
Interpersonal relationships | 0.028 | <0.001 | 0.014 | 0.280 | 0.264 |
NA, Not applicable (no mediation); Criteria (1), IV associated with mediator; Criteria (2), IV associated with DV; Criteria (3), Mediator associated with DV. Linear regression model adjusted for age, gender, and years of education.
Executive functioning
For analysis of FAST total score, including executive functioning in the third equation reduced the variance explained by MDD status (current MDD, HCs) from 24 to 22.5%. However, the direct effect of MDD status remained significant (p < 0.001) in equation 3, indicating partial mediation by executive functioning. Following regression analyses with FAST subdomains as DVs demonstrated that criteria (1), (2), and (3) were met for autonomy, subjective cognitive dysfunction, and leisure time (see Table 4). Analysis of autonomy indicated that MDD status explained 17.9% variance in equation 2, while the inclusion of spatial cognition as a mediator in equation 3 reduced variance explained by MDD status to 16.3%. MDD status explained 24.2% of the variance in subjective cognitive dysfunction in equation 2, whereas a lower 22.4% of variance was explained by MDD status after controlling for executive functioning in equation 3. Variance in leisure time explained by MDD status was reduced from 15% in equation 2 to 13.2% by the inclusion of executive functioning in equation 3. The direct effect of MDD status on FAST subdomains remained significant in equation 3 in all analyses (p < 0.001), indicating partial mediation of autonomy, subjective cognitive dysfunction, and leisure time by executive dysfunction.
Table 4.
FAST subdomains | Mediation criteria | Change in R2 owing to mediator | |||
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Criteria (1) | Criteria (2) | Criteria (3) | Equation 2 R2 | Equation 3 R2 | |
Autonomy | 0.054 | < 0.001 | 0.023 | 0.179 | 0.163 |
Occupational functioning | 0.054 | < 0.001 | 0.471 | NA | NA |
Subjective cognition | 0.054 | < 0.001 | 0.049 | 0.242 | 0.224 |
Leisure time | 0.054 | < 0.001 | 0.018 | 0.150 | 0.132 |
Financial issues | 0.054 | 0.319 | 0.015 | NA | NA |
Interpersonal relationships | 0.054 | < 0.001 | 0.152 | NA | NA |
NA, Not applicable (no mediation), Criteria (1), IV associated with mediator; Criteria (2), IV associated with DV; Criteria (3), Mediator associated with DV. Linear regression model adjusted for age, gender, and years of education.
Discussion
The current study examined whether the detrimental effect of a current episode of MDD on psychosocial functioning was mediated by deficits in cognitive functioning. Analyses of overall psychosocial functioning (i.e., FAST total) indicated that spatial cognition and executive functioning partially mediated the negative effect of current MDD on psychosocial functioning. In contrast, attention, immediate memory, delayed memory, and semantic fluency did not mediate psychosocial functioning. Follow-up analyses with spatial cognition and executive functioning as mediators of specific psychosocial issues (e.g., autonomy) indicated overlapping patterns of mediation in domains of autonomy and subjective cognition. In contrast, only spatial cognition mediated deficits in interpersonal relationships, while executive functioning alone mediated issues in leisure time. These findings provide new evidence that executive and spatial cognition play an important role in the pathology of specific psychosocial issues in current MDD.
To our knowledge, these results are the first to demonstrate that specific psychosocial issues, as opposed to overall functioning, are negatively affected by specific cognitive domains (3, 28). The overlapping mediation patterns in autonomy and subjective cognition suggest that these functional domains may be more sensitive to objective cognitive deficits than other functional issues (e.g., financial management), which may be primarily affected by other MDD symptoms (e.g., pessimism, social isolation). Given the primacy of autonomy and subjective cognition in the current results, it is worthwhile discussing the contribution of executive and spatial cognitive domains to these issues.
The role of executive functioning in autonomy is likely explained by the broad application of executive functions in a number of behavioral and cognitive abilities (e.g., forward planning, problem solving) (2, 22, 23), which have been shown to contribute to taking responsibility for daily duties, and maintaining functional independence (e.g., hygiene, grocery shopping) (19, 35). Losses in spatial cognition may negatively affect autonomy by increasing the probability of spatial errors and oversights (e.g., disorientation, driving errors), which could negatively affect self-confidence in maintaining autonomy, particularly in acutely depressed patients who are typically hypersensitive internalization of errors (40). Similarly, the negative effect of executive and spatial cognition on subjective cognitive dysfunction may be the result increased cognitive failures (e.g., missing appointments, forgetting names, failure to solve problems), which are associated with poor cognitive functioning (41) and greater self-perception of cognitive dysfunction (42).
It is noteworthy that deficits in spatial cognition, but not executive functioning, mediated deficits in interpersonal relationships. While deficits in spatial cognition have been noted in MDD (12), and in global functioning (43), their contribution to the pathology of deficits in interpersonal issues has not previously been identified. It is possible that spatial cognitive skills contribute to several indirect features of social interaction (e.g., arriving on time, personal space, physical contact). While these indirect spatial factors are a plausible mechanism for interpersonal issues in current MDD, spatial cognition may also be reflective of more complex cognitive processes related to social abilities. Specifically, some research has suggested that spatial skills are employed in visualization of social-cognitive concepts (e.g., in/out-groups, social constraint, “better/worse”) (44, 45), which could also explain the contribution of spatial skills in maintenance of interpersonal relationships. These findings point to the importance of spatial skills in acutely depressed patients, and call for further research on the role of spatial cognition in social/interpersonal functioning.
Executive functioning, but not spatial cognition, mediated the negative effect of current MDD on leisure time. Leisure time may be reliant on executive functioning due to the necessity to plan hobby/personal interest time effectively, balance work/leisure experiences, and maintain focus on leisure activities. Executive inhibition may be particularly crucial for maintenance of leisure time in MDD patients, as cognitive inhibition is key to blocking negative intrusions which can interfere with enjoyment of leisurely activities in currently depressed patients (46, 47). Specifically, failure to inhibit negative thoughts may result in negative material consuming cognitive resources in working memory and attention, decreasing engagement and focus in leisure activities (48). These findings suggest that cognitive treatments targeting executive functioning may be particularly beneficial for patients demonstrating loss of interest/motivation in leisure activities.
Additional findings of interest in the current study were the lack of mediation relationships detected for domains of immediate/delayed memory, attention, and semantic fluency. While several studies have demonstrated that poor cognitive function in these domains is associated with psychosocial dysfunction in depressed patients (4, 14), the present findings suggest that these associations do not play a major role in the mechanistic pathway between depressive illness and psychosocial dysfunction. This interpretation is supported by the current data, which demonstrated that immediate/delayed memory and attention were associated with psychosocial function, but not with depressive illness. Semantic fluency was not associated with depression or functional status, suggesting this domain may play a lesser role in functional and clinical status.
Broadly, the current results indicate that spatial cognition and executive functioning play an important role in functional disability in MDD. While cognitive dysfunction has been associated with functional deficits in general, these results are the first to demonstrate distinct mediation relationships between specific cognitive domains and specific psychosocial deficits. The domain-specific nature of our findings provide clear avenues for future research, which could investigate the roles of spatial cognition and executive function in psychosocial function in clinical subgroups (e.g., elderly, comorbid anxiety). Future research on this topic should also examine the pathological contribution of cognitive domains not included in the current study. For example, processing speed has been linked with major depression (49, 50), but could not be independently derived from cognitive tests in the CoFaMS study, and hence was not included in the present mediation analyses. Clinically, our results suggest that depressed patients with spatial cognitive or executive dysfunction should also be screened for functional deficits particularly in domains of leisure time and interpersonal relationships. Patients with cognitive dysfunction in these domains may also benefit broadly from cognitive training to remediate spatial cognitive or executive deficits (51).
It should be acknowledged that the present findings were drawn from a relatively modest sample size (N = 155), and hence require further validation with a larger and equally well characterized clinical population. In addition, the current findings should be replicated with more objective performance-based indications of psychosocial functioning [e.g., the Social Skills Performance Assessment (SSPA)] (52). While the FAST provided a comprehensive and domain-specific indication of psychosocial functioning, its results were derived from clinicians' judgment, and are hence vulnerable to clinician bias, highlighting the need for replication by performance-based measures (e.g., the SSPA). Further validation with self-reported measures of psychosocial function (e.g., the 36-Item Short Form Health Survey) would also be valuable. However, care should be taken when interpreting self-reported functional outcomes, which may be more closely associated with severity of mood symptoms than clinical interviews or objective measures (14).
Taken together, the current findings provide new evidence for the roles of executive and spatial cognitive dysfunction in psychosocial deficits in current MDD. Specifically, our findings suggest that MDD leads to deficits in executive and spatial cognition, which both contribute to functional issues in domains of autonomy and subjective cognitive dysfunction, and contribute differentially to leisure time, and social relationships. Emerging cognitive and adjunctive treatments for acute depression should consider executive and spatial domains as prime treatment targets (30), particularly in patients representing with issues in the psychosocial domains indicated above.
Ethics statement
This study will be carried out in accordance with the recommendations of the NHMRC national statement on ethical conduct in human research, Royal Adelaide Hospital HREC. The protocol was approved by the Royal Adelaide Hospital HREC and the University of Adelaide HREC. All subjects gave written informed consent in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki.
Author contributions
BB coordinated and supervised the CoFaMS study, from which the current results were derived. MK and BB mutually developed the hypotheses and conceptual outline of the present manuscript. MK analyzed data and wrote the manuscript, while BB edited and provided feedback during the writeup.
Conflict of interest statement
BB received speaker/consultation fees from: AstraZeneca, Lundbeck, Pfizer, Takeda, Servier, Bristol Myers Squibb, Otsuka, and Janssen-Cilag. The remaining author declares that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
Acknowledgments
The authors thank Tracy Air for support with recruitment and sample curation during the earlier stages of the project.
1The PEBL Card Sorting Test is a digital version of the original Wisconsin Card Sorting Test named Berg's Card Sorting Test, which is freely available and includes modified instructions suitable for the digital format.
2Predicted direction given variable coding: IV negatively related with mediator, IV positively related with DV, mediator negatively related with DV.
3Criteria 3 for was marginally significant for autonomy (see Table 3).
Funding. The study was supported by an unrestricted grant of the James and Diana Ramsay Foundation, Adelaide, Australia. The funding body had no impact on the design or the content of the presented work.
Supplementary material
The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyt.2018.00539/full#supplementary-material
References
- 1.Bortolato B, Carvalho A, McIntyre R. Cognitive dysfunction in major depressive disorder: a state-of-the-art clinical review. CNS Neurol Disord Drug Targets (2014) 13:1804–18. 10.2174/1871527313666141130203823 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.McIntyre RS, Lee Y. Cognition in major depressive disorder: a ‘Systemically Important Functional Index’(SIFI). Curr Opin Psychiatry (2016) 29:48–55. 10.1097/YCO.0000000000000221 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Buist-Bouwman M, Ormel J, De Graaf R, De Jonge P, Van Sonderen E, Alonso J, et al. Mediators of the association between depression and role functioning. Acta Psychiatr Scand. (2008) 118:451–8. 10.1111/j.1600-0447.2008.01285.x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Evans VC, Iverson GL, Yatham LN, Lam RW. The relationship between neurocognitive and psychosocial functioning in major depressive disorder: a systematic review. J Clin Psychiatry (2014) 75:1359–70. 10.4088/JCP.13r08939 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.McCall WV, Dunn AG. Cognitive deficits are associated with functional impairment in severely depressed patients. Psychiatry Res. (2003) 121:179–84. 10.1016/j.psychres.2003.09.003 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Woo YS, Rosenblat JD, Kakar R, Bahk W-M, McIntyre RS. Cognitive deficits as a mediator of poor occupational function in remitted major depressive disorder patients. Clin Psychopharmacol Neurosci. (2016) 14:1–16. 10.9758/cpn.2016.14.1.1 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Beekman A, Deeg D, Braam A, Smit J, Van Tilburg W. Consequences of major and minor depression in later life: a study of disability, well-being and service utilization. Psychol Med. (1997) 27:1397–409. 10.1017/S0033291797005734 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Bynum JP, Rabins PV, Weller W, Niefeld M, Anderson GF, Wu AW. The relationship between a dementia diagnosis, chronic illness, medicare expenditures, and hospital use. J Am Geriatr Soc. (2004) 52:187–94. 10.1111/j.1532-5415.2004.52054.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.McIntyre RS, Soczynska JZ, Woldeyohannes HO, Alsuwaidan MT, Cha DS, Carvalho AF, et al. The impact of cognitive impairment on perceived workforce performance: results from the International Mood Disorders Collaborative Project. Compr Psychiatry (2015) 56:279–82. 10.1016/j.comppsych.2014.08.051 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Mintz J, Mintz LI, Arruda MJ, Hwang SS. Treatments of depression and the functional capacity to work. Arch Gen Psychiatry (1992) 49:761–8. 10.1001/archpsyc.1992.01820100005001 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.McIntyre RS, Cha DS, Soczynska JK, Woldeyohannes HO, Gallaugher LA, Kudlow P, et al. Cognitive deficits and functional outcomes in major depressive disorder: determinants, substrates, and treatment interventions. Depress Anxiety (2013) 30:515–27. 10.1002/da.22063 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Baune BT, Miller R, McAfoose J, Johnson M, Quirk F, Mitchell D. The role of cognitive impairment in general functioning in major depression. Psychiatry Res. (2010) 176:183–9. 10.1016/j.psychres.2008.12.001 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Brewster GS, Peterson L, Roker R, Ellis ML, Edwards JD. Depressive symptoms, cognition, and everyday function among community-residing older adults. J Aging Health (2017) 29:367–88. 10.1177/0898264316635587 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Cambridge OR, Knight MJ, Mills N, Baune BT. The clinical relationship between cognitive impairment and psychosocial functioning in major depressive disorder: a systematic review. Psychiatry Res. (2018) 269:157–71. 10.1016/j.psychres.2018.08.033 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Godard J, Baruch P, Grondin S, Lafleur MF. Psychosocial and neurocognitive functioning in unipolar and bipolar depression: a 12-month prospective study. Psychiatry Res. (2012) 196:145–53. 10.1016/j.psychres.2011.09.013 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Knight MJ, Baune BT. Cognitive dysfunction in major depressive disorder. Curr Opin Psychiatry (2018) 31:26–31. 10.1097/YCO.0000000000000378 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Baune BT, Air T. Clinical, functional, and biological correlates of cognitive dimensions in major depressive disorder-rationale, design, and characteristics of the Cognitive Function and Mood Study (CoFaM-Study). Front Psychiatry (2016) 7:150 10.3389/fpsyt.2016.00150 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Daniel BD, Montali A, Gerra ML, Innamorati M, Girardi P, Pompili M, et al. Cognitive impairment and its associations with the path of illness in affective disorders: a comparison between patients with bipolar and unipolar depression in remission. J Psychiatric Pract. (2013) 19:275–87. 10.1097/01.pra.0000432597.79019.e2 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Gildengers AG, Butters MA, Chisholm D, Anderson SJ, Begley A, Holm M, et al. Cognition in older adults with bipolar disorder versus major depressive disorder. Bipolar Disord. (2012) 14:198–205. 10.1111/j.1399-5618.2012.00995.x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.McDermott LM, Ebmeier KP. A meta-analysis of depression severity and cognitive function. J Affect Disord. (2009) 119:1–8. 10.1016/j.jad.2009.04.022 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Xiang X, An R. The impact of cognitive impairment and comorbid depression on disability, health care utilization, and costs. Psychiatric Serv. (2015) 66:1245–8. 10.1176/appi.ps.201400511 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Hasselbalch BJ, Knorr U, Kessing LV. Cognitive impairment in the remitted state of unipolar depressive disorder: a systematic review. J Affect Disord. (2011) 134:20–31. 10.1016/j.jad.2010.11.011 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Knight MJ, Air T, Baune BT. The role of cognitive impairment in psychosocial functioning in remitted depression. J Affect Disord. (2018) 235:129–34. 10.1016/j.jad.2018.04.051 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Rock P, Roiser J, Riedel W, Blackwell A. Cognitive impairment in depression: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Psychol Med. (2014) 44:2029–40. 10.1017/S0033291713002535 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Alexopoulos GS, Meyers BS, Young RC, Kalayam B, Kakuma T, Gabrielle M, et al. Executive dysfunction and long-term outcomes of geriatric depression. Arch General Psychiatry (2000) 57:285–90. 10.1001/archpsyc.57.3.285 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Birnbaum HG, Kessler RC, Kelley D, Ben-Hamadi R, Joish VN, Greenberg PE. Employer burden of mild, moderate, and severe major depressive disorder: mental health services utilization and costs, and work performance. Depress Anxiety (2010) 27:78–89. 10.1002/da.20580 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Gualtieri CT, Morgan DW. The frequency of cognitive impairment in patients with anxiety, depression, and bipolar disorder: an unaccounted source of variance in clinical trials. J Clin Psychiatry (2008) 69:1122–30. 10.4088/JCP.v69n0712 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Kiosses DN, Alexopoulos GS. IADL functions, cognitive deficits, and severity of depression: a preliminary study. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry (2005) 13:244–9. 10.1097/00019442-200503000-00010 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.World Health Organization International Classification of Functioning, Disability and Health: ICF. World Health Organization; (2001). [Google Scholar]
- 30.Knight MJ, Baune BT. Psychosocial dysfunction in major depressive disorder–rationale, design, and characteristics of the Cognitive and Emotional Recovery Training Program for Depression (CERT-D). Front Psychiatry (2017) 8:280. 10.3389/fpsyt.2017.00280 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.American Psychiatric Association DSM IV Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association; (1994). [Google Scholar]
- 32.Randolph C, Tierney MC, Mohr E, Chase TN. The Repeatable Battery for the Assessment of Neuropsychological Status (RBANS): preliminary clinical validity. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol. (1998) 20:310–9. 10.1076/jcen.20.3.310.823 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Davis H, Keller F. Colorado Assessment Tests (CATS), Version 1.2. Colorado Springs, CO: Colorado Assessment Tests; (2002). [Google Scholar]
- 34.Mueller ST, Piper BJ. The psychology experiment building language (PEBL) and PEBL test battery. J Neurosci Methods (2014) 222:250–9. 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2013.10.024 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Rosa AR, Sánchez-Moreno J, Martínez-Aran A, Salamero M, Torrent C, Reinares M, et al. Validity and reliability of the Functioning Assessment Short Test (FAST) in bipolar disorder. Clin Pract Epidemiol Mental Health (2007) 3:5. 10.1186/1745-0179-3-5 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Sheehan D, Lecrubier Y, Sheehan KH, Sheehan K, Amorim P, Janavs J, et al. Diagnostic psychiatric interview for DSM-IV and ICD-10. J Clin Psychiatry (1998) 59:22–33. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Trivedi MH, Rush AJ, Wisniewski SR, Nierenberg AA, Warden D, Ritz L, et al. Evaluation of outcomes with citalopram for depression using measurement-based care in STAR* D: implications for clinical practice. Am J Psychiatry (2006) 163:28–40. 10.1176/appi.ajp.163.1.28 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Baron RM, Kenny DA. The moderator–mediator variable distinction in social psychological research: conceptual, strategic, and statistical considerations. J Pers Soc Psychol. (1986) 51:1173. 10.1037/0022-3514.51.6.1173 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Mackin RS, Areán PA. Impaired financial capacity in late life depression is associated with cognitive performance on measures of executive functioning and attention. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. (2009) 15:793–8. 10.1017/S1355617709990300 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Elliott R, Sahakian B, Herrod J, Robbins T, Paykel E. Abnormal response to negative feedback in unipolar depression: evidence for a diagnosis specific impairment. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry (1997) 63:74–82. 10.1136/jnnp.63.1.74 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Broadbent DE, Cooper PF, FitzGerald P, Parkes KR. The cognitive failures questionnaire (CFQ) and its correlates. Br J Clin Psychol. (1982) 21:1–16. 10.1111/j.2044-8260.1982.tb01421.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Hohman TJ, Beason-Held LL, Lamar M, Resnick SM. Subjective cognitive complaints and longitudinal changes in memory and brain function. Neuropsychology (2011) 25:125. 10.1037/a0020859 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Jaeger J, Berns S, Uzelac S, Davis-Conway S. Neurocognitive deficits and disability in major depressive disorder. Psychiatry Res. (2006) 145:39–48. 10.1016/j.psychres.2005.11.011 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.De Soto CB, London M, Handel S. Social reasoning and spatial paralogic. J Pers Soc Psychol. (1965) 2:513–21. 10.1037/h0022492 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Sinha C, De López KJ. Language, culture, and the embodiment of spatial cognition. Cognitive Linguistics (2000) 11:17–42. 10.1515/cogl.2001.008 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Gohier B, Ferracci L, Surguladze SA, Lawrence E, El Hage W, Kefi MZ, et al. Cognitive inhibition and working memory in unipolar depression. J Affect Disord. (2009) 116:100–5. 10.1016/j.jad.2008.10.028 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Joormann J, Gotlib IH. Updating the contents of working memory in depression: interference from irrelevant negative material. J Abnorm Psychol. (2008) 117:182. 10.1037/0021-843X.117.1.182 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Bugos JA, Perlstein WM, McCrae CS, Brophy TS, Bedenbaugh P. Individualized piano instruction enhances executive functioning and working memory in older adults. Aging Mental Health (2007) 11:464–71. 10.1080/13607860601086504 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Baune BT, Fuhr M, Air T, Hering C. Neuropsychological functioning in adolescents and young adults with major depressive disorder–a review. Psychiatry Res. (2014) 218:261–71. 10.1016/j.psychres.2014.04.052 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Douglas KM, Porter RJ. Longitudinal assessment of neuropsychological function in major depression. Aust N Z J Psychiatry (2009) 43:1105–17. 10.3109/00048670903279887 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Motter JN, Pimontel MA, Rindskopf D, Devanand DP, Doraiswamy PM, Sneed JR. Computerized cognitive training and functional recovery in major depressive disorder: a meta-analysis. J Affect Disord. (2016) 189:184–91. 10.1016/j.jad.2015.09.022 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Bowie CR, Harvey PD. Communication abnormalities predict functional outcomes in chronic schizophrenia: differential associations with social and adaptive functions. Schizophr Res. (2008) 103:240–7. 10.1016/j.schres.2008.05.006 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.