Visual Abstract
Abstract
Frontal eye field (FEF) in macaque monkeys contributes to visual attention, visual–motor transformations and production of eye movements. Traditionally, neurons in FEF have been classified by the magnitude of increased discharge rates following visual stimulus presentation, during a waiting period, and associated with eye movement production. However, considerable heterogeneity remains within the traditional visual, visuomovement, and movement categories. Cluster analysis is a data-driven method of identifying self-segregating groups within a dataset. Because many cluster analysis techniques exist and outcomes vary with analysis assumptions, consensus clustering aggregates over multiple analyses, identifying robust groups. To describe more comprehensively the neuronal composition of FEF, we applied a consensus clustering technique for unsupervised categorization of patterns of spike rate modulation measured during a memory-guided saccade task. We report 10 functional categories, expanding on the traditional 3 categories. Categories were distinguished by latency, magnitude, and sign of visual response; the presence of sustained activity; and the dynamics, magnitude and sign of saccade-related modulation. Consensus clustering can include other metrics and can be applied to datasets from other brain regions to provide better information guiding microcircuit models of cortical function.
Significance Statement
The contribution of a brain region cannot be understood without knowing the diversity, arrangement, and circuitry of constituent neurons. Traditional descriptions of frontal eye field include visual, visuosaccadic, and saccadic categories. Here, we use a novel consensus clustering method to identify more reliably functional categories in neural data. While confirming the traditional categories, consensus clustering distinguishes additional, previously unappreciated diversity in neural activity patterns. Such information is necessary to formulate correct microcircuit models of cortical function.
Introduction
Like all cortical areas, frontal eye field (FEF) is composed of neurons distinguished by morphology, neurochemistry, biophysics, layer, and connectivity. Biophysical distinctions can be made via action potential waveforms (McCormick et al., 1985; Mitchell et al., 2007; Cohen et al., 2009a; Ding and Gold, 2012; Thiele et al., 2016), calcium binding proteins (Pouget et al., 2009), and neuromodulatory receptors (Noudoost and Moore, 2011; Soltani et al., 2013). Neurons with distinct biophysical characteristics must play different roles in the cortical microcircuit (Lewis and Lund, 1993; DeFelipe, 1997; Pouget et al., 2009; Zaitsev et al., 2012). Connectivity studies find FEF connected with at least 80 cortical areas (Huerta et al., 1986, 1987; Schall et al., 1993, 1995a; Stanton et al., 1993, 1995; Markov et al., 2014), and most pyramidal neurons do not project to more than one cortical area (Markov et al., 2014; Ninomiya et al., 2012; Pouget et al., 2009). Numerous functional distinctions among FEF neurons have been reported, beginning with the traditional sorting into visual, visuomovement, and movement plus fixation and postsaccadic categories (Bruce and Goldberg, 1985; Schall, 1991). Subsequently, FEF neurons have been implicated in numerous functions including visual search (Schall et al., 1995b; Thompson et al., 1996; Lee and Keller, 2008; Zhou and Desimone, 2011; Purcell et al., 2012a; Fernandes et al., 2014; Costello et al., 2016), saccade preparation and inhibition (Hanes et al., 1998; Boucher et al., 2007; Ray et al., 2009), perceptual choice (Ding and Gold, 2012), visual attention (Bichot et al., 1996; Bichot and Schall, 2002; Gregoriou et al., 2009; Khayat et al., 2009; Zhou and Desimone, 2011; Schafer and Moore 2011; Noudoost et al., 2014; Thiele et al., 2016), visual working memory (Clark et al., 2012; Reinhart et al., 2012), trans-saccadic stability (Crapse and Sommer, 2008, 2012; Shin and Sommer, 2012; Joiner et al., 2013; Chen et al., 2018), planning saccade sequences (Phillips and Segraves, 2010), eye–head coordination (Elsley et al., 2007; Knight 2012; Sajad et al., 2015; Izawa and Suzuki, 2018), and anticipating reward (Roesch and Olson, 2003; Glaser et al., 2016). Can so many functions be accomplished by so few neuron categories?
The problem of classification is neither new to science nor unique to neurophysiology. Cluster analysis is a powerful statistical tool, which was developed to find self-segregating categories in gene expression (Sharp et al., 1986), psychiatric diagnostics (Lochner et al., 2005), linguistics (Gries and Stefanowitsch, 2010), and Scotch whisky (Lapointe and Legendre, 1994). It has also been used to describe the biophysical diversity of cortical neurons (Nowak et al., 2003; Druckmann et al., 2013; Ardid et al., 2015), expanding the in vivo description of putative excitatory and inhibitory cells. Cluster analysis should be similarly powerful for assessing the functional diversity that must parallel anatomic diversity and should reproduce the functional categories known to exist in FEF.
Cluster analysis requires strategic decisions about the method of grouping observations and how to calculate pairwise distance, which lacks rigorous specification for clustering the functional characteristics of neurons. Therefore, we applied multiple preprocessing pipelines to a large sample of FEF neurons then applied an agglomerative clustering algorithm to discover functional categories. Because a priori endorsement of any particular preprocessing pipeline is impossible, and each result is unique, the results of an individual clustering procedure are difficult to interpret. However, second-order clustering procedures known as consensus clustering combine outcomes from different pipelines (Strehl and Ghosh, 2002). Distinct consensus clustering procedures use different theoretical motivations and computational efficiencies (Goder and Filkov, 2008). We applied a procedure that operates on the median pairwise similarity across all preprocessing pipelines because it is tractable and efficient. This consensus clustering procedure identified 10 robust functional categories of FEF neurons, which elaborate conventional functional classifications.
Materials and Methods
Subjects and behavioral task
Three male macaque monkeys (Macaca radiata) participated in this study. All procedures were performed in accordance with the National Institutes of Health Guide for the Care and Use of Laboratory Animals and were approved by the Vanderbilt Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee. Monkeys were trained to perform a memory-guided saccade task (Bruce and Goldberg, 1985). Trials began when a central fixation point appeared. After fixating this point for 500 ms, a peripheral target was presented for one screen refresh (16.7 ms at 60 Hz refresh rate) at 8° eccentricity at one of eight locations separated by 45°. After a variable delay between 300 and 800 ms the fixation point was extinguished, and the monkey was required to shift gaze to and maintain fixation on the location of the peripheral stimulus. The peripheral stimulus was reilluminated after the saccade to provide a fixation stimulus. Fluid reward was delivered if the monkey maintained fixation on the peripheral, now reilluminated, stimulus for 500 ms. If the monkey broke fixation, made a saccade to an incorrect location, or made a saccade before the fixation point was extinguished, a 5000 ms time-out delay occurred.
Recording techniques
MRI-compatible headposts and recording chambers were placed over the arcuate sulcus. Surgery was conducted under aseptic conditions with animals under isoflurane anesthesia. Antibiotics and analgesics were administered postoperatively. Details have been described previously (Schall et al., 1995b; Sato et al., 2001; Cohen et al., 2009b). Data were streamed to a data acquisition system: Multi-Neuron Acquisition Processor (40 kHz; monkeys Da, Ga, and He, Plexon) or TDT System 3 (25 kHz; monkey Da, Tucker-Davis Technologies; Fig. 1a). Eye position was collected using EyeLink 1000 (SR Research). Eye position was calibrated daily, streamed to the data acquisition system, and stored at 1 kHz. Electrophysiological data were obtained from linear electrode arrays, either a 24-channel Plexon Uprobe (monkeys Ga and He) or a 32-channel NeuroNexus Vector Array (monkey Da). Both probes had a 150 μm recording contact spacing. Single units were identified on-line using a window discriminator (Plexon) or principle component analysis (Tucker-Davis Technologies). Units recorded from the TDT system were sorted off-line using Kilosort (Pachitariu et al., 2016).
Unit isolation was assessed by measuring waveform signal-to-noise ratio (SNR), interspike interval distributions, and baseline firing rate. SNR was calculated by dividing the voltage difference between the peak and trough of the mean action potential waveform by the Standard Deviation (SD) of concatenated waveform residuals (Joshua et al., 2007). A minimum SNR criterion was set for each recording system. Units with >10% of interspike intervals <2 ms were excluded. Units with a mean baseline discharge rate of <5 spikes/s were excluded. Of 1864 potential single units, 963 were excluded based on SNR, 22 were excluded based on interspike interval distribution, and 439 were excluded based on baseline firing rate. All together, these criteria excluded 1383 potential units, leaving 481 for analysis. An additional 15 units were excluded for lacking nonzero values in either the visual or perisaccadic epochs of their spike density function (SDF). These very conservative criteria resulted in only 25% of potential units being included in the categorization, which indicates that they were well isolated single units (Fig. 1b).
Neuron classification
SDFs were calculated by convolving the spike trains with a function that resembles the postsynaptic influence of each spike (Thompson et al., 1996). SDFs were calculated only for correct trials on which the visual stimulus was presented in the visual receptive field, and the saccade was made into the movement field. The number of trials contributing to characterizing each neuron ranged from as few as 4 to as many as 317 (median, 34 trials), with trials with fewer than five spikes excluded. If the spike density function was not a stable estimate, we did not include the neuron. A sequence of classification procedures was used. The first was based on the traditional criteria of Bruce and Goldberg (1985). A unit was considered to have visual activity if the firing rate between 50 and 150 ms after stimulus presentation was elevated >6 SDs above the baseline mean. A unit was considered to have movement activity if the firing rate in the 100 ms preceding the saccade was >6 SDs above the baseline mean and the SDF showed a positive correlation over time in the 20 ms preceding saccade. This prevents elevated delay activity with no presaccadic ramping from being considered movement-related activity. Visual units had visual responses with no movement activity, movement units had movement activity with no visual responses, and visuomovement units had both. Other units were considered uncategorized; we did not test for fixation or postsaccadic activity in this categorization.
Units were categorized via agglomerative hierarchical cluster analyses (Sokal and Michener, 1958; Everitt et al., 2011). These analyses iteratively combine units, or groups of units, based on the weighted average similarity of units. In each case, the analysis algorithm was identical, although the method for determining similarity differed due to the scaling of discharge rates across units (Fig. 1c), measurement of the response of the units (Fig. 1d), or the similarity metric (Fig. 1e). The agglomerative cluster analysis was performed as follows: first, the sample was considered as n groups, each with one member. Then, the two groups with the smallest pairwise distance were combined into one group, leaving (n − 1) groups, one of which had two members. The distances of this group to the other groups were determined by the weighted mean of the distances of the individuals in each group, as follows Eqn. 1:
(1) |
where I is the first group in consideration and J is the second, x are the members of group I, y are the members of group J, and nI and nJ are the number of members in groups I and J, respectively. The value of 2 in the denominator is required because the distances are symmetrical and thus represented twice in the numerator of the equation. More simply, this averages the pairwise distances of the members of I and J such that the similarity of two groups will not be skewed by uneven group sizes.
This procedure was repeated until all observations were agglomerated into a single group. Then, category identifications were made for a range of number of categories, k, by finding the most recent step in the algorithm at which k categories with a minimum of x members were present. For example, for a k value of five, the most recent set with five categories of at least x members was assigned as the final classification. Category membership for k was assessed between 1 and 20. The value of x was set to 10; only categories with ≥10 members were considered to assure robust results.
Category membership was assessed for six scaling procedures, four measurements of the response, and two similarity metrics (Fig. 1c–e). The mean skewness across time points was used to assess the quality of scaling for cross-unit comparisons. We refer to each combination of scaling procedure, SDF measurement, and similarity metric as a preprocessing pipeline. We evaluated categories derived from each pipeline but will show outcomes for only three.
Modulation of discharge rates was measured according to the following approaches. Three (mean, slope, and mean and slope) account for the firing rates during epochs: −200 to −100 ms before stimulus onset; 50–100 ms poststimulus; 100–150 ms poststimulus, −100 to −50 ms before saccade; −50 to 0 ms before saccade; and 50–100 ms postsaccade. The “mean” measurement was based on the mean firing rates in these epochs, the “slope” measurement was based on the slope of the firing rate changes in these epochs (i.e., the difference in mean firing rate at the beginning and end of the epoch), and “mean and slopes” were based on the concatenation of mean firing rate and slope. The “SDF” measurement did not parse the responses into epochs, and was instead based on the values of the SDF aligned on stimulus onset (−200 to 300 ms) or saccade (−300 to 200 ms) at each time point to millisecond resolution. To emphasize equally all epochs, means, and slopes, each measurement value was individually converted to a z score across the sample.
Pairwise distance was measured in the following two ways: Euclidean distance and correlation. Euclidean distance can be conceptualized as the physical distance between two points in multidimensional space while disregarding which dimensions contribute to this distance. Correlation assesses the relationships between the dimensions while disregarding the particular values of those dimensions. The different emphases of these two distance metrics can and often do assign two units to the same category via one metric but not the other.
Euclidean distance
Based on the firing rate each unit was placed in a multidimensional space. This multidimensional space had 6 (for mean measurement and slope measurement), 12 (for mean and slope measurement), or 1002 (for SDF measurement) dimensions. The Euclidean distance between the units in this space defines a pairwise distance matrix, as follows Eqn. 2:
(2) |
where e is the number of epochs (or milliseconds).
Correlation
Based on the firing rate, each unit was defined as a 6-, 12-, or 1002-element vector, depending on the measurement method, and the correlation between two vectors Eqn. 3:
(3) |
measures the similarity of the modulation patterns of two units while disregarding absolute differences in firing rates.
All of these preprocessing pipelines were tested, and all produced unique results. Some pipelines produced categories that were subjectively easy to endorse, while others produced subjectively poor categories. The results of three representative pipelines are presented, two that produced poor categorizations, for different reasons, and one that produced good categorizations. The first combination scales by z scoring relative to baseline and mean measurement, and uses Euclidean distance. The second combination does not scale the data before measurement, uses mean measurement, and uses correlation distance. The third combination scales by z scoring relative to the whole trial, and mean and slope measurement.
Consensus clustering
No a priori reason endorses the categories yielded by a particular SDF scaling procedure, summary of response modulation, or distance measurement. Therefore, we used a novel strategy of combining the results of multiple clustering pipelines. This algorithm considers the pairwise distance between units for each individual preprocessing pipeline tested by creating a composite distance matrix. Each individual distance matrix was z scored internally to correct for the absolute scale differences between different scaling procedures and distance measurements. Then, the median for each pair was selected to prevent skewing by nonoptimal pipelines. Thus, if the pairwise distance between two units was consistently small, then this composite measure was also small, whereas if the pairwise distance between two units was small in some processing pipelines but was generally larger, then the composite distance metric reflected the trend toward differences but accounted for the isolated cases of similarity. After creating this composite distance matrix, the same agglomerative algorithm used for each individual pipeline was applied to identify categories (Fig. 1f). Intuitively, this method was intended to distinguish units that were clustered together regardless of preprocessing pipeline from units that were members of different clusters regardless of preprocessing pipeline.
In essence, this procedure performs a clustering that operates on a distance matrix whose entries represent robustness of categorization across a number of individual procedures, or a “consensus clustering” problem (Goder and Filkov, 2008). Indeed, consensus clustering has been used to identify biophysical classes of neurons (Ardid et al., 2015). However, while conceptually similar, the previous “meta-clustering” and the present consensus clustering differ operationally in both the algorithm used for performing clustering and the preprocessing of input data. Ardid et al. (2015) performed K-means clustering, which does not provide a unique clustering solution and is highly sensitive to starting points (Bradley and Fayyad, 1998; Peña et al., 1999; Celebi and Kingravi, 2012), so their meta-clustering involved multiple iterations of the K-means procedure using the same input data and then assigning clusters via robust comembership across each iteration. Unlike Ardid et al. (2015), we used agglomerative clustering, which delivers unique solutions because no optimization steps are involved. However, we found that clustering outcomes were sensitive to the preprocessing pipeline, varying with the discharge rate scaling procedure, measurement, and distance metric. Hence, our consensus clustering approach was conceived to assess cluster assignment consistency over preprocessing steps, not local solutions to clustering one set of preprocessed data.
Assessing number of categories
The number of categories in individual clustering procedures was selected using a lenient version of the gap procedure of Tibshirani et al. (2001), which assesses the reduction in intracluster distance with respect to randomized null sets created with no intrinsic clustering. Valid splitting of clusters should have a greater than chance reduction in intracluster distance, assessed by the SDs of the intracluster distance in the null sets, whereas excessive splitting should have a reduction in intracluster distance within the SD of the null set. The strict version selects k categories as the first number of categories meeting this criterion. In the lenient version of this test, each occasion on which the above criterion is met was treated as potentially valid, and visual inspection was used to determine whether categorizations were either insufficient or excessive. In some cases, due to the difficulty in creating a reasonable null-set from physiologic data, categories were selected based on the properties of the gap curve. When a reasonable null-set could not be determined, an inflection in the gap curve was identified. This inflection identified the number of categories at which the reduction of intracluster distance was markedly less than that of the previous sequence of clusters.
For consensus categories and the composite distance matrix, the means for creating a null-set to use the gap procedure of Tibshirani et al. (2001) is unclear. Therefore, in such cases a pair of criteria for determining the maximum number of categories was set: not >10% of units was allowed to remain uncategorized, and each category required at least 10 members. The maximum number of categories that met both criteria was selected.
Comparing categorization schemes
The quality of alternative categorization schemes was assessed by calculating an index of member variability through the ratio of variances of the spike density values. Specifically, for each time point in the spike density functions, the within-category variance at that time point was divided by the variance of the category mean across time points. For each category, the average ratio was calculated, and then the grand average was taken. Because this ratio will decrease by definition as more categories are formed, a penalty for oversplitting was imposed by multiplying the grand average ratio by the square root of the number of categories. That is, for a given category the modulation strength was calculated as follows Eqn. 4:
(4) |
where c indexes category and t, time. Then, for each time point a ratio of variances (RoV) was calculated as follows Eqn. 5:
(5) |
These are then combined by averaging the values for each category over time, then across categories, then by applying a penalty for overclustering proportional to the square root of the number of categories identified as follows Eqn. 6:
(6) |
Small RoV values can be obtained either through large groupwise modulations over time or a lack of variability among the constituent members of the categories, whereas large RoV values are obtained through weak categorywise modulations or large variability. That is, smaller RoV values indicate better categorization, and vice versa. No benchmarks have been established for this index, so we interpret relative values in comparing the quality of two categorization schemes.
The similarity of two categorization schemes was assessed as follows by the Adjusted Rand Index (ARI; Hubert and Arabie, 1985) Eqn. 7:
(7) |
where ai and bj are the counts of category i or j in categorization procedure a or b, respectively, and ni,j is the number of observations in both category i in categorization scheme a and in category j in categorization scheme b. This quantity measures the similarity of two data categorizations and is adjusted by chance cocategorization produced through the two schemes. To assess significance, each categorization was randomly shuffled separately, destroying internal structure between the two schemes, and ARI was recalculated. This was repeated 1000 times and p was the proportion of shuffled ARI that exceeded the nonshuffled ARI.
;To visualize the overlap, for each pairwise categorization combination a signed χ2 was calculated as follows Eqn. 8:
(8) |
That is, the difference between the observed pairwise count and the count expected by the marginal probabilities of each individual categorization scheme was calculated, then normalized by that expected value. Each category assignment was shuffled separately 1000 times, and the signed χ2 was recomputed for each combination and iteration. A bootstrapped z score was then calculated for each category combination.
Biophysical characteristics
Several biophysical characteristics were calculated for each neuron to assess the identification of consensus categories with measurements that were not included in the clustering process. The spike width of the average waveform of each neuron was calculated as the time between the initial trough of the action potential and the following peak, or the time between the initial peak and the following trough for spikes with positive deflections. Coefficient of variation (CV), which measures firing rate variability, and local coefficient of variation (CV2), which measures CV across smaller time periods, were calculated as described previously (Holt et al., 1996). Local variation (LV), which is another metric of local firing rate variability, and revised LV (LVR), which accounts for a 5 ms refractory period, were also calculated as described previously (Shinomoto et al., 2003, 2009). The Fano factor was calculated by computing spike counts in 100 ms bins, then dividing the spike count variance by the mean spike count (Purcell et al., 2012b). Response field characteristics (maximum response, preferred location, and tuning width) were calculated by fitting a unimodal Gaussian function to the mean responses to the eight target locations. Response field characteristics were calculated separately for visual epochs (50–150 ms after target presentation) and movement epochs (50 ms preceding saccade initiation). Response field values were excluded from analysis if the Gaussian tuning function was unable to fit well (r 2 < 0.5).
Cross-validation
To verify the accuracy of the consensus clustering algorithm, a leave-one-out classification procedure was used. A singular value decomposition classifier (SVD) classifier with a linear kernel was trained. To preserve the consensus metrics, the basis for the classifier training set was the composite pairwise distance matrix. However, this matrix was underspecified, so principle components of the pairwise distance matrix were calculated. The classifier was trained, for example, for the first principle component, the first and second components, and the first through third components for the first 100 principle components. The cumulative variance explained ranged from 43.4% to 93.4%. Due to the possible presence of groups with few members, this classifier was trained on all units but one, and the remaining unit was tested. No explicit regularization was performed when training the classifier. Only units that were assigned a category by the consensus clustering algorithm were included (n = 422 of 466, 90.6%).
Results
This analysis is based on 466 units sampled in FEF from three macaque monkeys performing memory-guided saccades in pursuit of other research aims.
Traditional response categorization
First, units were categorized based on traditional criteria (Bruce and Goldberg, 1985; Schall, 1991), and the canonical visual, movement, and visuomovement units were identified (Fig. 2). Of 466 neurons sampled, 210 (45.1% of sampled neurons, 70.2% of presaccadic modulated neurons) were identified as visual, 16 (5.4%, 5.4%) were movement related and 73 (15.7%, 24.4%) were visuomovement related. The remainder exhibited other patterns of modulation. Earlier studies using different tasks than we used here reported more diversity among the three major groups. We will consider this in the Discussion. For now, this simpler categorization facilitates the motivation of this approach.
While the average discharge rates of these categories were as expected, the SDF of the individual units categorized into each type exhibited considerable variation. For reference with subsequent analyses, the RoV value was 50.89. Thus, while the traditional categorization methods captured general trends in the modulation patterns of FEF neurons, additional variation was present but unaccounted for.
Cluster pipeline 1: z score relative to baseline, mean measurement, Euclidean distance
To begin accounting for this excessive variability, a cluster analysis was performed on the SDFs that were z scored based on the mean and SD of the baseline firing rate. This captured the definition of visual and movement activity used above. However, the data-driven clustering procedure revealed functional categories that are similar in their firing rate modulations in more than just two epochs. Based on the mean firing rate in the six specific task epochs described in Materials and Methods, each unit was represented as a six-element vector. Based on Euclidean distance measures of pairwise distances, eight categories of units, numbered 1a to 8a to distinguish this set of results, were found (Fig. 3). Unlike the traditional categorization scheme, two categories of visual units were identified as categories 1a and 4a [17 of 466 (3.7%) and 11 of 466 (2.4%), respectively]. Both categories had modest visual responses and no perisaccadic activity. They were differentiated by the presence or absence of anticipatory activity before the target appeared and by delay period activity. An additional category, category 5a [37 of 466 (7.9%)], had a robust visual response with weak presaccadic ramping. Four categories of units had both visual and presaccadic responses. Two of these categories had robust visual responses and intermediate presaccadic ramping: categories 2a and 7a [40 of 466 (5.2%) and 112 of 466 (24.0%) respectively]. These categories were differentiated by the return to baseline after the saccade: category 7a had a typical slow return to baseline, whereas category 2a returned to baseline almost immediately. The other two categories, 6a and 8a [135 of 466 (29.0%) and 16 of 466 (3.4%) respectively], had only modest firing rate modulation in both epochs and were distinguished by the presence (8a) or absence (6a) of anticipatory activity. The final category, 3a [24 of 466 (5.2%)], did not show firing rate modulation during the trial. This demonstrates that additional diversity is present in FEF firing patterns that has been unaccounted for in the traditional scheme. However, this clustering approach failed to identify purely movement neurons or postsaccadic neurons.
The eight categories did not match the three traditional categories in FEF that should have been recovered through cluster analysis. However, similar to the traditional categorization scheme, these category means captured some general trends of the individual units comprising the categories. Variation was reduced (RoV = 18.36 relative to 50.89 for the traditional classification), but considerable variation was still evident. This was particularly pronounced in categories 4a and 8a, which were also much larger categories than the other six. Thus, these categories seem to be “catch-all” categories. Other categories seem to be nearly identical, though are clearly seen as different groups in the dendrogram (e.g., categories 2a and 5a). The dendrogram in Figure 3a also shows that units did not exhibit clear clustering. Instead, it appears as though small groups or individual units are progressively grouped together such that, at the clustering step when eight categories meet the membership criterion, 74 of 466 (16%) units were still uncategorized. That is, for the number of categories identified via the gap procedure of Tibshirani et al. (2001), 16% of neurons were so dissimilar to each other and the eight categories that they could not be placed in any of the eight categories or form a ninth separately. This may be so because the variation of the SDF of the identified units was high (mean skewness = 1.35). Together, these considerations indicate that this clustering procedure is insufficient.
Cluster pipeline 2: nonscaled SDF, mean measurement, correlation distance
To account for more of the excessive variability, a cluster analysis using a correlation distance measurement was performed on the non-scaled data. This approach captures relative rather than absolute changes in firing rate. That is, if two units were similarly modulated but have different firing rates, this procedure treated them as members of the same category. In other words, this approach emphasized the pattern of modulation of FEF neurons rather than the absolute discharge rate.
This procedure identified six categories, 1b to 6b (Fig. 4). These categories did not match the three traditional categories; movement neurons are missing. Instead, each of the six identified categories demonstrated modulation following visual stimulation to different degrees. Two of these categories, 1b (123 of 466) and 6b (122 of 466), showed visual modulation only and were differentiated by the baseline firing rate and degree of visual modulation. Three categories had both visual and movement-related activity: 2b (28 of 466), 4b (44 of 466), and 5b (24 of 466). Category 2b had modest visual modulation and no delay activity; category 4b had modest visual activity and some delay activity; and category 5b had robust visual modulation, prominent delay activity, and presaccadic ramping, but its activity fell off dramatically at the time of the saccade. The final category, 3b (121 of 466), had modest visual modulation and a sharp postsaccadic transient. Four of 466 neurons were placed in no category.
Considerable variability within categories remained and in fact increased relative to cluster pipeline 1 (RoV = 29.70 as opposed to 18.36), though the category means did capture category trends. This variability may be due to the skewed firing rates (mean skewness, 2.09), which allowed few units in one category to drive the modulations apparent in the category means. For example, the inclusion of some units with large visual responses and high firing rates in category 2b was a driving factor in the modest visual activity seen in the category mean. Thus, both the z score relative to baseline and unscaled firing rates may be ineffective for comparing across units, although they are useful for assessing within-unit modulation. However, the appearance of the dendrogram for this clustering outcome should be noted (Fig. 4a). Unlike that from pipeline 1, a more sensible structure is apparent when this combination of clustering parameters was used; fewer neurons are nonclassified, and the categories visibly self-segregate.
Cluster pipeline 3: z score relative to whole-trial, mean and slope measurement, and correlation distance
To account for more of the variability in modulation patterns, a different scaling procedure was used: z scoring across the entire trial and measuring the SDF with both the means of the SDF and the slopes during the relevant epochs were considered. The agglomerative clustering algorithm identified five categories, 1c to 5c (Fig. 5). Three of these categories had visual activity only: 1c (110 of 466), 2c (124 of 466), and 4c (124 of 466). Category 4c had pronounced delay activity. The other two were distinguished by the time of peak visual response, with the visual activity of category 1c peaking earlier, and that of category 2c, later. Category 3c (50 of 466) had robust visual and presaccadic modulation but did not have delay activity. The final category, 5c (58 of 466), showed robust presaccadic ramping and only modest, if any, visual activity. It should be noted that the presaccadic ramping activity in category 5c peaked after the saccade and showed a slow reduction of firing rate back to baseline, whereas the category with both visual and saccadic responses had a peak perisaccadic activity at the time of the saccade followed by a sharp return to baseline, but this sharp return is not as pronounced as the “clipped” movement neurons in category 5b. This indicates that the additional diversity evident in visual inspection of discharge rate modulation patterns is tangible and identifiable. Further, of the three cluster pipelines, pipeline 3 produced the classification most similar to the traditional. Visual and visuomovement categories were identified as well as a putative pure movement group (category 5c).
The range of values through this scaling was smaller and is less skewed (mean skewness, 0.73), suggesting that this scaling method provided a more equitable cross-unit comparison. As with pipeline 2, these categories are apparent in the dendrogram structure, though some additional heterogeneity can be observed, particularly among categories 1c and 2c. This may explain, in their particular cases, the seeming similarity between the two; it could be that splitting further would reveal additional heterogeneity, and, in the case of the category 3c, further splitting could reveal a second visuomovement group without clipped activity as well as a pure movement category. However, it should be noted that. unlike cluster pipelines 1 and 2, the category means were much more representative of the members (RoV = 5.19 as opposed to 18.36 and 29.70 for cluster pipelines 1 and 2, respectively).
Consensus clustering
We now address the problem of individual units being members of different categories following different analysis paths (Fig. 6). This occurs because different preprocessing pipelines resulted in different distance matrices on which the agglomerative clustering algorithm operates. Consequently, a given pair of units could be members of the same category following one pipeline but members of different categories following another pipeline. For example, all four individual units shown in Figure 6 belong to category 2c, but only three belong to category 6b; the unit on the top right belongs instead to category 1b. With no cluster pipeline being more confidently motivated or more certainly correct than another, should all four units be considered members of the same category or not? Nevertheless, assuming the existence of ground-truth categories, consistent with anatomic constraints, units that are actually members of the same ground-truth category should have small pairwise distances regardless of a scaling or clustering procedure. Likewise, units that are members of different ground-truth categories would have small pairwise distances only as an artifact of particular measurement parameters and clustering algorithm.
To address this fundamental problem, we used a second-order clustering procedure known as consensus clustering (Strehl and Ghosh, 2002; Goder and Filkov, 2008). We created a composite distance matrix by z scoring individual distance matrices from all of the preprocessing pipelines (Fig. 1f) and then calculated the median distance across all preprocessing pipelines. This composite distance metric was used to identify units that were consistently similar to one another across preprocessing pipelines and clustering algorithms. The agglomerative clustering algorithm was applied to this distance matrix to identify robust categories of units superordinate to any individual cluster analysis. Clusters were continually split until either of two membership criteria were no longer satisfied: (1) minimum number of units per cluster; and (2) maximum proportion of unclustered units.
Consensus clustering identified 10 categories, clearly distinguished in the dendrogram and evident in the distance matrix (Fig. 7). Of 466 neurons, 43 (9.2%) were not placed in any category. These categories were robust and consistent (RoV, 3.91). Even with the penalty for overclustering in the RoV metric, consensus clusters account for more of the variability in the neural data than the classification produced by the best individual classification.
Categories with visual responses only
We identified two categories of neurons that had visual, but not saccade-related, activity. These categories, 1con (74 of 466) and 2con (105 of 466), showed flat baseline activity and a sharp visual transient. The time of peak firing rate differentiated these two categories, with mean peak latencies of 74 ms (1con) and 136 ms (2con). Also, category 2con had persistent delay activity until the saccade.
Categories with visual and saccade-related facilitation
We identified five categories of neurons with both visual and presaccadic increases in firing rate. Two of these categories, 3con (21 of 466) and 4con (25 of 466), showed marked increases in firing rates following visual stimulation and were distinguished by the time of peak visual activity (mean values of 70 and 161 ms, respectively). They were also distinguished by the time and character of the presaccadic ramping. The firing rate of category 3con neurons peaked at the time of the saccade and quickly returned to baseline, whereas the firing rate of category 4con peaked after the saccade and returned to baseline more slowly. Two of the three remaining categories, 6con (35/466) and 7con (64/466), also had clipped movement activity with late, weak visual responses. These two categories are differentiated by the absence (6con) or presence (7con) of delay period activity. The final category, having both visual and movement activity, 8con (33 of 466), showed only modest visual activity and may be more accurately described as a pure movement category. In either case, the movement-related activity peaked just after the saccade but returned slowly to baseline. An additional category, 5con (23 of 466), was not movement related per se, but exhibited a strong postsaccadic transient with a modest, early visual response.
Categories with response attenuation
Two categories of units showed distinct decreases in firing rate. The first of these, 9con (11 of 466), was unique in having an “off” response to visual stimulation, but it also showed clipped presaccadic ramping that peaked just before the saccade. This provides a fruitful contrast with the final visuomovement category, category 8con, which showed only a modest increase in firing rate but robust, unclipped perisaccadic ramping. This distinction may be a useful criterion in future studies examining the differences in stimulus-driven or goal-directed saccades. The second category with an off response, 10con (31 of 466), showed little or no visual modulation, but was sharply inhibited around the time of the saccade, which is characteristic of fixation neurons.
Relation to other functional characteristics
To preclude that these categories are accidental and arbitrary, we quantified several other characteristics of each neuron. These included typical measures such as response field size and center location, baseline discharge rate, and maximal response for both the visual and motor periods of modulation. These were supplemented by the following discharge variability metrics: Fano factor, CV, CV2, LV, and LVR accounting for a 5 ms refractory period. Finally, spike width was also measured.
First, omnibus Kruskal-Wallis tests were performed for each of these factors. Factors with significant differences include visual response field width (χ2(9274) = 18.300; p = 0.032), maximum visual response (χ2(9267) = 20.881; p = 0.013), and baseline firing rate (χ2(9401) = 28.600; p = 0.001).
Second, to take a more targeted approach, categories 1con and 2con were considered visual; categories 3con, 4con, 6con, and 7con were considered visuomovement; and categories 8con and 9con were considered movement related. The analyses were repeated separately for these sets of consensus categories. Among the visual categories, receptive field width was significantly larger for category 2con (60.6 ± 31°) than for category 1con (52.4 ± 34.8°; χ2(1136) = 4.568; p = 0.033). For movement-related categories, category 8con had significantly wider spikes (266.0 ± 122.5 μs) than category 9con (173.6 μs ± 67.2 μs; χ2(1,42) = 5.625; p = 0.018). No significant differences or trends were identified among the visuomotor categories. Thus, these consensus clusters identify differences in neuron types, even when the factors for which differences were identified were not included as parameters for clustering.
Cross-validation analysis
Clustering analyses can be problematic because the algorithms involved will yield as many categories as are requested from any data sample, regardless of the underlying category structure. The use of a minimum membership criterion and a maximum uncategorized percentage criterion aim to mitigate this, but the contribution of these criteria to eliminating the problem of oversplitting the categories cannot be directly quantified. Instead, to quantitatively assess the quality of the categorization, a classification analysis was used. This analysis uses a leave-one-out cross-validation approach in which a classifier is trained on the consensus category membership of all recorded units except one, then assigns that remaining unit to one of the consensus categories. To prevent underspecifying the classifier, principle components of the composite consensus matrix were used (see Materials and Methods). Classification accuracy is assessed by the percentage of units that, when left out of the training set, are assigned to the same category as that specified by the full consensus clustering algorithm.
Peak accuracy was 86.7%, which was achieved when the classifier was trained with eight principle components, after which cumulative variance explained remained on a plateau (Fig. 8a). The units misclassified by the classifier were identified. The percentage of units from each consensus category misclassified in each classifier category is depicted as a matrix of consensus category rows and classifier prediction columns (Fig. 8c). Generally, misclassified units were found in adjacent categories, with misclassifications becoming less frequent as the categories become further apart (Fig. 8c). Given the purposeful ordering of these units on a visual–motor spectrum, this is not surprising. The highest percentage of misclassified units (9.1%) was in category 9con, which also has the fewest members according to the consensus clustering algorithm.
Category pairs with frequent misclassifications are superimposed in Figure 8b. Categories 6con and 7con both show robust movement activity and similar visual activity. Categories 4con and 8con have nearly identical late-peaking movement activity. Categories 7con and 9con also have highly similar movement related activity. Categories 2con and 10con both have suppressed activity at the time of saccade. Categories 8con and 9con are the two categories of nearly pure movement activity. Finally, categories 7con and 8con both have weak visual activity as well as robust movement activity, although with different timing. Overall, though there are differences among these pairs of categories, there are features that explain why misclassifications could be made between these pairs.
To confirm that the peak classification accuracy was indeed greater than the nominal chance value of 10%, the above procedure was used with the category assignments randomly shuffled. For these shuffled assignments, the first eight principle components were used to train the classifier because this corresponds to the peak in classifier accuracy. Including additional components decreases accuracy, most likely due to overfitting. Shuffling was performed 1000 times. No randomized classification accuracy exceeded the empirical classification accuracy (mean = 8.2%; SD = 3.7%; range = 1.0–21.6%). This indicates that the consensus clustering algorithm categorizes neurons in a highly internally consistent manner. The robust reclassification of the original set also allows for new data to be categorized according to the present consensus categories.
Discussion
We applied a consensus clustering technique and identified 10 robust functional categories in FEF based on modulation of discharge rates alone. This categorization includes but exceeds the traditional categories. We will discuss the relationship of the new functional categories to the traditional categorization and possible functional and anatomic implications of these consensus clusters. We will conclude by considering the limitations and extensions of these consensus clustering techniques.
Correspondence with traditional functional categories
To compare the traditional and this new categorization, we assessed their overlap by calculating the proportion of consensus clusters identified as visual, movement, visuomovement, or unclassified. The traditional scheme and our new consensus clustering procedure show significant overlap (Fig. 9; ARI = 0.0931, p < 0.001). Thus, our procedure complements the traditional categorization. In fact, although not sought specifically, both postsaccadic and fixation neurons were identified via consensus clustering. This unsupervised discovery increases confidence that consensus clustering identifies natural neural categories.
We now compare the proportions of different categories to previous surveys. Bruce and Goldberg (1985) identified 40% of their sample as visually related, 40% visuomovement, and 20% movement only, not including postsaccadic and fixation neurons. Postsaccadic and fixation neurons account for 17% and 7% of their total sample, respectively. Schall (1991) identified 17% of the sample as visual, 41% as visuomovement, 22% movement, and 13% postsaccadic. When using the traditional categorization, 45% of the current sample was visual, 16% was visuomovement, and only 3% was movement. This proportion of visual neurons with respect to the whole sample is consistent with the findings of Bruce and Goldberg (1985), but not with Schall (1991). The proportions of neurons are more similar to earlier descriptions when consensus categories are considered with 38% visual, 31% visuomovement, 9% movement, 7% fixation, and 5% postsaccadic. Still, visuomovement, movement, and postsaccadic neurons are still underrepresented in the current studies relative to previous studies. This under-representation of movement-related neurons seems curious, for these are thought to be large pyramidal neurons in layer 5 (Fries, 1984; Segraves and Goldberg, 1987; Sommer and Wurtz, 2001), which should be easier to isolate. Similarly, fixation neurons are also layer 5 pyramidal cells (Izawa and Suzuki, 2014), but are instead found in the same proportion as in the study by Bruce and Goldberg (1985). Perhaps the linear electrode array failed to sample layer 5 neurons. Future reconstructions of the recording sites will determine whether laminar differences can explain the differences in proportions of neurons.
Another possible explanation for differences in category proportions concerns the nature of the electrodes. The neural spiking analyzed for this study was obtained with linear electrode arrays (Plexon U-probe and Neuronexus vector probe). The studies cited above sampled neurons with a variety of sharp electrodes including glass-coated, platinum-iridium, and tungsten. The differential sampling characteristics of various electrodes in FEF require further investigation.
A third possibility involves the eccentricity represented by the neuron samples across studies. The central visual field is represented laterally and peripheral field medially in FEF and rostrally adjacent cortex (Suzuki and Azuma, 1983), and RF size increases with eccentricity (Mayo et al., 2015). Lateral and medial FEF have qualitatively and quantitatively different patterns of connectivity (Schall et al., 1995a; Babapoor-Farrokhran et al., 2013; Markov et al., 2014). Convergence from the dorsal and ventral processing streams occurs in lateral but not in medial FEF. Lateral FEF, which is responsible for generating short saccades, receives visual afferents from the foveal representation in retinotopically organized areas, from areas that represent central vision in inferotemporal cortex and from other areas having no retinotopic order. In contrast, medial FEF, which is responsible for generating longer saccades, is innervated by the peripheral representation of retinotopically organized areas, from areas that emphasize peripheral vision or are multimodal and from other areas that have no retinotopic order or are auditory. Hence, neural spiking samples from lateral and medial FEF are likely to differ in a variety of as yet uncertain ways. Here, all stimuli were placed at 8° eccentricity, whereas tested locations in the study by Bruce and Goldberg (1985) ranged from 5° and 45°, and the study by Schall (1991) used 15° horizontal and 8° vertical. Systematic mapping across eccentricities is needed to resolve this question.
A fourth possibility involves the nature of tasks and reward contingencies. As noted above, the particular memory-guided saccade task used here is not identical to tasks used in previous studies. Factors like stimulus luminance, chromaticity, and contrast will need to be explored systematically (Krock and Moore, 2016). Moreover, FEF neurons are sensitive to reward contingency (Roesch and Olson, 2003) and other cognitive processes (Ferrera et al., 2009; Middlebrooks and Sommer, 2012; Teichert et al., 2014).
Functional differences can arise from structural differences in connectivity (Schall et al., 1995a; Markov et al., 2014), in morphology (Lewis and Lund, 1993), and in biophysical properties (McCormick et al., 1985; Connors and Gutnick, 1990; Krimer et al., 2005 Vigneswaran et al.,2011; Casale et al., 2015; Rocco et al., 2016), although the relative contributions of these factors are unknown in FEF. Indeed, using a related consensus clustering approach on physiologic measures from monkey prefrontal cortex, Ardid et al. (2015) reported four broad spiking, putative pyramidal cell classes and three narrow spiking, putative inhibitory cell classes, which were distinguished by sparse, bursting, or regular spike trains. Using a related agglomerative clustering approach, Zaitsev et al. (2012) also reported four classes of neurons visually identified as pyramidal cells in vitro that were also distinguished by sparse, bursting, or regular spike trains. These authors also identified three classes of inhibitory interneurons identified morphologically in vitro that were distinguished by firing rate variability measures, and these classes were each associated with calcium binding proteins parvalbumin, calbindin, or calretinin.
In this sample, we found no significant differences in measures of firing rate variability across the consensus categories. However, spike width differed between the two movement-related categories, indicating some differences in biophysical characteristics. Curiously, spike width did not differ between consensus clusters identified as visual, movement related, and visuomovement, which is at odds with findings from previous studies (Cohen et al., 2009a; Ding and Gold, 2012, Thiele et al., 2016). Further investigation is necessary to determine the reason for this difference. To that end, the consensus clustering method can be extended to incorporate biophysical characteristics such as spike polarity and phase (Gold et al., 2009), spike width (Cohen et al., 2009a; Vigneswaran et al., 2011), spike timing patterns (Holt et al., 1996; Nawrot et al., 2008; Cohen et al., 2009a; Shinomoto et al., 2009), and Fano factor (Purcell et al., 2012b).
Possible functional implications
Most of the consensus categories were characterized by pronounced perisaccadic activity. Many such neurons also had pronounced visual responses (3con, 4con, 5con, 6con, and 7con) and will be discussed below. Categories 8con and 9con were distinguished by (1) weaker modulation of opposite signs after the target appeared, (2) time of peak saccade-related activity, (3) duration of activity after the saccade, and (4) spike width. Both patterns of modulation have been reported previously in FEF (Bruce and Goldberg, 1985; Hanes and Schall, 1996; Everling and Munoz, 2000; Sommer and Wurtz, 2001; Lawrence et al., 2005). The peak activity of category 9con neurons coincides with saccade initiation, and discharge rate is reset by saccade termination. Such clipped neurons have been reported in FEF by one study (Hanes et al., 1995) but not another (Segraves and Park, 1993). Clipped movement neurons have been associated with saccade dynamics in superior colliculus (Waitzman et al., 1991). Confirming the presence of clipped movement neurons in FEF would substantiate the hypothesis that FEF contributes to the dynamics of saccade production (Schiller et al., 1987; Dias and Segraves, 1999; Peel et al., 2014). The activity of category 8con has a more sluggish relationship to saccade timing, peaking after the saccade and resetting well after saccade termination. These properties are inconsistent with a direct role in saccade production. Further investigation with other task conditions is necessary. For example, another approach to determining whether individual neurons are involved in controlling saccade initiation involves testing with the saccade-countermanding task (Hanes et al., 1998; Murthy et al., 2009). Alternatively, distinct functions may be revealed when planning saccade sequences (Phillips and Segraves, 2010).
Relative to category 8con, category 9con neurons had significantly narrower spike widths. This does not entail, necessarily, that 9con neurons are inhibitory interneurons. In primary motor cortex, some identified corticospinal pyramidal neurons have narrow spike widths (Vigneswaran et al., 2011) presumably because these neurons have a fast potassium channel Kv3.1b subunit (Ichinohe et al., 2004). If corticotectal and corticopontine neurons are analogous to corticospinal neurons, then spike width may be misleading in the identification of projection neurons. However, whether layer 5 neurons in FEF stain positively for fast potassium channels is unknown.
The analysis also identified a consensus cluster with characteristics of fixation neurons (10con). These neurons seem involved in the active maintenance of fixation and may release inhibition on presaccadic movement neurons to produce saccades (Segraves and Goldberg, 1987; Hanes et al., 1998). The cluster exhibited a modest visual response. If these neurons do indeed inhibit presaccadic movement neurons, then this could be the origin of the brief reduction of discharge rate characteristic of the movement neuron consensus cluster 9con. Greater diversity of this category may be found if tested with pursuit eye movements (Izawa and Suzuki, 2014).
Most of the consensus categories were characterized by pronounced visual responses. This is consistent with previous descriptions of FEF neural properties (Mohler et al., 1973; Bruce and Goldberg, 1985; Schall, 1991). Two consensus clusters were distinguished by strong visual responses and no modulation associated with saccades (1con and 2con). Category 1con had an earlier peak response and no delay activity, whereas category 2con had a later peak response and clear activity during the memory delay. The receptive fields of category 1con were narrower than those of category 2con. The diversity of FEF visual responses along multiple dimensions is well known in the early visual pathway. For example, the well known distinction of transient and phasic visual responses (Cleland et al., 1971) is evident in FEF (Sato et al., 2001). The diversity of visual responses likely arises principally from the diversity of connectivity. As noted above, FEF is reciprocally connected with an unusually large number of extrastriate visual cortical areas, and pronounced differences in connectivity distinguish medial and lateral FEF (Schall et al., 1995a; Babapoor-Farrokhran et al., 2013; Markov et al., 2014). Subcortical afferents can also influence visual responses; neurons in FEF that are activated orthodromically by SC stimulation have visual and saccade-related responses (Sommer and Wurtz, 1998). The extent to which visual response properties vary with cortical and subcortical connectivity is unresolved. The diversity of visual responses in FEF also relates to the variety of cortical areas in which FEF axons terminate. For example, V4 is influenced by visual neurons in layer 2/3 of FEF (Pouget et al., 2009; Noudoost and Moore, 2011; Gregoriou et al., 2012). However, FEF neurons projecting to V4 receive input from area 46, whereas the FEF neurons projecting to MT receive input from area 46 plus supplementary eye field. (Ninomiya et al., 2012). Thus, intracortical projections from FEF convey different signals. How much such signals vary across cortical targets of FEF efferents is unknown.
Most of the categories with visual responses were also characterized by modulation associated with saccade production. These are typically referred to as visuomovement neurons. While these data support no conclusions about the unique functional contributions of the four visuomovement categories, several characteristics warrant discussion. First, categories 3con and 4con have noticeably stronger visual responses than categories 6con and 7con. Perhaps categories 3con and 4con occupy an earlier position in the visuomotor transformation. The saccade-related activity of these two categories is similar, so they may contribute equally to the production of saccades. However, the timing of the peak visual responses differs in a manner similar to categories 1con and 2con. The earlier peak activity of category 3con is consistent with receiving magnocellular pathway inputs, perhaps via category 1con neurons. Meanwhile, the later peak activity of category 4con is consistent with receiving parvocellular pathway inputs directly or via category 2con neurons.
Categories 6con and 7con have similar visual responses but are distinguished by the magnitude of delay activity and the return to baseline following saccade. Category 6con activity returns to baseline quickly after the saccade, indicating that this category may be more intimately involved in saccade dynamics than category 7con. This is consistent with the higher delay activity in category 7con, which may indicate that category 7con is primarily involved in maintaining the stimulus location in working memory and therefore occupies an executive role as opposed to a direct role in saccade production. Of course, the lack of corroborating differences in other factors, including tuning characteristics, spike timing, and spike widths, may indicate that these categories are an excessive parsing of one continuum or that the measures are insensitive, but additional work is warranted to determine whether this is the case. For example, during visual search tasks, all visually responsive neurons respond equivalently to a target or a distractor in the receptive field (Schall et al., 1995b). Visual neurons with transient responses do not contribute to the selection of the target from distractors, but visually responsive neurons with prolonged activity do select the target of a search array when saccades are accurate (14% transient and 86% sustained; Thompson et al., 1996). When arbitrary stimulus–response mapping is required after visual search, many visually responsive neurons select the attended stimuli, while others select only the endpoint of the saccade (Sato and Schall, 2003). Further research is needed with this task and these categorization methods to determine how this previously observed distinction maps onto these new functional categories. When monkeys perform visual search for a target among distractors of fixed features, ∼50% of visually responsive neurons in FEF exhibit features selective from the initial response (Bichot et al., 1996). The same distinction may also explain feature-based attention differences identified in FEF neurons in other studies (Bichot and Schall, 2002; Gregoriou et al., 2009; Zhou and Desimone, 2011). Other distinctions among FEF neuron categories have been described in perceptual choice (Ding and Gold, 2012). Finally, the relationship of the dynamics of visuomovement and movement neurons to saccade initiation can be distinguished using the saccade-countermanding task (Ray et al., 2009).
Further insight may be gained by testing how the different categories contribute to eye–head coordination and visual–motor reference frame updating (Sajad et al., 2015, 2016). FEF neurons have also been implicated in remapping and trans-saccadic stability (Umeno and Goldberg, 1997; Crapse and Sommer, 2008, 2012; Shin and Sommer, 2012). These operations require information about the just executed saccade. A single consensus cluster of postsaccadic neurons was identified (5con). These neurons also had visual responses. Previous research has suggested that this type of neuron can support remapping and trans-saccadic stability by signaling the vector of the saccade that was just executed (Goldberg and Bruce, 1990). To produce sequences of saccades without visual guidance, the vector of the most recent saccade would be subtracted from the vector from the initial fixation point to the location of the second stimulus to account for the location of the second stimulus relative to the new fixation point rather than the point from which the location was initially encoded.
Antidromic stimulation studies agree that movement and fixation neurons project to the SC and brainstem, but they disagree about the projection of visual and visuomovement neurons (Segraves and Goldberg, 1987; Sommer and Wurtz, 2001). Perhaps the disagreement may be resolved by considering more refined categories of neurons. For example, perhaps only visuomovement neurons belonging to categories 6con or 7con with modest visual response, relative to categories 3con and 4con, project from FEF to SC.
Limitations and extensions of clustering procedures
Each of the individual clustering procedures is limited by the distance measure used to calculate pairwise similarity, by the measurement of the responses, and by the quality of the discharge rate samples. Different distance measures emphasize different aspects of the variability across units. Although Euclidean distance emphasizes similarity in absolute discharge rates, correlation distance emphasizes similarity in the pattern of modulation of discharge rates. Different measurements of the variation of discharge rate emphasize different aspects of the variability across units. Measuring the mean firing rate in different epochs captures absolute discharge rates but ignores dynamics. Measuring the slopes of the SDF in different epochs ignores absolute discharge rates. However, measuring both the means and slopes across many epochs or, indeed, using the SDF from the entire trial can expose the clustering algorithm to excessive incidental variation.
Different approaches to scaling the SDF across units emphasize different aspects of the variability across units. As shown in Figures 3–5, different methods of scaling the SDF across units can result in category means that do not accurately represent the individuals comprising those categories. Naturally, different scaling procedures emphasize useful information about the units. For example, z scoring the SDF based on the prestimulus baseline activity emphasizes the magnitude of modulation relative to the variation in the baseline. On the other hand, z scoring the SDF based on the entire trial reduces the skewed variation of discharge rates. Analytical choices must be made; hence, confidence in the outcome of every particular clustering pipeline can be questioned.
Consensus clustering increases confidence in distinctions identified across measurements and clustering procedures by minimizing spurious classifications arising from incidental analysis choices or unreliable data. Moreover, the consensus clustering approach affords the opportunity to include as many other measures and clustering procedures as desired. In particular, biophysical spiking properties are certainly useful for categorizing neurons. The eventual inclusion of such features will surely add complexity but should certainly approach an accurate account of the true diversity of functional neuron categories in FEF. A correct account of such diversity is necessary to support the next generation of microcircuit models (Mitchell and Zipser, 2003; Brown et al., 2004; Hamker, 2006; Heinzle et al., 2007).
Acknowledgments
We thank Joshua Cosman and Wolf Zinke for sharing data, Andy Tomarken for statistical advice, Thilo Womelsdorf for discussions of previous versions of this manuscript, the Vanderbilt Advanced Center for Computing for Research and Education for access to the high-performance computing cluster, and the reviewers for helpful suggestions.
Synthesis
Reviewing Editor: Tatiana Pasternak, University of Rochester
Decisions are customarily a result of the Reviewing Editor and the peer reviewers coming together and discussing their recommendations until a consensus is reached. When revisions are invited, a fact-based synthesis statement explaining their decision and outlining what is needed to prepare a revision will be listed below. The following reviewer(s) agreed to reveal their identity: Tirin Moore. Note: If this manuscript was transferred from JNeurosci and a decision was made to accept the manuscript without peer review, a brief statement to this effect will instead be what is listed below.
The manuscript revised in response to the JN reviews has now been reviewed by two reviewers. While both reviewers appreciated the need for a more principled classification scheme of FEF neurons, they raised reservations about the lack of explicit quantitative analyses to validate the clustering approach, the reservations also raised in the initial review for JN. These issues must be addressed before the manuscript can be considered for publication in eNeuro. These are outlined below:
1) Address the issue of single neuron isolation outlined by Reviewer 2 by providing additional proof that the neurons included in the classification were well isolated
2) Validate your cluster classification method with additional methods, such as classification analysis (linear or non-linear classifiers that use cross validation)
3) Following the suggestion of the initial JN Reviewer 2 who commented that the paper is “very dense”, please expand Fig. 1 to highlight the difference between the different methods.
REVIEWER 1
1) The authors should have additional proof of single neuron isolation
2) They should consider some methods such as classification analysis (linear or non-linear classifiers that use cross validation) in order to validate their cluster classification method.
REVIEWER 2
Advances the Field (Required)
It is not yet sufficiently clear if the manuscript advances the field. More details are needed.
Comments to the Authors (Required)
Lowe and Schall deploy a novel clustering approach, consensus clustering, on the issue of functional classification of the responses of frontal eye field neurons during a memory-guided saccade task. Historically, only a few classes have been described (e.g. visual, visuomovement, movement) in spite of the fact that a great deal of heterogeneity is evident among neurons classified into the same category. Clearly, arriving at a more principled classification scheme will be needed in order to understand the contribution the FEF makes to its synaptic targets and to behavior, and so this work addresses an important problem, not only for the FEF, but for the classification of neurons in any structure.
Similar to the comments raised by the first two reviewers, I have a couple of important concerns regarding the analysis and presentation of the data.
1. Similar to reviewer 2, I was surprised at the marked lack of explicit quantitative analyses to describe and validate the clustering approach. In the rebuttal, the authors state that they have addressed this issue, but it is not terribly evidence. It would be extremely valuable to, for example, provide an alternative statistical validation of a particular cluster subtype (e.g. k-means clustering, etc) to illustrate clearly that the c.c. approach arrives at subclasses that clearly differ in a qualitative manner (e.g. separate modes) rather than just a quantitative one.
2. One important challenge that exists in any attempt to optimally identify qualitatively different functional subsets of neurons with extracellular recordings is the inevitable variation in the quality of single-neuron waveform isolation. Depending on the electrode impedance, cell body size, and other factors, some isolated waveforms may be more or less contaminated with the spikes from other neurons, or have spike trains that are undersampled. In both cases, the SDFs will be distorted in different, but not random, ways depending on when other neurons fire. These effects in and of themselves, it seems, may create distinct classes of response profiles. The authors should address this, and discuss how they can address it, if at all.
References
- Ardid S, Vinck M, Kaping D, Marquez S, Everling S, Womelsdorf T (2015) Mapping of functionally characterized cell classes onto canonical circuit operations in primate prefrontal cortex. J Neurosci 35:2975–2991. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2700-14.2015 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Babapoor-Farrokhran S, Hutchison RM, Gati JS, Menon RS, Everling S (2013) Functional connectivity patterns of medial and lateral macaque frontal eye fields reveal distinct visuomotor networks. J Neurophysiol 109:2560–2570. 10.1152/jn.01000.2012 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bichot NP, Schall JD (2002) Priming in macaque frontal cortex during popout visual search: feature-based facilitation and location-based inhibition of return. J Neurosci 22:4675–4685. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.22-11-04675.2002 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bichot NP, Schall JD, Thompson KG (1996) Visual feature selectivity in frontal eye fields induced by experience in mature macaques. Nature 381:697–699. 10.1038/381697a0 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Boucher L, Palmeri TJ, Logan GD, Schall JD (2007) Inhibitory control in mind and brain: an interactive race model of countermanding saccades. Psychol Rev 114:376–397. 10.1037/0033-295X.114.2.376 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bradley PS, Fayyad UM (1998) Refining initial points for k-means clustering In: ICML ’98, Proceedings of the Fifteenth International Conference on Machine Learning, pp 91–99. Burlington, MA: Morgan Kaufmann. [Google Scholar]
- Brown JW, Bullock D, Grossberg S (2004) How laminar frontal cortex and basal ganglia circuits interact to control planned and reactive saccades. Neural Netw 17:471–510. 10.1016/j.neunet.2003.08.006 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bruce CJ, Goldberg ME (1985) Primate frontal eye fields. I. Single neurons discharging before saccades. J Neurophysiol 53:603–635. 10.1152/jn.1985.53.3.603 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Casale AE, Foust AJ, Bal T, McCormick DA (2015) Cortical interneuron subtypes vary in their axonal action potential properties. J Neurosci 35:15555–15567. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1467-13.2015 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Celebi ME, Kingravi HA (2012) Deterministic initialization of the K-means algorithm using hierarchical clustering. Intern J Pattern Recognit Artif Intell 26:1250018 10.1142/S0218001412500188 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Chen X, Zirsnak M, Moore T (2018) Dissonant representations of visual space in prefrontal cortex during eye movements. Cell Rep 22:2039–2052. 10.1016/j.celrep.2018.01.078 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Clark KL, Noudoost B, Moore T (2012) Persistent spatial information in the frontal eye field during object-based short-term memory. J Neurosci 32:10907–10914. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1450-12.2012 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cleland BG, Dubin MW, Levick WR (1971) Sustained and transient neurones in the cat's retina and lateral geniculate nucleus. J Physiol 217:473–496. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cohen JY, Pouget P, Heitz RP, Woodman GF, Schall JD (2009a) Biophysical support for functionally distinct cell types in the frontal eye field. J Neurophysiol 101:912–916. 10.1152/jn.90272.2008 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cohen JY, Heitz RP, Woodman GF, Schall JD (2009b) Neural basis of the set-size effect in frontal eye field: timing of attention during visual search. J Neurophysiol 101:1699–1704. 10.1152/jn.00035.2009 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Connors BW, Gutnick MJ (1990) Intrinsic firing patterns of diverse neocortical neurons. Trends Neurosci 13:99–104. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Costello MG, Zhu D, May PJ, Salinas E, Stanford TR (2016) Task dependence of decision- and choice-related activity in monkey oculomotor thalamus. J Neurophysiol 115:581–601. 10.1152/jn.00592.2015 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Crapse TB, Sommer MA (2008) The frontal eye field as a prediction map. Prog Brain Res 171:383-390. 10.1016/S0079-6123(08)00656-0 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Crapse TB, Sommer MA (2012) Frontal eye field neurons assess visual stability across saccades. J Neurosci 32:2835–2845. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1320-11.2012 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- DeFelipe J (1997) Types of neurons, synaptic connections and chemical characteristics of cells immunoreactive for calbindin-D28K, parvalbumin and calretinin in the neocortex. J Chem Neuroanat 14:1–19. 10.1016/S0891-0618(97)10013-8 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dias EC, Segraves MA (1999) Muscimol-induced inactivation of monkey frontal eye field: effects on visually and memory-guided saccades. J Neurophysiol 81:2191–2214. 10.1152/jn.1999.81.5.2191 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ding L, Gold JI (2012) Neural correlates of perceptual decision making before, during, and after decision commitment in monkey frontal eye field. Cereb Cortex 22:1052–1067. 10.1093/cercor/bhr178 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Druckmann S, Hill S, Schürmann F, Markram H, Segev I (2013) A hierarchical structure of cortical interneuron electrical diversity revealed by automated statistical analysis. Cereb Cortex 23:2994–3006. 10.1093/cercor/bhs290 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Elsley JK, Nagy B, Cushing SL, Corneil BD (2007) Widespread presaccadic recruitment of neck muscles by stimulation of the primate frontal eye fields. J Neurophysiol 98:1333–1354. 10.1152/jn.00386.2007 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Everitt Bs, Landau S, Leese M, Stahl D (2011) Cluster analysis. Chichester, UK: Wiley. [Google Scholar]
- Everling S, Munoz DP (2000) Neuronal correlates for preparatory set associated with pro-saccades and anti-saccades in the primate frontal eye field. J Neurosci 20:387–400. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fernandes HL, Stevenson IH, Phillips AN, Segraves MA, Kording KP (2014) Saliency and saccade encoding in the frontal eye field during natural scene search. Cereb Cortex 24:3232–3245. 10.1093/cercor/bht179 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ferrera VP, Yanike M, Cassanello C (2009) Frontal eye field neurons signal changes in decision criteria. Nat Neurosci 12:1458–1462. 10.1038/nn.2434 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fries W (1984) Cortical projections to the superior colliculus in the macaque monkey: a retrograde study using horseradish peroxidase. J Comp Neurol 230:55–76. 10.1002/cne.902300106 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Glaser JI, Wood DK, Lawlor PN, Ramkumar P, Kording KP, Segraves MA (2016) The role of expected reward in frontal eye field during natural scene search. J Neurophysiol 116:1328–1343. 10.1152/jn.00119.2016 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Goder A, Filkov V (2008) Consensus clustering algorithms: comparison and refinement In: Proceedings of the Tenth Workshop on Algorithm Engineering and Experiments, ALENEX 2008, San Francisco, California, USA, January 19, 2008 (Munro JI. , Wagner D. , eds), pp 109–117. Philadelphia: Society for Industrial and Applied Mathematics. [Google Scholar]
- Gold C, Girardin CC, Martin KA, Koch C (2009) High-amplitude positive spikes recorded extracellularly in cat visual cortex. J Neurophysiol 102:3340–3351. 10.1152/jn.91365.2008 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Goldberg ME, Bruce CJ (1990) Primate frontal eye fields. III. Maintenance of a spatially accurate saccade signal. J Neurophysiol 64:489–508. 10.1152/jn.1990.64.2.489 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gregoriou GG, Gotts SJ, Zhou H, Desimone R (2009) High-frequency, long-range coupling between prefrontal and visual cortex during attention. Science 324:1207–1210. 10.1126/science.1171402 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gregoriou GG, Gotts SJ, Desimone R (2012) Cell-type-specific synchronization of neural activity in FEF with V4 during attention. Neuron 73:581–594. 10.1016/j.neuron.2011.12.019 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gries ST, Stefanowitsch A (2010) Cluster analysis and the identification of collexeme classes In: Empirical and experimental methods in cognitive/functional research (Rice S, Newman J, eds), pp 73–90. [Google Scholar]
- Hamker FH (2006) Modeling feature-based attention as an active top-down inference process. Biosystems 86:91–99. 10.1016/j.biosystems.2006.03.010 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hanes DP, Schall JD (1996) Neural control of voluntary movement initiation. Science 274:427–430. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hanes DP, Thompson KG, Schall JD (1995) Relationship of presaccadic activity in frontal eye field and supplementary eye field to saccade initiation in macaque: Poisson spike train analysis. Exp Brain Res 103:85–96. 10.1007/BF00241967 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hanes DP, Patterson WF, Schall JD (1998) Role of frontal eye field in countermanding saccades: visual, movement and fixation activity. J Neurophysiol 79:817–834. 10.1152/jn.1998.79.2.817 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Heinzle J, Hepp K, Martin KA (2007) A microcircuit model of the frontal eye fields. J Neurosci 27:9341–9353. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0974-07.2007 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Holt GR, Softky WR, Koch C, Douglas RJ (1996) Comparison of discharge variability in vitro and in vivo in cat visual cortex neurons. J Neurophysiol 75:1806–1814. 10.1152/jn.1996.75.5.1806 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hubert L, Arabie P (1985) Comparing partitions. J Classif 2:193–218. 10.1007/BF01908075 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Huerta MF, Krubitzer LA, Kaas JH (1986) Frontal eye field as defined by intracortical microstimulation in squirrel monkeys, owl monkeys, and macaque monkeys: I. Subcortical connections. J Comp Neurol 253:415–439. 10.1002/cne.902530402 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Huerta MF, Krubitzer LA, Kaas JH (1987) Frontal eye field as defined by intracortical microstimulation in squirrel monkeys, owl monkeys, and macaque monkeys. II. Cortical connections. J Comp Neurol 265:332–361. 10.1002/cne.902650304 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ichinohe N, Watakabe A, Miyashita T, Yamamori T, Hashikawa T, Rockland KS (2004) A voltage-gated potassium channel, Kv3.1b, is expressed by a subpopulation of large pyramidal neurons in layer 5 of the macaque monkey cortex. Neuroscience 129:179–185. 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2004.08.005 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Izawa Y, Suzuki H (2014) Activity of fixation neurons in the monkey frontal eye field during smooth pursuit eye movements. J Neurophysiol 112:249–262. 10.1152/jn.00816.2013 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Izawa Y, Suzuki H (2018) Motor action of the frontal eye field on the eyes and neck in the monkey. J Neurophysiol 119:2082–2090. 10.1152/jn.00577.2017 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Joiner WM, Cavanaugh J, Wurtz RH (2013) Compression and suppression of shifting receptive field activity in frontal eye field neurons. J Neurosci 33:18259–18269. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2964-13.2013 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Joshua M, Elias S, Levine O, Bergman H (2007) Quantifying the isolation quality of extracellularly recorded action potentials. J Neurosci Methods 163:267–282. 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2007.03.012 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Khayat PS, Pooresmaeili A, Roelfsema RP (2009) Time course of attentional modulation in the frontal eye field during curve tracing. J Neurophysiol 101:1813–1822. 10.1152/jn.91050.2008 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Knight TA (2012) Contribution of the frontal eye field to gaze shifts in the head-unrestrained rhesus monkey: neuronal activity. Neuroscience 225:213–236. 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2012.08.050 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Krimer LS, Zaitsev AV, Czanner G, Kröner S, González-Burgos G, Povysheva NV, Iyengar S, Barrionuevo G, Lewis LD (2005) Cluster analysis-based physiological classification and morphological properties of inhibitory eurons in layers 2-3 of monkey dorsolateral prefrontal cortex. J Neurophysiol 94:3009–3022. 10.1152/jn.00156.2005 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Krock RM, Moore T (2016) Visual sensitivity of frontal eye field neurons during the preparation of saccadic eye movements. J Neurophysiol 116:2882–2891. 10.1152/jn.01140.2015 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lapointe F-J, Legendre P (1994) A classification of pure malt scotch whiskies. Appl Stat 43:237–257. 10.2307/2986124 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Lawrence BM, White RL, III, Snyder LH (2005) Delay-period activity in visual, visuomovement, and movement neurons in the frontal eye field. J Neurophysiol 94:1498–1508. 10.1152/jn.00214.2005 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lee KM, Keller EL (2008) Neural activity in the frontal eye fields modulated by the number of alternatives in target choice. J Neurosci 28:2242–2251. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3596-07.2008 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lewis DA, Lund JS (1993) Local circuit neurons of developing and mature macaque prefrontal cortex: Golgi and immunocytochemical characteristics. J Comp Neurol 328:282–312. 10.1002/cne.903280209 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lochner C, Hemmings S, Kinnear C, Stein DJ (2005) Cluster analysis of obsessive-compulsive spectrum disorders in patients with obsessive-compulsive disorder: clinical and genetic correlates. Compr Psychiatry 46:14–19. 10.1016/j.comppsych.2004.07.020 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Markov NT, Vezoli J, Chameau P, Falchier A, Quilodran R, Huissoud C, Lamy C, Misery P, Giroud P, Ullman S, Barone P, Dehay C, Knoblauch K, Kennedy H (2014) Anatomy of hierarchy: feedforward and feedback pathways in macaque visual cortex. J Comp Neurol 522:225–259. 10.1002/cne.23458 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mayo JP, DiTomasso AR, Sommer MA, Smith MA (2015) Dynamics of visual receptive fields in the macaque frontal eye field. J Neurophysiol 114:3201–3210. 10.1152/jn.00746.2015 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McCormick DA, Connors BW, Lighthall JW, Prince DA (1985) Comparative electrophysiology of pyramidal and sparsely spiny stellate neurons of the neocortex. J Neurophysiol 54:782–806. 10.1152/jn.1985.54.4.782 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Middlebrooks PG, Sommer MA (2012) Neuronal correlates of metacognition in primate frontal cortex. Neuron 75:517–530. 10.1016/j.neuron.2012.05.028 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mitchell JF, Zipser D (2003) Sequential memory-guided saccades and target selection: a neural model of the frontal eye fields. Vision Res 43:2669–2695. 10.1016/S0042-6989(03)00468-1 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mitchell JF, Sundberg KA, Reynolds JH (2007) Differential attention-dependent response modulation across cell classes in macaque visual area V4. Neuron 55:131–141. 10.1016/j.neuron.2007.06.018 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mohler CW, Goldberg ME, Wurtz RH (1973) Visual receptive fields of frontal eye field neurons. Brain Res 61:385–389. 10.1016/0006-8993(73)90543-X [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Murthy A, Ray S, Shorter SM, Schall JD, Thompson KG (2009) Neural control of visual search by frontal eye field: effects of unexpected target displacement on visual selection and saccade preparation. J Neurophysiol 101:2485–2506. 10.1152/jn.90824.2008 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nawrot MP, Boucsein C, Rodriguez-Molina V, Riehle A, Aertsen A, Rotter S (2008) Measurement of variability dynamics in cortical spike trains. J Neurosci Methods 169:374–390. 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2007.10.013 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ninomiya T, Sawamura H, Inoue K, Takada M (2012) Segregated pathways carrying frontally derived top-down signals to visual areas MT and V4 in macaques. J Neurosci 32:6851–858. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.6295-11.2012 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Noudoost B, Moore T (2011) Control of visual cortical signals by prefrontal dopamine. Nature 474:372–375. 10.1038/nature09995 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Noudoost B, Clark K, Moore T (2014) A distinct contribution of the frontal eye field to the visual representation of saccadic targets. J Neurosci 34:3687–3698. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3824-13.2014 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nowak LG, Azouz R, Sanchez-Vives MV, Gray CM, McCormick D (2003) Electrophysiological classes of cat primary visual cortical neurons in vivo as revealed by quantitative analyses. J Neurophysiol 89:1541–1566. 10.1152/jn.00580.2002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pachitariu M, Steinmetz N, Kadir S, Carandini M, Harris KD (2016) Fast and accurate spike sorting of high-channel count probes with KiloSort In: Advances in Neural Information Processing Systems 29 (NIPS 2016) (Lee DD, Sugiyama M, Luxburg UV, Guyon I, Garnett R, eds). La Jolla, CA: Neural Information Processing Systems Foundation. [Google Scholar]
- Peel TR, Johnston K, Lomber SG, Corneil BD (2014) Bilateral saccadic deficits following large and reversible inactivation of unilateral frontal eye field. J Neurophysiol 111:415–433. 10.1152/jn.00398.2013 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Peña JM, Lozano JA, Larrañaga P (1999) An empirical comparison of four initialization methods for the K-Means algorithm. Pattern Recognit Lett 20:1027–1040. 10.1016/S0167-8655(99)00069-0 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Phillips AN, Segraves MA (2010) Predictive activity in macaque frontal eye field neurons during natural scene searching. J Neurophysiol 103:1238–1252. 10.1152/jn.00776.2009 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pouget P, Stepniewska I, Crowder EA, Leslie MW, Emeric EE, Nelson MJ, Schall JD (2009) Visual and motor connectivity and the distribution of calcium-binding proteins in macaque frontal eye field: implications for saccade target selection. Front Neuroanat 3:1–14. 10.3389/neuro.05.002.2009 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Purcell BA, Schall JD, Logan GD, Palmeri TJ (2012a) From salience to saccades: multiple-alternative gated stochastic accumulator model of visual search. J Neurosci 32:3433–3446. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4622-11.2012 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Purcell BA, Heitz RP, Cohen JY, Schall JD (2012b) Response variability of frontal eye field neurons modulates with sensory input and saccade preparation but not visual search salience. J Neurophysiol 108:2737–2750. 10.1152/jn.00613.2012 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ray S, Pouget P, Schall JD (2009) Functional distinction between visuomovement and movement neurons in macaque frontal eye field during saccade countermanding. J Neurophysiol 102:3091–3100. 10.1152/jn.00270.2009 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Reinhart RM, Heitz RP, Purcell BA, Weigand PK, Schall JD, Woodman GF (2012) Homologous mechanisms of visuospatial working memory maintenance in macaque and human. Properties and sources. J Neurosci 32:7711–7722. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0215-12.2012 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rocco BR, Sweet RA, Lewis DA, Fish KN (2016) GABA-synthesizing enzymes in calbindin and calretinin neurons in monkey prefrontal cortex. Cereb Cortex 26:2191–2204. 10.1093/cercor/bhv051 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Roesch MR, Olson CR (2003) Impact of expected reward on neuronal activity in prefrontal cortex, frontal and supplementary eye fields and premotor cortex. J Neurophysiol 90:1766–1789. 10.1152/jn.00019.2003 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sajad A, Sadeh M, Keith GP, Yan X, Wang H, Crawford JD (2015) Visual-motor transformation within frontal eye fields during head-unrestrained gaze shifts in the monkey. Cereb Cortex 25:3932–3952. 10.1093/cercor/bhu279 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sajad A, Sadeh M, Yan X, Wang H, Crawford JD (2016) Transition from target to gaze coding in primate frontal eye field during memory delay and memory-motor transformation. eNeuro 3:ENEURO.0040-16.2016. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sato TR, Schall JD (2003) Effects of stimulus-response compatibility on neural selection in frontal eye field. Neuron 38:637–648. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sato TR, Murthy A, Thompson KG, Schall JD (2001) Search efficiency but not response interference affects visual selection in frontal eye field. Neuron 30:583–591. 10.1016/S0896-6273(01)00304-X [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schafer RJ, Moore T (2011) Selective attention from voluntary control of neurons in prefrontal cortex. Science 332:1568–1571. 10.1126/science.1199892 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schall JD (1991) Neuronal activity related to visually guided saccades in the frontal eye fields of rhesus monkeys: comparison with supplementary eye fields. J Neurophysiol 66:559–579. 10.1152/jn.1991.66.2.559 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schall JD, Morel A, Kaas JH (1993) Topography of supplementary eye field afferents to frontal eye field in macaque: implications for mapping between saccade coordinate systems. Vis Neurosci 10:385–393. 10.1017/S0952523800003771 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schall JD, Morel A, King DJ, Bullier J (1995a) Topography of visual cortex connections with frontal eye field in macaque: convergence and segregation of processing streams. J Neurosci 15:4464–4487. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.15-06-04464.1995 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schall JD, Hanes DP, Thompson KG, King DJ (1995b) Saccade target selection in frontal eye field of macaque. I. Visual and premovement activation. J Neurosci 15:6905–6918. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.15-10-06905.1995 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schiller PH, Sandell JH, Maunsell JH (1987) The effect of frontal eye field and superior colliculus lesions on saccadic latencies in the rhesus monkey. J Neurophysiol 57:1033–1049. 10.1152/jn.1987.57.4.1033 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Segraves MA, Goldberg ME (1987) Functional properties of corticotectal neurons in the monkey's frontal eye field. J Neurophysiol 58:1387–1419. 10.1152/jn.1987.58.6.1387 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Segraves MA, Park K (1993) The relationship of monkey frontal eye field activity to saccade dynamics. J Neurophysiol 69:1880–1889. 10.1152/jn.1993.69.6.1880 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sharp PM, Tuohy TM, Mosurski KR (1986) Codon usage in yeast: cluster analysis clearly differentiates highly and lowly expressed genes. Nucleic Acids Res 14:5125–5143. 10.1093/nar/14.13.5125 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shin S, Sommer MA (2012) Division of labor in frontal eye field neurons during presaccadic remapping of visual receptive fields. J Neurophysiol 108:2144–2159. 10.1152/jn.00204.2012 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shinomoto S, Shima K, Tanji J (2003) Differences in spiking patterns among cortical neurons. Neural Comput 15:2823–2842. 10.1162/089976603322518759 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shinomoto S, Kim H, Shimokawa T, Matsuno N, Funahashi S, Shima K, Fujita I, Tamura H, Doi T, Kawano K, Inaba N, Fukushima K, Kurkin S, Kurata K, Taira M, Tsutsui K, Komatsu H, Ogawa T, Koida K, Tanji J, et al. (2009) Relating neuronal firing patterns to functional differentiation of cerebral cortex. PLoS Comput Biol 5:e1000433. 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1000433 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sokal RR, Michener CD (1958) A statistical method for evaluating systematic relationships. A statistical method for evaluating systematic relationships. University of Kansas Sci Bull 38:1409–1438. [Google Scholar]
- Soltani A, Noudoost B, Moore T (2013) Dissociable dopaminergic control of saccadic target selection and its implications for reward modulation. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 110:3579–3584. 10.1073/pnas.1221236110 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sommer MA, Wurtz RH (1998) Frontal eye field neurons orthodromically activated from the superior colliculus. J Neurophysiol 80:3331–3335. 10.1152/jn.1998.80.6.3331 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sommer MA, Wurtz RH (2001) Frontal eye field sends delay activity related to movement, memory, and vision to the superior colliculus. J Neurophysiol 85:1673–1685. 10.1152/jn.2001.85.4.1673 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stanton GB, Bruce CJ, Goldberg ME (1993) Topography of projections to the frontal lobe from the macaque frontal eye fields. J Comp Neurol 330:286–301. 10.1002/cne.903300209 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stanton GB, Bruce CJ, Goldberg ME (1995) Topography of projection to posterior cortical areas from the macaque frontal eye fields. J Comp Neurol 353:291–305. 10.1002/cne.903530210 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Strehl A, Ghosh J (2002) Cluster ensembles—a knowledge reuse framework for combining multiple partitions. J Mach Learn Res 3:583–617. 10.1162/153244303321897735 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Suzuki H, Azuma M (1983) Topographic studies on visual neurons in the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex of the monkey. Exp Brain Res 53:47–58. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Teichert T, Yu D, Ferrera VP (2014) Performance monitoring in monkey frontal eye field. J Neurosci 34:1657–1671. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3694-13.2014 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Thiele A, Brandt C, Dasilva M, Gotthardt S, Chicharro D, Panzeri S, Distler C (2016) Attention induced gain stabilization in broad and narrow-spiking cells in the frontal eye-field of macaque monkeys. J Neurosci 36:7601–7612. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0872-16.2016 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Thompson KG, Hanes DP, Bichot NP, Schall JD (1996) Perceptual and motor processing stages identified in the activity of macaque frontal eye field neurons during visual search. J Neurophysiol 76:4040–4055. 10.1152/jn.1996.76.6.4040 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tibshirani R, Walther G, Hastie T (2001) Estimating the number of clusters in a data set via the gap statistic. Journal of the Royal Statistical Society B 63:411–423. 10.1111/1467-9868.00293 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Umeno MM, Goldberg ME (1997) Spatial processing in the monkey frontal eye field. I. Predictive visual responses. J Neurophysiol 78:1373–1383. 10.1152/jn.1997.78.3.1373 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vigneswaran G, Kraskov A, Lemon RN (2011) Large identified pyramidal cells in macaque motor and premotor cortex exhibit “thin spikes”: implications for cell type classification. J Neurosci 31:14235–14242. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3142-11.2011 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Waitzman DM, Ma TP, Optican LM, Wurtz RH (1991) Superior colliculus neurons mediate the dynamic characteristics of saccades. J Neurophysiol 66:1716–1737. 10.1152/jn.1991.66.5.1716 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zaitsev AV, Povysheva NV, Gonzalez-Burgos G, Lewis DA (2012) Electrophysiological classes of layer 2/3 pyramidal cells in monkey prefrontal cortex. J Neurophysiol 108:595–609. 10.1152/jn.00859.2011 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zhou H, Desimone R (2011) Feature-based attention in the frontal eye field and area V4 during visual search. Neuron 70:1205–1217. 10.1016/j.neuron.2011.04.032 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]