Skip to main content
Wiley Open Access Collection logoLink to Wiley Open Access Collection
. 2017 Oct 6;48(8):2674–2683. doi: 10.1111/ejn.13672

Strong long‐range temporal correlations of beta/gamma oscillations are associated with poor sustained visual attention performance

Mona Irrmischer 1,, Simon‐Shlomo Poil 1,2, Huibert D Mansvelder 1, Francesca Sangiuliano Intra 1,3, Klaus Linkenkaer‐Hansen 1
PMCID: PMC6221163  PMID: 28858404

Abstract

Neuronal oscillations exhibit complex amplitude fluctuations with autocorrelations that persist over thousands of oscillatory cycles. Such long‐range temporal correlations (LRTC) are thought to reflect neuronal systems poised near a critical state, which would render them capable of quick reorganization and responsive to changing processing demands. When we concentrate, however, the influence of internal and external sources of distraction is better reduced, suggesting that neuronal systems involved with sustained attention could benefit from a shift toward the less volatile sub‐critical state. To test these ideas, we recorded electroencephalography (EEG) from healthy volunteers during eyes‐closed rest and during a sustained attention task requiring a speeded response to images deviating in their presentation duration. We show that for oscillations recorded during rest, high levels of alpha‐band LRTC in the sensorimotor region predicted good reaction‐time performance in the attention task. During task execution, however, fast reaction times were associated with high‐amplitude beta and gamma oscillations with low LRTC. Finally, we show that reduced LRTC during the attention task compared to the rest condition correlates with better performance, while increased LRTC of oscillations from rest to attention is associated with reduced performance. To our knowledge, this is the first empirical evidence that ‘resting‐state criticality’ of neuronal networks predicts swift behavioral responses in a sensorimotor task, and that steady attentive processing of visual stimuli requires brain dynamics with suppressed temporal complexity.

Keywords: alpha, DFA, LRTC, neuronal oscillations, performance

Introduction

Research on attention has a long tradition in investigating how we selectively process one object while ignoring others (Driver, 2001). In the face of limited processing abilities, task performance is strongly influenced by the level of attention paid to the task (Kahneman, 1973 in: (Sarter et al., 2006)). Still, competing influences such as additional sensory information (Theeuwes, 2010) or mind wandering (Smallwood & Schooler, 2015) can cause distraction from the task at hand and dramatically reduce performance.

Cognitive functions like mind wandering evolve across many time scales, from brief mental images to long‐lasting trains of thoughts (Buckner et al., 2008; Seli et al., 2014). The underlying mechanism therefore also needs to be capable of organizing the coordination of neuronal activity on many time scales (Linkenkaer‐Hansen et al., 2005). Indeed, both hemodynamic (Fox et al., 2006) and electrophysiological measures (Linkenkaer‐Hansen et al., 2001; Montez et al., 2009; Palva et al., 2005) indicate that ongoing brain activity is highly variable and shows organization on many time scales. Quantitatively, this coordination is reflected in long‐range temporal correlations (LRTC) of the type observed in non‐linear dynamical systems operating near the critical state (Chialvo, 2010; Kello et al., 2010; Poil et al., 2012). This is indicating that ongoing brain activity harbors a long‐term memory process known as 1/f noise (Gilden, 2001), a hallmark of healthy physiological systems with high demands for swift adaptation such as heartbeat (Goldberger et al., 2002) and gait (Hausdorff et al., 1996), as well as the ability to produce repetitive taps using the index finger (Torre et al., 2011) and threshold‐stimulus detection tasks (Palva et al., 2013). Deviations from the normal range of LRTC have been associated with diseases such as clinical depression (Linkenkaer‐Hansen et al., 2005), Alzheimer (Montez et al., 2009), epilepsy (Monto et al., 2007), Parkinson's (Hohlefeld et al., 2012), and autism (Lai et al., 2010). Therefore, associating the success of overt behavior to oscillatory brain dynamics can be valuable for understanding general functioning of the brain and may have prognostic properties for optimal functioning or disease.

LRTC have been suggested to reflect the degree to which the brain remains capable of quick reorganization (Deco et al., 2013; Linkenkaer‐Hansen et al., 2001; Singer, 2013; Tognoli & Kelso, 2014) and, thus, is responsive to different processing demands. The closer neuronal systems are to the critical point separating the ordered sub‐critical and the disordered super‐critical regime, the stronger the long‐range temporal correlations in activity fluctuations (Poil et al., 2012). The transition from resting state to attention‐task activity on the other hand shows a decrease in the long‐range memory of the signal as found during focused attention meditation in experienced meditators (Irrmischer et al., 2017), as well as during a cued response task in electroencephalography (EEG) (He et al., 2010) and in fMRI BOLD activations (He, 2011); however, large fluctuations in ongoing oscillations remain during task execution and these have been associated with trial‐by‐trial variability in performance (Linkenkaer‐Hansen et al., 2004b; He & Zempel, 2013). Still, it remains unknown whether this change in temporal structure of continuous amplitude modulations of oscillatory activity is a mere by‐product of the shift from idling resting state to task engagement or whether it is behaviorally relevant in terms of performance success. And, more specifically, whether brain states closer to the critical regime—previously associated with high adaptability and versatile information processing (Bak, 1996; Kinouchi & Copelli, 2006; Shew et al., 2009)—is beneficial for a sustained attention task, or whether a state characterized by less complex variability is better?

In this study, based on earlier findings of reduced LRTC during focused attention meditation (Irrmischer et al., 2017), we propose the working hypothesis that the human brain is poised near a critical state that makes attention inherently unstable and, consequently, a less volatile brain state is desired when sustained focus of attention is required. A key prediction derived from this hypothesis is that long‐range temporal correlations in human EEG oscillations are suppressed during sustained attention and that such suppression may be related to behavioral performance. To test this, we analyzed the changes in LRTC from rest to active state, and their correlation to performance in a sustained attention task, which due to its repetitive nature and length was specifically targeted at inducing distractions in the form of mind‐wandering.

Methods

Participants

The 57 participants were healthy students of the Vrije Universiteit in Amsterdam, and volunteers from the general population aged 20–48 years (M = 25 years, SD = 6.2; 35 females), with no history of neurological complications including ADHD, depression, or substance abuse. All participants signed the informed consent, and the protocol was approved by the Scientific and Ethical Review Board (VCWE) of the Faculty of Psychology and Education, VU University Amsterdam.

EEG measurements

EEG recordings were acquired using the Electrical Geodesics EEG system (GES200) with 128‐EGI HydroCel channel sponge‐based EEG‐caps at a sampling rate of 1000 Hz and using Cz as reference electrode. Participants were measured while sitting alone in the recording room in front of a computer screen. The experiment was programmed in OpenSesame (Mathôt et al., 2012).

Experimental conditions

Eyes‐closed rest

Participants were first measured during 5‐min eyes‐closed rest (ECR) while sitting on a chair. The instruction was ‘Please keep your eyes closed, relax, and try not to fall asleep’.

Continuous temporal expectancy task

Next, participants completed a sustained attention task (adaptation of CTET (O'Connell et al., 2009)), which was designed to measure lapses in attention through the number and timing of errors the participants make. The task consisted of centrally presented photographs of flowers shown at regular intervals (600 ms), resulting in a continuous stream of pictures. Participants were asked to attend to the temporal duration of each stimulus and press the space bar with their right hand when a stimulus was presented longer (1200 ms) than the standard duration (Fig. 1). Long‐duration stimuli occurred semi‐randomly (every 4th–10th stimuli) 100 times. Identifying the duration target is easy when fully attending to the stimuli; however, it quickly becomes demanding to continuously focus on the boring task resulting in great variation as well as occasional misses during the 7.5‐min task. This makes the continuous temporal expectancy task (CTET) (O'Connell et al., 2009) a measure of continuous deployment of attention to the time domain, that is, duration of repetitive events. The stimuli were made of naturalistic pictures taken from the International Affective Picture System (IAPS; (Lang et al., 1999)), with pictures specifically chosen for their low arousal values. Additionally, the color brightness, saturation, and size of the scenes were standardized to decrease stimulus perception‐dependent differences.

Figure 1.

Figure 1

The Continuous Temporal Expectancy Task (CTET). An adaptation of the original task (O'Connell et al., 2009) was used which consisted of centrally presented photographs of flowers shown at regular intervals (600 ms) and longer intervals (1200 ms), which occurred semi‐randomly (every 4th–10th stimuli) 100 times. The entire task took 7.5 min, and participants were asked to press the space bar with their right hand when a stimulus was presented longer.

Eyes‐open rest

In 23 subjects, we also recorded a 5‐min eyes‐open rest (EOR) condition with the instruction ‘Please keep your eyes open, relax, focus on the mark on the screen and try not to fall asleep’. This was performed to assess whether differences in brain dynamics between the resting state and the CTET experiment were solely due to having eyes open or not.

Rationale

The capability to monitor the duration of stimuli is increased when attention is actively oriented toward it (Nobre et al., 2007) and decreases if top‐down attentional effort is diminishing. Lapses in identifying targets are therefore a direct correlate of decreased attention, a phenomenon seen in many everyday life applications. Different from conceptually similar attention tasks such as the go/no‐go ‘Sustained attention to response task’ (SART; (Robertson et al., 1997)), every stimulus is a potential target and only discriminated by its longer presentation time and not by perceptual features. This solves the problematic issue of target salience and automatically engaged exogenous attention interfering with the continuous attentional aspect which is targeted in this study (Robertson & O'Connell, 2012). Finally, it has been shown that performance declines after just 3 min of task performance, but there is no broader decline over blocks indicating that the CTET is a useful paradigm for tracing drifts in the level of attention over time (O'Connell et al., 2009). Therefore, in this study, a single block of 7.5 continuous minutes was chosen to tap into long‐term sustained attention abilities of the participants.

EEG pre‐processing

The EEG signals were FIR‐filtered (0.5–45 Hz band‐pass, Blackman window with 1 Hz transition band), and noisy channels were removed using the pre‐processing functions available in EEGLAB and the Neurophysiological Biomarker Toolbox (NBT, http://www.nbtwiki.net) (Hardstone et al., 2012). All signals were visually inspected in windows of 10 s and transient artifacts, for example, caused by head movements or eye blinks were manually marked and omitted from the subsequent computations of spectral power and detrended fluctuation analysis (DFA). Typically, only 1–2 s around an artifact were marked. Subsequently, we re‐referenced the signals to the common average and applied Independent Components Analysis (Infomax) (Bell & Sejnowski, 1995; Makeig et al. 2000) for identification of independent components related to heartbeat or eye movements, which were visually identified and removed (on average only two components were removed). The remaining components were projected back to signal space.

Behavioral and EEG analysis

The observed reaction‐time averages were calculated from the point in time when the target stimulus was displayed longer than non‐target stimuli. The reaction time therefore includes both the time needed to notice the deviant and the time to react. The next stimulus is displayed 600 ms after and to prevent that wrong presses to non‐target stimuli would count as a very slow reaction to the target stimulus, we defined the maximum allowed reaction time to 900 ms. To avoid short reaction times in subjects responding very fast but also missing several trials, and to obtain a comprehensive performance measure that also included the misses and too slow responses, we defined misses to have the longest reaction time allowed (i.e., 900 ms). We note that reaction times and number of errors exhibited very similar associations with LRTC of neuronal oscillations during CTET and, therefore, we only report the results for the reaction‐time measure defined above.

For the EEG analysis, we computed the power in the five classical frequency bands (delta 1–4 Hz, theta 4–8 Hz, alpha 8–13 Hz, beta 13–30 Hz, and gamma 30–45 Hz) using the Welch method with a 4096‐point Hamming window and a frequency resolution of 0.25 Hz. The relative power was calculated by dividing the absolute power in each frequency band with the integrated power in the range 1–45 Hz. The EEG analysis was performed per channel with a non‐directional t‐test (significance level: < 0.05), and, due to the continuous nature of the reaction times, the parametric Pearson's correlation coefficient was used to test for correlations with reaction times (significance level: < 0.05). To prevent chance‐level effects, we used the binomial multiple‐comparison correction method, which tests whether a significant number of channels reach the significance level of < 0.05 within a specific frequency band. The likelihood of having 12 channels of 128 by chance is <2% (cf. binomial distribution) (Montez et al., 2009; Nikulin et al., 2012; Schiavone et al., 2014). All exponents reported in the main text are averages of significant electrodes across subjects, ± SEM.

Quantifying long‐range temporal correlations

To quantify the strength of LRTC in the amplitude modulation of the EEG oscillations, we first extracted the amplitude envelope of each frequency band using band‐pass filters (FIR‐filter, Blackman window with transition bandwidth of 1 Hz) and the Hilbert transform. Next, the root‐mean‐square fluctuation of the integrated and linearly detrended signals, F(t), was calculated as a function of time window size, t (with an overlap of 50% between windows) and plotted in double‐logarithmic coordinates. The DFA exponent is the slope of the fluctuation function F(t) in a given interval, which was set to 5–30 s for delta and theta band, from 2–30 s for alpha and 1–30 s for the beta and gamma bands. The lower time scale of fitting the power law is higher for the slow oscillations in order to exclude the temporal autocorrelations introduced by the band‐pass filters. A DFA exponent α = 0.5 indicates randomly fluctuating oscillation amplitudes (no temporal structure), whereas 0.5 < α < 1.0 indicates LRTC with the temporal inhomogeneity of fluctuations increasing with increasing DFA exponents. The main steps from the broadband signal to the quantification of LRTC using DFA have been described in detail previously (Hardstone et al., 2012; Linkenkaer‐Hansen et al., 2001).

Results

Sustained attention is associated with a reduction in LRTC

Numerous studies have documented the presence of significant LRTC during eyes‐closed rest; however, little is known about the impact of focused attention on the temporal complexity of ongoing oscillations. Comparing the CTET and ECR conditions, we observed a widespread reduction in LRTC in the theta (αECR = 0.66 ± 0.01, αCTET = 0.61 ± 0.01; t56 = −3.1, = 0.002), alpha (αECR = 0.71 ± 0.01, αCTET = 0.67 ± 0.01; t56 = −3.5, = 0.001), and beta (αECR = 0.66 ± 0.01, αCTET = 0.64 ± 0.01; t56 = −2.9, = 0.005) bands, especially at fronto‐parietal and occipital regions (Fig. 2). In a subset of 23 subjects, we had eyes‐open rest data available, which showed a similar effect, from EOR to CTET theta (αEOR = 0.69 ± 0.02, αCTET = 0.63 ± 0.01; t22 = −4.5, = 0.0002), alpha (αEOR = 0.75 ± 0.02, αCTET = 0.68 ± 0.01; t22 = −5.2, = 0.0003), beta (αEOR = 0.70 ± 0.01 αCTET = 0.66 ± 0.01; t22 = −3.1, = 0.0005) (Fig. 2). Thus, actively engaging in a sustained attention task is associated with reduced LRTC in occipito‐parietal and frontal scalp regions in the theta, alpha, and beta‐frequency bands relative to rest—whether resting with eyes open or closed.

Figure 2.

Figure 2

Strong reduction in long‐range temporal correlations (LRTC) during attention task compared to rest. Difference in detrended fluctuation analysis (DFA) exponents are shown in the left column for continuous temporal expectancy task (CTET) minus eyes‐closed rest (= 57), and in the right column for CTET minus eyes‐open rest (= 23). Individual‐subject exponent values averaged across significant electrodes are shown next to the topographic plots with lines connecting paired values. Subjects showing increases in LRTC are plotted in red, whereas decreases are plotted in blue. Following either rest condition, a widespread reduction in LRTC in the theta, alpha, and beta bands was found during CTET, especially in fronto‐parietal and occipital regions. White circles denote channels with < 0.05 (t‐test, multiple comparisons corrected).

High sensorimotor LRTC during rest predict fast reaction times

Previous studies have found that resting‐state oscillatory dynamics is predictive of temporal fluctuations in perceptual (Palva et al., 2013) and motor task performance (Smit et al., 2013). To test whether resting‐state LRTCs can predict performance in a sustained visual attention task (see Methods and Fig. 1), we correlated the LRTC of oscillations during eyes‐closed rest with the reaction‐time performance in the CTET that required speeded reactions to deviant stimulus durations. Interestingly, high LRTC of resting‐state oscillations in the alpha band over the sensorimotor region—contralateral to the right hand used in the subsequent attention task—predicted better reaction‐time performance (r 56  = −0.37, = 0.007; Fig. 3). We did not observe significant associations between normalized oscillation power and reaction time for any of the frequency bands (data not shown).

Figure 3.

Figure 3

Long‐range temporal correlations (LRTCs) during eyes‐closed rest (ECR) predict reaction times in the subsequent attention task. Correlations between the reaction‐time performance and LRTC during the rest condition for the alpha oscillations indicate significant negative associations over the contralateral sensorimoter region (white circles denote channels with < 0.05, multiple comparisons corrected). Participants with high detrended fluctuation analysis (DFA) in brain activity during resting state showed better performance (faster reaction times) during the subsequent attention task.

Good performance is associated with low LRTC and high spectral power during CTET

Next, we investigated the direct relationship between behavioral performance and brain oscillations during the CTET experiment. Positive correlations between LRTC and reaction times were most pronounced in occipital and parietal regions of the beta (r 56 = 0.44, = 0.001) and gamma (r 56 = 0.40, = 0.003) frequency bands (Fig. 4). For the theta (r 56 = 0.38, = 0.005) and alpha (r 56 = 0.39, = 0.004) oscillations, the correlations were also positive, with similar topographic distribution to that of the beta and gamma oscillations, but with fewer significant channels. Additionally, we found that traditional EEG spectral power in the beta (r 56 = −0.38, = 0.005) and gamma (r 56 = −0.43, = 0.001) frequency bands showed the opposite correlation with performance in similar regions to those that showed significant associations with LRTC (Fig. 4). Together, this indicates that focusing well and responding quickly to deviant image display times is facilitated by stable and strong high‐frequency oscillations.

Figure 4.

Figure 4

Low long‐range temporal correlations (LRTC) and high power in beta/gamma oscillations during high‐performance sustained visual attention. Left column: Correlation (Pearson's r) between LRTC during continuous temporal expectancy task (CTET) and reaction‐time performance in the theta, alpha, beta, and gamma frequency bands. Participants with lower detrended fluctuation analysis (DFA) in brain activity during the attention task showed faster reaction times. Right column: Correlation between the behavioral performance and the spectral power of the beta and gamma frequency bands (white circles denote channels with < 0.05, multiple comparisons corrected). Participants with higher power in brain activity during the attention task showed better performance (faster reaction times).

The change in LRTC from ECR to CTET also predicts performance

Additional analysis revealed that not only during the CTET task there is a positive correlation of LRTC with performance, but also the change of LRTC from ECR to CTET that is correlated with reaction time in the theta (r 56 = 0.37, = 0.006), alpha (r 56 = 0.41, = 0.002), beta (r 56 = 0.34, = 0.001), and gamma (r 56 = 0.39, = 0.004) bands (Fig. 5). A reduction in LRTC from rest condition to CTET correlates with better performance, while an increase is associated with reduced performance. EEG spectral power only showed significant associations in the gamma band over occipital regions (r 56 = −0.45, = 0.0007) and a trend in the beta band. This shows that not only the state of oscillatory properties during the task, but also changes and the direction of changes in LRTC from resting state to the attentional state that are related to performance.

Figure 5.

Figure 5

Suppressed long‐range temporal correlations (LRTC) and elevated power in beta/gamma oscillations during high‐performance sustained visual attention compared to rest. Left column: Correlation between the reaction‐time performance and the continuous temporal expectancy task (CTET) minus eyes‐closed rest (ECR) difference in detrended fluctuation analysis (DFA) exponents for theta, alpha, beta, and gamma frequency bands. Participants with little change or slight decrease in DFA during the attention task compared to the rest condition showed better performance. Right column: Oscillation power in beta and gamma bands shows the opposite pattern: Performance is best in participants with the strongest change in neuronal oscillation power from rest to the attention task (white circles denote channels with < 0.05, multiple comparisons corrected). Note: The correlation with the beta‐band power did not pass the binomial criterion, but the similarity with the topography of the association with LRTC of beta oscillations speaks for a valid trend. Here, the correlation with an occipital channel neighboring to O2 is shown.

Discussion

In this study, we have tested the hypothesis that the temporal complexity of oscillations is related to behavioral performance during sustained attention. We have investigated the change in amplitude and LRTC of neuronal oscillations in a task requiring a single and sustained focus. We found that performance success during a sustained attention task was related to ongoing oscillations in the human EEG: Good performance in the behavioral task was related to higher spectral power and lower LRTC of beta and gamma oscillations especially in occipital regions. Interestingly, the opposite relationship between reaction times and LRTC was observed in the resting state: the higher the LRTC of alpha oscillations, the better the performance. Therefore, we showed that good performance is not only associated with the state of weak LRTC during the task (Fig. 4), but also to the ability to suppress an increase in LRTC relative to the individual resting‐state values (Fig. 5). This was especially clear in the beta and gamma bands in occipital and mid‐frontal regions. Specifically, we note that the positive associations in Fig. 5 relates to the poor performance of participants presenting with an increase in LRTC during the attention task compared to rest, which is the opposite association to what one would have expected if the correlations were caused by the statistical phenomenon of regression to the mean (Barnett et al., 2005). Taken together, our results suggest that the framework of critical brain dynamics is relevant for understanding mass‐neuronal mechanisms of sustained attention.

Why would critical‐state dynamics be beneficial for attention?

The human attention system combines two seemingly opposing functional properties: the ability to stay focused for a sustained period of time and rapid switching in response to changing internal or external demands. This is conceptually similar to the properties of dynamical systems operating near a critical state in which meta‐stable patterns form and rapidly dissolve upon perturbations (Deco et al., 2013; Linkenkaer‐Hansen et al., 2001; Singer, 2013; Tognoli & Kelso, 2014). A system operating near criticality is poised between ordered (sub‐critical) and disordered (super‐critical) states, optimally combining the ability to form patterns while also responding swiftly to input. Indeed, critical‐state dynamics have been related to optimal information processing (Shew & Plenz, 2013) and capacity (Shew et al., 2009), high robustness against perturbations (Hahn et al., 2010), and largest dynamic range in sensory processing (Gautam et al., 2015; Kinouchi & Copelli, 2006). In a supercritical state, the brain would be in a state of hyperarousal, whereas in a subcritical state propagation of neuronal activation is suppressed. Empirical (Beggs & Plenz, 2003) and theoretical (Poil et al., 2012) studies have highlighted the importance of balanced excitation and inhibition for a network to exhibit critical dynamics, which may be an explanation for why scale‐free dynamics of oscillations is altered in many brain disorders, including depression (Linkenkaer‐Hansen et al., 2005); Alzheimer (Montez et al., 2009); epilepsy (Monto et al., 2007); Parkinson's (Hohlefeld et al., 2012), and autism (Lai et al., 2010). It could also be that the excitation–inhibition balance is altered during task engagement.

Why would focus change the temporal structure of the attentional system?

We showed that during the attention task the brain shifts from complex resting‐state dynamics to a temporally more homogeneous state. We interpret this change in dynamics as a transition from a ‘default’ close‐to‐critical resting state optimized for a broad range of environmental and internal demands (Beggs & Plenz, 2003) to a state of reduced propagation of input but increased attentional stability. Therefore, our observation allows for the hypothesis that changes in cognitive control according to demand can be quantified as an ability to suppress LRTC of ongoing neuronal oscillations.

It needs to be stated that it is not known which exact DFA exponent corresponds to the critical state, especially not if different oscillations have different maximum DFA exponents. That said, a high DFA exponent will always be closer to the critical state than a lower. Even if a network is in a super‐critical state DFA exponents will exhibit an increase when the network moves closer to the critical state. Therefore, our results support the idea that on the one hand, a state close to criticality may be beneficial when versatility is required, whereas continued attentive visual processing is associated with a reduction in criticality fluctuations. Possibly due to successfully suppressing, the occurrence of non‐task‐related information propagation such as mind‐wandering episodes and the increased variability these would cause. Similarly, dynamical changes have also been shown in a transition from resting state to task activity in EEG during a cued response task (He et al., 2010) and fMRI BOLD activations (He, 2011), which in both cases were associated with a decrease in the long‐range memory of the signal. A reduction in exponents indicates less autocorrelations and, therefore, less influence on future dynamics (Eke et al., 2002; Mandelbrot & Van Ness, 1968), hence possibly less distractions from the focused task at hand, for example the mind wandering off the task. Further, it has been argued that less temporal redundancy leads to more efficiency in processing (He, 2011), perhaps enhancing the conscious experience of the object of focus, therefore possibly allowing for faster recognition of the deviant display time and subsequent quicker reaction.

Therefore, we believe part of the importance of our results relate to showing that critical‐state dynamics of oscillations are not per se beneficial for the performance of a given task, despite much of the interest in critical brain dynamics has been motivated by the superior computational properties of neuronal networks poised at the critical state. Default mode network alpha oscillations have previously been associated with attention to internal as opposed to sensory information and could also have influenced the current task (Carhart‐Harris et al., 2014). However, the occipital topographies of effects—and especially the increasing beta/gamma power jointly with a reduced complexity of the temporal structure of these oscillations—suggest that successfully sustaining attention is reflected in an uninterrupted processing of the visual stimuli.

Importance of alpha oscillations during rest

The correlation of LRTC of resting‐state oscillations in alpha with reaction time was a noteworthy finding, because of its distinct sensorimotor region topography. Although we did not perform source modeling of oscillations, based on previous studies on alpha oscillations reactivity to finger movements (e.g., Fig. 2H in (Smit et al., 2013)) such as required in the present paradigm—it seems very likely that the correlation reflect sensorimotor oscillations. We see it as preliminary evidence that a motor region operating close to criticality is advantageous for a quick motor response, which warrants further investigation.

Importance of beta and gamma oscillations

The strongest correlations with good performance were found in the beta and gamma frequency bands. Both frequency ranges have been reported for their significance in attentive processing of visual stimuli (Jensen et al., 2007; Wróbel, 2000) and conscious perception (Keil et al., 1999; Meador et al., 2002). Oscillatory phase synchronization has been reported as a mechanism for long‐distance neuronal communication (Salinas & Sejnowski, 2001) and facilitation of selective sensory gating (Jensen & Mazaheri, 2010). We indeed find that higher power in the beta and gamma band during the visual task is related to better performance. Interestingly, however, the temporal structure decreased in both frequency bands with better performance. Showing that next to increased power and synchronization, information propagation through temporal coding in the form of LRTC is also of significance.

Observations are not confounded by signal‐to‐noise effects

A factor that could influence the DFA estimate is the signal‐to‐noise ratio of the signal. The lower this ratio, the more the estimated scaling is biased toward an uncorrelated noise signal (Linkenkaer‐Hansen et al., 2007). Intriguingly, we found that the DFA showed a negative correlation in relation to performance, while at similar scalp locations, the spectral power in the beta and gamma showed a positive correlation (Fig. 3). Thus, subjects presenting with high performance on the sustained visual attention task had the weakest LRTC and the highest power of beta and gamma oscillations. Therefore, the LRTC results cannot be accounted for by signal‐to‐noise ratio effects.

Impact of period stimulation on LRTC

The majority of past studies investigating LRTC in neuronal oscillations have focused on resting‐state recordings (Hardstone et al., 2012). Periodic stimulation as used here, however, can introduce a characteristic scale in the amplitude modulation of oscillations, such as the mu rhythm during periodic stimulation of the median nerve (Linkenkaer‐Hansen et al., 2004a). Importantly, periodic modulation—whether in the form of a reduction or an enhancement of oscillation amplitudes—cannot in itself give rise to scale‐free modulation of oscillations. This has been studied previously, also by simulating periodic modulation of amplitudes by stereotypical stimulus responses (Linkenkaer‐Hansen et al., 2004b, b). We, therefore, consider it unlikely that event‐related potentials or modulation of neuronal oscillations could explain the associations between LRTC and performance reported here. We find it a more likely interpretation that steady focus of attention has a ‘whitening effect’ on neuronal dynamics, suppressing the complexity of fluctuations while trying to maintain a steady brain state, which is an observation we have also made in the absence of sensory stimulation while meditators perform focused attention meditation (Irrmischer et al., 2017).

Observations are not due to the difference between eyes‐open test vs. eyes‐closed rest

To test whether changes in LRTC between CTET and ECR were dependent on the difference between eyes open vs. eyes closed, we did the control experiment in which we included an eyes‐open rest (EOR). Results showed a reduction in LRTC in virtually the same areas (Fig. 2). Therefore, we conclude that focused visual attention suppresses LRTC beyond that of the eyes‐open effect.

Outlook

Our results suggest that the framework of critical brain dynamics is relevant for understanding mass‐neuronal mechanisms of sustained attention and reaction‐time performance.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare no competing financial interests.

Author contributions

Mona Irrmischer involved in conceptualizing, conducting, recording and processing data, analyzing, and writing. Francesca Sangiuliano Intra involved in recording and processing the data. Simon‐Shlomo Poil involved in analysis. Huibert D. Mansvelder performed supervision and writing. Klaus Linkenkaer‐Hansen involved in conceptualizing, analyzing, supervision, and writing.

Data accessibility

Data are available on request by the authors.

Abbreviations

CTET

continuous temporal expectancy task

DFA

detrended fluctuation analysis

ECR

eyes‐closed rest

EEG

electroencephalography

EOR

eyes‐open rest

fMRI BOLD

blood‐oxygen level‐dependent functional magnetic resonance image

LRTC

long‐range temporal correlations

Supporting information

 

Acknowledgements

M.I. was funded by a Research Talent grant from the Netherlands Organization for Scientific Research (NWO/MaGW 406‐12‐160). We thank Simon Houtman, Sonja Simpraga and Arthur‐Ervin Avramiea for their contributions to the development of the Neurophysiological Biomarker Toolbox.

Edited by Heleen Slagter

Reviewed by Redmond O'Connell, Trinity College Dublin, Ireland; and Christopher Kello, University of California Merced, USA.

The associated peer review process communications can be found in the online version of this article.

References

  1. Bak, P. (1996) How Nature Works. Springer, New York. [Google Scholar]
  2. Barnett, A.G. , van der Pols, J.C. & Dobson, A.J. (2005) Regression to the mean: what it is and how to deal with it. Int. J. Epidemiol., 34, 215–220. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Beggs, J.M. & Plenz, D. (2003) Neuronal avalanches in neocortical circuits. J. Neurosci., 23, 11167–11177. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Bell, A.J. & Sejnowski, T.J. (1995) An information‐maximization approach to blind separation and blind deconvoluttion. Neural Comput., 7, 1129–1159. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Buckner, R.L. , Andrews‐Hanna, J.R. & Schacter, D.L. (2008) The brain's default network: anatomy, function, and relevance to disease. Ann. NY. Acad. Sci., 1124, 1–38. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Carhart‐Harris, R.L. , Leech, R. , Hellyer, P.J. , Shanahan, M. , Feilding, A. , Tagliazucchi, E. , Chialvo, D.R. & Nutt, D. (2014) The entropic brain: a theory of conscious states informed by neuroimaging research with psychedelic drugs. Front. Hum. Neurosci., 8, 20. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Chialvo, D.R. (2010) Emergent complex neural dynamics: the brain at the edge ∗. Nat. Phys., 750, 744–750. [Google Scholar]
  8. Deco, G. , Jirsa, V.K. & Mcintosh, A.R. (2013) Resting brains never rest : computational insights into potential cognitive architectures. Trends Neurosci., 36, 268–274. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Driver, J. (2001) A selective review of selective attention research from the past century. Brit. J. Psychol., 92(Part 1), 53–78. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Eke, A. , Herman, P. , Kocsis, L. & Kozak, L.R. (2002) Fractal characterization of complexity in temporal physiological signals. Physiol. Meas., 23, R1–R38. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Fox, M.D. , Snyder, A.Z. , Zacks, J.M. & Raichle, M.E. (2006) Coherent spontaneous activity accounts for trial‐to‐trial variability in human evoked brain responses. Nat. Neurosci., 9, 23–25. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Gautam, S.H. , Hoang, T.T. , McClanahan, K. , Grady, S.K. & Shew, W.L. (2015) Maximizing sensory dynamic range by tuning the cortical state to criticality. PLoS Comput. Biol., 11, 1–15. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Gilden, D.L. (2001) Cognitive emissions of 1/f noise. Psychol. Rev., 108, 33–56. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Goldberger, A.L. , Amaral, L.a.N. , Hausdorff, J.M. , Ivanov, P.C. , Peng, C.‐K. & Stanley, H.E. (2002) Fractal dynamics in physiology: alterations with disease and aging. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA., 99(Suppl 1), 2466–2472. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Hahn, G. , Petermann, T. , Havenith, M.N. , Yu, S. , Singer, W. , Plenz, D. & Nikolic, D. (2010) Neuronal avalanches in spontaneous activity in vivo. J. Neurophysiol., 104, 3312–3322. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Hardstone, R. , Poil, S.S. , Schiavone, G. , Jansen, R. , Nikulin, V.V. , Mansvelder, H.D. & Linkenkaer‐Hansen, K. (2012) Detrended fluctuation analysis: a scale‐free view on neuronal oscillations. Front. Physiol., 3, 450. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Hausdorff, J.M. , Purdon, P.L. , Peng, C.K. , Ladin, Z. , Wei, J.Y. & Goldberger, A.L. (1996) Fractal dynamics of human gait: stability of long‐range correlations in stride interval fluctuations. J. Appl. Physiol., 1448 LP, 1457. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. He, B.J. (2011) Scale‐free properties of the fMRI signal during rest and task biyu. J. Neurosci., 4, 13786–13795. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. He, B.J. & Zempel, J.M. (2013) Average is optimal: an inverted‐U relationship between trial‐to‐trial brain activity and behavioral performance. PLoS Comput. Biol., 9, 11. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. He, B.J. , Zempel, J.M. , Snyder, A.Z. & Raichle, M.E. (2010) The temporal structures and functional significance of scale‐free brain activity. Neuron, 66, 353–369. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Hohlefeld, F.U. , Huebl, J. , Huchzermeyer, C. , Schneider, G.H. , Schönecker, T. , Kühn, A.A. , Curio, G. & Nikulin, V.V. (2012) Long‐range temporal correlations in the subthalamic nucleus of patients with Parkinson's disease. Eur. J. Neurosci., 36, 2812–2821. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Irrmischer, M. , Houtman, S.J. , Mansvelder, H.D. , Tremmel, M. , Ott, U. & Linkenkaer‐Hansen, K.F. (2017) Meditation reduces complex amplitude fluctuations of neuronal oscillations. Landelijk Mindfulness Symposium, Leiden, 19 May, 2017. Available: https://www.radboudcentrumvoormindfulness.nl/wp-content/uploads/2016/02/Focused-attention-meditation-EEG-results-Mona-Irmischer.pdf
  23. Jensen, O. & Mazaheri, A. (2010) Shaping functional architecture by oscillatory alpha activity: gating by inhibition. Front. Hum. Neurosci., 4, 186. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Jensen, O. , Kaiser, J. & Lachaux, J.P. (2007) Human gamma‐frequency oscillations associated with attention and memory. Trends Neurosci., 30, 317–324. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Keil, A. , Müller, M.M. , Ray, W.J. , Gruber, T. & Elbert, T. (1999) Human gamma band activity and perception of a gestalt. J. Neurosci., 19, 7152–7161. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Kello, C.T. , Brown, G.D. , Ferrer‐I‐Cancho, R. , Holden, J.G. , Linkenkaer‐Hansen, K. , Rhodes, T. & Van Orden, G.C. (2010) Scaling laws in cognitive sciences. Trends Cogn. Sci., 14, 223–232. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Kinouchi, O. & Copelli, M. (2006) Optimal dynamical range of excitable networks at criticality. Nat. Phys., 2, 348–351. [Google Scholar]
  28. Lai, M.C. , Lombardo, M.V. , Chakrabarti, B. , Sadek, S.A. , Pasco, G. , Wheelwright, S.J. , Bullmore, E.T. , Baron‐Cohen, S. et al (2010) A shift to randomness of brain oscillations in people with autism. Biol. Psychiatry, 68, 1092–1099. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Lang, P. , Bradley, M. & Cuthbert, B. (1999) International affective picture system (IAPS): instruction manual and affective ratings. The Center for Research in Psychophysiology, University of Florida, Gainesville, FL. [Google Scholar]
  30. Linkenkaer‐Hansen, K. , Nikouline, V.V. , Palva, J.M. & Ilmoniemi, R.J. (2001) Long‐range temporal correlations and scaling behavior in human brain oscillations. J. Neurosci., 21, 1370–1377. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Linkenkaer‐Hansen, K. , Nikulin, V.V. , Palva, S. , Ilmoniemi, R.J. & Palva, J.M. (2004a) Prestimulus oscillations enhance psychophysical performance in humans. J. Neurosci., 24, 10186–10190. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Linkenkaer‐Hansen, K. , Nikulin, V.V. , Palva, J.M. , Kaila, K. & Ilmoniemi, R.J. (2004b) Stimulus‐induced change in long‐range temporal correlations and scaling behaviour of sensorimotor oscillations. Eur. J. Neurosci., 19, 203–211. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Linkenkaer‐Hansen, K. , Monto, S. , Rytsälä, H. , Suominen, K. , Isometsä, E. & Kähkönen, S. (2005) Breakdown of long‐range temporal correlations in theta oscillations in patients with major depressive disorder. J. Neurosci., 25, 10131–10137. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Linkenkaer‐Hansen, K. , Smit, D.J.A. , Barkil, A. , Van Beijsterveldt, T.E.M. , Brussaard, A.B. , Boomsma, D.I. , Van Ooyen, A. & De Geus, E.J.C. (2007) Genetic contributions to long‐range temporal correlations in ongoing oscillations. J. Neurosci., 27, 13882–13889. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Makeig, S. , Jung, T.P. , Ghahremani, D. , & Sejnowski, T.J. (2000) Independent component analysis of simulated ERP data In Nakada T. (Ed.). Integrated Human Brain Science: Theory, Method, Applications (Music), New York: Elsevier Science. [Google Scholar]
  36. Mandelbrot, B.B. & Van Ness, J.W. (1968) Fractional brownian motions, fractional noises and applications. SIAM Rev., 10, 422–437. [Google Scholar]
  37. Mathôt, S. , Schreij, D. & Theeuwes, J. (2012) OpenSesame: an open‐source, graphical experiment builder for the social sciences. Behav. Res. Meth., 44, 314–324. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Meador, K.J. , Ray, P.G. , Echauz, J.R. , Loring, D.W. & Vachtsevanos, G.J. (2002) Gamma coherence and conscious perception. Neurology, 59, 847–854. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Montez, T. , Poil, S.‐S. , Jones, B.F. , Manshanden, I. , Verbunt, J.P.A. , van Dijk, B.W. , Brussaard, A.B. , van Ooyen, A. et al (2009) Altered temporal correlations in parietal alpha and prefrontal theta oscillations in early‐stage Alzheimer disease. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA, 106, 1614–1619. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Monto, S. , Vanhatalo, S. , Holmes, M.D. & Palva, J.M. (2007) Epileptogenic neocortical networks are revealed by abnormal temporal dynamics in seizure‐free subdural EEG. Cereb. Cortex, 17, 1386–1393. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Nikulin, V.V. , Jönsson, E.G. & Brismar, T. (2012) Attenuation of long‐range temporal correlations in the amplitude dynamics of alpha and beta neuronal oscillations in patients with schizophrenia. NeuroImage, 61, 162–169. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Nobre, A. , Correa, A. & Coull, J. (2007) The hazards of time. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol., 17, 465–470. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. O'Connell, R.G. , Dockree, P.M. , Robertson, I.H. , Bellgrove, M.A. , Foxe, J.J. & Kelly, S.P. (2009) Uncovering the neural signature of lapsing attention: electrophysiological signals predict errors up to 20 s before they occur. J. Neurosci., 29, 8604–8611. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Palva, S. , Linkenkaer‐Hansen, K. , Näätänen, R. & Palva, J.M. (2005) Early neural correlates of conscious somatosensory perception. J. Neurosci., 25, 5248–5258. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Palva, J.M. , Zhigalov, A. , Hirvonen, J. , Korhonen, O. , Linkenkaer‐Hansen, K. & Palva, S. (2013) Neuronal long‐range temporal correlations and avalanche dynamics are correlated with behavioral scaling laws. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA, 110, 3585–3590. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Poil, S.‐S. , Hardstone, R. , Mansvelder, H.D. & Linkenkaer‐Hansen, K. (2012) Critical‐state dynamics of avalanches and oscillations jointly emerge from balanced excitation/inhibition in neuronal networks. J. Neurosci., 32, 9817–9823. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Robertson, I.H. & O'Connell, R. (2012) Vigilant attention In Attention and Time. Oxford University Press, Oxford. [Google Scholar]
  48. Robertson, I.H. , Manly, T. , Andrade, J. , Baddeley, B.T. & Yiend, J. (1997) “Oops!”: performance correlates of everyday attentional failures in traumatic brain injured and normal subjects. Neuropsychologia, 35, 747–758. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Salinas, E. & Sejnowski, T.J. (2001) Correlated neuronal activity and the flow of neural information. Nat. Rev. Neurosci., 2, 539–550. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Sarter, M. , Gehring, W.J. & Kozak, R. (2006) More attention must be paid: the neurobiology of attentional effort. Brain Res. Rev., 51, 145–160. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Schiavone, G. , Linkenkaer‐Hansen, K. , Maurits, N.M. , Plakas, A. , Maassen, B.A.M. , Mansvelder, H.D. , van der Leij, A. & van Zuijen, T.L. (2014) Preliteracy signatures of poor‐reading abilities in resting‐state EEG. Front. Hum. Neurosci., 8, 735. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Seli, P. , Carriere, J. & Smilek, D. (2014) Not all mind wandering is created equal : dissociating deliberate from spontaneous mind wandering. Psychol Res, 79, 750–758. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Shew, W.L. & Plenz, D. (2013) The Functional benefits of criticality in the cortex. Neurosci., 19, 88–100. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Shew, W.L. , Yang, H. , Petermann, T. , Roy, R. & Plenz, D. (2009) Neuronal avalanches imply maximum dynamic range in cortical networks at criticality. J. Neurosci., 29, 15595–15600. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Singer, W. (2013) Cortical dynamics revisited. Trends Cogn. Sci., 17, 616–626. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Smallwood, J. & Schooler, J.W. (2015) The Science of mind wandering: empirically navigating the stream of consciousness. Annu. Rev. Psychol., 66, 487–518. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Smit, D.J.A. , Linkenkaer‐Hansen, K. & de Geus, E.J.C. (2013) Long‐range temporal correlations in resting‐state alpha oscillations predict human timing‐error dynamics. J. Neurosci., 33, 11212–11220. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Theeuwes, J. (2010) Top‐down and bottom‐up control of visual selection. Acta Psychol., 135, 77–99. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Tognoli, E. & Kelso, J.A.S. (2014) The Metastable Brain. Neuron, 81, 35–48. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Torre, K. , Balasubramaniam, R. , Rheaume, N. , Lemoine, L. & Zelaznik, H.N. (2011) Long‐range correlation properties in motor timing are individual and task specific. Psychon. Bull. Rev., 18, 339–346. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Wróbel, A. (2000) Beta activity: a carrier for visual attention. Acta Neurobiol. Exp., 60, 247–260. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

 

Data Availability Statement

Data are available on request by the authors.


Articles from The European Journal of Neuroscience are provided here courtesy of Wiley

RESOURCES