Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2019 Dec 1.
Published in final edited form as: Atten Defic Hyperact Disord. 2018 Jul 26;10(4):285–295. doi: 10.1007/s12402-018-0259-8

Parent and child neurocognitive functioning predict response to behavioral parent training for youth with ADHD

Whitney D Fosco 1,2, Dustin E Sarver 3,, Michael J Kofler 4, Paula A Aduen 5
PMCID: PMC6224297  NIHMSID: NIHMS983935  PMID: 30051256

Abstract

Parental cognitive functioning is thought to play a key role in parenting behavior and may inform response to behavioral intervention. This open-label pilot study examined the extent to which parent and child cognition impacted response to behavioral parent training for children with ADHD. Fifty-four participants (27 parent–child dyads; Mages = 10.6 and 45.2 for children and parents, respectively) completed tasks assessing visuospatial and phonological working memory, inhibitory control, and choice-reaction speed at pre-treatment. Drift diffusion modeling decomposed choice-reaction time data into indicators of processing speed (drift rate) and response caution (boundary separation). Parents completed a 10-week manualized behavioral parent training program. Primary outcomes were pre- and post-treatment child ADHD and conduct problem severity, and parent-reported relational frustration and parenting confidence. Bayesian multiple regressions assessed parent and child cognitive processes as predictors of post-treatment outcomes, controlling for pre-treatment behavior. Better child visuospatial and phonological WM and higher parental response caution were associated with greater reductions in inattention. For conduct problems, better parental self-regulation (stronger inhibitory control and greater response caution) predicted fewer post-treatment conduct problems. Higher parental response caution also predicted lower post-treatment relational frustration and higher parental confidence. Bayesian evidence supported no relation between parent and child cognitive functions and treatment-related changes in hyperactivity. This pilot study demonstrates that cognitive processes central to etiologic theories of ADHD and models of parenting behavior can be successfully integrated into treatment outcome research to inform which families are most likely to benefit from behavioral interventions. This study demonstrates the feasibility of bridging the translational research gap between basic and applied clinical science and facilitates research on the role of cognition in psychosocial interventions.

Keywords: ADHD, Behavior therapy, Working memory, Processing speed, Inhibitory control

Introduction

Behavioral parent training (BPT) is a frontline evidence-based psychosocial intervention for attention-deficit/hyper-activity disorder (ADHD; Evans et al. 2018). Decades of research document its effectiveness in reducing ADHD symptoms, co-occurring conduct problems, and related impairments. For parents, participating in BPT often decreases parental stress and reduces negative parenting behaviors (Chronis-Tuscano et al. 2011; Chacko et al. 2009). Despite its overall effectiveness, BPT is not effective for every family (Evans et al. 2018; Owens et al. 2003). An important step toward enhancing the effectiveness of existing treatments and improving patient care requires understanding who is most likely to benefit from particular treatments (e.g., Kraemer et al. 2002).

Identifying treatment moderators has the potential to maximize clinical outcomes while reducing burden and cost (Hinshaw 2007; Kraemer et al. 2002). Yet, relatively little work in the BPT literature has sought to elucidate ADHD treatment moderators. Existing research has focused primarily on how limited demographic (e.g., sex, SES) and psychological (e.g., symptom severity, comorbidity profile) characteristics of children/families impact treatment response (e.g., Chronis-Tuscano et al. 2017; Hinshaw 2007; Sonuga-Barke et al. 2002). Investigations of parent characteristics aside from parental psychopathology are surprisingly scarce (see Johnston et al. 2012).

Though an important starting point, selection of extant moderators to date has been largely divorced from theory, which limits the translation of basic mechanisms involved in ADHD to clinical application (Johnston et al. 2012). The current study addresses this gap in the BPT literature by examining the moderating role of candidate cognitive processes (working memory [WM], inhibitory control, processing speed) that are steeped in both theories of ADHD etiology (e.g., Barkley 1997a, b; Castellanos and Tannock 2002; Rapport et al. 2008) and models of parenting behavior (see Crandall et al. 2015; Johnston et al. 2012).

ADHD is associated with multiple neurocognitive weaknesses suspected to possibly underlie its development (Nigg et al. 2018). As a group, individuals with ADHD across the lifespan demonstrate prominent deficits on tasks of WM (Kasper et al. 2012) and inhibitory control (Alderson et al. 2007; Lipszyc and Schachar 2010), and show medium-sized processing speed differences (Fosco et al. 2017; Karalunas et al. 2012), though substantial within-disorder heterogeneity exists (Fair et al. 2012). Given the strong heritability of ADHD (Faraone et al. 2005) and ADHD-related cognitive functions (Crosbie et al. 2013), parent and child neurocognitive abilities are strong candidate moderators of treatment response. Indeed, child cognitive function has been shown to predict response to stimulant medication (Molitor and Langberg 2017), and stimulant effects on inhibitory control and working memory partially mediate stimulant effects on classroom behavior (Hawk et al. in press); yet, a recent review summarizing the use of cognitive task performance as a tool for treatment planning highlighted that there is a dearth of research examining the extent to which cognitive function also informs response to psychosocial interventions (Molitor and Langberg 2017).

Within the broader parenting literature, several studies have shown that stronger parental WM predicts more positive parenting behaviors (Mazursky-Horowitz et al. 2017) and less harsh parenting in the context of challenging child behaviors (Deater-Deckard et al. 2010). Responsive parenting also correlates with stronger parental inhibitory control (Shaffer and Obradović 2017). To date, investigations studying parental neurocognitive function have focused exclusively on relating individual differences in cognition to parenting behavior. The current study is the first to assess whether parental cognitive function impacts the effectiveness of interventions specifically designed to improve parenting behavior.

Parenting is a transactional process between caregivers and children, and children’s own cognitive functioning may impact responsiveness to BPT intervention strategies. The only study to test whether specific child cognitive processes, beyond global intelligence (Owens et al. 2003), moderated behavioral treatment response showed that better processing speed, but not WM, predicted greater symptom reduction among children with ADHD-inattentive type during a multimodal psychosocial intervention (Adalio et al. 2018). However, both constructs were measured with the WISC-V index scores, which lack measurement specificity in isolating neurocognitive processes compared to laboratory-based tasks (Tarle et al. 2017), and show poor correspondence with criterion WM tests (Wells et al. 2018).

The present open-label pilot study extends this important initial work by examining the extent to which both parents’ and children’s WM, inhibitory control, and multiple components of processing speed impact BPT effectiveness. The primary aims were to (1) demonstrate the feasibility of collecting both parent and child cognitive data within the context of a treatment study and (2) provide initial evidence for associations between cognitive function and improvement in children’s symptoms, as well as in secondary outcomes of parent’s stress and parenting confidence. We predicted that poorer pre-treatment cognitive functioning in children and parents would be associated with attenuated BPT benefits. Given the established role of WM in parenting behavior (Deater-Deckard et al. 2010), WM was hypothesized to show stronger effects than inhibitory control or processing speed. Given prior findings (Adalio et al. 2018), we anticipated that children’s processing speed would most strongly predict BPT treatment response.

Methods

Participants

The modified CONSORT study flow diagram is shown in Fig. 1. Parents of children aged 8–13 years were consecutively referred to a university-based children’s learning clinic through community resources. Caregivers were provided with psychoeducational evaluations. Informed consent/assent was obtained from all individual participants included in this study; IRB approval was obtained. The final sample included 27 parent–child dyads (Total N = 54; see Table 1).

Fig. 1.

Fig. 1

Behavioral parent training modified CONSORT diagram (modified to emphasize open-label design)

Table 1.

Participant characteristics

Children Parents
Age 10.6 (1.5) 45.2 (6.9)
Sex (% male:% female) 63%:37% 15%:85%
IQ
VCI 109.4 (13.2)
PRI 111.48 (13.0) 11.8 (1.8)a
Ethnicity
n (%) caucasian, non-hispanic 21 (78%)
n (%) hispanic 3 (11%)
n (%) multiracial 3 (11%)
Baseline measures
 Inattention 67.0 (7.3) N/A
 Hyperactivity 71.7 (13.5) N/A
 Conduct problems 59.6 (13.4) N/A
 Relational frustration N/A 60.9 (10.2)
 Parenting confidence N/A 43.8 (9.2)

Unless otherwise stated, values represent the mean (SD)

VCI verbal comprehension index and PRI perceptual reasoning index from the WASI-II. Baseline symptoms (T-scores) were measured with the BASC-2 and baseline relational frustration and parenting confidence were measured with the PRQ

a

Parents were administered the Matrix Reasoning subscale from the WASI-II. Values represent the standard score

Inclusion/exclusion criteria

All children and caregivers completed an identical evaluation that included a semi-structured clinical interview (K-SADS; Kaufman et al. 1997). Pre-treatment parent and teacher ADHD ratings were obtained from the Behavior Assessment System for Children (BASC-2; Reynolds and Kamphaus 2004) and Child Symptom Inventory (CSI-IV; Gadow and Sprafkin 2002).

Study eligibility required: (1) DSM-5 diagnosis of ADHD (Combined Presentation n = 18; Inattentive n = 7; Hyperactive/impulsive n = 2) on the K-SADS; and (2) Borderline/clinical elevations on at least one parent and one teacher ADHD rating scale, or previous psychoeducational evaluation documenting cross-informant symptoms (e.g., for children prescribed medication that reduced ADHD symptoms at school).

Comorbidities reflect clinical consensus best estimates, and included oppositional defiant (37.0%), anxiety (18.5%), and depressive (11.1%) disorders. Fifteen children (55.6%) were currently prescribed psychostimulants that were withheld ≥ 24 h prior to pre-treatment cognitive testing. Continued medication was allowed during BPT treatment (Table 2).

Table 2.

Descriptive statistics for parent and child cognitive function

Children Parents
VSWM 2.69 (0.79) 3.57 (0.75)
PHWM 3.11 (0.82) 4.19 (0.36)
SSRT 330.74 (78.90) 281.71 (80.43)
SSD 269.10 (58.49) 314.80 (68.20)
Drift rate (v) 1.94 (0.93) 4.22 (1.73)
Boundary separation (a) 1.78 (1.04) 2.06 (1.27)

Values represent the mean (SD)

VSWM visuospatial working memory, PHWM phonological working memory, WM values are the average number of stimuli correct across trials, SSRT stop-signal reaction time, lower values reflect better inhibitory control, SSD stop-signal delay, higher values represent stronger inhibitory control

Children were excluded for gross neurological, sensory, or motor impairment, seizure disorder, psychosis, autism spectrum disorder, or intellectual disability, or non-stimulant medications that could not be withheld for testing.

Procedures

Children first attended an intake assessment to determine eligibility. Pre-treatment cognitive testing occurred during a larger battery of two, 3-h sessions (child) or one, 90-m session (parent). All tests were counterbalanced within/across sessions. Children and caregivers received brief breaks after each task, and preset longer breaks every 2–3 tasks. Pre-treatment symptom ratings were collected as part of the child’s initial assessment prior to the first treatment session. The median duration between the completion of the child assessment and the first treatment session was 31 days (M = 46.15, SD = 35.69), and the duration between parent pre-treatment testing and the first treatment session was 10.52 days (SD= 13.91).

Behavioral parent training

BPT was delivered according to manualized procedures in small group format (n = 23) or individually (n = 4) as needed to accommodate families’ schedules. Evidence-based BPT (Evans et al. 2018) was provided using the 10-session manualized Barkley (1997a, b) Defiant Children protocol. BPT was delivered by behaviorally trained, PhD-level clinicians (two licensed psychologists and one postdoctoral fellow). In all BPT sessions, clinicians covered the intervention content by conducting sessions with the treatment manual open as the session guide. Parent-reported treatment feasibility and acceptability were excellent (see Kofler et al. in press). As reported below, parent-reported ADHD symptom reductions in this group were consistent with recent reviews of BPT for ADHD (Evans et al. 2018). Four families discontinued treatment after approximately half of sessions. For these families, mid-treatment scores were used in place of post-treatment scores. All other families attended at least 8 out of 10 sessions.

Measures

Intellectual Functioning (IQ)

Child IQ was estimated using the WASI-II (Wechsler 2011). Parent IQ was estimated with the WASI-II Matrix Reasoning subscale.

Child Symptoms

Parent-reported ADHD and ODD symptoms were assessed using T-scores (age and gender norms) from the Behavior Assessment Scale for Children (BASC-2; Reynolds and Kamphaus 2004) attention problems, hyperactivity, and conduct problems subscales. Psychometric support for these subscales includes high internal consistency (attention problems α = .79–.90, hyperactivity α = .84–.88, conduct problems α = .87–.88), test–retest reliability (attention problems = .90, hyperactivity = .91, conduct problems = .87–.88), and expected correspondence with other broadband and narrowband indices of ADHD and ODD symptoms (e.g., Achenbach and Rescorla 2001; DuPaul et al. 2016). Higher scores indicate higher quantity/severity of ADHD/ODD symptoms.

Parental stress and confidence

The Parenting Relationship Questionnaire (PRQ; Kamphaus and Reynolds 2006) assesses the quality of the parent–child relationship. The current study utilized T-scores (age and gender norms) from the Relational Frustration and Parenting Confidence sub-scales of the PRQ. Relational frustration assesses a parent’s stress in relating to their child and managing their child’s behavior. Higher scores reflect greater stress/frustration. Parenting confidence involves the degree of perceived confidence in making parenting decisions and implementing parenting practices. Higher scores reflect more confidence. Psychometric support includes good internal consistency (frustration α = .86–.89, confidence α = .78–.80) and test–retest reliability (frustration = .82, confidence = .77).

Candidate neurocognitive moderators

Working memory

Children and parents completed a visuospatial (VS) and phonological (PH) WM task (Rapport et al. 2009). Twelve trials per set size were administered in ascending order at a rate of 1 stimuli per second. Brief breaks were given between blocks. Children completed set sizes of 3–6 stimuli per trial; caregivers completed set sizes of 3–7 stimuli to avoid ceiling effects. Five practice trials were administered before test trials (80% correct required).

Task outcome variables

Number of stimuli correct per trial were summed at each set size; total stimuli correct were then averaged across set sizes, within modality (phonological vs. visuospatial). Higher scores reflect better WM.

Inhibitory control

The stop-signal task (SST) assessed inhibitory control. Go stimuli were the letters “X” and “O” (equal probability) displayed in the center of a computer screen for 1000 ms. A 1000-Hz auditory tone (i.e., stop signal) was presented randomly on 25% of trials. Stop-signal delay (SSD)—the latency between presentation of go stimuli and stop stimuli—was initially set at 250 ms, and dynamically adjusted in 50-ms increments, contingent on participant performance. The algorithm is designed to approximate successful inhibition on 50% of the stop trials. After two practice blocks, participants completed four consecutive experimental blocks of 32 trials per block (24 go trials, 8 stop trials).

Task outcome variables

Data for one child were lost due to technical issues. The speed of the stop process (stop-signal reaction time; SSRT) was the primary SST outcome variable.1 To ensure that differentially skewed “go” reaction times (RT) did not impact SSRT estimates, SSRT was computed as median RT—mean SSD (Verbruggen et al. 2013). SSRT was computed for each block and then averaged across blocks. Smaller values reflect better inhibitory control.

Processing speed and response caution

A two-choice RT task required participants to press one of two keys to indicate whether a “Y” or “N” appeared on the screen on each trial. Stimuli were presented randomly, with equal probability. Max stimulus duration was 2500 ms (jittered ISI 800–2400 ms). Participants’ response terminated the trial. After a 10-trial practice block (100% correct required), participants completed 60 test trials.

Task outcome variables

Anticipatory responses (RTs < 150 ms) and outlying RTs (> 2500 ms) were screened and excluded. A three-parameter (boundary separation, drift rate, nondecision time) drift diffusion model (DDM) using Fast-DM and the Kolmogorov–Smirnov criterion (Voss et al. 2015) was fit to the data. The three-parameter model can provide robust parameter estimation even for small trial numbers (Lerche et al. 2017). In the current sample, model fit was acceptable for all participants (all ps > .20, with p < .05 representing poor fit).

The DDM separates RT data into psychologically-distinct processing components. Drift rate (v) represents the speed of information accumulation and was selected to indicate processing speed in the current study. Drift rate is advantageous over mean RT because it provides a purer assessment of processing speed that separates processing speed from overall accuracy, level of response caution and speed of motor output, all of which affect overall RT estimates (Lerche et al. 2017). Higher values indicate faster processing speed. Boundary separation (a) reflects response caution, such that higher values indicate a response style that emphasizes accuracy over speed, and lower values indicate an emphasis on speed over accuracy. Nondecision time (terr) reflects time for processes outside of the decision-making process, including stimulus encoding and motor response execution (Voss et al. 2015). Nondecision time was excluded from analysis because it is comprised of several non-cognitive processes that are unrelated to decision-making (e.g., stimulus encoding, skeletomotor response speed).

Data analytic plan

Data screening indicated no outliers (> 3 SDs) or problematic skewness/kurtosis for any variable used in primary analyses.

Bayesian analyses

The primary data analytic strategy included Bayesian analyses conducted in JASP 0.8.2 (JASP Team 2017), rather than traditional null hypothesis significance testing. The benefits of Bayesian methods over p-values are well documented (e.g., Wagenmakers et al. 2016); for our purposes, Bayesian analyses allow stronger conclusions by estimating the magnitude of support for both the alternative and null hypotheses (Rouder and Morey 2012), and because they are appropriate for smaller samples sizes (Miočević et al. 2017).

Default JZS noninformative priors were selected because informative priors were not available for any parent and most child predictors. Instead of a p value, these analyses provide BF10, which is the Bayes Factor of the alternative hypothesis against the null hypothesis. BF10 is an odds ratio, where values above 3.0 are considered significant evidence supporting the alternative hypothesis (i.e., statistically significant evidence for the alternative hypothesis). BF10 values above 10.0 are considered strong (> 30 = very strong, > 100 = decisive/extreme support; Wagenmakers et al. 2016). Conversely, BF01 is the Bayes Factor of the null hypothesis against the alternative hypothesis and is reported when the evidence indicates a lack of an effect (i.e., favors the null hypothesis). BF01 values are interpreted identically to BF10, but in support of the null hypothesis that a predictor does not affect an outcome (Rouder and Morey 2012).

Analyses were conducted in a manner similar to those reported by Adalio et al. (2018). Bayes factor linear multiple regressions were conducted with the inattention, hyperactivity, and conduct problems scales from the BASC-2 as outcomes. Each model included pre-treatment symptom severity for that domain as a nuisance variable (covariate) and all five cognitive predictors (VSWM, PHWM, inhibitory control, drift rate, boundary separation). Child and parent cognitive function were examined in separate models. For each Bayesian regression, the best-fitting model was selected (criteria: individual main effects or combination of main effects with highest BF10 ≥ 3), with the contribution of individual predictors tested relative to this best-fitting model (Rouder and Morey 2012).

Exploratory analyses were conducted by repeating the analyses above with the addition of child age, parent age, and child medication status as potential covariates. Generally, the pattern of evidence for the alternative hypothesis (BF10) remained the same regardless of whether these covariates were included, but the evidence against the alternative hypothesis (BF01) was weaker. For ease of interpretation and because this is the first study to examine both child and parent cognition in relation to behavioral treatment response, we present analyses without age and medication status but report footnotes where inclusion of covariates significantly impacted results.

Results

Preliminary analyses

Treatment led to significant improvements in parent-rated attention problems (BF10 = 20.74, d = 0.61), hyper-activity (BF10 = 6.86 × 105, d = 1.47), conduct problems (BF10 = 135.11, d = 0.85), and relational frustration (BF10 = 5.29, d = 0.54). Treatment did not significantly impact parents’ self-reported parenting confidence (BF01 = 2.20, d = 0.17). Correlations between BASC/PRQ scores at pre/post and each parent and child cognitive variable are presented as supplemental online resources (see Supplemental Tables 1–5). The remaining analyses were conducted controlling for pre-treatment scores.

Child cognitive function

Inattention

Stronger VSWM (BF10 = 5.44, β = −0.23) and PHWM (BF10 = 4.79, β = −0.35) were associated with greater treatment-related reductions in attention problems. There was moderate evidence against effects of child inhibitory control (BF01 = 3.44, β = −0.10), drift rate (BF01 = 3.48, β = 0.17), and boundary separation (BF01 = 3.07, β = 0.04) on changes in attention.

Hyperactivity

There was moderate evidence against effects of all five child neurocognitive abilities on treatment-related improvements in hyperactivity (BF01 = 3.25–5.08).

Conduct problems

There was significant evidence against effects of most child neurocognitive abilities on treatment-related changes in conduct problems (BF01 = 3.55–4.72) but inconclusive evidence regarding PHWM effects on treatment outcomes (BF01 = 2.68).

Parental stress and confidence

Evidence regarding the relation between children’s cognitive functioning and parent-reported changes in relational frustration was inconclusive, but leaned toward the absence of an association for all cognitive variables (BF01 = 2.46–2.66). Parental confidence followed a similar pattern, such that evidence was inconclusive but leaned toward the absence of an association (BF01 = 1.64–2.43).

Parent cognitive function

Inattention

Bayesian evidence provided moderate support that higher parental boundary separation predicted greater treatment-related reductions in inattention (BF10 = 4.27, β = −0.43). Contrary to hypotheses, there was significant evidence against effects of parental WM on treatment-related changes in child attention (VSWM: BF01 = 3.91, β = 0.01; PHWM: BF01 = 3.85, β = −0.14). Bayesian evidence regarding inhibitory control (BF01 = 2.19, β = −0.20) and drift rate (BF01 = 1.71, β = 0.09) were inconclusive.

Hyperactivity

Consistent with the results of child cognitive function and hyperactivity, Bayesian analyses provided significant evidence against effects of PHWM, SSRT, drift rate, and boundary separation (BF01 = 3.00–5.46) on child outcomes. Evidence against VSWM was inconclusive (BF01 = 1.44).

Conduct problems

The best-fitting Bayesian model provided strong support that the combination of inhibitory control, drift rate, and boundary separation predicted treatment-related reductions in conduct problems, (BF10 = 17.34).2 Specifically, better inhibitory control on the stop-signal task (BF10 = 15.32, β = 0.34), and higher boundary separation (i.e., response caution) on the choice RT task (BF10 = 17.21, β = −0.66), predicted lower post-treatment conduct problems. Surprisingly, higher drift rate, reflective of faster processing speed, was associated with less treatment-related improvement in conduct problems (BF10 = 17.18, β = 0.63). Parental WM did not predict post-treatment conduct problems (VSWM: BF01 = 3.91, β = 0.01; PHWM: BF01 = 4.75, β = 0.14).

Parental stress and confidence

The best-fitting model provided very strong support for the combination of boundary separation and drift rate predicting decreased relational frustration with treatment (BF10 = 30.75).3 Similar to the pattern observed for conduct problems, higher boundary separation predicted greater treatment improvements in parent-reported relational frustration (BF10 = 29.77, β = −0.89), but faster drift rate was associated with less change in relational frustration (BF10 = 29.77, β = 1.0). Neither working memory (VSWM: BF01 = 3.02, β = −0.15; PHWM: BF01 = 2.91, β = 0.04) nor inhibitory control (BF01 = 3.03, β = 0.21) predicted treatment changes in relational frustration. For parental confidence, the model with the strongest evidentiary support included the combination of PHWM, inhibitory control, drift rate, and boundary separation (BF10 = 4.87). However, examination of the relative contribution of individual predictors indicated that the effect was carried primarily by drift rate (BF10 = 4.61, β = −0.90) and boundary separation (BF10 = 4.64, β = 0.93; PHWM: BF10 = 2.43, β = −0.50; inhibitory control: BF10 = 1.72, β = −0.40).4 Evidence regarding VSWM was inconclusive (BF01 = 2.62, β = 0.22).

Discussion

Enhancing treatment outcomes for children with ADHD requires identifying moderators that impact treatment effectiveness (Hinshaw 2007; Kraemer et al. 2002). The present pilot study integrated etiologic theories of ADHD (Bark-ley 1997a, b; Castellanos and Tannock 2002; Rapport et al. 2008) and models of parenting behavior (Crandall et al. 2015) to evaluate the extent to which core parent and child cognitive functions central to both domains (working memory, inhibitory control, processing speed) predicted response to ADHD behavioral parent training (BPT).

For children, a clear pattern emerged in which better WM predicted greater treatment-related attention improvements. Children with poor WM may benefit less from treatment because they have a harder time maintaining in mind the changing environmental contingencies that occur as a result of BPT. In contrast, there was significant evidence against effects of inhibitory control and processing speed on attention. Our use of Bayesian statistics allowed stronger conclusions by providing significant support for the null (instead of just failing to reject it). To that end, our findings are consistent with prior experimental and longitudinal work linking WM deficits with attention problems in ADHD (Karalunas et al. 2017; Kofler et al. 2010); however, they directly contradict the only other study to date to investigate effects of child neurocognitive abilities on behavioral treatment-related improvements in attention (Adalio et al. 2018), which reported significant effects of processing speed but no significant evidence for effects of WM. This discrepancy likely reflects the large differences in construct measurement between the two studies, as the current study employed laboratory-based tasks and computational modeling to derive construct estimates, whereas Adalio and colleagues measured global processing speed and WM with the construct-insensitive WISC-IV subscales (Canivez et al. 2016; Snyder et al. 2015). They also included two active treatments—a traditional parent training similar to that utilized in the current study, and a multimodal intervention that involved parent, child, and teacher components. This difference may suggest that processing speed is more important for benefitting from non-parenting components of multimodal psychosocial treatment.

For parents, greater levels of response caution (higher boundary separation) predicted greater reduction in child inattention. A similar pattern was found for conduct problems, with stronger parental inhibitory control, higher response caution, and slower information accumulation leading to greater improvement in children’s conduct problems. Parents with higher response caution and slower information accumulation also experienced greater reduction in their level of relational frustration with their child and improved confidence in their parenting abilities. Taken together, this pattern indicates that BPT produces greater benefit when parental self-regulation is stronger, reflected by strong inhibition and response styles emphasizing accuracy over speed. It may reflect that parents with better cognitive self-regulation abilities were better able to learn to withhold reactive, dominant responses to child misbehavior, which would be consistent with the literature showing that reductions in negative parenting behaviors (as opposed to increases in positive parenting) primarily drive treatment-related improvement in child behavior (Chronis-Tuscano et al. 2011). This pattern may also suggest that when child behavior/compliance is unclear in the moment, parents with greater inhibition and response caution may grant extra time to evaluate contextual factors to determine whether children comply, follow instructions, or adhere to rules, which in turn may relate to lower levels of perceived stress and frustration. These hypothesized relations between cognitive function and actual parenting behavior are an important avenue for future research, as the current study was not designed to examine associations between cognitive function and parental behavior.

Limitations

The current open-label pilot study was limited by a small sample size and a lack of follow-up after post-treatment. Due to the timing of parent and child assessments, children experienced a greater gap between cognitive assessment and treatment, which may have contributed to the general lack of association between child cognitive function and treatment outcomes. Completing child and parent assessments in closer temporal proximity in future work will help determine the extent to which results for children’s cognitive functioning in the current study were influenced by timing of assessments.

Given our reliance on parent ratings to assess change in child symptoms and parenting stress and confidence, we could not assess which parental behaviors changed throughout treatment. The use of parent ratings, rather than observations of parenting or child behavior, may explain why we found significant evidence against parental WM predicting outcomes in any symptom domain,5 even though WM has been previously linked to parenting behavior (e.g., Deater-Deckard et al. 2010). The lack of an association between cognitive functioning and parental confidence may also be a function of the normative pre-treatment levels of parenting confidence of the sample (i.e., overall, parents remained confident in their parenting while experiencing significantly reduced parenting stress).

Finally, we included several relevant covariates, including child medication status and child and parent age, and demonstrated that results were largely unaffected. It will be important for future studies to consider additional variables that may impact the observed relations, such as caregiver sex, generation status (i.e., grandparent/parent), and caregiver relationship status (Chacko et al. 2009).

Future directions

The current pilot investigation focused on the influence of parent and child cognitive processes in treatment response, as this is an important direction for psychosocial research (Molitor and Langberg 2017). Contemporary parenting models also emphasize emotion regulation in addition to cognitive function (Crandall et al. 2015) because parents must first be able to modulate their own emotional reactions to a situation before implementing adaptive parenting strategies. ADHD-specific parenting work also highlights parent motivational factors (Johnston et al. 2012). Motivational dysfunction in ADHD primarily involves the need for consistent extrinsic reinforcement to motivate behavior and the preference for immediate rewards (Luman et al. 2005). Most parenting involves reinforcement that is delayed (e.g., ignoring a child’s crying in the moment to work toward a longer-term goal of extinguishing tantrum behaviors), or inconsistent (a child thanks the parents only infrequently), and involves consequences that may not be particularly rewarding to the parent; this makes many aspects of successful parenting particularly challenging for individuals with ADHD-related motivation difficulties. Moving forward, emotion regulation, motivational processes, and cognition should be examined concurrently in studies of parenting in ADHD to assess their individual and collective impact on parenting behavior and treatment outcome, given their interplay (Deater-Deckard et al. 2010; Mazursky-Horowitz et al. 2017). Future work will also need to include parental ADHD symptoms, along with these basic processes, to examine their unique and interactive effects on parent and child behavior.

Finally, it is important to keep in mind that medication was a concurrent intervention for many children in this study. Progress toward the long-term goal of person-centered treatment matching and tailored interventions (e.g., based on cognitive profiles) will require evaluation of which parent and child variables predict response to single treatments (behavior therapy or medication) versus their combination, as well as better understanding treatment mechanisms (Hawk et al. in press).

Conclusion

This open-label pilot study was the first to integrate both parent and child cognitive processes that are central to etiologic theories of ADHD and models of parenting behavior into a treatment outcome study. We provided initial evidence that children’s and parents’ performance on brief computerized tasks can inform children’s symptom improvement in treatment—across methods and across a several month interval. We hope that this initial study demonstrates the feasibility of bridging the translational research gap between basic and applied clinical science and facilitates research on the role of cognitive function in psychosocial interventions.

Supplementary Material

Supplemental Materials

Acknowledgements

This work was supported by a UVa Curry School of Education Foundation grant from the Galant family (PI: Kofler). During manuscript preparation, the authors were supported by the National Institutes of Health (R34 MH102499–01; PI: Kofler) and the Mississippi Council on Developmental Disabilities (4680-DD17-HE; PI: Sarver). The sponsors had no role in design and conduct of the study, collection, management, analysis, and interpretation of the data, or reparation, review, or approval of the manuscript. The authors wish to acknowledge the children and families who participated in the study, as well as the efforts of Dr. Hillary Schaefer, Erin Lunsford, and the many research assistants who contributed to the project.

Electronic supplementary material The online version of this article (https://doi.org/10.1007/s12402-018-0259-8) contains supplementary material, which is available to authorized users.

Compliance with ethical standards

Conflict of interest The authors declare that they have no conflict of interest.

Human and animal rights This study was approved by the institution’s IRB. All procedures performed in studies involving human participants were in accordance with the ethical standards of the institutional and/or national research committee and with the 1964 Helsinki declaration and its later amendments or comparable ethical standards.

Informed consent Informed/assent was obtained from all individual participants included in the study.

1

SSD was also computed due to the current debate in the literature regarding the optimal metric for estimating inhibitory control (Alder-son et al. 2007). The pattern of results was the same for SSD and SSRT.

2

When child and parent age and child medication status are included as covariates, the best-fitting model includes PHWM, inhibitory control, drift rate, and boundary separation (BF10 = 51.08).

3

When child and parent age and child medication status are included as covariates, the best-fitting model includes VSWM, inhibitory control, drift rate, and boundary separation (BF10 = 9.47).

4

When child and parent age and child medication status are included as covariates, the best-fitting model includes drift rate and boundary separation (BF10 = 14.70).

5

Parent working memory was included in the best-fitting model for predicting treatment-related improvements in parenting confidence but failed to provide sufficient evidence as an individual predictor (i.e., its BF10 of 2.4 fell below the accepted threshold of 3.0). Similarly, parent working memory improved model fit for predicting treatment-related improvements in child conduct problems and parent relational frustration, but only when demographic covariates were included, suggesting that these findings may not be particularly robust. We thus emphasize findings from our a priori rather than exploratory models, while noting this inconsistency as an important area for future research.

References

  1. Achenbach TM, Rescorla LA (2001) Manual for the Achenbach system of empirically based assessment school-age forms profiles. Aseba, Burlington, VT [Google Scholar]
  2. Adalio CJ, Owens EB, McBurnett K, Hinshaw SP, Pfiffner LJ (2018) Processing speed predicts behavioral treatment outcomes in children with Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder predominantly inattentive type. J Abnormal Child Psychol 46(4):701–711 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Alderson RM, Rapport MD, Kofler MJ (2007) Attention-Deficit/Hyper-activity Disorder and behavioral inhibition: a meta-analysis review of the Stop-Signal paradigm. J Abnorm Child Psychol 35:745–758 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Barkley RA (1997a) Behavioral inhibition, sustained attention, and executive functions: constructing a unifying theory of ADHD. Psychol Bull 121:65–94 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Barkley RA (1997b) Defiant children. Guilford Press, New York [Google Scholar]
  6. Canivez GL, Watkins MW, Dombrowski SC (2016) Factor structure of the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children-Fifth Edition: exploratory factor analyses with the 16 primary and secondary subtests. Psychol Assess 28:975–986 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Castellanos FX, Tannock R (2002) Neuroscience of attention-deficits/hyperactivity disorder: the search for endophenotypes. Nat Rev Neurosci 3:617–628 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Chacko A, Wymbs BT, Wymbs FA, Pelham WE, Swanger-Gagne MS, Girio E, Pirvics L, Herbst L, Guzzo J, Phillips C, O’Connor B (2009) Enhancing traditional behavioral parent training for single mothers of children with ADHD. J Clin Child Adolesc Psychol 38:206–218 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Chronis-Tuscano A, O’Brien KA, Johnston C, Jones HA, Clarke TL, Raggi VL, Rooney ME, Diaz Y, Pian J, Seymour KE (2011) The relation between maternal ADHD symptoms & improvement in child behavior following brief behavioral parent training is mediated by changes in negative parenting. J Abnorm Child Psychol 39:1047–1057 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Chronis-Tuscano A, Wang CH, Woods KE, Strickland J, Stein MA (2017) Parent ADHD and evidence-based treatment for their children: review and directions for future research. J Abnorm Child Psychol 45:501–517 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Crandall A, Deater-Deckard K, Riley AW (2015) Maternal emotion and cognitive control capacities an parenting: a conceptual framework. Dev Rev 36:105–126 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Crosbie J, Arnold P, Paterson A, Swanson J, Dupuis A, Li X, Shan J, Goodale T, Tam C, Strug LJ, Schachar RJ (2013) Response inhibition and ADHD traits: correlates and heritability in a community sample. J Abnorm Child Psychol 41:497–507 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Deater-Deckard K, Sewell MD, Petrill SA, Thompson LA (2010) Maternal working memory and reactive negativity in parenting. Psychol Sci 21:75–79 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. DuPaul GJ, Power TJ, Anastopoulos AD, Reid R (2016) ADHD rating scale? 5 for children and adolescents: checklists, norms, and clinical interpretation. Guilford Publications [Google Scholar]
  15. Evans SW, Owens JS, Wymbs BT, Ray AR (2018) Evidence-based psychosocial treatments for children and adolescents with attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder. J Clin Child Adolesc Psychol 47(2):157–198 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Fair DA, Bathula D, Nikolas MA, Nigg JT (2012) Distinct neuropsychological subgroups in typically developing youth inform heterogeneity in children with ADHD. Proc Natl Acad Sci 109:6769–6774 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Faraone SV, Perlis RH, Doyle AE, Smoller JW, Goralnick JJ, Holmgren MA, Sklar P (2005) Molecular genetics of Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder. Biol Psychiat 57:1313–1323 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Fosco WD, White CN, Hawk LW Jr (2017) Acute stimulant treatment and reinforcement increase the speed of information accumulation in children with ADHD. J Abnorm Child Psychol 45:911–920 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Gadow KD, Sprafkin JN (2002) Child symptom inventory 4: screening and norms manual. Checkmate Plus, Stony Brook, NY [Google Scholar]
  20. Hawk LW, Fosco WD, Colder CR, Waxmonsky JG, Pelham WE, Rosch KS (2018) How do stimulant treatments for ADHD work? Evidence for mediation by improved cognitive function. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 10.1111/jcpp.12917 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Hinshaw SP (2007) Moderators and mediators of treatment outcome for youth with ADHD: understanding for whom and how interventions work. Ambul Pediatr 7:91–100 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. JASP Team (2017) JASP. Version 0.8.1. [Computer Software]
  23. Johnston C, Mash EJ, Miller N, Ninowski JE (2012) Parenting in adults with attention- deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Clin Psychol Rev 32:215–228 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Kamphaus RW, Reynolds CR (2006) PRQ: parenting relationship questionnaire. Pearson, Minneapolis [Google Scholar]
  25. Karalunas SL, Huang-Pollock CL, Nigg JT (2012) Decomposing attention- deficit/hyperactivity disorder-related effects in response speed and variability. Neuropsychology 26:684–694 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Karalunas SL, Gustafsson HC, Dieckmann NF, Tipsord J, Mitchell SH, Nigg JT (2017) Heterogeneity in development of aspects of working memory predicts longitudinal attention deficit hyperactivity disorder symptom change. J Abnorm Psychol 126(6):774. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Kasper LJ, Alderson RM, Hudec KL (2012) Moderators of working memory deficits in children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD): a meta-analytic review. Clin Psychol Rev 32:605–617 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Kaufman J, Birmaher B, Brent D, Rao UMA, Flynn C, Moreci P, Williamson D, Ryan N (1997) K-SADS-PL: initial reliability and validity data. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 36:980–988 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Kofler MJ, Sarver DE, Austin K, Schaefer H, Holland E, Aduen PA, Wells EL, Soto EF, Irwin L, Chatschneider C, Lonigan CJ (in press) Can working memory training work for ADHD? Development of central executive training and comparison with behavioral parent training. J Consult Clin Psychol. 10.1037/ccp0000308 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Kofler MJ, Rapport MD, Bolden J, Sarver DE, Raiker JS (2010) ADHD and working memory: the impact of central executive deficits and exceeding storage/rehearsal capacity on observed inattentive behavior. J Abnorm Child Psychol 38:149–161 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Kraemer HC, Wilson GT, Fairburn CG, Agras WS (2002) Mediators and moderators of treatment effects in randomized clinical trials. Arch Gen Psychiatry 59:877–884 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Lerche V, Voss A, Nagler M (2017) How many trials are required for parameter estimation in diffusion modeling? A comparison of different optimization criteria. Behav Res Methods 49:513–537 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Lipszyc J, Schachar R (2010) Inhibitory control and psychopathology: a meta-analysis of the stop signal task. J Int Neuropsychol Soc 16:1064–1076 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Luman M, Oosterlaan J, Sergeant JA (2005) The impact of reinforcement contingencies on AD/HD: a review and theoretical appraisal. Clin Psychol Rev 25:183–213 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Mazursky-Horowitz H, Thomas SR, Woods KE, Chrabaszcz JS, Deater-Deckard K, Chronis-Tuscano A (2017) Maternal executive functioning and scaffolding in families of children with and without parent-reported ADHD. J Abnormal Child Psychol 1–13 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Miočević M, MacKinnon DP, Levy R (2017) Power in Bayesian mediation analysis for small sample research. Struct Equ Model Multi-discip J 1–18 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Molitor SJ, Langberg JM (2017) Using task performance to inform treatment planning for youth with ADHD: a systematic review. Clin Psychol Rev 58:157–173 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Nigg JT, Gustafsson HC, Karalunas SL, Ryabinin P, McWeeney S, Faraone SV, Mooney MA, Fair DA, Wilmot B (2018) Working memory and vigilance as multivariate endophenotypes related to common genetic risk for Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 57(3):175–182 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Owens EB, Hinshaw SP, Kraemer HC, Eugene AL, Abikoff HB, Cantwell DP, Conners CK, Elliott G, Greenhill LL, Hechtman L, Hoza B (2003) Which treatment for whom for ADHD? Moderators of treatment response in the MTA. J Consult Clin Psychol 71:540–552 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Rapport MD, Alderson RM, Kofler MJ, Sarver DE, Bolden J, Sims V (2008) Working memory deficits in boys with Attention-deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder: the contribution of central executive and subsystem processes. J Abnorm Child Psychol 36:825–837 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Rapport MD, Bolden J, Kofler MJ, Sarver DE, Raiker JS, Alderson RM (2009) Hyperactivity in boys with attention-deficit/hyper-activity disorder (ADHD): a ubiquitous core symptom or manifestation of working memory deficits? J Abnorm Child Psychol 37(4):521–534 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Reynolds CR, Kamphaus RW (2004) BASC-2: behavior assessment system for children. American Guidance Service, Circle Pines, MN [Google Scholar]
  43. Rouder JN, Morey RD (2012) Default Bayes factors for model selection in regression. Multivar Behav Res 47:877–903 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Shaffer A, Obradović J (2017) Unique contributions of emotion regulation and executive functions in predicting the quality of parent–child interaction behaviors. J Fam Psychol 31:150–159 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Snyder HR, Miyake A, Hankin BL (2015) Advancing understanding of executive function impairments and psychopathology: bridging the gap between clinical and cognitive approaches. Front Psychol 6:328. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Sonuga-Barke EJS, Daley D, Thompson M (2002) Does maternal ADHD reduce the effectiveness of parent training for pre-school children’s ADHD? J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 41:696–702 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Tarle SJ, Alderson RM, Patros CH, Lea SE, Hudec KL, Arrington EF (2017) Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder and phonological working memory: methodological variability affects clinical and experimental performance metrics. Neuropsychology 31:383. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Verbruggen F, Chambers CD, Logan GD (2013) Fictitious inhibitory differences: how skewness and slowing distort the estimation of stopping latencies. Psychol Sci 24:352–362 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Voss A, Voss J, Lerche V (2015) Assessing cognitive processes with diffusion model analyses: a tutorial based on fast-dm-30. Front Psychol 6:336. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Wagenmakers EJ, Marsman M, Jamil T, Ly A, Verhagen AJ, Love J, Morey RD (2016) Bayesian statistical inference for psychological science. Part I: theoretical advantages and practical ramifications. Psychon Bull Rev 25:169–176 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Wechsler D Psychological, Psychological Corporation (2011) WASI-II: Wechsler abbreviated scale of intelligence, 2nd edn. Psychological Corporation, San Antonio [Google Scholar]
  52. Wells EL, Kofler MJ, Soto EF, Schaefer HS, Sarver DE (2018) Assessing working memory in children with ADHD: minor administration and scoring changes may improve digit span backward’s construct validity. Res Dev Disabil 72:166–178 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

Supplemental Materials

RESOURCES