Abstract
The cognitive model of negative symptoms posits that defeatist performance beliefs— overgeneralized negative beliefs about one’s ability to successfully perform tasks—contribute to the development and maintenance of negative symptoms. However, a conceptually similar construct, reduced generalized self-efficacy—diminished confidence in one’s ability to effectively complete or respond to new or challenging tasks and situations—has also been linked to negative symptoms. To identify which beliefs might be most important to target to reduce negative symptoms, we examined: 1) the association between defeatist performance and selfefficacy beliefs and 2) which beliefs are more strongly associated with negative symptoms in a non-clinical sample of young adults (N = 941). Analyses revealed a significant, medium-sized correlation between defeatist performance and self-efficacy beliefs. Both beliefs types were significantly associated with negative symptoms, but defeatist performance beliefs were more strongly related to negative symptoms than self-efficacy beliefs. Defeatist performance and selfefficacy beliefs appear to be distinct, yet overlapping constructs. Findings support the cognitive model and indicate that defeatist performance beliefs may have a greater role in the manifestation of negative symptoms than self-efficacy beliefs. Thus, defeatist performance beliefs may be a uniquely promising treatment target for reducing or preventing negative symptoms.
Keywords: negative symptoms, defeatist beliefs, schizotypy, schizophrenia, cognitive model
1. Introduction
Negative symptoms, such as blunted affect and asociality, are prevalent in people with schizophrenia-spectrum disorders (Bobes et al., 2010). These symptoms have been identified in individuals at various junctures on the spectrum, including those with schizotypy—the 10% of the population exhibiting traits reflecting a putative genetic liability for schizophrenia (Lenzenweger and Korfine, 1992; Meehl, 1962, 1990). Negative symptoms are also strongly linked to reduced functional outcomes and quality of life in schizophrenia-spectrum populations (Cohen and Davis, 2009; Fervaha et al., 2014; Grant and Beck, 2009). However, extant pharmacological treatments have demonstrated little effectiveness in ameliorating these symptoms (Tandon et al., 2010). Accordingly, researchers have worked to identify novel factors that may be targeted in psychosocial treatments to improve negative symptoms in those with schizophrenia.
According to the cognitive model of negative symptoms, defeatist beliefs such as defeatist performance beliefs—overgeneralized negative beliefs about one’s ability to successfully perform tasks (Grant and Beck, 2009)—are key psychological factors that lead to the development and maintenance of negative symptoms (Beck et al., 2009). Specifically, the model posits that people with a vulnerability to developing schizophrenia have neurocognitive problems and related difficulties that contribute to reduced performance in areas such as school, work, or social relationships. These discouraging experiences can lead to the formation of defeatist beliefs about one’s performance (i.e., ―It is not worth trying because I will only fail.”), which can contribute to reductions in motivation and engagement in goal-directed or enjoyable activities. Further, this withdrawal from tasks can also act as a maladaptive behavior to prevent expected poor performance or failure; by limiting experiences to counter negative beliefs about one’s abilities, defeatist beliefs are strengthened (Couture et al., 2011; Perivoliotis et al., 2009). As a result, for some people, this cycle results in the development of negative symptoms, further contributing to reduced functioning.
Empirical support for the cognitive model comes from both cross-sectional and longitudinal studies. To date, several studies have found small to large cross-sectional associations between increased defeatist performance beliefs and negative symptoms both in those diagnosed with schizophrenia (see Campellone et al., 2016 for a review; Couture et al., 2011; Grant and Beck, 2009) and those with schizotypy (Fervaha et al., 2015; Luther et al., 2016). Importantly, the relationship between defeatist performance beliefs and negative symptoms has been found to be independent of depression symptoms (Grant and Beck, 2009; Fervaha et al., 2015). Similarly, several studies have found that greater defeatist performance beliefs are related to reduced functioning and quality of life across the schizophrenia-spectrum (Grant and Beck, 2009; Luther et al., 2016; Pillny and Lincoln, 2017). Studies have also found that compared to controls, defeatist performance beliefs are elevated in individuals with schizotypy (Luther et al., 2016), those at ultra-high risk of developing schizophrenia (Perivoliotis et al., 2009), those with recent-onset schizophrenia (Ventura et al., 2014), and those with more prolonged schizophrenia (Grant and Beck, 2009; Horan et al., 2010; Kiwanuka et al., 2014), supporting the validity of the cognitive model. Longitudinal studies have also largely corroborated these cross-sectional findings by demonstrating links between reduced defeatist beliefs and lower prospective negative symptoms and greater time in social and occupational activities (Luther et al., 2015; Thomas et al., 2017). Taken together, these studies suggest that defeatist performance beliefs are an important psychological factor that contributes to both the development and maintenance of negative symptoms and reduced functioning.
Although the cognitive model specifies that different subtypes of defeatist beliefs and expectancies may lead to the development of negative symptoms (Beck et al., 2009), most studies to date have focused on defeatist performance beliefs. However, a putatively related concept, reduced generalized self-efficacy—diminished confidence in one’s ability to effectively complete or respond to new or challenging tasks and situations (Schwarzer and Jerusalem, 1995)—may also play a role in negative symptoms. Indeed, self-efficacy has been posited to be a key factor for successful task performance, persistence, and completion (Bandura, 1977, 1997; Luszczynska, Gutiérrez Doña, et al., 2005). To date, among people with schizophrenia, researchers have found that compared to controls, self-efficacy is reduced in people with early (Ventura et al., 2014) and relatively prolonged schizophrenia (Bentall et al., 2010; Lincoln et al., 2014). However, it is unclear if these beliefs are reduced in those with schizotypy. In addition, small to large correlations between greater self-efficacy and lower negative symptoms have been found in a sample of people with schizophrenia-spectrum disorders as well as in a college student sample (Avery et al., 2009; Kurtz et al., 2013; Ventura et al., 2014, Wolfradt et al., 1999). Finally, some studies have also observed that lower self-efficacy is related to reduced quality of life and psychosocial functioning in people with schizophrenia-spectrum disorders (Cardenas et al., 2013; Hill and Startup, 2013; Pratt et al., 2005).
Several key questions regarding defeatist performance beliefs and self-efficacy emerge given that they are conceptually similar, both types of beliefs have been linked with negative symptoms, and both belief types are often impaired in people diagnosed with schizophreniaspectrum disorders. First, it is unclear what the magnitude of the relationship is between these two beliefs. Elucidating the overlap between these constructs will help to disentangle whether we are assessing overlapping or distinct constructs, which could have clinical implications (e.g., should they be independent treatment targets?). Similarly, it is also unclear which type of belief is more strongly related to negative symptoms. Identifying which beliefs are more strongly related to subclinical negative symptoms1 (i.e., negative schizotypy traits) may help researchers and clinicians to determine what type of thought content is specific to negative symptoms (i.e., cognitive content-specificity hypothesis; Beck, 1976) and identify beliefs that may be most important to target in efforts to reduce or prevent the development of more severe negative symptoms. Finally, although both increased defeatist performance beliefs and reduced selfefficacy beliefs have been found in those with schizophrenia-spectrum disorder diagnoses in several studies (cf., Grant and Beck, 2009; Ventura et al., 2014), no studies to our knowledge have examined whether self-efficacy is reduced, and only one study has examined if defeatist performance beliefs are increased in those with schizotypy. Ascertaining whether those with a putative liability for schizophrenia have greater defeatist performance beliefs and reduced selfefficacy beliefs can help to identify whether these belief alterations are enduring aspects of the schizophrenia-spectrum, which can inform identification and early intervention efforts.
To investigate these questions, we examined the relationships between defeatist performance beliefs, self-efficacy beliefs, and negative symptoms in a sample from a nonclinical setting. Examining these relationships in a non-clinical sample helps to ensure that any identified relationships are independent of potential confounds seen in clinical samples, such as medication effects, hospitalizations, and stigma, which are more prevalent in studies of people who have had psychosis for a prolonged period (Gooding et al., 2005; Cohen et al., 2015). Based on conceptual similarities, we hypothesized that there would be a medium-sized, inverse relationship between self-efficacy and defeatist performance beliefs. Since the cognitive model posits that defeatist performance beliefs specifically are related to the development of negative symptoms (Beck et al., 2009), we hypothesized that these beliefs would be more strongly related to negative symptoms compared to self-efficacy beliefs. If these hypotheses were supported, we planned to conduct two additional analyses to further understand the relationships between these beliefs and negative symptoms; 1) to ensure that the relationship between defeatist performance beliefs and negative symptoms was independent of self-efficacy beliefs, we conducted a partial correlation controlling for self-efficacy; 2) to identify whether defeatist performance beliefs was an intervening variable in the relationship between self-efficacy and negative symptoms, we conducted a mediation analysis.
To further characterize these beliefs, we also conducted several secondary analyses. First, because past work has linked defeatist performance beliefs and depression symptoms (Grant and Beck, 2009; Fervaha et al., 2015), we examined whether the relationship between defeatist performance and self-efficacy beliefs would be independent of depression symptoms. We hypothesized that the relationship between both defeatist performance and self-efficacy beliefs and negative symptoms would remain significant after controlling for depression symptoms. Second, we explored whether defeatist performance or self-efficacy beliefs were more strongly related to quality of life and depression as well as positive and disorganized symptoms. Finally, defeatist performance beliefs have been found to be increased in people with schizotypy compared to those without schizotypy (Luther et al., 2016); however, reductions in self-efficacy have yet to be established in a schizotypy sample. Thus, we aimed to both replicate the previously observed elevation of defeatist performance beliefs in the schizotypy sample and determine whether self-efficacy beliefs were also reduced in those with schizotypy compared to those without schizotypy.
2. Methods
2.1. Participants and procedure
Participants were undergraduate students from a large mid-western university in the United States who self-selected to complete the current study for course research credit. As part of informed consent, participants were told that they would complete an anonymous survey consisting of various measures of attitudes, symptoms, and functioning, with the primary goal of the study being to better understand the relationships between these domains in university students. Over the course of approximately four semesters,1,025 participants completed the survey. To identify those who provided random or invalid responses, we embedded ten items from the Chapman Infrequency Scale (Chapman and Chapman, 1983) in the survey. Following prior studies (Cohen and Davis, 2009; Minor et al., 2018), we used an abbreviated version of the scale to reduce participant burden. This scale includes items such as ―I find that I often walk with a limp, which is the result of a skydiving accident.” Participants (n = 84) were excluded if they endorsed (e.g., agreed or strongly agreed with) more than two items, which resulted in a final sample of 941 participants. Demographic characteristics of the included participants are displayed in Table 1. All procedures were approved by the institutional review board at Indiana University—Purdue University Indianapolis.
Table 1.
Means and Standard Deviations for Demographic Characteristics and Study Measures (n = 941)
% | N | |
---|---|---|
Gender (% female) | 78.5 | 739 |
Ethnicity (% Caucasian) | 77.0 | 725 |
M | SD | |
Age | 20.0 | 3.7 |
SPQ-BR – Negative | 15.1 | 4.8 |
SPQ-BR – Positive | 33.7 | 9.0 |
SPQ-BR – Disorganized | 23.8 | 6.1 |
Defeatist Performance Beliefs | 44.6 | 14.6 |
Self-Efficacy Beliefs | 30.5 | 4.1 |
PHQ-9 – Depressiona | 7.8 | 5.8 |
LEH – QOL – Subjective | 4.8 | 0.8 |
Note. SPQ-BR = Schizotypal Personality Questionnaire-Brief Revised; LEH – QOL = Lehman’s Quality of Life Measure; PHQ-9 =9-item Patient Health Questionnaire.
n = 918.
2.2. Measures
2.2.1. Schizotypy traits
In order to assess for positive, negative, and disorganized symptoms and to identify participants with and without schizotypy, we used the Schizotypal Personality QuestionnaireBrief Revised (SPQ-BR; Cohen et al., 2010). The SPQ-BR consists of 32 self-report items, and participants were asked to rate each item on a 5-point Likert scale from 1 (strongly disagree) to 5 (strongly agree). Items can be divided into positive (ideas of references, odd beliefs or magical thinking, suspiciousness, and unusual perceptual experiences items), negative (no close friends and constricted affect items), and disorganized (odd speech and odd or eccentric behavior items) symptom factors. A sample item from the negative factor is ―I find it hard to be emotionally close to other people.” Greater scores on each factor and the total score are indicative of higher symptoms, and score ranges are as follows: positive factor (14–70), negative factor (6–30), disorganized factor (8–40), and total score (32–160). Scores on the SPQ-BR have shown adequate content and convergent validity as well as good internal consistency in college samples (Callaway et al., 2014; Cohen et al., 2010). Scores for the SPQ-BR total score and factors demonstrated good to excellent internal consistency in the present sample (α’s range = 0.80 to 0.92).
2.2.2. Defeatist performance beliefs
The defeatist performance beliefs subscale (Grant and Beck, 2009) of the Dysfunctional Attitudes Scale (DAS; Weissman, 1978) was used to assess defeatist performance beliefs. The subscale consists of 15 self-report items that are rated on a 7-point Likert scale, ranging from 1 (agree totally) to 7 (disagree totally). A sample item is ―If you cannot do something well, there is little point in doing it at all.” Scores are coded so that a higher score reflects increased defeatist performance beliefs, and total scores range from 15 to 105. Consistent with prior studies with college students (Cane et al., 1986; Fervaha et al., 2015; Luther et al., 2015), the subscale demonstrated excellent internal consistency (α = 0.90).
2.2.3. Self-efficacy beliefs
The Generalized Self-Efficacy Scale (Schwarzer and Jerusalem, 1995) was used to determine participants’ global level of self-efficacy. This was chosen over more domain or taskspecific self-efficacy measures because defeatist performance beliefs are also not domain or taskspecific. The Generalized Self-Efficacy Scale assesses a person’s confidence in his/her ability to effectively complete or respond to new or challenging situations and associated obstacles. It assesses a broad sense of a person’s perceived competence to effectively complete or deal effectively with a range of challenging or demanding tasks. The scale involves 10 self-report items that are rated on a 4-point Likert Scale ranging from 1 (not at all true) to 4 (exactly true). A sample item is ―I can always manage to solve difficult problems if I try hard enough.” Total scores range from 10 to 40, and higher scores indicate stronger self-efficacy. This measure has shown acceptable construct validity, test-retest reliability, and internal consistency (Leganger et al., 2000; Schwarzer et al., 1999) in a range of samples, including college students (Schwarzer et al., 1997; Zhang and Schwarzer, 1995). Internal consistency in the present sample was good (α = 0.85).
2.2.4. Depression symptoms
The depression module of the Patient Health Questionnaire (PHQ; Spitzer et al., 1999; Kroenke et al., 2001) was used to assess depression symptoms. This 9-item measure, known as the PHQ-9 (Kroenke et al., 2001), consists of self-reported depression symptoms that are rated on a 4-point Likert scale ranging from 0 (not at all) to 3 (nearly every day). A sample symptom is ―Feeling bad about yourself—or that you are a failure or have let yourself or your family down.” Total scores range from 0 to 27, and higher scores indicate greater depression symptom severity. The PHQ-9 has demonstrated excellent test-retest reliability, construct validity, and criterion validity (Kroenke et al., 2001) and has been used with college student samples (Eisenberg et al., 2007; Granillo, 2012). The PHQ-9 score demonstrated good internal consistency in the present sample (α = 0.89).
2.2.5. Quality of life
Quality of life (QOL) was assessed using the self-report Lehman Quality of Life Questionnaire TL-30 (Lehman, 1996), which assesses similar domains as Lehman’s Quality of Life Interview—Brief Version (Lehman et al., 1995). The TL-30 contains 30 items and assesses both subjective and objective QOL across the following domains: living situation, daily activities, family and social relationships, finances, health, and safety. The objective items are rated on variable response scales and assess frequency of behaviors (e.g., ―How often do you talk to a member of your family on the telephone?”) or access to different resources (e.g., In the past month, did you have enough money for housing?). The subjective items (e.g., ―Select the item that best describes how you feel about the amount of friendship in your life.”) are rated on a 7point satisfaction scale ranging from 1 (terrible) to 7 (delighted), with the mean total score for the subjective QOL subscale also ranging from 1 to 7. Items are scored such that higher scores suggest greater QOL. The TL-30 has shown good convergent validity and test-retest reliability (Nørholm and Bech, 2007; Cohen et al., 2010) and similar versions of the questionnaire have been used with college students (Cohen and Davis, 2009; Cohen et al., 2010). In the current sample, internal consistency was good for the QOL subjective subscale (α = 0.82) but was poor for the objective subscale (α = 0.42). Consequently, we only used the subjective QOL subscale in further analyses.
2.3. Analyses
Analyses were conducted in several steps. First, we conducted descriptive statistics to characterize the sample and ensure that the data adhered to statistical assumptions. Next, we conducted a Pearson’s correlation in the entire sample to test our hypothesis that there would be a significant and medium-sized relationship between defeatist performance beliefs and selfefficacy beliefs. We also used the coefficient of determination (r2) to identify the amount of overlapping and non-overlapping variance between these two beliefs. To test our hypothesis that defeatist performance beliefs were more strongly related to negative symptoms than self-efficacy beliefs, we used Fisher’s r-to-z transformations to compare the magnitude of the correlations between the two beliefs and negative symptoms. Once these hypotheses were supported, we then examined whether the relationship between defeatist performance beliefs and negative symptoms was independent of self-efficacy beliefs by conducting a partial correlation controlling for selfefficacy. We also examined whether defeatist performance beliefs was a significant mediator between self-efficacy beliefs and negative symptoms using the PROCESS macro (Hayes, 2013). To test for mediation, we examined the indirect effect (ab) of self-efficacy beliefs on negative symptoms through defeatist performance beliefs by conducting two ordinary least square regressions within the macro: 1) to obtain a, defeatist performance beliefs was regressed onto self-efficacy beliefs; 2) negative symptoms were then regressed onto both defeatist performance beliefs (produces b) and self-efficacy beliefs (produces c’, which is the direct effect of selfefficacy on negative symptoms independent of the pathway through defeatist performance beliefs). We used a 95% bias-corrected bootstrap confidence interval based on 10,000 bootstrap samples for the indirect effect; significant mediation is indicated if the confidence interval does not include zero (Hayes, 2013).
To test our secondary hypothesis that the relationship between both beliefs and negative symptoms would be independent of depression symptoms, we conducted partial correlations controlling for depression symptoms. For exploratory analyses, we conducted Pearson’s correlations to explore the relationships between both defeatist performance beliefs and selfefficacy beliefs and symptoms (positive, disorganized, and depression symptoms) and subjective QOL. To explore which beliefs (defeatist performance beliefs or self-efficacy beliefs) were more strongly related to these symptoms and QOL, we used Fisher’s r-to-z transformations. For all correlations, we interpreted the magnitude based on Cohen’s (1992) recommendation for correlations where 0.10 is small, 0.30 is medium, and 0.50 is large. To account for the multiple comparisons, we used the Bonferroni correction, resulting in a significance level of p < .003 (.05/15 total correlations) for all correlations.
Finally, we used two independent samples t-tests to test our hypothesis that defeatist performance beliefs would be increased and self-efficacy beliefs would be reduced in those with schizotypy (> 1.65 standard deviations (95th percentile) above the mean of the total SPQ-BR score) compared to those without schizotypy (< 1.65 standard deviations on the mean of the total SPQ-BR score). Following prior procedures (Cohen et al., 2012; Minor et al., 2015), the schizotypy and non-schizotypy groups were identified after controlling for gender and ethnicity. Based on evidence indicating that schizotypy has a population prevalence of about 10% (Lenzenweger, 2006; Meehl, 1962), we followed Cohen et al. (2012) and Minor et al. (2015) and adopted a conservative approach of including only the top 5% of scorers in the schizotypy group in attempts to reduce false positives. Finally, we calculated Cohen’s d as a measure of effect size and followed Cohen (1992) in categorizing effect sizes of 0.2 as small, 0.5 as medium, and 0.8 as large.
3. Results
3.1. Measure descriptives
Measure descriptives are in Table 1. Notably, the three mean SPQ-BR subscale scores all suggested that on average, participants responded to symptom experience items with either disagree or neutral. Similarly, the mean belief scores indicated that on average, participants slightly disagreed with the defeatist performance beliefs items and indicated that the self-efficacy statements were moderately true.
3.2. Correlations with defeatist performance and self-efficacy beliefs
Consistent with our hypothesis, defeatist performance beliefs were significantly and inversely related to self-efficacy beliefs (r = -0.31, p < .001), showing a medium correlation. Notably, the coefficient of determination indicated that these beliefs only share 9.61% of underlying variance, while 90.39% of the variance in each belief is not accounted for by the other belief. In addition, greater defeatist performance beliefs and lower self-efficacy beliefs were also significantly related to increased negative symptoms. The magnitude of the relationship between negative symptoms and defeatist performance beliefs was medium (r =0.43, p < 0.001), while the relationship between negative symptoms and self-efficacy was small (r = -0.26, p < 0.001); both relationships remained significant after controlling for depression (see Table 2). As hypothesized, defeatist performance beliefs were more strongly related to negative symptoms than self-efficacy beliefs (Z = 4.20, p < 0.001), and the difference remained significant after controlling for depression symptoms (Z = 3.88, p < 0.001).
Table 2.
Correlations with defeatist performance and self-efficacy beliefs (n = 941)
Defeatist Performance Beliefs | Self-Efficacy Beliefs | Statistical Difference (Z) | |
---|---|---|---|
SPQ-BR – Negative | 0.43** | –0.26** | 4.20*** |
SPQ-BR – Negative (controlling for depression)a, b | 0.33** | –0.16** | 3.88*** |
SPQ-BR – Positive | 0.38** | –0.16** | 5.17*** |
SPQ-BR – Disorganized | 0.28** | –0.12** | 3.62*** |
PHQ-9 – Depressionb | 0.40** | –0.30** | 2.44* |
LEH – QOL – Subjective | –0.38** | 0.35** | 0.75 |
Note. SPQ-BR = Schizotypal Personality Questionnaire-Brief Revised; LEH – QOL = Lehman’s Quality of Life Measure; PHQ-9 = 9-item Patient Health Questionnaire.
p < 0.05
p < 0.003 or Bonferroni-corrected significance value for correlations
p < 0.001
Partial correlation.
n = 918.
Given that our hypotheses were supported, we also conducted additional partial correlations (as well as a mediation analysis; see below). The relationship between defeatist performance beliefs and negative symptoms remained significant when controlling for selfefficacy (r = .38, p < .001). We explored whether this was also true with self-efficacy and negative symptoms, finding that the relationship between self-efficacy and negative symptoms remained significant when controlling for defeatist performance beliefs (r = -.14, p < .001).
In additional exploratory analyses, higher defeatist performance beliefs and lower selfefficacy beliefs were significantly related to greater positive and disorganized symptoms, greater depression symptoms, and lower subjective QOL (see Table 2). When we compared the magnitude of these correlations, defeatist performance beliefs were more strongly related to positive and disorganized symptoms and depression symptoms than self-efficacy beliefs. The magnitude of the correlations between the two beliefs and QOL did not statistically differ.
3.3. Defeatist performance beliefs as a mediator
Mediation results are reported in Table 3 and Figure 1. Defeatist performance beliefs significantly mediated the relationship between self-efficacy beliefs and negative symptoms. Specifically, those who had reduced self-efficacy beliefs also reported greater defeatist performance beliefs (a = -1.09, p < .001), which in turn led to increased negative symptoms (b = .13, p < .001). The 95% bias-corrected bootstrap confidence interval for the indirect effect (ab = -.14) did not include zero [-.18, -.11], indicating significant mediation. After accounting for the defeatist performance beliefs pathway, there was also a significant direct effect of self-efficacy beliefs on negative symptoms (c’ = -.16, p < .001).
Table 3.
Mediation analysis model coefficients (n = 941)
Antecedent | Consequent | |||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Defeatist Performance Beliefs |
Negative Symptoms |
|||||||
Coefficient | SE | p | Coefficient | SE | p | |||
Self-Efficacy Beliefs | a | −1.09 | .11 | < .001 | c’ | −.16 | .04 | < .001 |
Defeatist Performance Beliefs | – | – | – | – | b | .13 | .01 | < .001 |
Constant | ii | 77.85 | 3.36 | < .001 | i2 | 14.16 | 1.31 | < .001 |
R2 = .10 | R2 = .20 | |||||||
F(1,939) = 99.71, p < .001 | F(2,938) = 118.83, p < .001 |
Figure 1.
Defeatist performance beliefs as a mediator between self-efficacy beliefs and negative symptoms. * p < .001.
3.4.. Group differences on defeatist performance and self-efficacy beliefs
As hypothesized, the schizotypy group (n = 44) reported significantly more defeatist performance beliefs (M = 56.7, SD = 17.2, p < .001) and less self-efficacy beliefs (M = 28.6, SD = 4.6, p < .001) compared to the non-schizotypy group (n = 58) (defeatist, M = 30.0 SD = 10.1; self-efficacy, M = 33.1 SD = 3.9). Both of these effect sizes were large (defeatist, d = 1.89; selfefficacy, d = -1.06).
4. Discussion
DEFEATIST AND SELF-EFFICACY BELIEFS
Efforts to treat the negative symptoms of schizophrenia have led to interest in the identification of psychological treatment targets. Informed by the cognitive model of negative symptoms (Beck et al., 2009), the primary aim of this study was to examine the magnitude of the overlap between two types of beliefs, defeatist performance and self-efficacy beliefs, as well as their association with negative symptoms in a sample from a non-clinical setting. In line with our hypothesis, we observed a significant, inverse relationship that was medium in magnitude between the two belief types. Defeatist performance beliefs demonstrated a significant and medium-sized association with negative symptoms, while self-efficacy beliefs evidenced a small but significant association with negative symptoms. As hypothesized, defeatist performance beliefs were more strongly related to negative symptoms than self-efficacy beliefs.
Our findings indicate that defeatist performance and self-efficacy beliefs are overlapping, yet distinct constructs. Specifically, the magnitude of the association between these two beliefs indicates that only 9.61% of the underlying variance in these constructs is shared, suggesting that over 90% of the variance is unique to each construct. Accordingly, these beliefs may be thought of as independent treatment targets. Further, the magnitude of the correlation is consistent with Ventura et al. (2014), who also found a medium-sized association between more task specific self-efficacy and defeatist performance beliefs in people with early psychosis. However, based on the theoretical similarities between defeatist performance beliefs and generalized selfefficacy, one might have expected a stronger correlation in the current study; this was likely not observed for several reasons. First, several of the items on the self-efficacy scale reference novel or unexpected events, whereas items on the defeatist performance belief scale mostly focus on more general experiences such as doing well or asking a question. Given the uncertainty surrounding novel or unexpected events, it is likely that a person may have divergent beliefs about their ability to perform more general experiences compared to novel, unknown experiences. In addition, 14 of the 15 items on the defeatist performance beliefs scale are negatively phrased, while all the items on the self-efficacy scale are positively phrased. Some studies have found differential response patterns to positively and negatively worded items (DiStefano and Motl, 2009; Lindwall et al., 2012). For example, one study found that students were more likely to strongly agree with positively phrased items than to strongly disagree with negatively phrased items (Weems et al., 2010). Other studies have found that personality traits such as neuroticism are associated with a greater likelihood of endorsing negatively worded items (Quilty et al., 2006). It will be helpful for future research to determine whether people, including those on the schizophrenia-spectrum, have differential responses to these belief items when they are positively or negatively phrased.
Consistent with the cognitive model of negative symptoms, our findings suggest that greater dysfunctional beliefs (i.e., increased defeatist performance and reduced self-efficacy beliefs) are related to increased negative symptoms. Notably, these findings build on prior studies that have found associations between these two beliefs and negative symptoms in people with a diagnosis of a schizophrenia-spectrum disorder (cf., Horan et al., 2010; Avery et al., 2009). Further, the current findings, along with prior studies by Fervaha et al. (2014), Luther et al. (2016), and Wolfradt et al. (1999), extend the associations between both defeatist performance and self-efficacy beliefs and negative symptoms to a non-clinical sample. Importantly, this suggests that these associations are not reducible to effects associated with a schizophrenia diagnosis, such as stigma and antipsychotic medication effects. Similarly, it suggests that these beliefs also contribute to negative symptoms even among healthy samples. Additionally, we also found that each belief type was uniquely associated with negative symptoms as the relationship between defeatist performance beliefs and negative symptoms remained significant after controlling for self-efficacy beliefs (and vice versa); this further indicates that these beliefs may be independent constructs and treatment targets, particularly for negative symptoms.
Importantly, while our findings indicate that these beliefs may be distinct treatment targets, they also suggest that defeatist performance beliefs may be a more effective target for negative symptoms. Indeed, we observed a significantly stronger relationship between defeatist performance beliefs and negative symptoms than with self-efficacy beliefs. Moreover, the stronger association between defeatist performance beliefs and negative symptoms remained even when controlling for depression symptoms. This suggests that independent of depression symptoms, negative beliefs about one’s ability to perform tasks contribute to negative symptoms to a greater extent than confidence in one’s ability to effectively complete or respond to new or challenging tasks and situations. These findings are in accord with the cognitive contentspecificity hypothesis (Beck, 1976) and suggest that defeatist performance beliefs have greater specificity for negative symptoms than self-efficacy beliefs. Further supporting the key role of defeatist performance beliefs in negative symptoms, we also found that self-efficacy’s effect on negative symptoms was mediated by defeatist performance beliefs. In other words, defeatist performance beliefs serve as an intermediary link that helps to explain how reduced self-efficacy leads to increased negative symptoms. Although longitudinal studies are needed to confirm these potential causal links, overall these findings point to defeatist performance beliefs in particular as a key mechanism and treatment target for reducing negative symptoms. Notably, these findings also support the validity of the cognitive model, and specifically the role the model places on defeatist performance beliefs in negative symptoms. Taken together, these results suggest that targeting defeatist performance beliefs in psychosocial treatments may lead to greater reductions in negative symptoms than targeting self-efficacy beliefs. Future research may more decisively test this by examining the differential impact on negative symptoms when independently targeting defeatist performance beliefs and self-efficacy beliefs using cognitive therapy approaches (cf., Granholm et al., 2016; Grant et al., 2012).
We also explored whether one belief type was more strongly related to additional schizophrenia-spectrum symptoms as well as depression and subjective quality of life. Both increased defeatist performance beliefs and lower self-efficacy beliefs were significantly associated with greater positive, disorganized, and depression symptoms, and reduced quality of life. Importantly, the relationships between these symptoms were significantly stronger for defeatist performance beliefs than for self-efficacy beliefs. However, the magnitude of the correlations did not differ for quality of life. This may suggest that while both beliefs similarly contribute to quality of life, defeatist performance beliefs have greater specificity for these symptoms. Indeed, theories of self-efficacy largely describe these beliefs as more proximal predictors of task completion or behaviors relevant to quality of life (Bandura, 1977, 1997; Luszczynska, Scholz, et al., 2005), whereas the cognitive model places greater emphasis on the role that defeatist performance beliefs have on symptoms (Beck et al., 2009). Thus, our results suggest that in addition to improving negative symptoms, targeting defeatist performance beliefs (to a greater extent than self-efficacy beliefs) may help to improve positive, disorganized, and depression symptoms. Further, targeting both beliefs may lead to improvements in quality of life. Indeed, this is consistent with findings that targeting defeatist beliefs with cognitive therapy approaches can not only reduce negative symptoms but also reduce positive symptoms and improve functioning and life satisfaction among people with schizophrenia (Granholm et al., 2013, 2014; Grant et al., 2012).
A final aim of our study was to ascertain whether these beliefs were altered in those with schizotypy compared to those without schizotypy. Consistent with the cognitive model and our hypothesis, we found that defeatist performance beliefs were increased and self-efficacy beliefs were reduced in those with schizotypy compared to those without schizotypy, with both evidencing large effect sizes. These findings replicate the results of prior work demonstrating that defeatist performance beliefs were also increased among people with schizotypy that completed study measures in a laboratory setting (Luther et al., 2016). Further, our results extend prior work demonstrating that self-efficacy is reduced in people with a schizophrenia-spectrum diagnosis (Lincoln et al., 2014; Ventura et al., 2014) by identifying that these beliefs are also reduced in people with a putative liability for schizophrenia. These findings suggest that alterations in these beliefs develop prior to a formal diagnosis (rather than in response to the diagnosis) and are present in those even on the higher end of functioning on the schizophreniaspectrum. Taken together with studies that have found that both of these beliefs are impaired in people with early (Ventura et al., 2014) and prolonged psychosis (Grant and Beck, 2009; Lincoln et al., 2014), these beliefs may represent enduring aspects of the schizophrenia-spectrum. Thus, both beliefs may be important to screen for and target in early intervention services. However, because the effect size was relatively larger for defeatist performance beliefs and these beliefs were more strongly associated with schizophrenia-spectrum symptoms, defeatist performance beliefs may be a more primary target compared to self-efficacy beliefs.
Although a strength of this study was the sample size, the sample was recruited from a single university, which may limit the generalizability of these findings. Additionally, the crosssectional nature of this study limits our ability to identify the direction or causal nature of the observed relationships. Further, although we used infrequency items to reduce random and invalid responses, this study was based on self-report measures that were completed online. It will be important for future research to replicate and extend these findings using laboratorybased assessments. In addition, while this study was conducted in a non-clinical setting, future studies should also screen for mental health disorders and consider contextual factors that might impact university students’ responses and symptom levels (e.g., point in semester, course load). Finally, our investigation focused on overall negative symptoms and on one belief subtype specified in the cognitive model of negative symptoms; future research is needed to examine the content specificity of other dysfunctional beliefs described in the model, such as low expectancies for success (Beck et al., 2009), for overall and specific negative symptoms (e.g., alogia, anhedonia).
In summary, this study adds additional support for the cognitive model of negative symptoms and also highlights the increased specificity defeatist performance beliefs have for negative symptoms compared to self-efficacy beliefs. In a sample from a non-clinical setting, we found that defeatist performance beliefs were distinct, yet overlapped with self-efficacy beliefs and were more strongly correlated with negative symptoms (as well as positive, disorganized, and depression symptoms) than self-efficacy beliefs. Both increased defeatist performance and reduced self-efficacy beliefs also demonstrated similar relationships with reduced subjective quality of life. In line with the cognitive model, these findings suggest that even at the subclinical level, increased defeatist thinking is associated with increased negative symptomology and reductions in quality of life. Further, we also found that those with schizotypy evidenced relatively larger deficits in defeatist performance beliefs compared to self-efficacy beliefs. Taken together, these findings point to defeatist performance beliefs as a uniquely promising treatment target for efforts to reduce or prevent the development of more severe schizophrenia symptomology, particularly negative symptoms.
Highlights.
Links tested among defeatist performance and self-efficacy beliefs and negative symptoms
There was a medium-sized, inverse correlation between the belief types
Both beliefs correlated with negative symptoms; defeatist performance beliefs link was stronger
These two belief types seem to be distinct but overlapping constructs
Defeatist performance beliefs may have a greater role in negative symptoms
Role of the funding source:
This work was supported by a Predoctoral Award (to LL) from the Indiana Clinical and Translational Sciences Institute, which is funded in part by a National Institutes of Health (NIH), National Center for Advancing Translational Sciences, Clinical and Translational Sciences Award (# UL1TR001108) and by a NIH training grant (to RLF) (# 5T32MH096682). These funding sources had no further role in the study design, data collection, analysis, and interpretation or in the writing of the manuscript and the decision to submit the article.
Footnotes
Recognizing that someone may have a symptom of a disorder without being diagnosed or clinically treated, we have retained the simpler term symptom throughout.
Declarations of interest: none
Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final citable form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.
References
- Avery R, Startup M, Calabria K, 2009. The role of effort, cognitive expectancy appraisals and coping style in the maintenance of the negative symptoms of schizophrenia. Psychiatry Res. 167, 36–46. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bandura A 1977. Self-efficacy: toward a unifying theory of behavioral change. Psychol. Rev 84, 191–215. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bandura A, 1997. Self-efficacy: The Exercise of Control. Freeman, New York. [Google Scholar]
- Beck AT, 1976. Cognitive Therapy and the Emotional Disorders. International Universities Press, New York. [Google Scholar]
- Beck AT, Rector NA, Stolar N, Grant P, 2009. Schizophrenia: Cognitive Theory, Research, and Therapy. Guilford Press, New York. [Google Scholar]
- Bentall RP, Simpson PW, Lee DA, Williams S, Elves S, Brabbins C, et al. , 2010. Motivation and avolition in schizophrenia patients: The role of self efficacy. Psychosis. 2, 12–22. [Google Scholar]
- Bobes J, Arango C, Garcia-Garcia M, Rejas J, 2010. Prevalence of negative symptoms in outpatients with schizophrenia spectrum disorders treated with antipsychotics in routine clinical practice: Findings from the CLAMORS study. J. Clin. Psychiatry 71, 280–286. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Callaway DA, Cohen AS, Matthews RA, Dinzeo T, 2014. Schizotypal personality questionnaire-brief revised: Psychometric replication and extension. Personal. Disord 5, 32–38. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Campellone TR, Sanchez AH, Kring AM, 2016. Defeatist performance beliefs, negative symptoms, and functional outcome in schizophrenia: A meta-analytic review. Schizophr. Bull 42, 1343–1352. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cane DB, Olinger LJ, Gotlib IH, Kuiper NA, 1986. Factor structure of the dysfunctional attitudes scale in a student population. J. Clin. Psychol 42, 307–309. [Google Scholar]
- Cardenas V, Abel S, Bowie CR, Tiznado D, Depp CA, Patterson TL, et al. , 2013. When functional capacity and real-world functioning converge: the role of selfefficacy. Schizophr. Bull 39, 908–916. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chapman LJ, Chapman JP, 1983. Infrequency Scale. Unpublished test. Madison, Wisconsin. Cohen, J., 1992. A power primer. Psychol. Bull. 112, 155–159. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cohen AS, Davis TE, 2009. Quality of life across the schizotypy spectrum: Findings from a large nonclinical adult sample. Compr. Psychiatry. 50, 408–414. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cohen AS, Matthews RA, Najolia GM, Brown LA, 2010. Toward a more psychometrically sound brief measure of schizotypal traits: Introducing the SPQ-Brief Revised. J. Pers. Disord 24, 516–537. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cohen AS, Callaway DA, Najolia GM, Larsen JT, Strauss GP, 2012. On ―risk” and reward: Investigating state anhedonia in psychometrically defined schizotypy and schizophrenia. J. Abnorm. Psychol 121, 407–415. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cohen AS, Mohr C, Ettinger U, Chan RC, Park S, 2015. Schizotypy as an organizing framework for social and affective sciences. Schizophr. Bull 41, S427–S435. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Couture SM, Blanchard JJ, Bennett ME, 2011. Negative expectancy appraisals and defeatist performance beliefs and negative symptoms of schizophrenia. Psychiatry Res. 189, 43–48. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- DiStefano C, Motl RW, 2009. Further investigating method effects associated with negatively worded items on self-report surveys. Stuct. Equ. Modeling 13, 440–464. [Google Scholar]
- Eisenberg D, Gollust SE, Golberstein E, Hefner JL, 2007. Prevalence and correlates of depression, anxiety, and suicidality among university students. Am. J. Orthopsychiatry 77, 534–542. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fervaha G, Foussias G, Agid O, Remington G, 2014. Motivational and neurocognitive deficits are central to the prediction of longitudinal functional outcome in schizophrenia. Acta Psychiatr. Scand 130, 290–299. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fervaha G, Zakzanis KK, Foussias G, Agid O, Remington G, 2015. Distress related to subclinical negative symptoms in a non-clinical sample: Role of dysfunctional attitudes. Psychiatry Res. 230, 249–254. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gooding DC, Tallent KA, Matts CW, 2005. Clinical status of at-risk individuals 5 years later: Further validation of the psychometric high-risk strategy. J. Abnorm. Psychol 114, 170–175. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Granholm E, Holden J, Link PC, McQuaid JR, 2014. Randomized clinical trial of cognitive behavioral social skills training for schizophrenia: Improvement in functioning and experiential negative symptoms. J. Consult. Clin. Psychol 82, 1173–1185. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Granholm E, Holden J, Link PC, McQuaid JR, Jeste DV, 2013. Randomized controlled trial of cognitive behavioral social skills training for older consumers with schizophrenia: Defeatist performance attitudes and functional outcome. Am. J. Geriatr. Psychiatry 21, 251–262. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Granholm EL, McQuaid JR, Holden JL, 2016. Cognitive-behavioral social skills training for schizophrenia: A practical treatment guide. Guilford Publications, New York. [Google Scholar]
- Granillo MT, 2012. Structure and function of the Patient Health Questionnaire-9 among Latina and non-Latina White female college students. J. Soc. Social Work Res 3, 80–93. [Google Scholar]
- Grant PM, Beck AT, 2009. Defeatist beliefs as a mediator of cognitive impairment, negative symptoms, and functioning in schizophrenia. Schizophr. Bull 35, 798–806. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Grant PM, Huh GA, Perivoliotis D, Stolar NM, Beck AT, 2012. Randomized trial to evaluate the efficacy of cognitive therapy for low-functioning patients with schizophrenia. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 69, 121–127. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hayes AF, 2013. Introduction to Mediation, Moderation, and Conditional Process Analysis: A Regression-Based Approach. Guilford Press, New York. [Google Scholar]
- Hill K, Startup M, 2013. The relationship between internalized stigma, negative symptoms and social functioning in schizophrenia: The mediating role of self-efficacy. Psychiatry Res. 206, 151–157. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Horan WP, Rassovsky Y, Kern RS, Lee J, Wynn JK, Green MF, 2010. Further support for the role of dysfunctional attitudes in models of real-world functioning in schizophrenia. J. Psychiatr. Res 44, 499–505. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kiwanuka JN, Strauss GP, McMahon RP, Gold JM, 2014. Psychological predictors of functional outcome in people with schizophrenia. Schizophr. Res 157,299–304. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kroenke K, Spitzer RL, Williams JBW, 2001. The PHQ-9: Validity of a brief depression severity measure. J. Gen. Intern. Med 16, 606–613. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kurtz MM, Olfson RH, Rose J, 2013. Self-efficacy and functional status in schizophrenia:Relationship to insight, cognition and negative symptoms. Schizophr. Res 145, 69–74. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Leganger A, Kraft P, Røysamb E, 2000. Perceived self-efficacy in health behaviour research:Conceptualisation, measurement and correlates. Psychol. Health 15, 51–69. [Google Scholar]
- Lehman A, Kernan E, Postrado, 1995. Toolkit evaluating quality of life for persons with severe mental illness Center for Mental Health Services Research, Department of Psychiatry, University of Maryland School of Medicine, Baltimore, Maryland. [Google Scholar]
- Lehman AF, 1996. Quality of Life Interview: Self-Administered Short Form (TL-30S Version). Center for Mental Health Services Research, Department ofPsychiatry, University of Maryland, Baltimore, Maryland. [Google Scholar]
- Lenzenweger MF, Korfine L, 1992. Confirming the latent structure and base rate of schizotypy: A taxometric analysis. J. Abnorm. Psychol 101, 567–571. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lenzenweger MF, 2006. Schizotaxia, schizotypy, and schizophrenia: Paul E. Meehl's blueprint for the experimental psychopathology and genetics of schizophrenia. J. Abnorm. Psychol 115, 195–200. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lincoln TM, Möbius C, Huber MT, Nagel M, Moritz S, 2014. Frequency and correlates of maladaptive responses to paranoid thoughts in patients with psychosis compared to a population sample. Cogn. Neuropsychiatry 19, 509–526. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lindwall M, Barkoukis V, Grano C, Lucidi F, Raudsepp L, Liukkonen J, et al. , 2012. Method effects: The problem with negatively versus positively keyed items. J. Pers. Assess 94, 196–204. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Luszczynska A, Gutiérrez Doña B, Schwarzer R, 2005. General self efficacy in various domains of human functioning: Evidence from five countries. Int. J. Psychol 40, 80–89. [Google Scholar]
- Luszczynska A, Scholz U, Schwarzer R, 2005. The general self-efficacy scale: Multicultural validation studies. J. Psychol 139, 439–457. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Luther L, Fukui S, Firmin RL, McGuire AB, White DA, Minor KS, et al. , 2015. Expectancies of success as a predictor of negative symptoms reduction over 18 months in individuals with schizophrenia. Psychiatry Res. 229, 505–510. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Luther L, Salyers MP, Firmin RL, Marggraf MP, Davis B, Minor KS, 2016. Additional support for the cognitive model of schizophrenia: Evidence of elevated defeatist beliefs in schizotypy. Compr. Psychiatry 68, 40–47. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Meehl PE, 1962. Schizotaxia, schizotypy, schizophrenia. Am. Psychol 17, 827–838. [Google Scholar]
- Meehl PE, 1990. Toward an integrated theory of schizotaxia, schizotypy, and schizophrenia. J. Pers. Disord 4, 1–99. [Google Scholar]
- Minor KS, Luther L, Auster TL, Marggraf MP, Cohen AS, 2015. Category fluency in psychometric schizotypy: how altering emotional valence and cognitive load affects performance. Cogn. Neuropsychiatry 20, 542–550. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Minor KS, Davis BJ, Marggraf MP, Luther L, Robbins ML, 2018. Words matter: Implementing the electronically activated recorder in schizotypy. Personal Disord. 9, 133–143. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nørholm V, Bech P, 2007. Quality of life assessment in schizophrenia: Applicability of the Lehman Quality of Life Questionnaire (TL-30). Nord. J. Psychiatry 61, 438–442. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Perivoliotis D, Morrison AP, Grant PM, French P, Beck AT, 2009. Negative performance beliefs and negative symptoms in individuals at ultra-high risk of psychosis: A preliminary study. Psychopathology. 42, 375–379. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pillny M, Lincoln TM, 2017. Predictors of improved functioning in patients with psychosis: The role of amotivation and defeatist performance beliefs. Psychiatry Res. 244, 117–122. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pratt SI, Mueser KT, Smith TE, Lu W, 2005. Self-efficacy and psychosocial functioning in schizophrenia: A mediational analysis. Schizophr. Res 78, 187–197. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Quilty LC, Oakman JM, Risko E, 2006. Correlates of the Rosenberg Self-Esteem Scale method effects. Struct. Equ. Modeling. 13, 99–117. [Google Scholar]
- Schwarzer R, Jeursalem M, 1995. Generalized Self-Efficacy scale, in Weinman J, Wright S, Johnston M (Eds.), Measures in Health Psychology: A User’s Portfolio. Nfer-Nelson, Windsor, pp. 35–37. [Google Scholar]
- Schwarzer R, Bäßler J, Kwiatek P, Schröder K, Zhang JX, 1997. The assessment of optimistic self beliefs: Comparison of the German, Spanish, and Chinese versions of the general self efficacy scale. Appl. Psychol 46, 69–88. [Google Scholar]
- Schwarzer R, Mueller J, Greenglass E, 1999. Assessment of perceived general self-efficacy on the internet: Data collection in cyberspace. Anxiety Stress Coping. 12, 145–1610. [Google Scholar]
- Spitzer RL, Kroenke K, Williams JBW, the Patient Health Questionnaire Primary Care Study Group, 1999. Validation and utility of a self-report version of PRIME-MD: The PHQ primary care study. JAMA, 282, 1737–1744. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tandon R, Nasrallah HA, Keshavan MS, 2010. Schizophrenia, ―Just the Facts” 5. Treatment and prevention. Past, present, and future. Schizophr. Res 122, 1–23. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Thomas EC, Luther L, Zullo L, Beck AT, Grant PM, 2017. From neurocognition to community participation in serious mental illness: the intermediary role of dysfunctional attitudes and motivation. Psychol. Med 47, 822–836. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ventura J, Subotnik KL, Ered A, Gretchen-Doorly D, Hellemann GS, Vaskinn A, et al. , 2014. The relationship of attitudinal beliefs to negative symptoms, neurocognition, and daily functioning in recent-onset schizophrenia. Schizophr. Bull 40, 1308–1318. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Weems GH, Onwuegbuzie AJ, Schreiber JB, Eggers SJ, 2012. Characteristics of respondents who respond differently to positively and negatively worded items on rating scales. Assess. Eval. High. Educ 28, 587–606. [Google Scholar]
- Weissman A, 1978. The Dysfunctional Attitudes Scale: A validation study (published doctoral dissertation). University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia, Pennsylvania. [Google Scholar]
- Wolfradt U, Oubaid V, Straube ER, Bischoff N, Mischo J, 1999. Thinking styles, schizotypal traits and anomalous experiences. Pers. Individ. Dif 27, 821–830. [Google Scholar]
- Zhang JX, Schwarzer R, 1995. Measuring optimistic self-beliefs: A Chinese adaptation of the General Self-Efficacy Scale. Psychologia, 38, 174–181. [Google Scholar]