Skip to main content
eLife logoLink to eLife
. 2018 Nov 16;7:e39648. doi: 10.7554/eLife.39648

Hedonic processing in humans is mediated by an opioidergic mechanism in a mesocorticolimbic system

Christian Buchel 1,, Stephan Miedl 1,, Christian Sprenger 1
Editors: Geoffrey Schoenbaum2, Catherine Dulac3
PMCID: PMC6239433  PMID: 30444488

Abstract

It has been hypothesized that the pleasure of a reward in humans is mediated by an opioidergic system involving the hypothalamus, nucleus accumbens and the amygdala. Importantly, enjoying the pleasure of a reward is distinct from incentive salience induced by cues predicting the reward. We investigated this issue using a within subject, pharmacological challenge design with the opioid receptor antagonist naloxone and fMRI. Our data show that blocking opioid receptors reduced pleasure associated with viewing erotic pictures more than viewing symbols of reward such as money. This was paralleled by a reduction of activation in the ventral striatum, lateral orbitofrontal cortex, amygdala, hypothalamus and medial prefrontal cortex. Crucially, the naloxone induced activation decrease was observed at reward delivery, but not during reward anticipation, indicating that blocking opioid receptors decreases the pleasure of rewards in humans.

Editorial note: This article has been through an editorial process in which the authors decide how to respond to the issues raised during peer review. The Reviewing Editor's assessment is that all the issues have been addressed (see decision letter).

Research organism: Human

Introduction

Goal directed behaviors such as finding a sexual mate or searching for food are crucial for survival and subserved by a mainly dopaminergic motivational system including the ventral striatum (Morton et al., 2006). However, in the rodent it has been shown that the ensuing pleasure of these goal directed behaviors that is the hedonic aspect of reward consumption is mediated by an additional, opioidergic system (Peciña and Berridge, 2005). This is in line with the observation that opioids play an important role in reproduction, an effect mainly mediated by the hypothalamus and amygdala (Kostarczyk, 1986; Le Merrer et al., 2009).

In humans, behavioral studies employing opioid antagonists suggest a role of endogenous opioids in relationship to positively valenced stimuli such as attractive faces (Chelnokova et al., 2014), food (Yeomans and Gray, 2002) and social aspects (Hsu et al., 2013). However, evidence with respect to the role of opioids in reward processing in humans is contradictory, as some studies show negative effects of opioid blockade on reward processing (Petrovic et al., 2008), whereas others have shown positive effects (Porchet et al., 2013).

Additionally, human neuroimaging studies using positron emission tomography with an opioid tracer have revealed a correlation between social acceptance (Hsu et al., 2013), positive emotions induced by erotic stimuli (Koepp et al., 2009) or food (Nummenmaa et al., 2018) and activity in the ventral striatum and the amygdala. However, PET studies have not been able to dissociate the temporal aspects of reward processing such as the effects during anticipation reflecting incentive salience and the hedonic aspects or pleasure related to a reward. This dissociation is neurobiologically important, as previous animal studies have revealed a temporally distinct response pattern of the mesolimbic system in particular dissociating responses for reward anticipation and outcome (Schultz et al., 1997; Smith et al., 2011). In this context, it has been shown that anticipatory or conditioned stimuli that predict a reward are associated with incentive salience, but only the actual reward delivery (sucrose) was associated with hedonic effects (Peciña and Berridge, 2005; Smith et al., 2011). More importantly, the hedonic effect linked to the outcome phase was exclusively opioid dependent, whereas the effect of incentive salience during anticipation was also dopamine dependent.

In humans, anticipation and outcome related neuronal effects can reliably be dissociated by fMRI (Knutson et al., 2000; Yacubian et al., 2006). Consequently, functional MRI in combination with an opioid antagonist could offer a comprehensive view on the mechanisms of hedonic processing. We therefore performed a within subject, cross-over, pharmacological challenge study with the opioid receptor antagonist naloxone in combination with fMRI and investigated the role of the opioidergic system in processing the pleasure of rewards. To even further dissociate incentive salience from reward related pleasure, we compared erotic stimuli, which directly resemble a pleasurable reward to pictures of monetary outcomes, which are merely visual representations of what a participant can redeem after the experiment (Sescousse et al., 2010).

With respect to the task, we aimed for high motivational involvement of the volunteer, which is ideally met by an incentive delay task (Knutson et al., 2000). Consequently, we extended the well-established monetary incentive delay task (Knutson et al., 2000) with erotic pictures, which have been shown to reliably activate the mesolimbic system (Redouté et al., 2000; Beauregard et al., 2001; Arnow et al., 2002; Hamann et al., 2004; Ponseti et al., 2006; Paul et al., 2008; Sescousse et al., 2010; Sescousse et al., 2013). In this task, volunteers were cued with the expectable reward magnitude at the beginning of each trial, characterizing the possible outcome. Erotic pictures evoking low pleasure showed women in swimsuits, whereas highly pleasurable stimuli depicted total nudity (Figure 1). In monetary trials small and large amounts of money served as rewards. As in classical incentive delay tasks, volunteers had to press a button as soon as a neutral target stimulus appeared on the screen (Figure 1). If their response was registered within a defined response window the trial was considered successful and volunteers were shown the reward (erotic picture or picture showing money). Afterwards, they rated the pleasure of viewing the picture in case of reward trials, or how frustrated they were not to be shown the picture in missed reward trials.

Figure 1. Adapted Monetary Incentive Delay (MID) task trial structure.

An initial cue signaled potential gain for each trial (high/low pleasure erotic picture or high/low monetary reward). After a variable delay, a target briefly appeared. Responding during target display yielded the indicated gain, whereas late or early responses yielded no gain. Target durations were adapted to approximate 67% hit rate for each subject. In case of gain trials volunteers could watch the outcome picture (erotic picture or money) for 1.5 s. In case of loss trials a scrambled image was shown.

Figure 1.

Figure 1—figure supplement 1. Behavioral rating data.

Figure 1—figure supplement 1.

Top row: Pleasure and frustration ratings for received and missed monetary rewards. Bottom row: Pleasure and frustration ratings for received and missed erotic picture rewards.

Twenty-one heterosexual, healthy, male volunteers (mean age 25.5 years) took part in the experiment. In monetary trials the amounts of possible monetary rewards were approximately matched for value with the high and low erotic stimuli using a pre-experiment procedure. The order of monetary and erotic trials was randomized and had the same structure, involving high or low possible gains. Volunteers were investigated on two days with either placebo or naloxone. The treatment order was randomized across volunteers. Each erotic stimulus was only presented once and randomized across the placebo and naloxone days.

Results

Behavioral and autonomic data

Behavioral mood and side effects ratings did not differ between treatments (Supplementary file 1 & 2). Hedonic ratings in the placebo condition indicated that volunteers perceived high erotic stimuli (total nudity) as more pleasurable compared to low erotic (swimsuit) stimuli (mean high erotic >low erotic: T(18)=7.74; p<0.00001). A similar pattern emerged for high and low monetary rewards (Supplementary file 3). Not receiving an erotic picture reward led to a comparable pattern of frustration ratings (Supplementary file 3). However, absolute ratings for monetary rewards were higher as compared to erotic rewards (Figure 1—figure supplement 1).

Interestingly, skin conductance data showed the opposite pattern, with significantly higher values for erotic rewards as compared to monetary rewards (Figure 2). This discrepancy was not unexpected as volunteers’ ratings on sensitive items such as erotic pictures have been shown to be influenced by social desirability (Tourangeau and Yan, 2007). In particular, a high erotic reward induced a significantly stronger SCR response as compared to a high monetary reward (T(13)= 2.87; p=0.007; Figure 2). The same was observed comparing low erotic to low monetary rewards (T(13)= 3.16; p=0.004). In addition, only weak responses were observed for the anticipation phase (i.e. evoked by the cue).

Figure 2. Z-transformed skin conductance responses for anticipation and reward.

Figure 2.

Data is shown for n = 14 volunteers during the placebo treatment only, because naloxone is known to directly affect autonomic regulation of SCR (Traore et al., 1998).

In a next step, we investigated the effect of naloxone on perceived pleasure. A repeated measures ANOVA with factors treatment and condition (Supplementary file 4) revealed a main effect of condition F(3.94, 67.12) = 16.709; p=1.79*10−09, a trend for a main effect of treatment F(1,17)=4.06; p=0.06 and importantly a treatment by condition interaction F(5.22, 88.68)=2.60; p=0.03. An additional between subject effect of treatment order (i.e. Nlx – Placebo or Placebo - Nlx), revealed neither a significant main effect (F(1,17)=2.78; p=0.11), nor an interaction with (i) the main effect of treatment (F(1,17)=1.47;p=0.24), (ii) condition (F(3.95,67.12) = 1.68; p=0.17) or (iii) the treatment by condition interaction (F(5.22, 88.68)=1.64; p=0.16).

In particular, pleasure ratings at the outcome phase of the experiment, that is directly after viewing the erotic picture, in gain trials were reduced during naloxone treatment. This effect was most pronounced for the high pleasure condition (T(18)=3.90; p<0.001; Figure 3; Figure 1—figure supplement 1 and Supplementary file 5). Motivation to attain a goal has been shown to affect the level of frustration when thwarted (Dollard et al., 1939). We therefore also investigated whether naloxone has an effect on frustration ratings, that is when in missed reward trials the erotic picture was not shown, and observed a significant reduction by naloxone (T(18)=2.80; p<0.006). In addition we observed a significantly stronger reduction of frustration ratings for the missed reward trials for erotic as compared to monetary outcomes (T(18)=2.61; p=0.0088; Figure 3 right; Supplementary file 5). A trend for this interaction was also observed for ratings of received rewards (T(18)=1.66; p=0.0572; Figure 3 second from right; Supplementary file 5). The effect of naloxone on the low erotic stimuli, and monetary rewards was weaker and only significant at the uncorrected level for high monetary rewards (T(18)=2.14; p=0.0229; Supplementary file 5). For low rewards no significant interaction between erotic and monetary outcomes was observed.

Figure 3. Behavioral data shows that the opioid antagonist naloxone significantly reduced ratings for high erotic rewards (dark gray bars).

Figure 3.

Frustration ratings for missed erotic rewards show a stronger decrease by naloxone as compared to monetary rewards (right). (* denotes p<0.05; * between bars denote p<0.05 for the interaction).

Functional neuroimaging

Based on previous reports (Redouté et al., 2000; Beauregard et al., 2001; Arnow et al., 2002; Sescousse et al., 2010; Sescousse et al., 2013; Morelli et al., 2015; Noori et al., 2016) on activations related to erotic and monetary rewards, we focused our analysis on the ventral striatum, the orbitofrontal cortex, the amygdala, the hypothalamus and the medial prefrontal cortex (see Materials and methods and Supplementary file 6 for the exact definition of these regions of interest based on multiple individual studies and meta-analyses).

Our analysis revealed a reduction of BOLD responses by naloxone to erotic image presentation in bilateral ventral striatum at the outcome phase (right: T(18)=2.99; p=0.004; p(corrected)=0.031; left: T(18)=3.47; p=0.001; p(corrected)=0.011; Figure 4 and Figure 4—figure supplement 1), lateral OFC (right: T(18)=2.84; p=0.006; p(corrected)=0.044; left: T(18)=1.94; p=0.034; p(corrected)=0.275), bilateral amygdalae (right: T(18)=2.85; p=0.005; p(corrected)=0.043; left: T(18)=2.83; p=0.006; p(corrected)=0.044), medial prefrontal cortex (T(18)=3.52; p=0.001; p(corrected)=0.010) and the hypothalamus (T(18)=1.87; p=0.039; p(corrected)=0.309). See Supplementary file 7 for an overview. For the monetary trials no significant effects of naloxone were observed at the outcome phase (Supplementary file 9 and Figure 4—figure supplement 2). Finally, we investigated whether the opioid antagonist has the same effect on activation during the anticipation phase. This analysis revealed only a weak effect of naloxone in the medial prefrontal cortex (T(18)=1.90; p=0.037; p(corrected)=0.294) and right lateral OFC (T(18)=1.82; p=0.042; p(corrected)=0.339) for monetary trials (Supplementary file 9 and Figure 4—figure supplement 3) but not for erotic trials (Figure 4—figure supplement 4). Directly comparing naloxone effects on erotic rewards with its effects on monetary rewards, we observed a stronger effect of naloxone with respect to erotic rewards in all regions of interest (Figure 4 and Supplementary file 8), with the most significant effect in the amygdala (right: T(18)=3.40; p=0.0016; p(corrected)=0.0128; left: T(18)=3.50; p=0.0013; p(corrected)=0.0103). The effects of all conditions in each ROI for saline and naloxone are shown in Figure 4—figure supplements 512.

Figure 4. Region of interest brain activity for high versus low erotic picture (dark gray) and high versus low monetary reward (light gray) outcomes shows naloxone related reduced activation for erotic trials.

Under naloxone treatment, activation in the ventral striatum, medial prefrontal cortex lateral orbitofrontal cortex, the amygdala and the hypothalamus is significantly reduced. This reduction is larger for erotic rewards as compared to monetary rewards. Activations for high versus low erotic pictures comparing placebo to naloxone at p<0.005 (uncorrected, t-test) are overlaid on a mean structural image also indicating the predefined volumes of interest (light gray) in the ventral striatum, medial prefrontal cortex and lateral orbitofrontal cortex, amygdala and hypothalamus. (** denotes p<0.05 corrected for multiple comparisons; *p<0.05 uncorrected; * or ** between bars denote p values for the interaction).

Figure 4.

Figure 4—figure supplement 1. Region of interest contrasts of brain activity for high versus low erotic picture reward outcome for placebo and naloxone treatment.

Figure 4—figure supplement 1.

**p<0.05 corrected for multiple comparisons; *p<0.05 uncorrected.
Figure 4—figure supplement 2. Region of interest contrasts of brain activity for high versus low monetary reward outcome for placebo and naloxone treatment.

Figure 4—figure supplement 2.

Figure 4—figure supplement 3. Region of interest contrasts of brain activity for high versus low monetary reward anticipation for placebo and naloxone treatment (*p<0.05, uncorrected).

Figure 4—figure supplement 3.

Figure 4—figure supplement 4. Region of interest contrasts of brain activity for high versus low erotic picture reward anticipation for placebo and naloxone treatment.

Figure 4—figure supplement 4.

Figure 4—figure supplement 5. Right ventral striatum (nucleus accumbens) fMRI responses (arbitrary units) for all stimuli (money/erotic, high/low) and time-points (anticipation, outcome: reward, outcome: miss) under placebo (Plac) and naloxone (Nlx).

Figure 4—figure supplement 5.

Error bars represent standard error of the mean.
Figure 4—figure supplement 6. Left ventral striatum (nucleus accumbens) fMRI responses (arbitrary units) for all stimuli (money/erotic, high/low) and time-points (anticipation, outcome: reward, outcome: miss) under placebo (Plac) and naloxone (Nlx).

Figure 4—figure supplement 6.

Error bars represent standard error of the mean.
Figure 4—figure supplement 7. Right lateral orbitofrontal cortex fMRI responses (arbitrary units) for all stimuli (money/erotic, high/low) and time-points (anticipation, outcome: reward, outcome: miss) under placebo (Plac) and naloxone (Nlx).

Figure 4—figure supplement 7.

Error bars represent standard error of the mean.
Figure 4—figure supplement 8. Left lateral orbitofrontal cortex fMRI responses (arbitrary units) for all stimuli (money/erotic, high/low) and time-points (anticipation, outcome: reward, outcome: miss) under placebo (Plac) and naloxone (Nlx).

Figure 4—figure supplement 8.

Error bars represent standard error of the mean.
Figure 4—figure supplement 9. Right amygdala fMRI responses (arbitrary units) for all stimuli (money/erotic, high/low) and time-points (anticipation, outcome: reward, outcome: miss) under placebo (Plac) and naloxone (Nlx).

Figure 4—figure supplement 9.

Error bars represent standard error of the mean.
Figure 4—figure supplement 10. Left amygdala fMRI responses (arbitrary units) for all stimuli (money/erotic, high/low) and time-points (anticipation, outcome: reward, outcome: miss) under placebo (Plac) and naloxone (Nlx).

Figure 4—figure supplement 10.

Error bars represent standard error of the mean.
Figure 4—figure supplement 11. Medial prefrontal cortex fMRI responses (arbitrary units) for all stimuli (money/erotic, high/low) and time-points (anticipation, outcome: reward, outcome: miss) under placebo (Plac) and naloxone (Nlx).

Figure 4—figure supplement 11.

Error bars represent standard error of the mean.
Figure 4—figure supplement 12. Hypothalamus fMRI responses (arbitrary units) for all stimuli (money/erotic, high/low) and time-points (anticipation, outcome: reward, outcome: miss) under placebo (Plac) and naloxone (Nlx).

Figure 4—figure supplement 12.

Error bars represent standard error of the mean.

In addition we investigated whether the individual decrease of pleasure ratings due to naloxone were related to the reduction of brain activation. Using a linear regression analysis (Supplementary file 10), we observed a significant correlation in the hypothalamus (T(17)=3.24; p=0.0024; p(corrected)=0.0194; Figure 5; Supplementary file 10) and a weaker correlation in the left ventral striatum (T(17)=2.09; p=0.0262; p(corrected)=0.2097; Figure 5; Supplementary file 10). This indicates that the BOLD signal difference in the hypothalamus due to naloxone is linearly related to the individual decrease in pleasure ratings.

Figure 5. Positive correlation between naloxone induced decreases in fMRI signal in the hypothalamus and decreases in ratings for erotic rewards.

Figure 5.

The more naloxone reduced fMRI signal comparing high versus low erotic rewards during outcome, the more the rating difference between high and low erotic rewards was reduced (See also Table S10). Activations showing this correlation at p<0.005 (uncorrected, t-test) are overlaid on a mean structural image also indicating the predefined volumes of interest (light gray) in the hypothalamus and amygdala.

Discussion

Observing that naloxone can reduce pleasure ratings in a cross-over pharmacological challenge design causally implicates the opioidergic system in hedonic processing of erotic stimuli. Furthermore, functional neuroimaging revealed that an opioid receptor antagonist led to a reduction of neuronal activation in the ventral striatum, amygdala, orbitofrontal and medial prefrontal cortex. In addition, the decrease of activation in the hypothalamus due to naloxone was correlated to individual reductions in pleasure ratings, implicating this structure in opioid mediated hedonic processing. Our findings show that a similar system mediates pleasure as it does in rodents (Peciña and Berridge, 2005; Ismail et al., 2009; Le Merrer et al., 2009; Smith et al., 2011). Not only did we observe a strong effect of opioid antagonists on pleasurable erotic stimuli, but this effect was specifically related to the outcome phase.

Elegant previous molecular imaging (PET) studies have indicated a relationship between social rejection (Hsu et al., 2013), food stimuli (Nummenmaa et al., 2018) or erotic stimulus material (Koepp et al., 2009) and the opioid system. However, by the nature of the imaging modality, these studies were not in a position to answer the crucial question of whether reward anticipation or outcome is responsible for the observed effect. The rather high temporal resolution of our pharmacological fMRI approach allowed us to dissociate the anticipation from the outcome phase and thus to attribute hedonic reward processing to an opioidergic system.

In a sequential conditioning task in rodents in which a first conditioned stimulus (CS1) was followed by a second (CS2) which was then followed by a sucrose reward, it could be demonstrated that hedonic effects were only observed for the outcome phase, but neither for CS1 nor CS2 (Smith et al., 2011). This study has also shown that incentive salience attribution was higher for cues more proximal to the actual reward (i.e. CS2) and although responses to the CS increased after opioid stimulation of the Nacc, they remained much smaller as compared to sucrose reward. Furthermore, the effect of incentive salience at CS2 was dopamine and opioid dependent, whereas the hedonic effect linked to the outcome phase was only opioid dependent. Therefore, one might argue that the effect of opioid blockade at the outcome stage observed in our study could be related to incentive salience (i.e. comparable to conditioned stimulus, CS2) rather than to the hedonic outcome. However, if the erotic reward in our study only represents a CS (e.g. predicting copulatory action) rather than a pleasurable reward per se, this contingency (and any possible incentive salience) would quickly extinguish, because this CS is never reinforced. In addition, attributing incentive salience rather than hedonic outcomes to our erotic rewards is unlikely for other reasons: (i) It has been argued (Sescousse et al., 2010) that an important property of erotic pictures is that they directly resemble a reward, which is in contrast to pictures of monetary outcomes that are merely visual representations of what the participant can redeem after the experiment. (ii) Smith and colleagues (Smith et al., 2011) observed weak to non-existing facial responses indicative of hedonic processing for CS1 and CS2, but strong responses for the sucrose reward. This closely resembles the pattern of our skin conductance data (Figure 2), where very weak responses were observed during the anticipation phase (which could be considered a CS), but very strong responses to the outcome phase (Figure 2).

With respect to opposite sex human stimuli, a design involving attractive faces offered first evidence that pharmacological manipulation of the human opioid system can affect motivation for viewing opposite-sex faces (Chelnokova et al., 2014). In contrast to these findings, a study involving affective pictures, which included erotic material, could not reveal an effect of naloxone on pleasure ratings (Kut et al., 2011). However, the latter study only involved passive viewing of affective pictures, which is in contrast to our study, where volunteers had to successfully perform a task to view erotic pictures. This suggests that the role of opioids in mediating pleasure is modulated by motivational aspects, analogous to observations in the dopaminergic system (Coricelli et al., 2005).

Previous studies observed a correlation of hypothalamic activity with pleasure ratings and penile tumescence in the context of visual erotic stimuli (Redouté et al., 2000; Arnow et al., 2002; Paul et al., 2008; Georgiadis et al., 2012) and direct sexual activity (Georgiadis et al., 2010). Importantly, our data suggests that this effect is mediated by opioids, because the individual reduction of pleasure ratings for erotic stimuli by naloxone was correlated with the reduction of brain activation in the hypothalamus.

The hypothalamus is an important structure for behavioral, autonomic and endocrine responses in relation to reproductive behavior (Le Merrer et al., 2009). Opioid receptor agonists injected directly into the hypothalamus inhibited or delayed sexual behavior (van Furth et al., 1995; Le Merrer et al., 2009). In contrast, opioid antagonists facilitate male sexual behavior. In addition, rodent studies have revealed that opioid receptor antagonists (Agmo and Gómez, 1993) but not dopamine antagonists (Ismail et al., 2009) in the hypothalamus can block conditioned place preference (CPP) linked to sexual behavior, indicating a distributed system of hypothalamic and limbic regions for mediating the effects of sexual rewards (Le Merrer et al., 2009). Given that sexual behavior in rodents is increased by opioid antagonists, whereas we observed decreased pleasure in viewing of erotic pictures under naloxone emphasizes that additional processes contribute to sexual behavior as compared to viewing erotic pictures. The pivotal role of the hypothalamus is further underlined by the observation that infusion of an opioid antagonist into the nucleus accumbens did not reduce the reinforcing properties of ejaculation. This strong observation led the authors to conclude that the hypothalamus is the site where sexual reward is produced (Agmo and Gómez, 1993). This resonates with our finding that the hypothalamus was the region where we observed the strongest individual relationship between reduction of pleasure ratings by naloxone and the ensuing reduction of the BOLD signal.

We investigated fMRI activation differences between high and low magnitude trials. The alternative, that is comparing high (or low) magnitude trials to a resting baseline includes unspecific effects such as those evoked by gross differences in visual stimulation. Therefore, showing that an opioid antagonist can block differential responses for high versus low erotic stimuli directly implies opioids in mediating these effects. Furthermore, our behavioral data is in line with previous studies, showing that the effect of an opioid antagonist was strongest for the most valuable stimuli (Chelnokova et al., 2014).

Interestingly, we observed higher average ratings for monetary rewards as compared to erotic rewards, which seems to contradict our attempt to equate the value of monetary and erotic stimuli in a pre-experiment. However, previous studies have also observed a dissociation of ratings and the amount of work volunteers are willing to perform to increase viewing time of attractive faces (Aharon et al., 2001). Nevertheless, our calibration procedure was sufficient to equate the range of ratings for monetary and erotic stimuli to be captured by the same visual analogue scale and thus avoid ceiling or floor effects. Importantly, the focus of our study was on how ratings change under naloxone treatment within a stimulus category and our results clearly indicate that although absolute ratings were higher for monetary rewards, the relative difference due to opioid blockade is significantly larger for erotic rewards. Furthermore, our skin conductance data from the placebo session clearly indicates that SCR responses are significantly larger for erotic rewards as compared to monetary rewards. These findings together with the observation that fMRI responses in all regions of interest were stronger for erotic stimuli indicate that lower ratings for erotic stimuli as compared to monetary stimuli might at least in part be related to social desirability (Tourangeau and Yan, 2007). However, we cannot rule out that there is a genuine difference between ratings and autonomic and neural responses with respect to erotic and monetary stimuli.

When comparing high to low magnitude monetary rewards, for the placebo treatment we only observed weak effects. This is not surprising as previous studies have established that brain areas including the ventral striatum and vmPFC adapt their dynamic range to the overall value range of the stimuli employed in an experiment (Benedek and Kaernbach, 2010; Bostwick and Bucci, 2008; Boucsein et al., 2012; Calhoun et al., 2017). Based on these observations it is to be expected that highly rewarding and pleasurable outcomes such as erotic pictures can adaptively down-regulate the responses to less arousing monetary rewards. This notion is also supported by the skin conductance data indicating that (i) SCR responses are significantly larger for erotic rewards as compared to monetary rewards and (ii) responses to erotic rewards are much larger compared to activation during anticipation (Figure 2).

Although our approach revealed an effect of opioids on processing of pleasurable rewards this does not rule out the role of other modulatory neurotransmitter systems such as the endocannabinoid system which is known to have effects on emotional processing (Laviolette and Grace, 2006). A further limitation of our study is the considerably small sample size. This is unfortunately caused by the great effort of a cross-over pharmacological challenge study using fMRI. However, using a longitudinal design we could minimize between subjects variance.

Our data also sheds light on the emerging clinical application of opioid antagonists in treating internet sex and pornography addiction (Bostwick and Bucci, 2008; Raymond et al., 2010; Kraus et al., 2015) as well as in treating adolescent sexual offenders (Ryback, 2004). Numerous case reports have documented a very high effectiveness of the opioid antagonist Naltrexone for treating severely affected patients. Importantly, in the course of treatment patients have described a diminished sense of ‘overwhelming pleasure’ (Kraus et al., 2015) which is in line with our data showing a major effect of opioid blockade on the hedonic aspects of reward processing. In the context of a classical conditioning framework the pleasure of reward can be considered as part of the unconditioned stimulus (UCS) whereas cues that predict this represent conditioned stimuli (CS). Consequently, by reducing reward pleasure the predictors of reward will also be devalued which in turn can lead to an overall therapeutic success including a reported reduction of craving (Kraus et al., 2015).

Materials and methods

Participants

Currently, no study has investigated the effect of naloxone on pleasure ratings related to erotic stimuli or monetary rewards. For our power calculation, we were therefore guided by the effect sizes reported in a previous study on the effect of naloxone on affective ratings in a monetary gambling task (Petrovic et al., 2008). By careful visual inspection of Figure 1 in Petrovic et al., 2008, we estimated the mean effect of naloxone on ratings (μ-μ0) to −3.53, with a standard deviation (σ) of 6.31. Based on the following equation and setting power (1-β) to 80% and type I error rate (α) to 5% we estimated the sample size (n)

n=(σZ1-α+Z1-βμ-μ0)2

(http://powerandsamplesize.com) to be 19.75.

Consequently, 21 heterosexual male volunteers (Mean ± SD age, 25.48 ± 4.55 years) with no history of neurological or psychiatric disorders participated in this study. Sexual arousability was measured with the Sexual Arousability Inventory (SAI) (Hoon and Chambless, 1998) (mean score SAI: 88.45 ± 11.91), ensuring that subjects showed a normal sexual arousability.

Data for two volunteers could not be used due to technical problems (scanner artefacts), leaving a final sample size of nineteen. The study was conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki. All subjects gave written informed consent to be part of the study, which was approved by the ethics committee of the Chamber of Physicians, Hamburg, Germany (PV3906). The informed consent also included the consent to publish the data.

Task

Our task extended the classical monetary incentive delay task (MID) (Knutson et al., 2000) by a condition in which erotic pictures were employed instead of money. As in the classical MID task, trials started with a cue phase. The cue indicated the nature and magnitude of the possible reward in this trial: A heart indicated a possible erotic stimulus, whereas a Euro symbol (€) indicated a possible monetary reward. Magnitude of either type of stimulus was indicated by either a single (low) or three (high) horizontal lines. In erotic trials low magnitude was defined as showing female models in swimsuits, whereas high magnitude related to showing completely nude pictures. 110 erotic pictures were selected from the Internet according to two objective criteria: women had to be alone and their face was not displayed. After a variable delay between 1.5 and 4.5 s the target (white square) appeared and volunteers had to indicate this with a button press as quickly as possible. If the response occurred within a response window, the trial was considered successful and volunteers were shown the picture. In a pretest we estimated a time windows so that 67% of all responses were valid. In case of an erotic picture trial they were able to view the picture for 1.5 s, in case of a monetary trial they were shown their monetary gain for the same time period. In case of a non-successful trial a scrambled picture was shown. Immediately afterwards, they rated the pleasure of the outcome (i.e. either viewing the erotic picture or the picture of the monetary reward in gain trials, or how frustrated they felt in missed reward trials). Rating was performed by moving a cursor on a continuous visual analog scale ranging from 0 to 100.

We approximately equated pleasure ratings for the two modalities to be able to use the same visual analogue scale in both conditions (i.e. to guarantee that the scale covers the range of pleasure ratings for both modalities and to avoid any ceiling or floor effects). In a pre-experiment session we estimated how much work (clicks) volunteers are willing to perform (Aharon et al., 2001) for fixed monetary values (0.32, 0.39, 0.49, 0.61, 0.76, 0.94, 1.17, 1.46, 1.81, 2.26, 2.81, 3.49, 4.35, 5.41, 6.73, 8.37, 10.41, 12.95, 16.11, 20.04) and erotic pictures (high and low). The logarithmic grading of these fixed values was chosen to be more sensitive in the low value range. In this pretest we used a modified incentive delay task in which volunteers had to press a button as many times as they want in a 5 s period. They were told that the more often they press within a 5 s period, the more likely it will be to gain money or be able to view the erotic picture. We estimated the maximum number of button presses (in a 5 s interval) for each individual before this test by asking them to press a button as often as possible in a 5 s period. In analogy to a Becker-deGroot-Marschak auction (Becker et al., 1964) for each trial we generated a random number in the interval between 0 and 150(%) of their maximum button press rate. If that number was lower than the amount of button presses for this trial a gain occurred. Consequently, volunteers would gain in 66% of trials if they performed at their maximum response rate in each trial.

We then fitted an exponential function to the monetary data. Finally, we took the intersection of the amount of work (clicks) volunteers were willing to spend to see the high and low erotic pictures and the fitted exponential function to identify the monetary equivalent of watching a high or a low erotic picture. In case this procedure could not reveal meaningful amounts, default values of 0.4€ for low and 1.8€ for the high monetary amount were selected. The actual amounts used ranged from 0.1 to 5.6€ (0.85 ± 1.09€; mean ± sd) for the low amount and from 0.8 to 8.1€ (mean ± sd 2.71 ± 1.32€) for the high amount.

Drug administration

Volunteers were investigated in a cross-over design on two days (~48 hr apart) with either the application of naloxone or placebo (order randomized across volunteers). At ~15 min before the start of the experiment, we administered a bolus dose of 0.15 mg/kg naloxone (Naloxon-ratiopharm, Ratiopharm, Ulm, Germany) or the same volume of saline via an intravenous line inserted into the antecubital vein of the left arm. Because naloxone has a relatively short half-life (~70 min in blood plasma; Summary of Product Characteristics, Ratiopharm) and its clinically effective duration of action can be even shorter (Gutstein and Akil, 2006), we additionally administered an intravenous infusion dose of 0.2 mg/kg/h naloxone for the duration of the experiment (diluted in saline) or the same volume of saline, starting shortly after bolus administration. This dosing regime leads to a stable concentration of naloxone in blood plasma over the length of the experiment (Eippert et al., 2009; Schoell et al., 2010) and is sufficient to block central opioid receptors almost completely (Mayberg and Frost, 1990).

Subjects were informed about naloxone, including its pharmacological properties, its general clinical use, and its possible side effects. Subjects were also informed that they would most likely not notice that they had received naloxone, as it generally does not have noticeable effects on mood at this dose (Grevert and Goldstein, 1978; Petrovic et al., 2008; Kut et al., 2011). After each experiment the experimenter (S.M.) recorded mood and possible side effects using a 5-point Likert scale (not at all - very) with 12 items for mood and a 7-point Likert scale (not present - extreme) with seven items for side effects. The mood rating scale included the following items: satisfied, rested, restless, bad, worn out, calm, tired, good, uneasy, cheerful, unwell and relaxed. The seven item side effect scale included the following items: dry mouth, dry skin, blurred vision, lethargy, sickness, dizziness, and headache. Naturally, we could not inform subjects about the true purpose of naloxone administration in this study, which was done during debriefing. The experimenter (S.M.) who interacted with the subjects was blinded as to which drug was given. Blinding and assignment of treatment order by a random number was performed by another experimenter (C.S.). Unblinding occurred after the experiment. In the final sample ten volunteers received saline on day 1 and naloxone on day 2, nine volunteers received naloxone on day 1 and saline on day 2.

Behavioral data

Pleasure and frustration ratings for each trial type (monetary low, monetary high, erotic low, erotic high) for naloxone and placebo were compared using a repeated measures ANOVA with factors trial type and treatment. Given the strong a priori hypothesis that naloxone would decrease pleasure ratings (Petrovic et al., 2008), we additionally performed planned individual paired one-sided t-tests. Results were corrected for multiple comparisons using a Bonferroni correction.

Autonomic data

Electrodermal activity was measured during fMRI with MRI-compatible electrodes on the palm of the left hand (thenar and hypothenar sites) connected to carbon leads (Biopac, Lead108). The signal was amplified using an analog amplifier (Biopac, MP150) and sampled at 1000 Hz using CED 1401 analog-digital converter (Cambridge Electronic Design). After temporal smoothing using a Gaussian convolution kernel with a full-width-at-half-maximum of 0.4 s and subsequent downsampling to 10 Hz, we computed the phasic skin conductance drive (SCR) using a deconvolution technique (Benedek and Kaernbach, 2010) as implemented in Ledalab 3.4.8 and used these to assess the autonomic arousal associated with individual stimuli in a time window from 1 to 4 s after stimulus onset (Boucsein et al., 2012). To account for differences in electrode position, skin moisture and other between subject effects (Boucsein et al., 2012), SCR estimates were z-transformed within volunteers and session, and then averaged across sessions. We only analyzed SCR data from the placebo treatment condition, as it has been shown that naloxone suppresses the descending bulbar inhibitory mechanisms on SCR responses (Traore et al., 1998) and thus alters SCR responses. Due to artefacts (i.e. MR gradient switching artefacts, cable movement), skin conductance data was only available from 14 volunteers for the placebo day.

Imaging

Scanning was performed with a 3T whole-body magnetic resonance imager (TIM Trio, Siemens, Erlangen, Germany). fMRI data acquisition was divided into four sessions. In each session we acquired between 311 and 332 volumes (depending on response timings) per session with 36 slices in descending order (2 mm slice thickness with 1 mm gap) using a gradient-echo T2*-weighted pulse sequence (EPI). The time to repetition (TR) for volume acquisition was set to 2160 ms and the time to echo (TE) to 25 ms. In-plane resolution was 108 × 108 with a field of view of 216 × 216 mm. For anatomical reference, a 3D magnetization prepared gradient-echo sequence of the whole brain was obtained with TR of 6.8 ms and a TE of 3.2 ms.

Image preprocessing and analyses were performed with SPM12 software (Wellcome Trust Centre for Neuroimaging, London). For structural preprocessing, we used DARTEL to spatially normalize individually segmented T1-weighted scans to a template (Template_X_IXI555_MNI152.nii; http://brain-development.org/) as provided by the CAT 12 toolbox (http://dbm.neuro.uni-jena.de/cat12). Functional images were realigned and resliced in a two pass approach (initially to the first volume, then to the mean of all volumes). In addition, a mean functional image was created for each volunteer and used to derive a deformation field for spatial normalization into MNI space using the unified segmentation approach. A direct estimation of the deformation field from the functional images has been shown to outperform a combined coregistration approach in some cases (Calhoun et al., 2017). Single-subject statistical models analyzed the resliced data for anticipation (i.e. when the cue was shown) and outcome (i.e. when the outcome was presented) for low and high monetary and erotic trials. Each condition was defined separately for successful (gain) and unsuccessful (no gain) trials. Trials in which subjects failed to respond were modeled as error trials. Rigid body movement parameters from the realignment procedure were included as six additional nuisance covariates. Next, contrast images of the parameter estimates were created for each subject. Single-subject contrast images were created by applying the deformations as estimated from the unified segmentation normalization of the mean functional images to the contrast images, which were subsequently resampled with a resolution of 1.5 × 1.5 × 1.5 mm3 and smoothed with a Gaussian kernel of 8 mm full-width-at-half-maximum (FWHM). Normalized and smoothed single-subject contrast images were then entered into a second-level random effects analysis (paired t-test contrasting the naloxone with the placebo treatment condition) reflecting a stimulus (high minus low erotic stimuli or high minus low monetary reward) by treatment (naloxone versus placebo) interaction analysis. Furthermore, we tested for an interaction comparing the naloxone versus placebo contrast image using a paired t-test. Finally, we performed a regression analysis, in which we added a covariate to the second level statistical model, coding the rating difference between the naloxone and the placebo scan.

Functional imaging analyses were based on regions of interest (ROIs) based on averaged coordinates from previous individual studies (Redouté et al., 2000; Beauregard et al., 2001; Arnow et al., 2002; Sescousse et al., 2010; Sescousse et al., 2013) and meta-analyses (Kühn and Gallinat, 2012; Morelli et al., 2015; Noori et al., 2016) on monetary and erotic rewards. According to these studies spheres of 6 mm radius around peak coordinates were located in ventral striatum (left: −10 10–5; right: 10 7–6 mm), lateral OFC (left: −36 25–3; right: 34 19–5 mm) and amygdala (left: −19–4 −20; right: 21–1 −18 mm). In addition we employed 10 mm radius spherical ROIs centered on the medial prefrontal cortex (1 40 0 mm) and the hypothalamus (0–6 −8 mm) (Supplementary file 7).

Acknowledgements

We would like to thank the radiographer team at the Department for Systems Neuroscience for help with scanning, Brian Knutson and Stefanie Brassen for comments on an earlier version of this manuscript and Christian Gaser for his CAT12 toolbox. CB is supported by the DFG, SFB T-CRC 134 project C08 and SFB 936 project A06. CS was supported by the ERC, ERC-2010-AdG_20100407.

Funding Statement

The funders had no role in study design, data collection and interpretation, or the decision to submit the work for publication.

Contributor Information

Christian Buchel, Email: buechel@uke.de.

Geoffrey Schoenbaum, National Institute on Drug Abuse, National Institutes of Health, United States.

Catherine Dulac, Harvard University, United States.

Funding Information

This paper was supported by the following grants:

  • Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft SFB 936 project A6 to Christian Buchel, Christian Sprenger.

  • H2020 European Research Council ERC-2010-AdG_20100407 to Christian Buchel, Christian Sprenger.

  • Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft SFB TR 134 Project C08 to Christian Buchel.

Additional information

Competing interests

Reviewing editor, eLife.

No competing interests declared.

Author contributions

Conceptualization, Supervision, Funding acquisition, Investigation, Visualization, Writing—original draft, Project administration, Writing—review and editing.

Data curation, Investigation, Methodology, Writing—review and editing.

Supervision, Investigation, Methodology, Writing—original draft, Writing—review and editing.

Ethics

Human subjects: The study was conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki. All subjects gave written informed consent to be part of the study, which was approved by the ethics committee of the Chamber of Physicians, Hamburg, Germany (PV3906). The informed consent also included the consent to publish the data.

Additional files

Supplementary file 1. Side effects ratings.

Two sided paired Wilcoxon signed rank test comparing mood between naloxone (_nlx) and saline (_nacl) sessions.

elife-39648-supp1.docx (29.4KB, docx)
DOI: 10.7554/eLife.39648.020
Supplementary file 2. Mood ratings.

Two sided paired Wilcoxon signed rank test comparing mood between naloxone (_nlx) and saline (_nacl) sessions.

elife-39648-supp2.docx (30.3KB, docx)
DOI: 10.7554/eLife.39648.021
Supplementary file 3. Rating differences for successful and missed reward trials in the placebo treatment condition.
elife-39648-supp3.docx (28.6KB, docx)
DOI: 10.7554/eLife.39648.022
Supplementary file 4. Effects of Naloxone on pleasure ratings.

Repeated measures ANOVA, Huynh-Feldt nonsphericity correction.

elife-39648-supp4.docx (30.5KB, docx)
DOI: 10.7554/eLife.39648.023
Supplementary file 5. Naloxone effects (relative to placebo) on pleasure ratings.
elife-39648-supp5.docx (29.8KB, docx)
DOI: 10.7554/eLife.39648.024
Supplementary file 6. Coordinates from individual studies to define the center of ROIs.
elife-39648-supp6.docx (51.5KB, docx)
DOI: 10.7554/eLife.39648.025
Supplementary file 7. Naloxone effects (relative to placebo) on the high versus low erotic image outcome contrast.
elife-39648-supp7.docx (29.8KB, docx)
DOI: 10.7554/eLife.39648.026
Supplementary file 8. Naloxone effects (relative to placebo) comparing erotic versus monetary outcomes.
elife-39648-supp8.docx (29.7KB, docx)
DOI: 10.7554/eLife.39648.027
Supplementary file 9. Naloxone effects (relative to placebo) on other contrasts.
elife-39648-supp9.docx (32.3KB, docx)
DOI: 10.7554/eLife.39648.028
Supplementary file 10. Regression analysis relating the change in pleasure ratings to the change in fMRI signal.
elife-39648-supp10.docx (29.5KB, docx)
DOI: 10.7554/eLife.39648.029
Transparent reporting form
DOI: 10.7554/eLife.39648.031

Data availability

Only freely available data analysis tools were used (SPM, Ledalab). Data are available online via Dryad (http://dx.doi.org/10.5061/dryad.11j304c)."

The following dataset was generated:

Christian Buchel, Stephan Miedl, Christian Sprenger. 2018. Data from: Hedonic processing in humans is mediated by an opioidergic mechanism in a mesocorticolimbic system. Dryad Digital Repository.

References

  1. Agmo A, Gómez M. Sexual reinforcement is blocked by infusion of naloxone into the medial preoptic area. Behavioral Neuroscience. 1993;107:812–818. doi: 10.1037/0735-7044.107.5.812. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Aharon I, Etcoff N, Ariely D, Chabris CF, O'Connor E, Breiter HC. Beautiful faces have variable reward value: fMRI and behavioral evidence. Neuron. 2001;32:537–551. doi: 10.1016/S0896-6273(01)00491-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Arnow BA, Desmond JE, Banner LL, Glover GH, Solomon A, Polan ML, Lue TF, Atlas SW. Brain activation and sexual arousal in healthy, heterosexual males. Brain. 2002;125:1014–1023. doi: 10.1093/brain/awf108. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Beauregard M, Lévesque J, Bourgouin P. Neural correlates of conscious self-regulation of emotion. The Journal of Neuroscience. 2001;21:RC165. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.21-18-j0001.2001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Becker GM, DeGroot MH, Marschak J. Measuring utility by a single-response sequential method. Behavioral Science. 1964;9:226–232. doi: 10.1002/bs.3830090304. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Benedek M, Kaernbach C. Decomposition of skin conductance data by means of nonnegative deconvolution. Psychophysiology. 2010;47:647–658. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2009.00972.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Bostwick JM, Bucci JA. Internet sex addiction treated with naltrexone. Mayo Clinic Proceedings. 2008;83:226–230. doi: 10.1016/S0025-6196(11)60846-X. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Boucsein W, Fowles DC, Grimnes S, Ben-Shakhar G, roth WT, Dawson ME, Filion DL. Society for psychophysiological research ad hoc committee on electrodermal measures (2012) Publication recommendations for electrodermal measurements. Psychophysiology. 2012;49:1017–1034. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2012.01384.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Calhoun VD, Wager TD, Krishnan A, Rosch KS, Seymour KE, Nebel MB, Mostofsky SH, Nyalakanai P, Kiehl K. The impact of T1 versus EPI spatial normalization templates for fMRI data analyses. Human Brain Mapping. 2017;38:5331–5342. doi: 10.1002/hbm.23737. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Chelnokova O, Laeng B, Eikemo M, Riegels J, Løseth G, Maurud H, Willoch F, Leknes S. Rewards of beauty: the opioid system mediates social motivation in humans. Molecular Psychiatry. 2014;19:746–747. doi: 10.1038/mp.2014.1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Coricelli G, Critchley HD, Joffily M, O'Doherty JP, Sirigu A, Dolan RJ. Regret and its avoidance: a neuroimaging study of choice behavior. Nature Neuroscience. 2005;8:1255–1262. doi: 10.1038/nn1514. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Dollard J, Miller ME, Doob LW, Mowrer OH, Sears RR. Frustration and aggression. [March 6, 2017];1939 http://doi.apa.org/psycinfo/2004-16227-000
  13. Eippert F, Bingel U, Schoell ED, Yacubian J, Klinger R, Lorenz J, Büchel C. Activation of the opioidergic descending pain control system underlies placebo analgesia. Neuron. 2009;63:533–543. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2009.07.014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Georgiadis JR, Farrell MJ, Boessen R, Denton DA, Gavrilescu M, Kortekaas R, Renken RJ, Hoogduin JM, Egan GF. Dynamic subcortical blood flow during male sexual activity with ecological validity: a perfusion fMRI study. NeuroImage. 2010;50:208–216. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.12.034. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Georgiadis JR, Kringelbach ML, Pfaus JG. Sex for fun: a synthesis of human and animal neurobiology. Nature Reviews Urology. 2012;9:486–498. doi: 10.1038/nrurol.2012.151. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Grevert P, Goldstein A. Endorphins: naloxone fails to alter experimental pain or mood in humans. Science. 1978;199:1093–1095. doi: 10.1126/science.343250. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Gutstein HB, Akil H. In: Goodman and Gilman’s the Pharmacological Basis of Therapeutics (Brunton LL, Lazo JS, Parker KL, Eds. New York, NY: McGraw-Hill; 2006. Opioid analgesics; pp. 547–590. [Google Scholar]
  18. Hamann S, Herman RA, Nolan CL, Wallen K. Men and women differ in amygdala response to visual sexual stimuli. Nature Neuroscience. 2004;7:411–416. doi: 10.1038/nn1208. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Hoon EF, Chambless D. In: Handbook of Sexuality-Related Measures. Thousand Oaks: Sage; 1998. Sexual arousability inventory and sexual arousability inventory—expanded; pp. 71–77. [Google Scholar]
  20. Hsu DT, Sanford BJ, Meyers KK, Love TM, Hazlett KE, Wang H, Ni L, Walker SJ, Mickey BJ, Korycinski ST, Koeppe RA, Crocker JK, Langenecker SA, Zubieta JK. Response of the μ-opioid system to social rejection and acceptance. Molecular Psychiatry. 2013;18:1211–1217. doi: 10.1038/mp.2013.96. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Ismail N, Girard-Bériault F, Nakanishi S, Pfaus JG. Naloxone, but not flupenthixol, disrupts the development of conditioned ejaculatory preference in the male rat. Behavioral Neuroscience. 2009;123:992–999. doi: 10.1037/a0017096. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Knutson B, Westdorp A, Kaiser E, Hommer D. FMRI visualization of brain activity during a monetary incentive delay task. NeuroImage. 2000;12:20–27. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2000.0593. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Koepp MJ, Hammers A, Lawrence AD, Asselin MC, Grasby PM, Bench CJ. Evidence for endogenous opioid release in the amygdala during positive emotion. NeuroImage. 2009;44:252–256. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.08.032. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Kostarczyk EM. The amygdala and male reproductive functions: I. Anatomical and endocrine bases. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews. 1986;10:67–77. doi: 10.1016/0149-7634(86)90019-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Kraus SW, Meshberg-Cohen S, Martino S, Quinones LJ, Potenza MN. Treatment of Compulsive Pornography Use With Naltrexone: A Case Report. American Journal of Psychiatry. 2015;172:1260–1261. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2015.15060843. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Kühn S, Gallinat J. The neural correlates of subjective pleasantness. NeuroImage. 2012;61:289–294. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.02.065. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Kut E, Candia V, von Overbeck J, Pok J, Fink D, Folkers G. Pleasure-related analgesia activates opioid-insensitive circuits. Journal of Neuroscience. 2011;31:4148–4153. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3736-10.2011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Laviolette SR, Grace AA. Cannabinoids potentiate emotional learning plasticity in neurons of the medial prefrontal cortex through basolateral amygdala inputs. Journal of Neuroscience. 2006;26:6458–6468. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0707-06.2006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Le Merrer J, Becker JA, Befort K, Kieffer BL. Reward processing by the opioid system in the brain. Physiological Reviews. 2009;89:1379–1412. doi: 10.1152/physrev.00005.2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Mayberg HS, Frost JJ. Opiate Receptors. New York, NY: Lippincott Williams and Wilkins; 1990. [Google Scholar]
  31. Morelli SA, Sacchet MD, Zaki J. Common and distinct neural correlates of personal and vicarious reward: A quantitative meta-analysis. NeuroImage. 2015;112:244–253. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.12.056. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Morton GJ, Cummings DE, Baskin DG, Barsh GS, Schwartz MW. Central nervous system control of food intake and body weight. Nature. 2006;443:289–295. doi: 10.1038/nature05026. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Noori HR, Cosa Linan A, Spanagel R. Largely overlapping neuronal substrates of reactivity to drug, gambling, food and sexual cues: A comprehensive meta-analysis. European Neuropsychopharmacology. 2016;26:1419–1430. doi: 10.1016/j.euroneuro.2016.06.013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Nummenmaa L, Saanijoki T, Tuominen L, Hirvonen J, Tuulari JJ, Nuutila P, Kalliokoski K. μ-opioid receptor system mediates reward processing in humans. Nature Communications. 2018;9:1500. doi: 10.1038/s41467-018-03848-y. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Paul T, Schiffer B, Zwarg T, Krüger TH, Karama S, Schedlowski M, Forsting M, Gizewski ER. Brain response to visual sexual stimuli in heterosexual and homosexual males. Human Brain Mapping. 2008;29:726–735. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20435. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Peciña S, Berridge KC. Hedonic hot spot in nucleus accumbens shell: where do mu-opioids cause increased hedonic impact of sweetness? Journal of Neuroscience. 2005;25:11777–11786. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2329-05.2005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Petrovic P, Pleger B, Seymour B, Klöppel S, De Martino B, Critchley H, Dolan RJ. Blocking central opiate function modulates hedonic impact and anterior cingulate response to rewards and losses. Journal of Neuroscience. 2008;28:10509–10516. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2807-08.2008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Ponseti J, Bosinski HA, Wolff S, Peller M, Jansen O, Mehdorn HM, Büchel C, Siebner HR. A functional endophenotype for sexual orientation in humans. NeuroImage. 2006;33:825–833. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.08.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Porchet RI, Boekhoudt L, Studer B, Gandamaneni PK, Rani N, Binnamangala S, Müller U, Clark L. Opioidergic and dopaminergic manipulation of gambling tendencies: a preliminary study in male recreational gamblers. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience. 2013;7:138. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2013.00138. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Raymond NC, Grant JE, Coleman E. Augmentation with naltrexone to treat compulsive sexual behavior: a case series. Annals of Clinical Psychiatry : Official Journal of the American Academy of Clinical Psychiatrists. 2010;22:56–62. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Redouté J, Stoléru S, Grégoire MC, Costes N, Cinotti L, Lavenne F, Le Bars D, Forest MG, Pujol JF. Brain processing of visual sexual stimuli in human males. Human Brain Mapping. 2000;11:162–177. doi: 10.1002/1097-0193(200011)11:3&#x0003c;162::AID-HBM30&#x0003e;3.0.CO;2-A. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Ryback RS. Naltrexone in the treatment of adolescent sexual offenders. The Journal of Clinical Psychiatry. 2004;65:982–986. doi: 10.4088/JCP.v65n0715. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Schoell ED, Bingel U, Eippert F, Yacubian J, Christiansen K, Andresen H, May A, Buechel C. The effect of opioid receptor blockade on the neural processing of thermal stimuli. PLOS ONE. 2010;5:e12344. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0012344. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Schultz W, Dayan P, Montague PR. A neural substrate of prediction and reward. Science. 1997;275:1593–1599. doi: 10.1126/science.275.5306.1593. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Sescousse G, Redouté J, Dreher JC. The architecture of reward value coding in the human orbitofrontal cortex. Journal of Neuroscience. 2010;30:13095–13104. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3501-10.2010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Sescousse G, Barbalat G, Domenech P, Dreher JC. Imbalance in the sensitivity to different types of rewards in pathological gambling. Brain. 2013;136:2527–2538. doi: 10.1093/brain/awt126. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Smith KS, Berridge KC, Aldridge JW. Disentangling pleasure from incentive salience and learning signals in brain reward circuitry. PNAS. 2011;108:E255–E264. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1101920108. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Tourangeau R, Yan T. Sensitive questions in surveys. Psychological Bulletin. 2007;133:859–883. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.133.5.859. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Traore M, Maitte M, Delerm B, Roy JC, Sequeira H. Opioid peptide involvement in the bulbar inhibition of electrodermal activity in the cat. Journal of Physiology-Paris. 1998;92:17–23. doi: 10.1016/S0928-4257(98)80018-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. van Furth WR, van Emst MG, van Ree JM. Opioids and sexual behavior of male rats: involvement of the medial preoptic area. Behavioral Neuroscience. 1995;109:123–134. doi: 10.1037/0735-7044.109.1.123. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Yacubian J, Gläscher J, Schroeder K, Sommer T, Braus DF, Büchel C. Dissociable systems for gain- and loss-related value predictions and errors of prediction in the human brain. Journal of Neuroscience. 2006;26:9530–9537. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2915-06.2006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Yeomans MR, Gray RW. Opioid peptides and the control of human ingestive behaviour. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews. 2002;26:713–728. doi: 10.1016/S0149-7634(02)00041-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Decision letter

Editor: Geoffrey Schoenbaum1
Reviewed by: Boris Quednow2

In the interests of transparency, eLife includes the editorial decision letter, peer reviews, and accompanying author responses.

[Editorial note: This article has been through an editorial process in which the authors decide how to respond to the issues raised during peer review. The Reviewing Editor's assessment is that all the issues have been addressed.]

Thank you for submitting your article "Hedonic processing in humans is mediated by an opioidergic mechanism in a mesocorticolimbic system" for consideration by eLife. Your article has been reviewed by three peer reviewers, including Geoffrey Schoenbaum as the Reviewing Editor, and the evaluation has been overseen by a Senior Editor. The reviewers have opted to remain anonymous.

The Reviewing Editor has highlighted the concerns that require revision and/or responses, and we have included the separate reviews below for your consideration. If you have any questions, please do not hesitate to contact us.

Summary:

In this study, the authors test the hypothesis that hedonic responses to rewards are opioid mediated in a circuit including the hypothalamus, accumbent, and amygdala in humans. Human subjects were trained in an "incentive delay task" to associate pictures with high and low value monetary and erotic outcomes. After learning the task, they performed after challenge with naloxone or placebo while BOLD signal, SCR and subjective ratings were acquired. The authors report that naloxone blunted responses, especially to high value erotic outcomes during the reward phase of the task. This was associated with BOLD signal changes in the hypothesized regions.

Major concerns:

In the discussion, there were a number of concerns that were generally about the selective manner in which the results – particularly behavioral – were presented and analyzed. While the SCRs are presented in a relatively clear manner, they are analyzed by t-test. Reward ratings were presented and analyzed pairwise. And there was no distinction between outcome and anticipatory phase data. This then carried on into the manner in which the BOLD was analyzed. One might argue this was targeted, but the targets were not a priori and the justification was not clear. This selective focus made it hard to evaluate the task (reviewer 1, point 1) and caused some questions about whether other contrasts might have been more appropriate or shown different results (reviewer 1, point 1 and reviewer 3, point 2 and 6). It was felt that a fuller analysis and consideration of these questions was necessary. Relatedly, reviewer 2 felt that softening the strong distinction the authors are making between phases that distinguish incentive versus hedonic responses. This would perhaps go along with a fuller presentation and analysis of anticipatory vs outcome related behavior. Finally reviewer 3 had a variety of concerns regarding limitations that should be added to the manuscript – the impact of naloxone on mood, which presumably might underlie what they see and the low N for example should be mentioned.

Separate reviews (please respond to each point):

Reviewer #1:

In this study, the authors test in humans the hypothesis that pleasure during outcome presentation is selectively mediated by opioid mechanisms. Subjects performed a simple delay task in which they responded to cues that predicted the delivery of high versus low value erotic versus monetary rewards. After obtaining baseline measures, the same task was done following challenge with naloxone or placebo. They report that naloxone significantly affected ratings and SCR for the erotic but not the monetary outcomes, and this effect was associated with reduced activation in ventral striatum, orbitofrontal cortex, amygdala, hypothalamus and medial prefrontal cortex. The result is generally consistent with the idea that opioids mediate the pleasure of receiving reward distinct from anticipation and incentive salience and provides evidence of this in humans along with the potential circuit mediating the effects. Overall the experiments seem well designed and cleanly conducted and the results important.

However, while I found it interesting and potentially it seems like a novel and important study, I was a bit frustrated with how the results – particularly the behavioral results – were presented. Possibly my concerns are quite minor and may just involve a fuller presentation and analysis of the results. But if I follow the design correctly, it seems to me that the analysis should be a simple multifactor ANOVA comparing effects of placebo and drug on the different measures (SCR, ratings) over the different rewards – money and pictures. Yet instead the presentation of these data focuses in on specific comparisons and reports t-tests. I don't think these were a priori comparisons really, so this is not really appropriate. I think the authors need to present the results from each type of trial and analyze for the effects of the different factors. This could be extended to showing SCR measures during the anticipatory versus outcome periods as well. This level of detail would make the results much easier to appreciate and more meaningful I think. As it is, I feel like I do not have a full picture of the behavior.

The second issue I have is with the explanation for why the ratings and results were so different for the two types of rewards. The authors seem to minimize this (e.g. social desireability – what is this – embarrassment?), but I wonder whether this difference is important and potentially reflects the fact that different systems are mediating the response to one (erotic, primarily emotional and directly experienced) versus more cognitive and abstract to the other (money, the actual outcomes must be thought about). The authors allude to this at one point, but then do not say much about it. If this is a real difference, it should be addressed more, especially as many fMRI studies use money as the primary reward.

Additional data files and statistical comments:

My major concern is partly statistics – as outlined above.

Reviewer #2:

This manuscript by Buchel, Miedl, and Sprenger presents an interesting and compelling demonstration that hedonic reactions to pleasant outcomes are opioid mediated in people.

The experiments seem well designed, the data are convincing, and the entire article is clear and scholarly. The authors deserve congratulations on a fine study. I have no major criticisms.

Minor point:

While I agree with the authors' primary conclusion that they demonstrate an opioid mediation of outcome hedonic impact (the naloxone effects are especially compelling), and the authors also are perfectly right to divide anticipation vs. outcome temporal phases into psychological incentive salience vs. hedonic impact predominant categories. However, it should also be noted that the temporal anticipatory/consummatory divide is only a rough guide for motivation/hedonic categories, and temporal moments dos not always map on perfectly to the psychological categories.

That is, even if anticipatory phase is primarily 'wanting', there can be some cue-elicited hedonic impact in the anticipatory phase (albeit low level) as a conditioned hedonic reaction as the authors note. There can also be an augmentation of incentive salience in the outcome phase, as in drug-priming of craving, or the 'you can't eat just one chip' or cocktail peanut priming effect of a food morsel. It is plausible that sexual incentive salience could also be heightened during outcome phase of sexual images. In my view, this consideration might justify slightly softening the authors assignment of psychological components to anticipatory/consummatory phases, but does not detract from their main conclusion.

Similarly, regarding the authors' description in Discussion: "In a sequential conditioning task in rodents in which a first conditioned stimulus (CS1) was followed by a second (CS2) which was then followed by a sucrose reward, it could be demonstrated that hedonic effects were only observed for the outcome phase, but neither for CS1 nor CS2 (Smith et al., 2011)..….Furthermore, the effect of incentive salience at CS2 was dopamine and opioid dependent, whereas the hedonic effect linked to the outcome phase was only opioid dependent."

I think in the Smith et al., 2011 study, there actually was also an opioid enhancement of conditioned anticipatory hedonic reactions to CS1 and CS2 (as well as a larger hedonic enhancement to sucrose outcome). The conditioned hedonic reactions were very low to begin with, as the authors note, but became slightly larger after NAc opioid stimulation. As the authors say, the UCS outcome hedonic enhancement was much greater, in keeping with their conclusions. Possibly the description could be slightly adjusted.

Reviewer #3:

Buchel et al. investigated the impacts of the opioid receptor antagonist naloxone on reward processing in healthy human volunteers using an adapted incentive delay task with sexual and monetary stimuli. Naloxone treatment was associated with a stronger reduction of subjectively reported pleasure while viewing erotic pictures compared to viewing symbols of monetary reward. Moreover, naloxone decreased the activation of reward-related areas primarily during "reward delivery".

The manuscript is well-written and the research question is of high interest. The imaging methods are sound. However, the behavioral pharmacology part has some major flaws. Below you will find the several major concerns that have to be addressed.

1) The authors did not assess mood/affective state ratings, which is a major shortcoming of the study. The reviewer disagrees with the statement that "[naloxone] generally does not have noticeable effects on mood (Grevert and Goldstein, 1978; Petrovic et al., 2008; Kut et al., 2011)". There are a number of other studies clearly showing that naloxone infusions can induce aversive states such as dysphoria (Cohen et al., 1983, Arch Gen Psychiatry; Martin del Campo et al., 1994, Psychopharmacology; 2000, J Affect Disorder). Thus, the overall effect of mood on pleasure ratings remains unclear here. This has to be discussed as a strong limitation.

2) The reviewer is concerned regarding the choice of the sexual stimuli and the contrast build on the related picture categories (low vs. high erotic reward). Most likely, the supposed "low erotic reward" (women in swimsuits) category is already inducing strong activations of the reward system. This interpretation is supported by the fact that women in swimsuits ("low erotic reward") induced much stronger SCR responses than high monetary rewards but that there was no difference compared to high erotic reward pictures. Interestingly a recent study has shown that explicit erotic pictures (comparable with "high erotic reward" stimuli used here) did not induce higher sexual arousal ratings in comparison to implicit erotic pictures (comparable with "low erotic reward" stimuli) (Schmid et al., 2015, Eur Neuropsychopharmacol). Thus, the question arises as to whether the chosen contrast is ideal for investigating the rewarding nature of sexual stimuli. More neutral pictures might have been a better choice. This has to be discussed as a major limitation.

3) In light of the replication crisis in human neuroscience, the sample size is considered small (n=19) – a further limitation that has to be discussed. Related to that point, the authors report corrected and uncorrected p-values for the behavioral analyses but tested only one-tailed hypotheses. A more conservative approach would be appreciated, e.g., reporting and discussing only corrected results.

4) Did the authors control for potential effects of order? Specifically, the very short test-retest interval (48h) suggests that order effects should be investigated, as naloxone may impact up- or down-regulation of opioid receptors far beyond 48h (Sirohi et al., 2007, J Pharmacol Exp Ther; Jenab and Inturrisi 1994, Mol Brain Res; Belcheva et al., 1992; Mol Pharmacol). Thus, the administration of naloxone on the first test day may have influenced results from the placebo condition on the second test day. The very short interval is another major weakness of the study.

5) Interpretation and nature of effects: While the behavioral data suggest that naloxone reduces both reward and frustration ratings (in line with hedonic blunting), the fMRI analysis exclusively focuses on reward. How does naloxone affect the neural processing of no-reward outcomes (and how do these trial types enter the GLM)? By extension, are the effects appropriately interpreted as decreases in the pleasure of rewards (e.g. Abstract) or do they also decrease the disappointment induced by not obtaining reward?

Additionally, the third paragraph of the Discussion seems to argue that the erotic stimuli but not the conditioned stimuli of the present task constitute rewards associated with incentive salience. By contrast, the fourth and sixth paragraph seems to argue that the conditioned stimuli carried motivational significance, which can be understood as a synonym of incentive salience, and yet that viewing of these stimuli was different from reward consumption. The potential for ensuing confusion should be minimized in the Discussion.

6) Data display: Figure 4 and the related supplementary figures show contrasts, of e.g., high versus low outcomes. To fully appreciate the effects of naloxone, the contrast estimates should also be split up, so that high, low and no outcomes are shown separately for placebo and naloxone in further supplementary figures. This is also relevant for reinforcing the discussion point that adaptive coding explains the relatively small differences between high and low rewards. In the presence of full adaptive coding, the difference of high vs. no reward should be similar to the difference of low vs. no reward and both differences should be significantly larger than zero. Is this the case?

7) Data reporting: For future reference and meta-analyses, tables with whole-brain findings at p<0.001 uncorrected would be helpful. Moreover, the authors state that they do not report SCR data for the naloxone condition as naloxone has been shown to reduce the inhibition of electrodermal activity by the bulbar reticular formation. Was the SCR elevated here? Could relative differences in SCR not be interpreted over and above a mean increase?

8) Materials and methods: The last paragraph of the subsection “Task”, describes the estimated monetary equivalence ranges of erotic pictures. However, as far as I could see the amounts actually used (and those won in total) in the task are not specified (also not in the first paragraph where one may expect them).

9) The second paragraph of the subsection “Task”, mentions maximum button press rate without specifying how it was determined.

10) A 10 mm radius seems rather large for a hypothalamus ROI. Can the authors independently confirm that the identified cluster indeed lies within the hypothalamus?

eLife. 2018 Nov 16;7:e39648. doi: 10.7554/eLife.39648.035

Author response


Major concerns:

In the discussion, there were a number of concerns that were generally about the selective manner in which the results – particularly behavioral – were presented and analyzed. While the SCRs are presented in a relatively clear manner, they are analyzed by t-test. Reward ratings were presented and analyzed pairwise. And there was no distinction between outcome and anticipatory phase data. This then carried on into the manner in which the BOLD was analyzed. One might argue this was targeted, but the targets were not a priori and the justification was not clear. This selective focus made it hard to evaluate the task (reviewer 1, point 1) and caused some questions about whether other contrasts might have been more appropriate or shown different results (reviewer 1, point 1 and reviewer 3, point 2 and 6). It was felt that a fuller analysis and consideration of these questions was necessary. Relatedly, reviewer 2 felt that softening the strong distinction the authors are making between phases that distinguish incentive versus hedonic responses. This would perhaps go along with a fuller presentation and analysis of anticipatory vs outcome related behavior. Finally reviewer 3 had a variety of concerns regarding limitations that should be added to the manuscript – the impact of naloxone on mood, which presumably might underlie what they see and the low N for example should be mentioned.

We have addressed all these points in the paper and in addition provide detailed explanations of our changes when addressing individual points of all reviewers (see below).

In addition during the proof reading stage we found a little rounding error in the paper. We reported the effect in the left amygdala to be T(18)=2.83; p=0.006; p(corrected)=0.041. This was a rounding error, the corrected p-value before rounding is 0.0441 which must be rounded to 0.044.

Separate reviews (please respond to each point):

Reviewer #1:

In this study, the authors test in humans the hypothesis that pleasure during outcome presentation is selectively mediated by opioid mechanisms. Subjects performed a simple delay task in which they responded to cues that predicted the delivery of high versus low value erotic versus monetary rewards. After obtaining baseline measures, the same task was done following challenge with naloxone or placebo. They report that naloxone significantly affected ratings and SCR for the erotic but not the monetary outcomes, and this effect was associated with reduced activation in ventral striatum, orbitofrontal cortex, amygdala, hypothalamus and medial prefrontal cortex. The result is generally consistent with the idea that opioids mediate the pleasure of receiving reward distinct from anticipation and incentive salience and provides evidence of this in humans along with the potential circuit mediating the effects. Overall the experiments seem well designed and cleanly conducted and the results important.

We thank the reviewer for the positive assessment of our study as important.

However, while I found it interesting and potentially it seems like a novel and important study, I was a bit frustrated with how the results – particularly the behavioral results – were presented. Possibly my concerns are quite minor and may just involve a fuller presentation and analysis of the results. But if I follow the design correctly, it seems to me that the analysis should be a simple multifactor ANOVA comparing effects of placebo and drug on the different measures (SCR, ratings) over the different rewards – money and pictures. Yet instead the presentation of these data focuses in on specific comparisons and reports t-tests. I don't think these were a priori comparisons really, so this is not really appropriate. I think the authors need to present the results from each type of trial and analyze for the effects of the different factors.

We now provide the results of a repeated measures ANOVA with factors treatment and condition. This analysis revealed a significant main effect of condition, a trend for the main effect of treatment and importantly, a treatment by condition interaction. We have also included the order of treatment as a between-subject factor to test for residual order effects (see comment by reviewer #3). Neither the main effect of treatment order, nor the interaction with the other effects was significant.

However, we would like to stress that our study was motivated and designed shortly after a paper appeared which had tested the hypothesis of decreased pleasure and fMRI activation in the brain after application of naloxone (Petrovic et al., 2008). Indeed they observed that naloxone led to an attenuation of pleasure ratings for larger reward outcomes. Yet they did not observe any difference in the ventral striatum, as one could have expected based on rodent literature. We reckoned that the null result in the ventral striatum might be related to the little hedonic value of monetary rewards and were inspired by work of the Dreher lab (Sescousse et al., 2010) to combine an MID (Knutson et al., 2000) with erotic pictures to elicit more hedonic drive.

Accordingly, we hypothesized that naloxone would decrease pleasure ratings. Consequently we used paired t-tests (one-sided according to our hypothesis) for the different conditions and corrected for multiple comparisons.

This could be extended to showing SCR measures during the anticipatory versus outcome periods as well.

The SCR measures in Figure 2 show anticipation and outcome. However, as mentioned in the paper, we refrain from analyzing the SCR data with respect to the treatment effect, as it has been shown that SCR is directly affected by naloxone (Traore et al., 1998). This is supported by our raw SCR data which differed substantially between the naloxone and the placebo sessions (raw SCR values in arbitrary units of the analog-digital-converter were 502.2 ± 40.2 for placebo and 824.8 ± 107.3 for naloxone, averaged across all conditions).

This level of detail would make the results much easier to appreciate and more meaningful I think. As it is, I feel like I do not have a full picture of the behavior.

The second issue I have is with the explanation for why the ratings and results were so different for the two types of rewards. The authors seem to minimize this (e.g. social desireability – what is this – embarrassment?), but I wonder whether this difference is important and potentially reflects the fact that different systems are mediating the response to one (erotic, primarily emotional and directly experienced) versus more cognitive and abstract to the other (money, the actual outcomes must be thought about). The authors allude to this at one point, but then do not say much about it. If this is a real difference, it should be addressed more, especially as many fMRI studies use money as the primary reward.

This is an interesting point. Absolute ratings show higher pleasure for money than erotic pictures, but only a significant decrease of pleasure by naloxone for erotic pictures. SCR for the placebo condition clearly show higher responses for erotic than for monetary outcomes. fMRI responses in all areas of interest also show higher responses for erotic outcomes. Based on the fact that two measures (SCR and fMRI) agree and the observation that behavioral ratings can be subject to social desirability (or embarrassment), we have argued that the absolute ratings between monetary and erotic rewards are difficult to compare, whereas social desirability would not affect the difference in ratings between naloxone and placebo treatments. However, we agree with this reviewer that we cannot rule out that different systems are mediating the response to one (erotic, primarily emotional and directly experienced) versus more cognitive and abstract to the other (money, the actual outcomes must be thought about). We have added this point to the Discussion.

Additional data files and statistical comments:

My major concern is partly statistics – as outlined above.

Reviewer #2:

This manuscript by Buchel, Miedl, and Sprenger presents an interesting and compelling demonstration that hedonic reactions to pleasant outcomes are opioid mediated in people.

The experiments seem well designed, the data are convincing, and the entire article is clear and scholarly. The authors deserve congratulations on a fine study. I have no major criticisms.

Thank you.

Minor point:

While I agree with the authors' primary conclusion that they demonstrate an opioid mediation of outcome hedonic impact (the naloxone effects are especially compelling), and the authors also are perfectly right to divide anticipation vs. outcome temporal phases into psychological incentive salience vs. hedonic impact predominant categories. However, it should also be noted that the temporal anticipatory/consummatory divide is only a rough guide for motivation/hedonic categories, and temporal moments dos not always map on perfectly to the psychological categories.

That is, even if anticipatory phase is primarily 'wanting', there can be some cue-elicited hedonic impact in the anticipatory phase (albeit low level) as a conditioned hedonic reaction as the authors note. There can also be an augmentation of incentive salience in the outcome phase, as in drug-priming of craving, or the 'you can't eat just one chip' or cocktail peanut priming effect of a food morsel. It is plausible that sexual incentive salience could also be heightened during outcome phase of sexual images. In my view, this consideration might justify slightly softening the authors assignment of psychological components to anticipatory/consummatory phases, but does not detract from their main conclusion.

We agree with this reviewer and have now slightly softened this point.

Similarly, regarding the authors' description in Discussion: "In a sequential conditioning task in rodents in which a first conditioned stimulus (CS1) was followed by a second (CS2) which was then followed by a sucrose reward, it could be demonstrated that hedonic effects were only observed for the outcome phase, but neither for CS1 nor CS2 (Smith et al., 2011)..….Furthermore, the effect of incentive salience at CS2 was dopamine and opioid dependent, whereas the hedonic effect linked to the outcome phase was only opioid dependent."

I think in the Smith et al., 2011 study, there actually was also an opioid enhancement of conditioned anticipatory hedonic reactions to CS1 and CS2 (as well as a larger hedonic enhancement to sucrose outcome). The conditioned hedonic reactions were very low to begin with, as the authors note, but became slightly larger after NAc opioid stimulation. As the authors say, the UCS outcome hedonic enhancement was much greater, in keeping with their conclusions. Possibly the description could be slightly adjusted.

We have now adjusted this statement.

Reviewer #3:

Buchel et al. investigated the impacts of the opioid receptor antagonist naloxone on reward processing in healthy human volunteers using an adapted incentive delay task with sexual and monetary stimuli. Naloxone treatment was associated with a stronger reduction of subjectively reported pleasure while viewing erotic pictures compared to viewing symbols of monetary reward. Moreover, naloxone decreased the activation of reward-related areas primarily during "reward delivery".

The manuscript is well-written and the research question is of high interest. The imaging methods are sound. However, the behavioral pharmacology part has some major flaws. Below you will find the several major concerns that have to be addressed.

We thank this reviewer for considering our research question of high interest. We regret that this reviewer considers our behavioral pharmacology as having major flaws. However, we hope that the points below change this view.

1) The authors did not assess mood/affective state ratings, which is a major shortcoming of the study.

We have to apologize for not including our mood and side effects ratings in the previous version of the paper. Mood and possible side effects were coarsely quantified using a 5-point Likert scale (not at all – very) with 12 items for mood and a 7-point Likert scale (not present – extreme) with 7 items for side effects. The mood rating scale included the following items: satisfied, rested, restless, bad, worn out, calm, tired, good, uneasy, cheerful, unwell, relaxed. The 7 item side effect scale included the following items: dry mouth, dry skin, blurred vision, lethargy, sickness, dizziness, headache. No significant differences between the naloxone and placebo session were observed. We therefore conclude that naloxone has no major side effects on mood at the dose used in our study.

The reviewer disagrees with the statement that "[naloxone] generally does not have noticeable effects on mood (Grevert and Goldstein, 1978; Petrovic et al., 2008; Kut et al., 2011)". There are a number of other studies clearly showing that naloxone infusions can induce aversive states such as dysphoria (Cohen et al., 1983, Arch Gen Psychiatry; Martin del Campo et al., 1994, Psychopharmacology; 2000, J Affect Disorder). Thus, the overall effect of mood on pleasure ratings remains unclear here. This has to be discussed as a strong limitation.

Apart from our mood and side effects ratings, which showed no difference between naloxone and placebo, we like to point out that we used an initial dose of 0.15mg/kg followed by a continuous infusion for a steady state (0.2mg/kg/h). Yet, the studies cited by this reviewer used higher bolus doses, e.g. the first study mentioned (Cohen et al., 1983) used doses between 0.3 and 4(!)mg/kg. The second study (Martin del Campo et al., 1994) used 0.2 mg/kg and 1 mg/kg naloxone. Even for the lower dose (which was higher than our bolus) they observed “no effect on overall mood (POMS),…” only effects in the afternoon after drug administration. The last study (Martín del Campo et al., 2000) again used a higher dose (0.2mg/kg) than we applied and half of the 14 participants investigated were depressed patients. Consequently, these studies are in good agreement with our observation that with a naloxone bolus of 0.15mg/kg no significant side effects and no effects on mood occur.

2) The reviewer is concerned regarding the choice of the sexual stimuli and the contrast build on the related picture categories (low vs. high erotic reward). Most likely, the supposed "low erotic reward" (women in swimsuits) category is already inducing strong activations of the reward system.

As can be seen in new Figure 4—figure supplements 5-12 (as requested by this reviewer), this is not the case. In all regions of interest, we observed higher activation for high as compared to low erotic reward. This is also supported by the pleasure ratings.

This interpretation is supported by the fact that women in swimsuits ("low erotic reward") induced much stronger SCR responses than high monetary rewards but that there was no difference compared to high erotic reward pictures.

We agree with this reviewer that skin conductance responses to erotic stimuli were significantly stronger as compared to monetary rewards, whereas SCR differences between high and low erotic stimuli were small. This could indeed indicate a ceiling effect in SCR responses for these stimuli. However, our other measures (behavioral VAS ratings and fMRI activation) actually show a consistent difference between low and high erotic pictures.

Interestingly a recent study has shown that explicit erotic pictures (comparable with "high erotic reward" stimuli used here) did not induce higher sexual arousal ratings in comparison to implicit erotic pictures (comparable with "low erotic reward" stimuli) (Schmid et al., 2015, Eur Neuropsychopharmacol). Thus, the question arises as to whether the chosen contrast is ideal for investigating the rewarding nature of sexual stimuli. More neutral pictures might have been a better choice. This has to be discussed as a major limitation.

We like to point out a few details of the study mentioned here (Schmid et al., 2015). The major difference is that a mixed sample of 15 male and 15 female volunteers was studied. The study employed an active condition in which volunteers could prolong the viewing time of stimuli by button presses. Surprisingly, the results show that volunteers prolonged viewing time of neutral stimuli much more than any erotic stimuli (both p<0.001). This could be indicative of a strong social desirability effect, where volunteers find it embarrassing to “work” for watching erotic stimuli. Nevertheless, this study also reported that “methylphenidate but not MDMA increased ratings of sexual excitation by visual stimuli with explicit sexual content …”. This modulation could not have been observed if only implicit stimuli were used. This is similar to our study, where the pharmacological effect was most pronounced for the high erotic condition. As can be seen in our new Figure 4—figure supplements 5-12 the effect of naloxone is evident for high erotic rewards, but not for low erotic rewards (and not for monetary stimuli). Only by including high erotic stimuli, we were able to reveal this important role of the opiate system, which we would have clearly missed had we only employed low erotic stimuli. In addition, we consider it important to use a parametric approach (i.e. comparing high to low) as opposed to comparing high erotic stimuli to baseline, as this is less specific and as this reviewer points out, can also be confounded by unspecific effects.

3) In light of the replication crisis in human neuroscience, the sample size is considered small (n=19) – a further limitation that has to be discussed. Related to that point, the authors report corrected and uncorrected p-values for the behavioral analyses but tested only one-tailed hypotheses. A more conservative approach would be appreciated, e.g., reporting and discussing only corrected results.

We agree with this reviewer and now mention this in the Discussion. However, we like to mention that previous studies have highlighted the advantage of longitudinal designs which is based on the fact that “each patient [is studied] as their own control” (e.g. (Martín del Campo et al., 2000) cited by this reviewer above).

4) Did the authors control for potential effects of order?

Yes potential order effects were reduced by randomizing the order of naloxone and placebo treatment.

Specifically, the very short test-retest interval (48h) suggests that order effects should be investigated, as naloxone may impact up- or down-regulation of opioid receptors far beyond 48h (Sirohi et al., 2007, J Pharmacol Exp Ther; Jenab and Inturrisi 1994, Mol Brain Res; Belcheva et al., 1992; Mol Pharmacol). Thus, the administration of naloxone on the first test day may have influenced results from the placebo condition on the second test day. The very short interval is another major weakness of the study.

We added a between subject factor “order” to the repeated measures ANOVA for the behavioral data (see above). This analysis did not reveal a significant main effect of order or an interaction with condition, treatment or a treatment by condition interaction. This suggests that randomization and the time between treatments was sufficient to avoid systematic carry over effects.

5) Interpretation and nature of effects: While the behavioral data suggest that naloxone reduces both reward and frustration ratings (in line with hedonic blunting), the fMRI analysis exclusively focuses on reward. How does naloxone affect the neural processing of no-reward outcomes (and how do these trial types enter the GLM)? By extension, are the effects appropriately interpreted as decreases in the pleasure of rewards (e.g. Abstract) or do they also decrease the disappointment induced by not obtaining reward?

This is a very important point. We now provide additional figures showing the effects of all conditions in each ROI for saline and naloxone (Figure 4—figure supplements 5-12). From these figures, it is clearly visible that the effect of naloxone in all regions is restricted to the reward (i.e. gain trials). In the miss trials most areas show no modulation by naloxone.

Additionally, the third paragraph of the Discussion seems to argue that the erotic stimuli but not the conditioned stimuli of the present task constitute rewards associated with incentive salience. By contrast, the fourth and sixth paragraph seems to argue that the conditioned stimuli carried motivational significance, which can be understood as a synonym of incentive salience, and yet that viewing of these stimuli was different from reward consumption. The potential for ensuing confusion should be minimized in the Discussion.

We thank this reviewer for pointing out this confusing paragraph. Rodent studies have converged on the observation that direct sexual behavior is decreased by opioids and increased by opioid antagonists. We have now put this directly in perspective with our findings and suggest that although the pleasure of viewing erotic pictures can be reduced by an opioid antagonist, this is distinct from direct sexual behavior which explicitly includes additional processes (e.g. motor output).

6) Data display: Figure 4 and the related supplementary figures show contrasts, of e.g., high versus low outcomes. To fully appreciate the effects of naloxone, the contrast estimates should also be split up, so that high, low and no outcomes are shown separately for placebo and naloxone in further supplementary figures. This is also relevant for reinforcing the discussion point that adaptive coding explains the relatively small differences between high and low rewards. In the presence of full adaptive coding, the difference of high vs. no reward should be similar to the difference of low vs. no reward and both differences should be significantly larger than zero. Is this the case?

We now provide the requested figures (Figure 4—figure supplements 5-12; one figure for each region of interest with 12 subplots for the different conditions and each subplot containing the data for the saline and for the naloxone run.

In particular, the figures for bilateral amygdala (Figure 4-figure supplement 9 and 10; row 2 column 3) and ventral striatum (Figure 4—figure supplement 5 and 6; row 2 column 3) highlight what we refer to as “adaptive”: The strongest responses is seen in the high erotic outcome condition for placebo treatment, and responses to other outcomes such as high monetary rewards (and low erotic and monetary rewards are blunted). We therefore argue that the system shifts its dynamic range to be able to represent the most rewarding stimuli (reward outcome high erotic), at the expense of other stimuli.

7) Data reporting: For future reference and meta-analyses, tables with whole-brain findings at p<0.001 uncorrected would be helpful.

We agree that meta-analyses can be of value. However, meta-analyses based on peak coordinates are prone to bias (e.g. coordinates in SPM are reported only with 8mm distance and effects with a p value slightly bigger than 0.001 might contribute significantly to an effect). We therefore, would rather encourage colleagues to exploit the contrast estimate images (which will be publicly available) for potential meta-analyses.

Moreover, the authors state that they do not report SCR data for the naloxone condition as naloxone has been shown to reduce the inhibition of electrodermal activity by the bulbar reticular formation. Was the SCR elevated here?

The raw SCR values (in arbitrary units of the analog-digital-converter and averaged across all conditions) were lower for placebo (502.2 ± 40.2) compared to naloxone (824.8 ± 107.3).

Could relative differences in SCR not be interpreted over and above a mean increase?

Absolute SCR values heavily depend on skin condition, environmental temperature and other factors. Therefore, the most sensitive way to analyze SCR within an experiment is to normalize (Z transform) the data. Unfortunately, in a longitudinal experiment as performed here, where SCR was measured on 2 days under naloxone and saline Z transforming each experiment separately potentially removes differences between treatments. Transforming the data across both treatments is problematic due to differences in electrode position, skin temperature etc. Furthermore, as mentioned in the manuscript SCR was only available for 14 volunteers.

8) Materials and methods: The last paragraph of the subsection “Task”, describes the estimated monetary equivalence ranges of erotic pictures. However, as far as I could see the amounts actually used (and those won in total) in the task are not specified (also not in the first paragraph where one may expect them).

The actual amounts used ranged from 0.1 – 5.6€ (0.85 ± 1.09€; mean ± sd) for the low amount and from 0.8 to 8.1€ (mean ± sd 2.71 ± 1.32€) for the high amount. This has been added to the manuscript. The valid response window for the MID was adjusted individually, so total gains cannot be meaningfully compared across volunteers or treatments.

9) The second paragraph of the subsection “Task”, mentions maximum button press rate without specifying how it was determined.

We estimated the maximum number of button presses for each individual before the equivalence experiment, by asking them to press a button as often as possible in a 5s period. This has been added to the manuscript.

10) A 10 mm radius seems rather large for a hypothalamus ROI. Can the authors independently confirm that the identified cluster indeed lies within the hypothalamus?

The radius of the hypothalamus ROI is larger as compared to the other ROIs, because it is centered close to the midline and thus covers the left and the right hypothalamus.

Associated Data

    This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

    Data Citations

    1. Christian Buchel, Stephan Miedl, Christian Sprenger. 2018. Data from: Hedonic processing in humans is mediated by an opioidergic mechanism in a mesocorticolimbic system. Dryad Digital Repository. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]

    Supplementary Materials

    Supplementary file 1. Side effects ratings.

    Two sided paired Wilcoxon signed rank test comparing mood between naloxone (_nlx) and saline (_nacl) sessions.

    elife-39648-supp1.docx (29.4KB, docx)
    DOI: 10.7554/eLife.39648.020
    Supplementary file 2. Mood ratings.

    Two sided paired Wilcoxon signed rank test comparing mood between naloxone (_nlx) and saline (_nacl) sessions.

    elife-39648-supp2.docx (30.3KB, docx)
    DOI: 10.7554/eLife.39648.021
    Supplementary file 3. Rating differences for successful and missed reward trials in the placebo treatment condition.
    elife-39648-supp3.docx (28.6KB, docx)
    DOI: 10.7554/eLife.39648.022
    Supplementary file 4. Effects of Naloxone on pleasure ratings.

    Repeated measures ANOVA, Huynh-Feldt nonsphericity correction.

    elife-39648-supp4.docx (30.5KB, docx)
    DOI: 10.7554/eLife.39648.023
    Supplementary file 5. Naloxone effects (relative to placebo) on pleasure ratings.
    elife-39648-supp5.docx (29.8KB, docx)
    DOI: 10.7554/eLife.39648.024
    Supplementary file 6. Coordinates from individual studies to define the center of ROIs.
    elife-39648-supp6.docx (51.5KB, docx)
    DOI: 10.7554/eLife.39648.025
    Supplementary file 7. Naloxone effects (relative to placebo) on the high versus low erotic image outcome contrast.
    elife-39648-supp7.docx (29.8KB, docx)
    DOI: 10.7554/eLife.39648.026
    Supplementary file 8. Naloxone effects (relative to placebo) comparing erotic versus monetary outcomes.
    elife-39648-supp8.docx (29.7KB, docx)
    DOI: 10.7554/eLife.39648.027
    Supplementary file 9. Naloxone effects (relative to placebo) on other contrasts.
    elife-39648-supp9.docx (32.3KB, docx)
    DOI: 10.7554/eLife.39648.028
    Supplementary file 10. Regression analysis relating the change in pleasure ratings to the change in fMRI signal.
    elife-39648-supp10.docx (29.5KB, docx)
    DOI: 10.7554/eLife.39648.029
    Transparent reporting form
    DOI: 10.7554/eLife.39648.031

    Data Availability Statement

    Only freely available data analysis tools were used (SPM, Ledalab). Data are available online via Dryad (http://dx.doi.org/10.5061/dryad.11j304c)."

    The following dataset was generated:

    Christian Buchel, Stephan Miedl, Christian Sprenger. 2018. Data from: Hedonic processing in humans is mediated by an opioidergic mechanism in a mesocorticolimbic system. Dryad Digital Repository.


    Articles from eLife are provided here courtesy of eLife Sciences Publications, Ltd

    RESOURCES