Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2019 Jul 1.
Published in final edited form as: J Pers Assess. 2017 Oct 31;100(4):363–374. doi: 10.1080/00223891.2017.1381969

Alternative Approaches to Addressing Non-Normal Distributions in the Application of IRT Models to Personality Measures

Steven P Reise a, Anthony Rodriguez a, Karen L Spritzer b, Ron D Hays b
PMCID: PMC6252010  NIHMSID: NIHMS1508313  PMID: 29087217

Abstract

It is generally assumed that the latent trait is normally distributed in the population when estimating logistic item response theory model parameters. This assumption requires that the latent trait be fully continuous and the population homogenous (i.e., not a mixture). When this normality assumption is violated, models are misspecified, and item and person parameter estimates are inaccurate. When normality cannot be assumed, it may be appropriate to consider alternative modeling approaches: (a) a zero-inflated mixture, (b) a log-logistic, (c) a Ramsay curve, or (d) a heteroskedastic-skew model. The first two models were developed to address modeling problems associated with so-called “quasi-continuous” or “unipolar” constructs, which apply only to a subset of the population, or are meaningful at one end of the continuum only. The second two models were developed to address non-normal latent trait distributions and violations of homogeneity of error variance, respectively. To introduce these alternative IRT models and illustrate their strengths and weaknesses, we performed real data application comparing results to those from a graded response model. We review both statistical and theoretical challenges in applying these models and choosing among them. Future applications of these and other alternative models (e.g., unfolding, diffusion) are needed to advance understanding about model choice in particular situations.


Well known assumptions of unidimensional IRT models are unidimensionality, local independence, and monotonicity. When estimating item parameters using full-information maximum likelihood, it is commonly assumed that the underlying latent trait is normally distributed in the population. In specifying a normal distribution, it is implicitly assumed that the latent variable scale and the estimated item parameters apply to everyone in the calibration population (i.e., there is no mixture). Further, it is assumed that the latent variable is a continuous “bipolar trait” that has substantively meaningful variation across the range of the latent variable (Lucke. 2015, p. 273).

When the normality assumption, or its subsidiary assumptions, are violated, parameter estimates can be highly inaccurate (Azevedo, Bolfarine, & Andrade, 2011; DeMars, 2012; Kirischi, Hsu, & Yu, 2001; Sass, Scmitt, & Walker, 2008; Seong, 1990; Wall, Park, and Moustaki, 2015). While reviewing the extensive psychometric literature on the effects of normality violations on IRT item parameter and person estimates is beyond the scope of this paper, Woods and Thissen (2006, p. 283) nicely summarize the consequences of non-normality:

“There is fairly consistent evidence that, when normality of g(θ) is assumed, MML estimates of more extreme item parameters (e.g., thresholds around ±2) are nontrivially biased when the true population distribution is platykurtic or skewed, and if g(θ) is skewed, the bias increases as the skewness increases.” Monroe and Cai (2014, p. 365) provide a compelling example of the negative consequences of a misspecified normal distribution in the context of a Drug Abuse Treatment Outcome Studies measure of mental health and emotional distress.

Non-normal latent trait distributions present particular challenges in the application of standard logistic IRT models to personality and psychopathology measures (Reise & Rodriguez, 2016) because it is arguable that for many traits in these domains (e.g., self-esteem (Gray-Little et al., 1997)); borderline personality disorder (Michonski et al., 2013), or dark triad traits ( Webster & Jonason, 2013), an assumed normal distribution in a general population may be untenable. In such cases, researchers need to consider alternative IRT models designed to estimate non-normal distributions.

Herein, we describe the strengths and limitations of two such approaches, one non-parametric and the other parametric (i.e., assumes a particular distributional form). Specifically, we review a Ramsay curve model (Woods & Thissen, 2006) that estimates the shape of the latent trait distribution simultaneously with the estimation of the item parameters. We also review a heteroskedastic-skew model (Molenaar, Dolan, and De Boeck, 2012) that estimates both the skewness of the latent trait and allows for error variances that increase or decrease as a function of the latent trait. Using a real data set, the results of these models will be compared with the results under a normality assumption.

A skewed latent trait distribution is one way that personality and psychopathology data deviate from the normality assumption. An additional complexity is that some personality and psychopathology constructs are not fully continuous with meaningful individual difference variation across the full-range of the latent trait continuum. Although some constructs such as extraversion (vs. introversion), conscientiousness (vs. irresponsible), and subjective wellbeing (vs. subjective distress) are, arguably, continuous and bipolar, other important constructs such as substance use/abuse, agoraphobia, and somatic complaints, are not bipolar or fully continuous. We argue that for such constructs, one would not expect a normal distribution; more likely would be a highly skewed or a half-normal distribution.

The challenges that certain personality and psychopathology constructs present for IRT modeling have been long noted. Almost 30 years ago Reise and Waller (1990, p. 57) stated that some “personality traits may have an inherently quasi-categorical rather than a full range continuum structure.” Observing that clinical assessment instruments have highly peaked information functions in the high (pathological) trait range and a notable lack of items that provide discriminations among individuals in low trait ranges, Reise and Waller (2007, p. 31) stated, “we believe that the peaked information function for many clinical scales reflects the quasi-trait status of many psychopathology constructs. By the term ‘quasi-trait,’ we mean that the trait is unipolar (relevant only in one direction) and that variation at the low end of the scale is less informative in both a substantive as well as a psychometric sense.”

Reise and Waller (1990; 2007) merged the concepts of unipolar trait and quasi-continuous trait (as opposed to fully continuous) to reference certain types of constructs that are potentially problematic when fitting IRT models using a normality assumption. Here we use the term “quasi-trait” to refer to constructs such as positive psychotic symptoms, where low scores on symptoms ratings reflect the absence or irrelevance of the disorder for the individual. We use the term “unipolar” trait to refer to constructs that are most substantively meaningful at one end of the continuum (e.g., alienation, aggression).

Admittedly, the distinction between “quasi” (apply only to a subset of the population) and “unipolar” (only meaningful at one end of the continuum) construct is murky in practice, but needs to be drawn here because recently introduced IRT models were designed explicitly to handle these two types of measurement situations.

Specifically, we review two alternative IRT modeling approaches potentially applicable for quasi- and unipolar traits. The first is a zero-inflated mixture model (Wall, Park, & Moustaki, 2015) designed to handle the IRT modeling of quasi-traits – when the population is heterogeneous and the continuous trait is only applicable for a subset of the population. This model treats zero and near zero scores as a distinct latent class and then estimates IRT item parameters with a normality assumption only for a “traited class.” We also review a log-logistic model (Lucke, 2015) explicitly designed to handle unipolar traits – traits that are not fully continuous and are only substantively meaningful at one end of the trait continuum.

In what follows, we review emerging IRT models that may be viable alternatives when the normality assumption for the latent trait in IRT is implausible, either because the latent trait distribution is suspected to be skewed, and/or the construct is not fully continuous or a unipolar trait. For comparison purposes, we fit a logistic graded response model (GRM; Samejima, 1969) to 29 items from the Patient Reported Outcomes Measurement Information Systems (PROMIS®) Anger scale (Pilkonis et al., 2011). We then compare this “business-as-usual” analysis with the four alternative models cited above.

Each of the four alternative models makes different assumptions about the origin of the normality violation. Our specific goal in each comparison is not only to highlight the strengths and limitations of alternative modeling approaches, but also to demonstrate how the models may yield different substantive results, or not, in this particular dataset. Our overarching goals are to raise awareness of alternative IRT models, as well as to motivate researchers to think more critically about latent distributions.

Example Data and Psychometric Characteristics

The calibration sample consisted of 1498 non-clinical adults who responded to 29 items administered in the development of the PROMIS Anger measure (content available in Supplementary Table 1). Anger is an historically important construct in normal range personality, health-outcomes, and in psychopathology research and is one of three constructs relevant to Negative Affect available through PROMIS (Pilkonis et al., 2011) initiative. As such it has been extensively evaluated 1 (e.g., Shalet et al., 2016). This measure was selected because it is well suited for illustrating the strengths and limitations of the alternative models presented here. We also believe the data are representative of typical IRT applications to clinical measures as reviewed in Reise and Waller (2007) – unipolar, high skew for summed scores, and few if any items providing discrimination in the low trait range (see analyses below).

The first set of columns in Table 1 displays the percentage of responses in each category (0=Never, 1=Rarely, 2=Sometimes, 3=Often, and 4=Always)2. Relatively few individuals respond in the two extreme categories, and for many items, approximately 50% are responding 0=Never. The last set of columns displays the item-scale (minus the item) correlations, item means, and standard deviations. When summed scores are calculated, coefficient alpha =.97, M =25.55, S = 21.22, skewness = 0.96, and kurtosis =.50.

Table 1.

Category Response Percentages and Descriptive Statistics for the PROMIS

Response Percentages Descriptives
Scale Itema 0 1 2 3 4 rit M S
Item 1 Ang01 0.28 0.32 0.29 0.09 0.02 0.62 1.25 1.03
Item 2 Ang03 0.36 0.27 0.26 0.09 0.03 0.74 1.14 1.08
Item 3 Ang04 0.57 0.24 0.14 0.04 0.01 0.64 0.68 0.94
Item 4 Ang05 0.20 0.30 0.41 0.08 0.01 0.61 1.40 0.93
Item 5 Ang06 0.45 0.27 0.20 0.06 0.02 0.78 0.93 1.02
Item 6 Ang07 0.66 0.19 0.10 0.04 0.01 0.75 0.55 0.91
Item 7 Ang09 0.28 0.36 0.28 0.07 0.01 0.79 1.17 0.95
Item 8 Ang10 0.44 0.25 0.20 0.08 0.02 0.68 0.98 1.07
Item 9 Ang11 0.72 0.13 0.10 0.04 0.01 0.75 0.50 0.91
Item 10 Ang15 0.56 0.23 0.14 0.06 0.01 0.82 0.74 0.99
Item 11 Ang16 0.45 0.27 0.20 0.06 0.02 0.78 0.92 1.01
Item 12 Ang17 0.52 0.27 0.15 0.05 0.01 0.76 0.76 0.95
Item 13 Ang18 0.49 0.23 0.20 0.06 0.02 0.71 0.89 1.06
Item 14 Ang21 0.47 0.27 0.18 0.07 0.01 0.79 0.88 1.00
Item 15 Ang22 0.60 0.19 0.16 0.04 0.01 0.75 0.66 0.93
Item 16 Ang25 0.57 0.24 0.13 0.05 0.01 0.81 0.68 0.93
Item 17 Ang26 0.53 0.24 0.16 0.05 0.02 0.76 0.77 0.99
Item 18 Ang28 0.49 0.26 0.18 0.06 0.02 0.81 0.84 1.01
Item 19 Ang30 0.26 0.37 0.29 0.07 0.01 0.75 1.21 0.95
Item 20 Ang31 0.40 0.33 0.21 0.06 0.01 0.72 0.94 0.95
Item 21 Ang35 0.20 0.36 0.34 0.09 0.01 0.72 1.35 0.93
Item 22 Ang37 0.46 0.29 0.18 0.06 0.01 0.82 0.86 0.97
Item 23 Ang42 0.57 0.25 0.13 0.04 0.01 0.80 0.68 0.93
Item 24 Ang45 0.41 0.28 0.22 0.07 0.02 0.75 0.99 1.02
Item 25 Ang47 0.47 0.27 0.19 0.06 0.02 0.80 0.90 1.02
Item 26 Ang48 0.70 0.14 0.11 0.04 0.01 0.77 0.51 0.91
Item 27 Ang54 0.44 0.30 0.20 0.06 0.01 0.78 0.92 1.00
Item 28 Ang55 0.43 0.28 0.21 0.06 0.02 0.79 0.95 1.02
Item 29 Ang56 0.72 0.15 0.09 0.03 0.01 0.70 0.48 0.88
a

Note.Response options include 0=Never, 1=Rarely, 2=Sometimes, 3=Often, 4=Always. Item-test correlation denoted (rit). For raw scores, M = 25.55, S = 21.22, skewness = 0.96, and kurtosis = 0.5.

As seen in Figure 1, the distribution of summed scores appears to be more of a truncated-normal or half-normal distribution with zero-inflation; best fitting skewed normal (solid-line) and normal distributions (long-dashed line) are superimposed for illustrative purposes. While the distribution of summed scores is not necessarily a good indicator of the distribution of the latent trait,3 it is still critically important to inspect it for observed non-normality in the data. Is the latent distribution really normal but observed skew is due to “faulty” item construction or is the latent distribution skewed, possibly with zero-inflation? Are excess zeros caused by poor sampling, or is this attributable to a unipolar or quasi-trait? How a researcher answers these questions impacts model choice.

Figure 1.

Figure 1.

Histogram of Composite Scores for Anger Scale with Best Fitting Skewed Normal (solid line) and Normal Distribution (dashed line).

The Graded Response Model

The graded response model (GRM; Samejima, 1969) is commonly employed in the IRT modeling of personality, psychopathology, and health outcomes data. This model has well known relations with the parameters resulting from item-level ordinal factor analysis (i.e., factor loadings and intercepts); In fact, some IRT software packages report both the factor analytic and IRT parameterization alongside each other in standard output. Herein, we consider the GRM as the “default” or “business-as-usual” model. In estimating the GRM model, it is commonly assumed that there exists an underlying normally distribution latent variable (“trait”) with meaningful variation across the full range of the trait continuum.

In the GRM for each item (i), one slope parameters (αi) is estimated; Items with larger slope parameters are considered more discriminating or informative. For each item, K - 1 intercept parameters γi(j = 1...K−1) are also estimated where K is the number of response options. These intercepts are then transformed into K - 1 location parameters βi(j = 1...K−1), where βij=γijαij. Thus, the location parameter is not independent of the slope parameter, and, consequently, highly discriminating items tend to have locations that are clustered around zero, and less discriminating items have locations more spread out.

Item parameter estimates for the Anger Scale under the GRM are shown in Table 2. These parameters were estimated using marginal maximum likelihood as implemented in MIRT (Chalmers, 2012) assuming a normally distributed latent trait with a mean of 0 and variance of 1 in the population (for identification). The default number of quadrature nodes used in MIRT is 61 (range specified to be −4 to 4). For informational purposes, the log-likelihood of the model is −38135.64, Akaike Information Criterion (AIC) = 76561.28, Bayesian Information Criterion (BIC) = 77331.5, Root Mean Square Error of Approximation (RMSEA) =0.04 (95% CI =0.038 to 0.049), Standardized Root Mean Square Residual (SRMSR) =.040, and Comparative Fit Index (CFI) =.989. The M2 fit index (Maydeu-Olivares & Joe, 2006) is 1027.31 on 290 df, p <.01. Thus, judging by the practical fit indices, the estimated parameters recover the data well, but the M2 statistical index suggests that a closer examination of fit at the item level is needed.

Table 2.

Item Slope, Location, and Intercept Parameter Estimates Under the Graded Response Model, Assuming a Normally Distributed Latent Trait.

Slope Location Intercept
α β1 β2 β3 β4 γ1 γ2 γ3 γ4
Item 1 1.71 −0.77 0.40 1.72 2.98 1.31 −0.69 −2.94 −5.09
Item 2 2.25 −0.40 0.46 1.52 2.48 0.90 −1.03 −3.42 −5.58
Item 3 1.80 0.26 1.18 2.21 3.26 −0.47 −2.12 −3.98 −5.86
Item 4 1.57 −1.21 0.04 1.96 3.60 1.90 −0.06 −3.08 −5.65
Item 5 2.65 −0.13 0.70 1.69 2.55 0.34 −1.86 −4.48 −6.75
Item 6 2.92 0.47 1.16 1.81 2.70 −1.38 −3.39 −5.28 −7.87
Item 7 2.85 −0.66 0.42 1.59 2.75 1.87 −1.20 −4.53 −7.86
Item 8 1.97 −0.17 0.69 1.68 2.77 0.34 −1.36 −3.32 −5.46
Item 9 3.14 0.65 1.13 1.77 2.65 −2.03 −3.55 −5.56 −8.32
Item 10 3.35 0.18 0.89 1.62 2.52 −0.61 −2.97 −5.42 −8.46
Item 11 2.76 −0.12 0.71 1.67 2.53 0.33 −1.95 −4.61 −6.98
Item 12 2.60 0.08 0.94 1.85 2.79 −0.20 −2.45 −4.81 −7.25
Item 13 2.25 0.01 0.76 1.76 2.52 −0.02 −1.72 −3.96 −5.67
Item 14 2.75 −0.05 0.75 1.63 2.84 0.14 −2.06 −4.48 −7.80
Item 15 2.62 0.32 0.97 1.97 2.94 −0.83 −2.54 −5.16 −7.69
Item 16 3.30 0.21 0.98 1.75 2.69 −0.70 −3.23 −5.78 −8.88
Item 17 2.62 0.13 0.90 1.82 2.55 −0.33 −2.34 −4.76 −6.67
Item 18 3.23 0.01 0.76 1.64 2.42 −0.03 −2.47 −5.30 −7.83
Item 19 2.49 −0.76 0.39 1.62 2.75 1.88 −0.98 −4.04 −6.86
Item 20 2.20 −0.28 0.76 1.90 3.11 0.62 −1.67 −4.17 −6.82
Item 21 2.19 −1.05 0.20 1.60 2.96 2.30 −0.45 −3.51 −6.47
Item 22 3.21 −0.08 0.78 1.68 2.62 0.25 −2.51 −5.38 −8.42
Item 23 3.27 0.21 1.01 1.84 2.54 −0.68 −3.31 −6.03 −8.31
Item 24 2.38 −0.24 0.64 1.69 2.72 0.57 −1.52 −4.01 −6.45
Item 25 3.00 −0.07 0.72 1.62 2.47 0.21 −2.17 −4.87 −7.41
Item 26 3.33 0.59 1.11 1.84 2.54 −1.98 −3.69 −6.12 −8.46
Item 27 2.68 −0.16 0.73 1.71 2.61 0.43 −1.96 −4.57 −7.00
Item 28 2.79 −0.17 0.65 1.65 2.53 0.47 −1.82 −4.60 −7.05
Item 29 2.42 0.69 1.34 2.06 2.79 −1.66 −3.23 −4.97 −6.75
Mean 2.63 −0.09 0.76 1.75 2.73 0.10 −2.08 −4.59 −7.09
SD 0.50 0.47 0.30 0.15 0.26 1.12 0.95 0.86 1.04
Meanp 2.17 −0.19 0.81 2.04 3.15
SDp 0.43 0.65 0.55 0.43 0.47

Note. α = slope, β = location, γ = intercept,

p

indicates Pilkonis et al. (2011) results.

To understand the parameters of the GRM, in Figure 2 we display three plots based on the results for Item #1. In the top panel, is the log-odds of responding in and above categories 1, 2, 3, and 4, respectively, as a function of the latent variable. This plot makes clear the interpretation of the K-1 = 4 intercepts – they are the log-odds of responding in or above a category j = 1, 2, 3, and 4, for individuals with trait levels of 0 (the mean). Moreover, the item slope parameter reflects the steepness of these functions; for Item #1 with a slope = 1.7, the log-odds of responding in the next highest category or above increases by a factor of 1.7 for a 1 standard deviation unit change on the latent variable.

Figure 2.

Figure 2.

Log-Odds, Threshold Response Curves, and Category Response Curves for PROMIS Anger Item 1 Under the Graded Response Model.

In the middle panel of Figure 2 is shown the more familiar threshold response curves (TRCs) for Item #1. These represent the probability of responding in and above categories j = 1, 2, 3, and 4, respectively, as a function of the latent variable. The vertical dashed lines show that the location parameters represent the point on the latent variable continuum where the individual has a 50% chance of responding in and above a given category j=1…4. Finally, for the bottom panel, if we label the K-1 TRCs as TRC1, TRC2, TRC3, and TRC4, respectively, then the probability of responding in a particular category is 1 – TRC1, TRC1 – TRC2, TRC2 – TRC3, TRC3 – TRC4, and TRC4 – 0. These are called category response curves (CRCs). For any point on the latent variable, the sum of the CRCs equals 1.

Returning to Table 2, all PROMIS Anger items are highly discriminating, but there is a large range. Item #4 (“I disagreed with people”) has a slope of 1.57, and Item #26 (“I felt like I needed help for my anger”) has a slope of 3.33. Item #26 is about 4.5 times more informative or discriminating than Item #4 (3.332/1.572), or, it would take about 4.5 items like Item #4 to achieve the same precision as one Item #26.

Despite there being five response options designed to spread measurement precision across the trait range, there are no items with location parameters below −1.21 (Item #4 – the least discriminating item). With few exceptions, even the first location parameter tends to be around zero, or only slightly negative, suggesting that an individual has to be around the mean on the latent variable to have a fifty percent chance of responding in the second category or above. On the positive side of the theta continuum, the 4th location parameters are extreme, suggesting that to respond in the highest category, individuals must be around 2.5 to 3.6 standard deviations above the mean on the latent variable. Clearly, the Anger Scale is a “peaked test,” with location parameters clustered in the positive end of the continuum, and few, if any, items that discriminate best in the low trait range. This is the type of psychometric situation that Reise and Waller (1990; 2007) referred to in their discussion of quasi-continuous traits.

Models for Quasi- and Unipolar Traits

The above described GRM assumed a continuous, normally distributed latent variable. Nevertheless, the observed item and summed scores are highly skewed, and when a model was fit, threshold parameters were highly concentrated at the high end of the trait continuum. To justify this incongruity, one might attribute this to problems with the measure or the data, that is, faulty item construction (e.g., not enough response options, or, the anchors somehow are too extreme and thus cannot distinguish between low trait individuals), or oversampling individuals from low trait ranges. Alternatively, we can reconceptualize the construct with a corresponding change in model and assumptions. In the following two sections, we consider two alternative modeling strategies.

A Zero-Inflated Mixture Model

Wall, Park, and Moustaki (2015) developed a “zero-inflated” mixture (ZIM) IRT model in the context of psychiatric symptom measurement where it is common to find many individuals responding with zero or few symptoms. In other words, the model was developed explicitly to handle measurement situations where the construct is a so-called “quasi-trait” (a trait applicable to only a subset of the population) and the population is heterogeneous – for one “pathological” class of individuals, the trait is meaningful and symptoms can be used to scale individuals along a continuum, whereas for another “no traited” or “non-pathological” class, the construct is inapplicable. Wall, Park, and Moustaki (2015) cite studies that documented the severe bias in IRT item parameter estimates, especially the discrimination parameter, when a normal distribution is assumed, but the data are zero-inflated. In turn, the authors demonstrated that their ZIM model yields more accurate calibration.

The basic idea underlying the zero-inflated mixture model is to estimate the percentages in the population that belong to the traited and no traited classes and then estimate the item parameters for the GRM in the traited class only. The latent trait is assumed to be normal in both classes, but one class is degenerate – item parameters are not estimated for the degenerate class. This is similar to, but not exactly the same, as discarding cases scoring zero or near zero, and estimating item parameters based on the remaining cases. The authors argue that the mixture approach has a superior statistical justification because due to measurement error, some people with zero raw scores are likely in the “traited” class, while some people with non-zero raw scores likely belong to the “untraited” class.

We estimated a zero-inflated mixture model using Mplus (Muthén & Muthén, 2016) code supplied by the authors, with modification for the present data. Results showed that the percentage of individuals estimated to be in the untraited class was 5.5% (a subset of the 6.0% of individuals with 0 raw total scores) and, thus, 94.5% was in the traited class. In comparison to GRM parameter estimates shown in Table 2, the slope parameters in the ZIM model for the traited class are much lower (mean = 1.94 vs 2.63); controlling for the zero-inflation leads to smaller slope parameters (see Supplementary Table 2).

When test information curves are drawn that reflect the precision of measurement across the trait range, the test information functions for the GRM and mixture model are peaked at relatively high trait levels, but the mixture model, which is presumably more accurate, provides about half the information of the GRM (see Figure 3), and thus larger standard errors (which equal approximately 1 divided by the square root of test information). Finally, the Pearson correlation between latent trait estimates for the 1,406 individuals in the traited class from the mixture and GRM was.994. Thus, the models provide essentially the same relative ordering of individuals albeit with much larger standard errors in the ZIM model.

Figure 3.

Figure 3.

A Comparison of Test Information for the Graded Response Model (solid line) and the Zero-Inflated Mixture Model (dashed line).

There are three important limitations of the mixture model applied in the present context. First, the model assumes that the non-normality arises from a degenerate class, and once this degenerate class is removed from the calibration sample (down weighted during estimation), the distribution is normal. The model, as presently implemented, does not allow for a skewed distribution to be applied after removing the untraited class. Thus, the parameters may still be biased due an incorrect latent distribution.

Second, and resulting from the first, one loses sample size because no meaningful latent trait estimates can be derived for the degenerate class. If this approach were to be applied in multivariate research where many constructs are measured, it is not at all clear how researchers are to proceed with such missing data when the data are missing as a consequence of construct irrelevance. Our third concern is purely substantive. As noted, the model was derived in the context of psychiatric constructs (alcohol use/abuse in particular) where the mixture formulation of non-pathological and pathological groups may make relatively more substantive sense. It is not clear to us what the interpretation of a class of “no anger” group would mean substantively, unless we view anger as measured in the PROMIS items as a pathological condition.

A Unipolar Log-Logistic Model

In the model presented in this section, a skewed distribution is treated as an inherent result of the measurement of unipolar constructs, especially for disorders such as alcohol, nicotine, and substance abuse where it makes little sense to create a norm-referenced score. Lucke (2015, p. 272) states, “it makes little or no sense to assert that a person has a below-average level of addiction to alcohol or an above-average level of addiction to gambling.” He then further argues that “The anchor for the scale should therefore be “no disorder.”

To put these views in practice, Lucke (2015) proposed a log-logistic (LL) model for dichotomous item response data. This model was proposed in the context of unipolar traits and applied to a measure of gambling addiction. In the LL model, the latent trait begins at zero and continues to positive infinity. Response patterns of all zeros are assigned θ = 0. A polytomous log-logistic threshold response curve (TRC) for responding in or above category j (j = 1…4) is then defined:

TRCij=P(xj|θ)=λijθηi1+λijθηi

Category response curves are defined in the same way as the GRM: CRC0 = 1−TRC1, CRC1 = TRC1−TRC2; CRC2=TRC2−TRC3; CRC3=TRC3−TRC4; CRC4=TRC4−0. Model parameters are defined as follows. The λij parameters, K – 1 per item, are analogous to the intercept parameters in the GRM and have been referred to as “easiness” parameters. The λ parameters are always positive, and higher values signify that a large proportion of people respond in or below a given category. The ηi parameter, one per item, is a discrimination parameter and analogous to the slope in the GRM; in fact its exactly the same so items are no more or less relatively discriminating in either model. Finally, for each item, K-1 location parameters – the point on the latent trait where the probability of responding in or above category j=1...K-1 is.50 is δij=(1λij)1ηij.

The parameters of the dichotomous or polytomous log-logistic model can be estimated using Bayesian methods. However, for present purposes, we take advantage of the fact that logistic models and log-logistic models are transformations of each other (e.g., if the latent variable in the GRM is normal with a mean of zero and standard deviation of 1, then in the LL model, the distribution is log-normal with the same mean and standard deviation). 4Thus, using the parameters for the GRM shown in Table 2, we can transform to a log-logistic model as follows: λ = exp(γ); η = α; and δ = exp(β). Latent trait scores estimated in the GRM can also be transformed to the log-logistic metric as θ^LL=exp(θ^GRM).

Item parameter estimates for the log-logistic model are available in Supplementary Table 3. Understanding the difference between the GRM and LL rests on understanding the effects of the transformation of the latent scale. To clarify, what the LL model does is massively compress negative theta estimates and estimates that are around 0 in the GRM metric. For example consider that exp(−3.0) = 0.049; exp(−2.0)= 0.135; exp(−1.0) = 0.367; exp(0) = 1.0; exp(1.0) = 2.71; exp(2.0)= 7.38; and exp(3.0)=20.08. For this reason, theta estimates are very highly skewed in the log-logistic model. In turn, this new metric has profound implications for the item and test information functions. Due to the metric “squeezing” at the low end and expansion at the high end, test information is very peaked and extremely high at the low end, as shown in Figure 4. The corresponding standard errors, also shown, indicate that the standard errors for a trait level estimate changes remarkably as a function of theta. The correlation between trait level estimates in the GRM and LL is r =.72. Despite the fact that the two estimates are simple non-linear tranforms of each other where the rank ordering remains the same, the correlation is far from perfect due to scale compression and expansion noted above.

Figure 4.

Figure 4.

Test Information (solid line) and Standard Errors (dashed line) in the Log-Logistic Model.

In our view, the log-logistic model has many virtues to recommend it in terms of the present Anger measure. Most importantly, it does not assume normality, it allows for the scoring of all individuals, and appears consistent with theory if the researcher believes the construct to be a unipolar trait. On the other hand, this is a relatively recently proposed model, and much remains unknown (e.g., how to evaluate fit, test for differential item functioning, its robustness to zero-inflation, and so on). Some of our concerns are practical (e.g., accuracy of parameter estimation under varying distribution conditions) and some are technical (e.g., the information function in this model has some implausible properties – it gets very high at low trait levels due to the “squeezing” effect of the transformation).

Models for Non-Normal Latent Traits

The above models treat the non-normality in the data as arising from very different mechanisms, zero-inflation and the unipolar nature of the construct, respectively. In this section, we review two models that assume a continuous underlying latent distribution, but allow that distribution to be non-normal.5

Ramsay-Curve IRT

When a normal latent trait distribution is (incorrectly) assumed during parameter estimation, this misspecification can lead to distorted item parameter estimates. One possible remedy to this problem is to estimate the shape of the latent trait distribution and then estimate parameters based on a correctly specified latent trait distribution. In Ramsay-Curve (RC) IRT (Woods, 2006; Woods, 2015; Woods & Thissen, 2006), the latent trait distribution is estimated at the same time as the item parameters.

At its most basic level, the latent trait distribution in RC-IRT is estimated using a smooth function to describe the density for the latent trait; this is a non-parametric technique, but there are limits on the type of distribution that can be reasonably approximated. To date, all research on parameter recovery under RC-IRT relies on creating non-normal distributions through the mixture of normal distributions. We know of no studies of parameter recovery in the presence of zero-inflation. In this analysis, we used RCLOG V2 (Woods, 2006) to estimate a latent density underlying the Anger items. There are several important user options in running RCLOG. For the sake of brevity, for technical details and suggested user defaults, we refer readers to the original research and user manual.

In this analysis, we allowed RC-IRT to evaluate solutions running from 2 degrees and 2 knots (a normal distribution) to 6 degrees and 6 knots. Knots and degrees are technical jargon. Degrees refers to the degrees of the polynomial for the Ramsay curves used to approximate the latent distribution. Knots refers to the number of joinings of the Ramsay curves. Together, degrees and knots refer to the flexibility of the possible distribution; fewer are more restricted with the limiting case of a normal distribution, while more allow for greater departures from non-normality (i.e., skew and kurtosis). Examination of fit indices output from RCLOG suggested that the solution with 6 knots and 2 degrees was “best,” although alternative solutions were very close. In this solution, the log-likelihood was −38102.47, which differed significantly from a normal distribution (χ2 = 67.11 on 4 df). Most importantly, skewness of the latent distribution was estimated to be −1.96 and kurtosis was estimated to be 8.78. The specific distribution estimated is shown in Figure 5, which displays one small hump at low trait levels, and then an essentially normal distribution.

Figure 5.

Figure 5.

The Latent Density Estimated by RC-LOG.

Item parameter estimates based on the best fitting model are available in Supplementary Table 4. Comparing these values to the GRM, the glaring difference is that the slopes are all much higher in the RC model than GRM (M = 3.75 in RC and M = 2.63 in GRM), implying that the RC model yields more precise trait level estimates. However, we caution that these parameter estimates may be misleading. The basis of our concern is the negative skew estimate and unusually high kurtosis. In particular, the negative skew estimate for data that are clearly positively skewed indicates that the estimation may be problematic due to the excess zero distribution or preponderance of people clustered around raw total scores of zero.6 The correlation between trait level estimates in the GRM and RC models is.99.

The Heteroskedastic-Skew Graded Response Model

The above model attempts to estimate a non-parametric but smooth density function to represent the latent trait distribution. The model in this section, called the heteroskedastic-skew (HS) model (Molenaar, Dolan, & De Boeck, 2012), also attempts to estimate a non-normal density, but with a specific parametric form, namely, a skewed normal distribution (Azzalini & Capatanio, 1999). In addition, the HS model also attempts to account for violations of homogeneity of variance, which is one possible, but seldom discussed source of observed non-normality in item response.

Three features of the HS are critical to understand. First, the model is based on the normal-ogive version of the GRM. This makes the model akin to an item-level ordinal factor analytic model – a “factor loading and intercept” parameterization easily transformable into an IRT “slope and threshold” parameterization. For example, we can describe item functioning as:

yi*=νi+λiθ+ϵi

yi* is a continuous normal response propensity which is “polytomized” through the K ordinal item response categories, νi is the item intercept (the expected score on yi* when θ = 0), λi is the (unstandardized) factor loading (regression slope), and εi is a residual with variance, σϵ2. For each item, a linear regression is estimated with the latent variable as the predictor and a normally distributed latent response propensity as the outcome. In the above, error variances for each item are assumed to be homoscedastic, with an expected value of zero.

Second, the logistic GRM described previously imposes symmetric category response curves, which can lead to problems in scoring individuals on the latent variable (Samejima, 2000). By virtue of allowing for heteroskedastic errors, the HS model does not necessarily produce symmetric category response curves (see Molenaar, Dolan, & De Boeck, 2012, p. 473). Third, the developers of the HS model note that observed skewness in the data can be caused by at least two factors: (a) the latent trait distribution could be skewed, and, (b) heteroskedasticity of residuals. In theory, the HS model allows for skewness to be estimated after separating out the effects of heteroskedastic error and, thus, can provide a “cleaner” estimate.

With this foundation in mind, the basic idea of the HS-GRM is to simultaneously estimate: (a) the parameters of the normal-ogive GRM, (b) a heterogeneity parameter for each item that allows items to violate homoscedasticity, and (c) the skewness of the latent trait distribution. This latter estimate is based on assuming a parametric skewed-normal distribution. For specific details of the model, and converson of factor analytic to item response theory model parameters, we refer readers to Molenaar, Dolan, and De Boeck (2012).

We estimated two nested models using an OpenMx (Boker, et al., 2011) program provided by the model developers: (a) baseline model with no skew or heteroscedasticity, and a full model with both skew and heteroscedasticity estimated. To identify the model, we fixed the first two threshold parameters to their values estimated in the GRM. Item parameter estimates (i.e., factor loadings, thresholds, intercepts, residual variance, heteroscedastic residuals, and IRT slope) for the baseline and full models are available in Supplementary Tables 5 and 6.

Most important is the comparison of the baseline model with the full model. For the baseline model (skewness = 0, heteroskedasticity = 0), model fit indices were −2 the log-likelihood = 76733.50, df = 43297, AIC = −9860.50, and BIC = −119924.62. For the full model, fit indices were −2 the log-likelihood = 76610.57, df = 43267, AIC = −9923.43, and BIC = −119876.40. The chi-square difference between the baseline and full model was 122.93 on 30 df, which is significant at p <=.01. The estimated skewness of the latent trait was 0.28, which is essentially indistinguishable from a normal distribution. The factor loadings (M=0.96 vs. M=0.92) and IRT slopes (M=2.22 vs. M=2.05) are slightly lower in the full than the baseline model. All other item parameter estimates in the full model are essentially the same as the baseline, with the exception of the heteroskedasticity parameters (δ1), which are now estimated.

The critical issue with these parameters is whether they are of sufficient magnitude to impact the CRCs relative to the baseline model. To explore this issue, in the top two panels of Figure 6 are displayed the category response curves for Item 19 under the baseline and full models. This item had a large positive heteroskedasticity parameter (0.48). It appears that the CRCs under the two models are nearly identical. In the bottom panel, we compare the item response curves for Item #19 under the two models; they are nearly overlapping except in the high trait ranges where the expected item scores are lower for the full model. In Figure 7, we provide the comparison of test response curves (expected summed score as a function of the latent trait) under the two models. As one would predict from the slightly lower factor loadings in the full model, expected scores are slightly lower in the full model. This is likely a difference that makes no practical difference. Note that trail level estimates from the HS models were not actually estimated due to software limitations, but given the similarity in item parameters with the GRM it is reasonable to expect that their correlation with the GRM would be near 1.0.

Figure 6.

Figure 6.

Category Response Curves and Item Response Curves for Item 19 under Baseline Model and Heteroskedastic-Skew Model.

Figure 7.

Figure 7.

Test Response Curves for Baseline (solid line) and Heteroscedastic-Skew (dashed line) Models.

In review, although the HS full model with estimated skewed distribution is statistically superior than the baseline GRM with normal distribution, just as in the RC-IRT analysis, we question whether the present data are consistent with the estimated latent distribution. In RC-IRT the distribution is smooth but non-parametric, and thus avoids misspecifications caused by assuming a specific form. In the HS model, what is estimated is the skew of a parametric skewed normal distribution. In this regard, it is critically important to recognize that the limit of this distribution is a skewness of 1.0. As a consequence, no matter how skewed the true latent distribution is, the model can only accommodate that skew up to a certain point. Clearly, more research is needed to clarify parameter estimates under the HS model with unipolar traits, extreme skew, and/or excess zeros present in the data. In addition, the substantive interpretation and practical implications of heterogeneous residuals needs further research. In the present research, the role of estimating heterogeneous residuals was merely to decontaminate the estimate of skewness from one source of possible bias.

Discussion

IRT models are valuable psychometric tools when the model-derived latent variable scale (θ) accurately reflects individual differences on the trait the researcher is trying to measure and the estimated item parameters faithfully reflect the relation between trait levels and the probability of category response. To be used effectively, however, models such as the logistic GRM (Samejima, 1969) make many assumptions about the latent trait (causative, not emergent7), the item response data (local independence), the calibration sample (homogeneous, representative), the nature and shape of the latent variable (continuous, normal), the distribution of errors, and the parametric form of the model (linear relation between theta and log-odds of responding). The validity of the conclusions drawn from any IRT model application is threatened to the degree that any of these assumptions are violated.

Similar to Pilkonis et al. (2011), we applied the logistic GRM, with normality assumption, to responses to a 29-item measure of Anger. We then considered four alternative models that relaxed one or more of the assumptions listed above. The RC-IRT (non-parametric) and HS (parametric) models, allow the researcher to estimate a fully continuous latent distribution simultaneously with the item parameters, and thus relax the normality assumption. Both approaches also allow a likelihood ratio test to compare the non-normal versus normal distribution model.

The ZIM and LL models relax normality in different ways, and neither model allows for a simple likelihood ratio statistical test of the whether it is a significant improvement over the GRM. The ZIM assumes that the population is heterogeneous and the observed normality violations are caused by a “no traited” or “not pathological” latent class. When this subsample is estimated and removed, the GRM with normality assumption is then applied to the “traited” or “pathological” group. The LL model replaces the logistic function in the GRM with the log-logistic, and replaces the normality assumption with an assumption of log-normality. It can be used in the same situations as the ZIM model.

Review of Practical Differences

An important practical concern is determining whether the alternative models yield either different scalings of individual differences, or provide a different view on the psychometric properties of the items and the scale (category response curves, scale response curves, scale information). In this section, we review model differences for the Anger Scale in terms of scoring and psychometric evaluation.

Correlations of latent trait estimates generated from the GRM with estimates from the ZIM (for those in the traited class), RC, and HS models are all nearly perfect. These results suggest that these models make little difference in terms of relative standing on the estimated latent trait. The only real difference for the Anger data is in the standard error, which would be larger in the ZIM model (because of lower slopes) and smaller in the RC model (because of the higher slopes).

The one distinctive model in terms of scoring was the LL model where latent trait estimates correlated r =.72 with those from the GRM. Although some may view this as a high linear correlation, implying similar patterns of external relations, it is important to note two differences. First, the anchor for the scale and the interpretation is very different. In the GRM, the anchor is the mean of θ = 0 and scores are interpreted relative to that mean. In the log-logistic, the anchor is θ = zero – no disorder – and scores reflect severity of the disorder. Second, relative to the GRM, differences between people near the low end of the scale are compressed while differences between people toward the high end of the scale are expanded. Thus, the substantive (heritability coefficients, correlates with neurobiolgical parameters or life outcomes) and psychometric results (indices of clinically important differences) based on the Anger data under these two models could differ dramatically.8

In terms of psychometric properties, as noted above, one major difference was the reduced slope parameters in the ZIM model compared to the GRM, suggesting that they are inflated in the GRM due to “excess zeros” – roughly, more zero scores than expected under a normal distribution. The implication is that if a sample contains fewer or greater non-cases, the slope parameters of the GRM will change accordingly. On the other hand, the RC model suggested higher slopes once a non-normal latent trait distribution was estimated. Nevertheless, we believe that the RC results are untrustworthy in this particular application due the extreme skew caused by the excess zeros identified in the ZIM model.

This same concern with the possibility of excess zeros applies to the interpretability of the HS model as well. The HS model that contained item heterogeneity parameters (allowing error variances to increase or decrease as a function of trait level) and estimated a skewed normal distribution fit better than the GRM. Moreover, because the estimated skewness was very small, the better fit could be mostly attributable to the estimated item heterogeneities. Nevertheless, allowing for heterogenous variances did not result in item or test response curves that differed appreciably from the GRM. Because of a lack of research on this model, we do not know the degree to which possible “excess zeros” lead to biases in heterogeneity or skewness estimates.

Finally, as in the case of scoring, the LL model provided the largest contrast with the normal theory GRM. In relative terms, items are just as discriminating in the GRM and LL model, but where that discrimination is located is vastly different. In the LL model, information is very high in the near zero trait range, indicating that the item set yield’s a precise discrimination between people who are low on the anger (i.e., the majority of subjects) and those who are not. In terms of differentiating among individuals at the positive end of scale, standard errors are relatively larger. These psychometrics differences have implications for all types of applications of IRT models including linking, computerized adaptive testing, and the study of differential item functioning. We note in closing that just as GRM slope parameters can be inflated by excess zeros, so can the analogous parameters in the LL model. In short, the LL results presented here may be misleading if one considers some zero scores as “excess” zeros.

Deciding Between Approaches in Practice

Throughout, we have not considered whether the alternative models provide a statistically “better fit” than the GRM that assumes normality; with a large enough sample, we assume that any model without a restrictive normality assumption will display a superior statistical fit. In practice, there are no ready fit index or rule-of-thumb for deciding between the models considered here. Rather, what is required is the thoughtful consideration of mostly theoretical questions. For example, if a researcher considers the latent variable to be fully continuous with meaningful variation on both ends of the scale (bipolar), but the latent trait distribution may be skewed, then RC and HS models are viable candidates.

However, we warn that not only do these methods have limitations in the type of distribution that can be estimated, the item response data must allow for the accurate estimation of a non-normal latent trait. For short scales (e.g., 5 items), and for scales that do not include items that discriminate well across the trait range9, the ability of any algorithm to correctly estimate a latent distribution is severely compromised. Moreover, if the skew is caused by “excess zeros,” possibly due to poor sampling, estimating a true latent distribution would be nearly impossible.

If the construct is considered unipolar or a quasi-trait, then the ZIM and LL models can be considered. In deciding between these two models a researcher must ask questions such as: what do low scores reflect – low trait standing or absence of the trait? A critical difference between these models is that the ZIM assumes a normal distribution for the population, but the sample is contaminated by excess zeros. To obtain correct population parameters, one needs to identify and eliminate these cases from the calibration. The LL assumes a highly skewed distribution in the population. If that assumption is justifiable, and it make sense to reference scores and clinical change relative to a zero anchor, the LL model may be the more appropriate choice. It is clear to us that more research is needed on the robustness of LL model parameters to excess zeros. It is also possible, in theory, to develop a LL model with excess zeros analogous to the ZIM model.

Conclusion

Lucke (2015) argued that a chief virtue of IRT modeling is that it allows researchers to develop measurement models that are consistent with the theory of the construct (see also Asparouhov & Muthén, 2015). Indeed, his LL model was selected not merely because it can account for highly skewed response data – dozens of monotonically increasing functions can do that – but rather because the log-logistic model and log-normal trait scale are potentially more consistent with the cognitive neuroscience of addictive behavior. Lucke is by no means alone in proposing that measurement models need to be consistent with the hypothesized underlying response processes and what is known about the nature of specific constructs.

Stark et al. (2006) and Weekers, Anke, and Meijer (2008) have considered the IRT modeling of personality data in terms of an unfolding response process.10 Van der Mass et al. (2011) consider a diffusion model for the response process that may be appropriate for bipolar traits, but not for unipolar traits that are anchored at no ability or no trait at the low end. Although neither of these models were detailed here, they are examples of new psychometric developments of potential value as we move toward the next generation of IRT applications in the personality, psychopathology, and health outcomes domains. We hope that the present article provides motivation for researchers to more carefully consider the nature of the latent trait, and to explore the application of alternative models. Only then can we obtain additional substantive insights and findings from these models.

Supplementary Material

Supp1

Acknowledgments

This research was supported in part through the National Cancer Institute (1U2-CCA186878–01). Ron D. Hays was also supported by the NIA (P30-AG021684) and the NIMHD (P20-MD000182).

Footnotes

2

Item #29 used a different response format: not at all, a little bit, somewhat, quite a bit, and very much.

3

The observed summed score distribution is a function of both the true latent trait distribution and the properties of the items (Lord, 1953). Thus, even if the true distribution is normal, if a test was either too “easy” or “difficult,” or if the location parameters are not symmetric around zero, the observed scores will be skewed.

4

With the caveat that if the parameters in the GRM are biased, their translation must be in error as well. This is why future work should consider the Bayesian estimation of the log-logistic GRM where a researcher can have better control over prior distributions.

5

In the original presentation of the Anger item bank (Pilkonis et al., 2011), the authors assumed a continous latent variable but acknowledged that the normality assumption for the calibration may be questionable. Nevertheless, they concluded that the effects of violations, if any, were mininal, and proceeded with a standard calibration with normality assumption.

6

We note that Woods (2015) successfully implemented Davidian curves to Anger items drawn from the PROMIS project. However, there is currently no available software to implement this method so we could not explore that alterative to Ramsay curves here.

7

It is also worth noting that not all constructs are best conceived as latent variables, some constructs are best represented as emergent variables (Bollen & Lennon, 1990; Fayers, Hand, Bjordal & Groenvold,1997). IRT or factor analytic model are inappropriate for this latter type of construct. Full discussion of this issue is beyond the scope of the present article. We have assumed for simplicity that the latent variable measure framework is sensible.

8

We note that IRT models have been criticized (Goldstein, 1980) exactly because if you change the basic model from a logistic, very different scaling of individual differences may occur. Historically, the choice of a logistic function was not based on any substantive consideration or proven validity, but rather simply on mathematical convenience. This remains true today.

9

We note that the sole psychometric justification for having multiple, ostensibly, ordered response options, rather than yes/no, true/false, is to allow for better differentiation among individuals across the assumed latent trait continuum. When location parameters in the GRM are bunched at one end of the continuum (e.g., all in the positive trait range), the items are not differentiating among individuals across the continuum. One possible reason is that the low end of the trait doesn’t exist – it is a unipolar or quasi-trait.

10

By response process, we mean the theory of how trait levels are linked to item responses. Traditional test theory models are dominance models – the probability of item endorsement depends on the degree to which a individual’s trait level is higher than an item’s location (e.g., easy vs. hard). In an “unfolding” response process, the probability of endorsement is determined by the absolute distance between trait level and item location. The closer the trait level is to item location, the more likely an endorsement.

References

  1. Asparouhov T & Muthén B (2015). Structural equation models and mixture models with continuous non-normal skewed distributions. Structural Equation Modeling: A Multidisciplinary Journal, 34(6), 1041–1058.DOI: 10.1080/10705511.2014.947375. [Google Scholar]
  2. Azzalini A, & Capatanio A (1999). Statistical applications of the multivariate skew normal distribution. Journal of the Royal Statistical Society, Series B, 61, 579–602.
 doi:10.1111/1467-9868.00194 [Google Scholar]
  3. Azevedo CL, Bolfarine H, & Andrade DF (2011). Bayesian inference for a skew-normal IRT model under the centered parameterization. Computational Statistics & Data Analysis, 55, 353–365. doi:10.1016/j.csda.2010.05.003 [Google Scholar]
  4. Boker S, Neale M, Maes H, Wilde M, Spiegel M, Brick T,… & Mehta P (2011). OpenMx: an open source extended structural equation modeling framework. Psychometrika, 76(2), 306–317. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Bollen K, & Lennox R (1991). Conventional wisdom on measurement: A structural equation perspective. Psychological Bulletin, 110, 305–314. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.110.2.305 [Google Scholar]
  6. Chalmers PR (2012). mirt: A Multidimensional Item Response Theory Package for the R Environment. Journal of Statistical Software, 48(6), 1–29. [Google Scholar]
  7. DeMars CE (2012). A Comparison of Limited-Information and Full-Information Methods in Mplus for Estimating Item Response Theory Parameters for Nonnormal Populations. Structural Equation Modeling: A Multidisciplinary Journal, 19(4), 610–632. [Google Scholar]
  8. Fayers PM, Hand DJ, Bjordal K, & Groenvold M (1997). Causal indicators in quality of life research. Quality of Life Research, 6(5), 393–406. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Goldstein H (1980). Dimensionality, bias, independence and measurement scale problems in latent trait test score models. British Journal of Mathematical and Statistical psychology, 33(2), 234–246. [Google Scholar]
  10. Gray-Little B, Williams VS, & Hancock TD (1997). An item response theory analysis of the Rosenberg Self-Esteem Scale. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 23(5), 443–451. [Google Scholar]
  11. Kirisci L, Hsu TC, & Yu L (2001). Robustness of item parameter estimation programs to assumptions of unidimensionality and normality, Applied Psychological Measurement, 25, 146–162. doi:10.1177/0146621012203197 [Google Scholar]
  12. Lucke JF (2015). Unipolar item response models. In Reise SP & Revicki DA (Eds.), Handbook of Item Response Theory Modeling: Applications to Typical Performance Assessment (pp. 272–284). New York, NY: Routledge/Taylor & Francis Group. [Google Scholar]
  13. Maydeu-Olivares A, & Joe H (2006). Limited information goodness-of-fit testing in multidimensional contingency tables. Psychometrika, 71, 713–732. doi:10.1007/s11336-005-1295-9 [Google Scholar]
  14. Michonski JD, Sharp C, Steinberg L, & Zanarini MC (2013). An item response theoryanalysis of the DSM-IV borderline personality disorder criteria in a population-based sample of 11-to 12-year-old children. Personality Disorders: Theory, Research, and Treatment, 4(1), 15. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Molenaar D, Dolan CV, & de Boeck P (2012). The heteroscedastic graded response model with a skewed latent trait: Testing statistical and substantive hypotheses related to skewed item category functions. Psychometrika, 77, 455–478. doi:10.1007/s11336-012-9273-5 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Muthén LK, & Muthén BO (2016). Mplus: Statistical analysis with latent variables: User’s guide (pp. 1998–2012). Los Angeles: Muthén & Muthén. [Google Scholar]
  17. Reise SP, & Rodriguez A (2016). Item response theory and the measurement of psychiatric constructs: Some empirical and conceptual issues and challenges. Psychological Medicine, 46, 2025–2039. doi:10.1017/S0033291716000520 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Samejima F (1969). Estimation of latent ability using a response pattern of graded scores. Psychometrika Monograph Supplement. 17. [Google Scholar]
  19. Samejima F (2000). Logistic positive exponent family of models: virtue of asymmetric item characteristic curves. Psychometrika, 65, 319–335.
 doi:10.1007/BF02296149 [Google Scholar]
  20. Sass DA, Schmitt TA, & Walker CM (2008). Estimating non-normal latent trait distributions within item response theory using true and estimated item parameters. Applied Measurement in Education, 21, 65–88. doi:10.1080/08957340701796415 [Google Scholar]
  21. Schalet BD, Pilkonis PA, Yu L, Dodds N, Johnston KL, Yount S,… Cella D (2016). Clinical validity of PROMIS depression, anxiety, and anger across diverse clinical samples. Journal of Clinical Epidemiology. doi:10.1016/j.jclinepi.2015.08.036 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Seong TJ (1990). Sensitivity of marginal maximum likelihood estimation of item and ability parameters to the characteristics of the prior ability distributions. Applied Psychological Measurement, 14, 299–311. doi:10.1177/014662169001400307 [Google Scholar]
  23. van der Maas HL, Molenaar D, Maris G, Kievit RA, & Borsboom D (2011). Cognitive psychology meets psychometric theory: on the relation between process models for decision making and latent variable models for individual differences. Psychological Review, 118(2), 339. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Wall MM, Park JY, & Moustaki I (2015). IRT modeling in the presence of zero-inflation with application to psychiatric disorder severity. Applied Psychological Measurement, 1–15. Advance online publication. doi:10.1177/0146621615588184 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Webster GD, & Jonason PK (2013). Putting the “IRT” in “Dirty”: Item Response Theory analyses of the Dark Triad Dirty Dozen—An efficient measure of narcissism, psychopathy, and Machiavellianism. Personality and Individual Differences, 54(2), 302–306. [Google Scholar]
  26. Weekers AM, and Meijer RR (2008). Scaling response processes on personality items using unfolding and dominance models.” European Journal of Psychological Assessment 24, no. 1 (2008): 65–77. [Google Scholar]
  27. Woods CM (2006). Ramsay-curve item response theory (RC-IRT) to detect and correct for nonnormal latent variables. Psychological Methods, 11, 253–270. doi:10.1037/1082-989X.11.3.253 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Woods CM (2015). Estimating the latent density in unidimensional IRT to permit non-normality In Reise SP & Revicki DA (Eds.), Handbook of Item Response Theory Modeling: Applications to Typical Performance Assessment (pp. 60–84). New York, NY: Routledge; /Taylor & Francis Group. [Google Scholar]
  29. Woods CM, & Thissen D (2006). Item response theory with estimation of the latent population distribution using spline-based densities. Psychometrika, 71, 281–301. doi:10.1007/s11336-004-1175-8 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

Supp1

RESOURCES