Skip to main content
Frontiers in Genetics logoLink to Frontiers in Genetics
. 2018 Nov 19;9:554. doi: 10.3389/fgene.2018.00554

Spatially Dense 3D Facial Heritability and Modules of Co-heritability in a Father-Offspring Design

Hanne Hoskens 1,2,*, Jiarui Li 2,3, Karlijne Indencleef 2,4, Dorothy Gors 2,3, Maarten H D Larmuseau 5, Stephen Richmond 6, Alexei I Zhurov 6, Greet Hens 4, Hilde Peeters 1, Peter Claes 2,3,7,*
PMCID: PMC6252335  PMID: 30510565

Abstract

Introduction: The human face is a complex trait displaying a strong genetic component as illustrated by various studies on facial heritability. Most of these start from sparse descriptions of facial shape using a limited set of landmarks. Subsequently, facial features are preselected as univariate measurements or principal components and the heritability is estimated for each of these features separately. However, none of these studies investigated multivariate facial features, nor the co-heritability between different facial features. Here we report a spatially dense multivariate analysis of facial heritability and co-heritability starting from data from fathers and their children available within ALSPAC. Additionally, we provide an elaborate overview of related craniofacial heritability studies.

Methods: In total, 3D facial images of 762 father-offspring pairs were retained after quality control. An anthropometric mask was applied to these images to establish spatially dense quasi-landmark configurations. Partial least squares regression was performed and the (co-)heritability for all quasi-landmarks (∼7160) was computed as twice the regression coefficient. Subsequently, these were used as input to a hierarchical facial segmentation, resulting in the definition of facial modules that are internally integrated through the biological mechanisms of inheritance. Finally, multivariate heritability estimates were obtained for each of the resulting modules.

Results: Nearly all modular estimates reached statistical significance under 1,000,000 permutations and after multiple testing correction (p ≤ 1.3889 × 10-3), displaying low to high heritability scores. Particular facial areas showing the greatest heritability were similar for both sons and daughters. However, higher estimates were obtained in the former. These areas included the global face, upper facial part (encompassing the nasion, zygomas and forehead) and nose, with values reaching 82% in boys and 72% in girls. The lower parts of the face only showed low to moderate levels of heritability.

Conclusion: In this work, we refrain from reducing facial variation to a series of individual measurements and analyze the heritability and co-heritability from spatially dense landmark configurations at multiple levels of organization. Finally, a multivariate estimation of heritability for global-to-local facial segments is reported. Knowledge of the genetic determination of facial shape is useful in the identification of genetic variants that underlie normal-range facial variation.

Keywords: 3D imaging, (co-)heritability, face, ALSPAC, geometric morphometrics, spatially dense, modularity

Introduction

The human face is a complex trait displaying a strong genetic component (Kohn, 1991), as evidenced by remarkable facial similarity between identical twins, clear facial resemblances within families, geographic populations (Hopman et al., 2014) and the sexes (Claes et al., 2012b), and finally the distinctive facial features associated with particular genetic conditions (Hammond, 2007; Baynam et al., 2013). This suggests that inter-individual variation in craniofacial morphology is largely determined by genetic variation, most likely in combination with diverse environmental influences.

Studies on craniofacial heritability provide insight into the relative contribution of genetic versus environmental effects on craniofacial parameters. These studies highlight similarities as well as differences in patterns of inheritance, resulting from differences in the study population (sample size, age of assessment, sex, ethnicity), in the capturing technique (2D or 3D), in the way facial shape is measured and finally in the statistical methods yielded. Most craniofacial heritability studies are performed on twin and family (siblings or parent-offspring) databases and many of these apply 2D imaging techniques, limiting their results due to the loss of information when quantifying the facial phenotype in two dimensions only (Tables 1, 2). More recently, 3D imaging techniques are being used to characterize facial morphology (Tables 13). All studies, 2D as well as 3D, start from sparse descriptions of facial shape using a limited set of landmarks, with one recent exception that uses spatially dense landmarking instead (Tsagkrasoulis et al., 2017). Variation in these landmarks is simplified by the projection of multivariate landmark configurations onto principal components (PC) or by measuring geometric features such as distances, curvature, ratios and/or angles from the landmarks. Subsequently, a heritability score is computed for each individual PC or geometric feature separately. However, any preselection of individual PCs or geometric features fails to capture the full-range of facial variations as combinations of these measurements are not considered. Finally, heritability studies today do not investigate co-inheritance between different facial features.

Table 1.

Literature review of craniofacial heritability – twin studies.

Study sample Measures and Techniques Effect Reference
NUMBER
79 twin pairs (33 MZ, 46 DZ)
SEX DISTRIBUTION
MZ: 17 male, 16 female
DZ: 14 male, 14 female, 18 male-female
AGE DISTRIBUTION
9-16 years
mean 12.1 years
ETHNIC BACKGROUND
Belgium
CAPTURING TECHNIQUE
2D lateral cephalograms
STATISTICAL ANALYSIS
Model-fitting
FACIAL PHENOTYPE
23 measures (linear, angular)
RANGE
45.3–91.2%
GENETIC DETERMINATION
Total anterior facial height (male)
Anterior cranial base
Nasion horizontal
Incisor superior vertical
Incisor inferior vertical
ENVIRONMENTAL INFLUENCE
Saddle angle
Gonial angle
Relative sagittal position of the mandible and maxilla to the anterior cranial base
NOTES
h2: vertical > horizontal measures
Carels et al., 2001

NUMBER
26 twin pairs (10 MZ, 16 DZ)
SEX DISTRIBUTION
MZ: 5 male, 5 female
DZ: 3 male, 7 female, 6 male-female
AGE DISTRIBUTION
6-42 years
Mean 12 years
ETHNIC BACKGROUND
Mixed ethnicity
CAPTURING TECHNIQUE
3D facial scans
STATISTICAL ANALYSIS
Intrapair differences (no h2-values)
FACIAL PHENOTYPE
18 landmarks
28 measures (linear)
Surface measures (curvature)
GENETIC DETERMINATION
Nasal height and width
Left eye width
Intercanthal width
NOTES
h2: vertical > horizontal measures
High h2 for central midfacial structures (triangular area encompassing the nose, orbital rims, intercanthal area)
Naini and Moss, 2004.

NUMBER
20 twin pairs (10 MZ, 10 DZ)
SEX DISTRIBUTION
MZ: 5 male, 5 female
DZ: 3 male, 7 female
AGE DISTRIBUTION
mean 12 years
CAPTURING TECHNIQUE
3D facial scans
STATISTICAL ANALYSIS
Surface shape analysis (no h2-values)
FACIAL PHENOTYPE
Surface measures (curvature)
GENETIC DETERMINATION
Brow ridges
Nasion
Infraorbital margins
ENVIRONMENTAL INFLUENCE
Chin
Cheeks
Lips
Moss, 2006

NUMBER
50 twin pairs (25 MZ, 25 DZ)
SEX DISTRIBUTION
MZ: 13 male, 12 female
DZ: 13 male, 12 female
AGE DISTRIBUTION
13.4–20.1 years
mean 16.4 years
ETHNIC BACKGROUND
Iran
CAPTURING TECHNIQUE
2D lateral cephalograms
STATISTICAL ANALYSIS
classical correlation analysis
FACIAL PHENOTYPE
33 measures (linear, angular)
RANGE
6–162%
GENETIC DETERMINATION
Gonial angle
Saddle angle
Total anterior facial height
Lower anterior facial height
Relative sagittal position of the mandible
Relative sagittal position of the maxilla
Anterior cranial base
ENVIRONMENTAL INFLUENCE
Upper anterior facial height
Mandibular body length
NOTES
h2: vertical > horizontal measures
High heritability found in lower third of the face
Low heritability for dento-alveolar variables
Amini and Borzabadi-Farahani, 2009

NUMBER
21 twin pairs (10 MZ, 11 DZ)
SEX DISTRIBUTION
Same-sex twins
AGE DISTRIBUTION
5–12 years
Mean 9.3 years
ETHNIC BACKGROUND
United States Caucasian
CAPTURING TECHNIQUE
3D facial scans
STATISTICAL ANALYSIS
Classical correlation analysis
FACIAL PHENOTYPE
13 landmarks
17 measures (PC)
RANGE
90–100%
GENETIC DETERMINATION
Breadth of orbital and nasal structures
Nasal length, breadth and projection
Upper lip height and projection
NOTES
High h2 for central midfacial structures
Weinberg et al., 2013

NUMBER
37 twin pairs (19 MZ, 18 DZ)
SEX DISTRIBUTION
MZ: 9 male, 10 female
DZ: 7 male, 3 female, 8 male-female
AGE DISTRIBUTION
15.5 years
ETHNIC BACKGROUND
United Kingdom (ALSPAC)
CAPTURING TECHNIQUE
3D facial scans
STATISTICAL ANALYSIS
Shape analysis (no h2-values)
FACIAL PHENOTYPE
21 landmarks
Surface measures
GENETIC DETERMINATION
Supraorbital and infraorbital ridges
Forehead, lower lip and nasal bridge (males)
Eyes, philtrum and lower part of the cheeks (females)
ENVIRONMENTAL INFLUENCE
Lower third of the face
NOTES
High h2 for central midfacial structures
Djordjevic et al., 2013

NUMBER
141 twin pairs (90 MZ, 51 DZ)
SEX DISTRIBUTION
MZ: 29 male, 61 female
DZ: 20 male, 31 female
AGE DISTRIBUTION
15.3–39.6 years
Mean 21.7 years
ETHNIC BACKGROUND
Lithuania
CAPTURING TECHNIQUE
2D lateral cephalograms
STATISTICAL ANALYSIS
Model-fitting
FACIAL PHENOTYPE
39 measures (linear, angular)
RANGE
20–84%
GENETIC DETERMINATION
Incision inferior to nasion–basion distance
Sagittal position of the mandible
Gonial angle
ENVIRONMENTAL INFLUENCE
Mandibular body length
Ramus width and height
NOTES
h2: horizontal > vertical measures
h2: form (angular) > size (linear)
h2: skeletal > dentoalveolar measures
High h2 for the ‘polygon of the facial profile similarity’ (area determined by the angles SNB, NSAr, ArGoMe)
Šidlauskas et al., 2016

ETHNIC BACKGROUND
United Kingdom (TwinsUK)
CAPTURING TECHNIQUE
3D facial images
STATISTICAL ANALYSIS
model-fitting
FACIAL PHENOTYPE
21 landmarks
210 pairwise distances
RANGE
0–66%
GENETIC DETERMINATION
nasal region
mouth
de Jong et al., 2016.

NUMBER
604 twin pairs (263 MZ, 341 DZ)
172 unpaired twins (75 MZ, 97 DZ)
SEX DISTRIBUTION
Female twins
CAPTURING TECHNIQUE
3D facial images
STATISTICAL ANALYSIS
Classical correlation analysis
RANGE
Facial form (uPC): 38.8–78.5%
Facial shape (sPC): 30.5–84.8%
Djordjevic et al., 2016
AGE DISTRIBUTION
23.6-86.5 years
Mean 58.8 years
ETHNIC BACKGROUND
United Kingdom (TwinsUK)
FACIAL PHENOTYPE
51 landmarks
1317 measures (linear, scaled PCs, unscaled PCs)
GENETIC DETERMINATION
Lips prominence
Inter-ocular distance
Facial size (height)
Nasal width, prominence & height
ENVIRONMENTAL INFLUENCE
Mandibular ramus height
Horizontal facial asymmetry

NUMBER
476 twin pairs (197 MZ, 279 DZ)
SEX DISTRIBUTION
Female twins
AGE DISTRIBUTION
Mean 59.3 years
ETHNIC BACKGROUND
United Kingdom (TwinsUK)
CAPTURING TECHNIQUE
3D facial images
STATISTICAL ANALYSIS
Model-fitting
FACIAL PHENOTYPE
4,096 landmarks
20 distances (Euclidean, Geodesic)
16,384 surface measures (curvature)
RANGE
0–78.9%
GENETIC DETERMINATION
Chin
Nasal region
Nasolabial folds
Nasion
Upper lips
Zygomatic bones
Inner canthi
ENVIRONMENTAL INFLUENCE
Orbits
Lips
Tsagkrasoulis et al., 2017

NUMBER
1,567 individuals
SEX DISTRIBUTION
female twins
AGE DISTRIBUTION
mean 59.3 years
ETHNIC BACKGROUND
United Kingdom (TwinsUK)
CAPTURING TECHNIQUE
3D facial images
STATISTICAL ANALYSIS
classical correlation analysis
FACIAL PHENOTYPE
PCs for orbital and profile subregion
RANGE
76.1–81.5%
GENETIC DETERMINATION
eyes subregion
profile subregion
Crouch et al., 2018

NUMBER
200 twin pairs (37 MZ, 163 DZ)
ETHNIC BACKGROUND
United Kingdom (TwinsUK)
CAPTURING TECHNIQUE
3D facial images
STATISTICAL ANALYSIS
Model-fitting
FACIAL PHENOTYPE
225 measures (coordinates, distances, areas, angles)
RANGE
0–87%
GENETIC DETERMINATION
Area: (left) corner mouth, (left) alae nasi and (left) outer corner eye
Distance: (right) inner corner eye, (left) alae nasi
Area: (left) corner mouth, (left) alae nasi & (left) outer corner eye
x-coordinate: (left) alae nasi
de Jong et al., 2018

NUMBER
26 twin pairs (13 MZ, 13 DZ)
SEX DISTRIBUTION
MZ: 7 male, 6 female
DZ: 7 male, 6 female
AGE DISTRIBUTION
Mean 39 years
ETHNIC BACKGROUND
Korea
CAPTURING TECHNIQUE
2D lateral cephalograms
STATISTICAL ANALYSIS
Classical correlation analysis
FACIAL PHENOTYPE
23 landmarks
47 measures (linear, angular, ratio)
RANGE
-131.8–219%
GENETIC DETERMINATION
Horizontal relationship between maxilla, mandible and anterior cranial base °
Vertical ratios of anterior facial height
shape of cranial base
Location of occlusal plane within skeletal framework
Vertical relationship among cranial base, palatal plane and mandibular plane °
Lower gonial angle
Mandibular body length
ENVIRONMENTAL INFLUENCE
Anterior and posterior facial height
Ramus height
NOTES
h2: horizontal > vertical > mandible > cranial base > dental measures
Kim et al., 2018.

The first column (‘Study sample’) contains information on the study population. The second column (‘Measures and Techniques’) specifies the methodology. The third column (‘Effect’) summarizes the most important findings of the study (see also column 4 ‘Reference’). Heritability was considered to be low if h2< 35% (i.e., ‘environmental influence’) and moderate to high if h2 > 35% and h2 > 65%, respectively (i.e., ‘genetic determination’). MZ, monozygotic; DZ, dizygotic.

Table 2.

Literature review of craniofacial heritability – family studies.

Study sample Measures and techniques Effect Reference
Siblings

NUMBER
138 siblings
SEX DISTRIBUTION
68 males, 70 females
AGE DISTRIBUTION
Mean 23 years
ETHNIC BACKGROUND
Turkey
CAPTURING TECHNIQUE
2D lateral cephalograms
STATISTICAL ANALYSIS
Model-fitting
FACIAL PHENOTYPE
12 measures (soft-tissue, ratio)
RANGE
30–109%
GENETIC DETERMINATION
Total depth index
Soft-tissue chin thickness
Merrifield angle
Holdaway angle
Soft-tissue facial angle
Upper to lower facial height
NOTES
h2: depth > vertical measures
Baydaş et al., 2007

Parent-offspring

NUMBER
363 6-year-olds
182 16-year-olds
SEX DISTRIBUTION
6y: 184 males, 179 females
16y: 97 males, 85 females
ETHNIC BACKGROUND
Iceland
CAPTURING TECHNIQUE
2D lateral cephalograms
STATISTICAL ANALYSIS
Regression analysis
FACIAL PHENOTYPE
22 landmarks
33 measures (linear, angular, ratio)
RANGE
F–S: (6y) -28–62%; (16y) -47–98%
M–S: (6y) -33–65%; (16y) -26–107%
F–D: (6y) -54–77%; (16y) -33–87%
M–D: (6y) -47–83%; (16y) -63–104%
GENETIC DETERMINATION
Position of the lower jaw
Anterior and posterior facial height
Cranial base dimensions
Nasal bone length and prominence
ENVIRONMENTAL INFLUENCE
Dental variables
NOTES
h2: daughters > sons
h2: 16-year-olds > 6-year-olds
Johannsdottir et al., 2005

NUMBER
24 families
AGE DISTRIBUTION
children: 17-35 years
parents: 35-65 years
ETHNIC BACKGROUND
Saudi Arabia
CAPTURING TECHNIQUE
2D lateral cephalograms
STATISTICAL ANALYSIS
regression analysis
FACIAL PHENOTYPE
15 landmarks
28 measures (linear, angular, ratio)
RANGE
F–S: 1–147%
M–S: 2–85%
F–D: 11–118%
M–D: 1–113%
GENETIC DETERMINATION
mandibular variables (°)
facial height dimensions
mandibular body length
NOTES
h2: daughters > sons
h2: father-offspring > mother-offspring
h2: linear > angular measures
h2: mandibular > maxillary variables
AlKhudhairi and AlKofide, 2010

NUMBER
140 individuals from 35 families
AGE DISTRIBUTION
geq 16 years
ETHNIC BACKGROUND
India
CAPTURING TECHNIQUE
2D digital photographs
STATISTICAL ANALYSIS
Correlation analysis (no h2-values)
FACIAL PHENOTYPE
27 measures (linear, ratio)
GENETIC DETERMINATION
Mandibular position
Chin prominence
Nasal prominence & width
Lip length at philtrum
Total facial height
Lip prominence
ENVIRONMENTAL INFLUENCE
Nose and lip form
NOTES
h2: daughters > sons
Lahoti et al., 2013

NUMBER
762 father-offspring pairs
SEX DISTRIBUTION
358 males, 404 females
AGE DISTRIBUTION
Children: 15.5 years
Fathers: 40–75 years
CAPTURING TECHNIQUE
3D facial scans
STATISTICAL ANALYSIS
Multivariate regression analysis
FACIAL PHENOTYPE
7,160 landmarks
63 facial segments
RANGE
Sons: 34–82%
Daughters: 32–72%
GENETIC DETERMINATION
Global face
Upper facial part
Nose
Current study

ETHNIC BACKGROUND
United Kingdom (ALSPAC)
Orbital region
ENVIRONMENTAL INFLUENCE
Cheeks
Small segments around philtrum
NOTES
h2: sons > daughters


Nuclear and extended families

NUMBER
1,918 individuals from 342 families
SEX DISTRIBUTION
Children: 598 males, 464 females
Parents: 390 males, 466 females
AGE DISTRIBUTION
6–72 years
Mean 21.5 years
ETHNIC BACKGROUND
India
CAPTURING TECHNIQUE
Direct anthropometric measurements
STATISTICAL ANALYSIS
Model-fitting
FACIAL PHENOTYPE
23 measures (linear, craniofacial, soft-tissue)
RANGE
25–61%
GENETIC DETERMINATION
Bizygomatic breadth
Nasal breadth and height
Head breadth and length
Facial height
NOTES
h2: craniofacial > linear measures
h2: breadth measures = circumference
Arya et al., 2002

NUMBER
1,406 individuals from 357 families
SEX DISTRIBUTION
733 males, 673 females
AGE DISTRIBUTION
17–90 years
ETHNIC BACKGROUND
Russia
CAPTURING TECHNIQUE
Direct anthropometric measurements
STATISTICAL ANALYSIS
Classical correlation analysis
model-fitting
FACIAL PHENOTYPE
10 measures, 2 latent factors (f)
RANGE
52–72%
GENETIC DETERMINATION
Horizontal component (f)
Bizygomatic breadth
Minimum frontal breadth
Head breadth and length
Vertical component (f)
Nasal height nasion
NOTES
h2: horizontal > vertical measures
Ermakov et al., 2005

NUMBER
298 subjects from 54 families
SEX DISTRIBUTION
127 males, 171 females
CAPTURING TECHNIQUE
3D models of the skull
STATISTICAL ANALYSIS
Model-fitting
RANGE
0–86.7%
GENETIC DETERMINATION
External alveolar breadth
Carson, 2006
AGE DISTRIBUTION
Adults
ETHNIC BACKGROUND
Austria, Hallstatt population
CAPTURING TECHNIQUE
3D models of the skull
STATISTICAL ANALYSIS
Model-fitting
FACIAL PHENOTYPE
58 landmarks
33 measures (linear)
Nasal height
Bimaxillary breadth
Nasion-prosthion height
ENVIRONMENTAL INFLUENCE
Bifrontal breadth
Nasal breadth
Biorbital breadth
NOTES
h2: vertical > horizontal measures
h2: neurocranial > facial measures


NUMBER
1,263 individuals from 373 families
SEX DISTRIBUTION
686 males, 577 females
AGE DISTRIBUTION
18–81 years
ETHNIC BACKGROUND
India
CAPTURING TECHNIQUE
Direct anthropometric measurements
STATISTICAL ANALYSIS
Classical correlation analysis
Model-fitting
FACIAL PHENOTYPE
11 measures, 2 latent factors (f)
RANGE
41–83%
GENETIC DETERMINATION
Nasal height nasion
Vertical head factor (f)
Horizontal head factor (f)
Bizygomatic breadth
Minimum frontal breadth
Physiognomic super facial height
NOTES
h2: horizontal = vertical measures
Karmakar et al., 2007

NUMBER
474 individuals from 119 families
SEX DISTRIBUTION
238 males, 236 females
CAPTURING TECHNIQUE
Direct anthropometric measurements
STATISTICAL ANALYSIS
Classical correlation analysis
Model-fitting
RANGE
52–80%
GENETIC DETERMINATION
Head breadth
Bizygomatic breadth
Jelenkovic et al., 2008

AGE DISTRIBUTION
17–72 years
ETHNIC BACKGROUND
Belgium
FACIAL PHENOTYPE
14 measures, 4 latent factors (f)
Horizontal head factor 1 (f)
Horizontal facial factor (f)
External biocular breadth
Horizontal head factor 2 (f)
Nose breadth
NOTES
h2: horizontal > vertical measures
h2: facial > head phenotypic measures


NUMBER
607 individuals from 90 families
SEX DISTRIBUTION
328 males, 279 females
AGE DISTRIBUTION
13–75.5 years
(observation closest to the participant’s 18th birthday was chosen for analysis)
ETHNIC BACKGROUND
Ohio (Fels Longitudinal Study) European ancestry
CAPTURING TECHNIQUE
2D lateral cephalograms
STATISTICAL ANALYSIS
Model-fitting
FACIAL PHENOTYPE
10 landmarks
10 measures (linear, angular)
RANGE
34–71%
GENETIC DETERMINATION
Anterior basicranial length (S-N)
Sella-vertex (ectocranial)
Basocranial flexion (Ba-S-N°)
Sella-sphenoethmoidale
Facial positioning (S-N-A°)
Total basicranial length (Ba-N)
ENVIRONMENTAL INFLUENCE
Posterior base (Ba-S)
Sherwood et al., 2008

NUMBER
355 subjects
SEX DISTRIBUTION
211 males, 144 females
AGE DISTRIBUTION
Adults
ETHNIC BACKGROUND
Austria, Hallstatt population
CAPTURING TECHNIQUE
3D models of the skull
STATISTICAL ANALYSIS
Model-fitting
FACIAL PHENOTYPE
65 landmarks
58 measures (linear)
RANGE
0–43%
GENETIC DETERMINATION
Nasal height and length
Orbital breadth (frontomalare orbitale)
Zygomatic height
Orbital length
ENVIRONMENTAL INFLUENCE
Nasal breadth
Zygomatic breadth
NOTES
h2: basicranial = neurocranial = facial
Martínez-Abadías et al., 2009

NUMBER
509 individuals from 122 families
SEX DISTRIBUTION
251 males, 258 females
AGE DISTRIBUTION
13–72 years
ETHNIC BACKGROUND
Belgium
CAPTURING TECHNIQUE
Direct anthropometric measurements
STATISTICAL ANALYSIS
Model-fitting
FACIAL PHENOTYPE
18 craniofacial measures (skeletal, soft-tissue)
RANGE
46–72%
GENETIC DETERMINATION
External biocular breadth
Lips height
Head breadth
Minimum frontal breadth
Bigonial breadth
Physiognomic facial height
Bizygomatic breadth
Jelenkovic et al., 2010
NOTES
h2: skeletal > soft-tissue measures


NUMBER
229 individuals from 38 families
SEX DISTRIBUTION
94 males, 135 females
AGE DISTRIBUTION
Children: mean 36.0 years
Parents: mean 55.2 years
ETHNIC BACKGROUND
Korea
CAPTURING TECHNIQUE
2D digital photographs
STATISTICAL ANALYSIS
Model-fitting
FACIAL PHENOTYPE
25 landmarks
14 measures, 3 latent factors (f)
RANGE
25–61%
GENETIC DETERMINATION
Intercanthal width
Lower face portion (f)
Nose width
Orbital region (f)
Vertical length (f)
ENVIRONMENTAL INFLUENCE
Mouth width
Lower facial height
Kim et al., 2013

NUMBER
1,379 individuals from 127 families
SEX DISTRIBUTION
655 males, 724 females
CAPTURING TECHNIQUE
2D lateral cephalograms
STATISTICAL ANALYSIS
Model-fitting
RANGE
10–60%
GENETIC DETERMINATION
Nasion-sella-basion (°)
Pogonion to nasion-basion
Šešelj et al., 2015

AGE DISTRIBUTION
8–95 years
(observation closest to the participant’s 18th birthday was chosen for analysis)
ETHNIC BACKGROUND
Ohio (Fels Longitudinal Study)
European ancestry
FACIAL PHENOTYPE
48 landmarks
75 measures (linear, angular)
Gonial angle
Lower facial height
Sella to nasion
ENVIRONMENTAL INFLUENCE
Molar relation
Palatal plane
Ramus position
Lip protrusion
NOTES
h2: neurocranial > basicranial and facial measures

The first column (‘Study sample’) contains information on the study population. The second column (‘Measures and Techniques’) specifies the methodology. The third column (‘Effect’) summarizes the most important findings of the study (see also column 4 ‘Reference’). Heritability was considered to be low if h2< 35% (i.e., ‘environmental influence’) and moderate to high if h2 > 35% and h2 > 65%, respectively (i.e., ‘genetic determination’). F, father; M, mother; S, son; D, daughter.

Table 3.

Literature review of craniofacial heritability – population studies.

Study sample Measures and techniques Effect Reference
NUMBER
3480 individuals
SEX DISTRIBUTION
44.4% males
55.6% females
AGE DISTRIBUTION
3–21 years
70% in 7–12 age bracket
ETHNIC BACKGROUND
Tanzania
Bantu children, Mwanza region
CAPTURING TECHNIQUE
3D facial scans
STATISTICAL ANALYSIS
Model-fitting (GCTA)
>15 million common SNPs
FACIAL PHENOTYPE
29 landmarks
38 measures (PC, linear, size)
RANGE
28.3–66.9%
GENETIC DETERMINATION
Nasal root shape, mouth width
Total facial width
Allometry
Centroid size
Nasion-midendocanthion distance
Nasal width
Nose width, mandible height
Total facial shape
Midfacial landmark network around nose and mouth
ENVIRONMENTAL INFLUENCE
Upper vermilion height
Nasal width, maxillary prognathism
Lower lip height
Chin height, nasion protrusion
NOTES
h2: horizontal > vertical and depth measures
>90% of the narrow-sense h2 can be explained by common genetic variation
High absolute genetic correlations between most traits: large overlap in underlying genetic loci
Cole et al., 2017

The first column (‘Study sample’) contains information on the study population. The second column (‘Measures and Techniques’) specifies the methodology. The third column (‘Effect’) summarizes the most important findings of the study (see also column 4 ‘Reference’). Heritability was considered to be low if h2< 35% (i.e., ‘environmental influence’) and moderate to high if h2 > 35% and h2 > 65%, respectively (i.e., ‘genetic determination’).

In this work, we present a global-to-local analysis of heritability and co-heritability of multivariate facial modules in a father-offspring design. First, we establish spatially dense quasi-landmarks representing complete facial shape and compute 3D landmark heritability as well as 3D pairwise landmark co-heritability. Subsequently, we apply hierarchical spectral clustering to these values and define 63 modules (multiple 3D landmarks grouped together) of co-inheritance, which when analyzed as groups, provide a multivariate estimation of heritability for various facial segments, ranging from the full face (global) to smaller facial regions (local). Finally, in the discussion we embed our results in an elaborate overview of related craniofacial heritability studies published from the year 2000 and onward (Tables 13).

Materials and Methods

Sample and Recruitment

Data were collected from the Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children (ALSPAC), a UK-based birth cohort study designed to explore genetic and environmental influences on child health and development. In brief, all pregnant women with an expected delivery date between 1 April 1991 and 31 December 1992 inclusive, were eligible to participate in ALSPAC. A total of 14,541 pregnant women were recruited as part of Phase I (Boyd et al., 2013; Fraser et al., 2013). Detailed information and biological samples have been collected from these women and their offspring at various time points. The study website contains details of all the data that is available through a fully searchable data dictionary1.

3D facial surface scans were obtained for 4,731 adolescents at the 15-year-old follow-up clinic. A total of 3,663 fathers, from the ALSPAC cohort, were also invited to take part in the study. Additional phenotypic measures included demographic descriptors (e.g., sex, age, self-reported ethnicity), basic physical characteristics (e.g., height, weight) and information regarding the pregnancy. Children and their corresponding fathers could be linked by a unique pregnancy identifier, which resulted in 992 pairs (979 first and 13 second born). Participants with missing information on sex, age, height, weight (N = 21) and self-reported ethnic background (N = 45) were excluded from this study.

The current study (B2409: “Exploring the heritability of facial features in fathers and offspring using spatially dense geometric morphometrics”) was approved by the ALSPAC Ethics and Law Committee and the Local Research Ethics Committees. Written informed consent was obtained from all children and their fathers in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki.

3D Facial Imaging and Preprocessing

3D facial images were captured with two high-resolution Konica Minolta Vivid (VI900) laser scanners (Konica Minolta Sensing Europe Company, Milton Keynes, United Kingdom). The set of left and right facial images of each scanned subject were processed, registered and merged to generate 3D images of the face (Toma et al., 2008). Prior to the scanning procedure, participants were asked to remain still and present a neutral facial expression.

3D images were imported into an in-house scan cleaning program and hair, ears and any dissociated polygons were removed. Rough facial orientation was established by indicating five crude positioning landmarks, followed by non-rigidly mapping2 an anthropometric mask (AM) onto the images (Snyders et al., 2014; Claes et al., 2018). The AM is a predefined surface template covering the facial area of interest and its mapping results in the standardization of image data from all individuals by creating homologous spatially dense (∼7,160) quasi-landmark configurations (Claes et al., 2012a). Subsequently, generalized Procrustes analysis (GPA) was performed to eliminate differences in position, orientation and size of both original and reflected configurations combined, where the latter could be constructed by changing the sign of the x-coordinate (Claes et al., 2011). The average of an original and its reflected configuration constitutes the symmetric component, while the difference between the two constitutes the asymmetric component. Because faces display bilateral symmetry, aspects of symmetry and asymmetry are preferably considered separately when examining facial shape (Claes et al., 2012b). Although patterns of asymmetry may be informative, in this work we concentrate on the symmetric component only.

Facial Quality Control

Outlier faces were detected by establishing z-scores for each face as described by Claes et al. (2018). Manual inspection of faces with a z-score equal to or larger than 2 led to the removal of imaging and mapping errors (N = 24) and participants displaying non-neutral facial expressions (N = 19) or whose images were obstructed by facial hair (N = 85). A further reduction was done by excluding participants with self-reported non-European ancestry (N = 28) and by randomly selecting one sibling from each multiple pregnancy (N = 8), so that only one child per family was included. A total of 762 father-child pairs were retained for analysis, including 358 sons and 404 daughters. Distribution statistics for age and BMI can be found in Supplementary Table S1 for all three cohorts (sons, daughters, and fathers). Lastly, GPA was applied to superimpose and symmetrize the facial shapes.

Regression-Based Heritability Estimation

Patterns of heritability can be explored from the regression of offspring on parents (Falconer and Mackay, 1996). A multivariate generalization was proposed by Monteiro et al. (2002) based on the Procrustes distance. The Procrustes distance serves as a measure of shape difference and was used to compute a multivariate shape coefficient of determination (R2), which could then be transformed to a regression coefficient, reflecting the shape heritability. An extension for the use of high-dimensional data is provided here. Partial least squares regression (PLSR; function plsregress from Matlab 2017b) was performed to predict facial variation in children given the father’s facial variation. PLSR was preferred for this task because it allows to work with two blocks of multivariate and high-dimensional data. Furthermore, PLSR, in contrast to an ordinary multiple regression, is not constrained by collinearity in the data, which for 3D landmarks is practically always present (Zelditch et al., 2004; Tøndel et al., 2011). In essence, PLSR decomposes the dependent and independent variables into pairs of (unobserved) latent variables by maximizing the covariance between the two, which makes it a better prediction model (Zelditch et al., 2004; Shrimpton et al., 2014). Transformation of the variance explained by the regression model (R2) to a multivariate regression coefficient was done according to Monteiro et al. (2002). Given a one-parent one-offspring design, the heritability can directly be estimated by multiplying the regression coefficient by two (Falconer and Mackay, 1996). Note that the reported R2-coefficient was equal to the multivariate shape coefficient of determination defined in terms of the Procrustes distance.

Facial Segmentation and Modules of Co-inheritance

First, the symmetrized facial shapes were adjusted for the confounding effects of age, sex, and BMI using PLSR (Claes et al., 2018). This was done for fathers and children separately. Next, each quasi-landmark was used as a 3D shape variable [x,y,z] and multivariate heritability estimates were obtained as described in Section “Regression-Based Heritability Estimation.” For both sons and daughters combined, a quasi-landmark of the offspring was regressed on the corresponding as well as all other quasi-landmarks of the father. This was done for all the quasi-landmarks in the offspring and the result was a squared similarity matrix (N × N, with N the number of quasi-landmarks). The heritability of each quasi-landmark was located on the diagonal of this matrix, while the co-inheritance between different quasi-landmarks was located in the off-diagonal elements. The mean squared error (MSE), generated by the function plsregress, was used to evaluate the quality of the PLSR model.

The symmetrized similarity matrix was used as input to perform a hierarchical spectral clustering with five levels. A detailed description of the clustering technique is provided by Claes et al. (2018). Quasi-landmarks with strong co-inheritance were grouped together into a series of facial segments or modules, rather than the clustering of highly correlated quasi-landmarks as observed in the structural segmentation (Claes et al., 2018). In order to assess whether differences existed between both segmentations, we computed the normalized mutual information (NMI). NMI values range from 0 to 1, with high values indicating a substantial overlap between two alternative segmentations (Claes et al., 2018).

All quasi-landmarks of the resulting 63 modules were subjected to a new GPA for both children and fathers combined, thereby creating a shape space for each of the facial modules. These shape spaces were constructed independently of the other modules and their relative positioning within the full face, so that only shape information was retained. Yet, integration of the modules was preserved through the hierarchical construction. Subsequently, each shape space was spanned by an orthogonal basis of PCs and parallel analysis was applied to determine the number of significant PCs contributing to facial shape. In contrast to related work using PCs, the resulting PCs are always used together and never individually, to provide a single multivariate description of modular shape variations.

Facial Heritability Per Module

Sons and daughters were treated separately to estimate the heritability and co-heritability for all facial modules (cf. Regression-Based Heritability Estimation). The multidimensional nature of shape was preserved by performing PLSR on all PCs simultaneously and the quality of the regression model was assessed through the MSE. Labels between fathers and children were randomized and 1,000,000-fold permutation tests were undertaken for all 63 modules to determine any significant differences. The significance threshold correcting for the multiple-testing burden was determined at α = 1.3889 × 10-3 (i.e., 0.05/36), corresponding to an adjustment for the number of effective independent tests. The effective number was computed from the eigenvalues of the correlation matrix containing pairwise multivariate correlations of all 63 modules (Li and Ji, 2005). The reduction in effective tests was expected because of the dependency between neighboring quasi-landmarks and the hierarchical and overlapping construction of the facial modules (Claes et al., 2018). Finally, we experimentally determined the extent to which the heritability estimation is affected by the sample size in our multivariate approach by computing the heritability of randomly generated subsamples of different sizes.

The construction of the modular shape spaces on data of children and fathers altogether allowed us to evaluate whether similar patterns of variation in fathers and children were correlated, e.g., does variation in the nasal breadth of children reflect the same pattern of variation in fathers. When corresponding modules in children–father pairs were considered, shape variations encoded by the extracted latent variables each represented a particular direction within the same shape space. Both directions could be depicted graphically by creating morphs and by plotting the normal displacement map between the upper and lower extremes.

Results

3D Landmark (Co-)heritability and Modules of Co-inheritance

Facial heritability maps of both sons and daughters are shown in Figure 1. As expected, these maps were symmetric and coherent, without abrupt changes in the estimates of neighboring quasi-landmarks. Regions in the face with the greatest heritability included the areas encompassing the nasion and zygomas, as well as the nose and forehead. Differences between both cohorts were found in the lower part of the face. For example, higher estimates were obtained in the chin area for daughters compared with sons, while the latter showed higher heritability estimates in the philtrum area. All MSE values were close to zero (Supplementary Figure S1), yet slightly more variation was observed around the chin, nose and forehead.

FIGURE 1.

FIGURE 1

Facial heritability maps. 3D landmark heritability (%) for sons and daughters as obtained from the regression on fathers. The red-blue spectrum represents regions of high and low heritability, respectively. The maximum value was set to 80% for visualization purposes.

Starting from 3D landmark heritability and 3D pairwise landmark co-heritability (Figure 2) in children, we subdivided facial shape into 63 modules of co-inheritance (Figure 3). First, the midface was separated from the rest of the face and was further partitioned into the philtrum area (quadrant 3, starting at segment 6) and nose (quadrant 4, starting at segment 7). The same was supported by the facial maps of co-heritability (Figures 2A,B,E) in which the nose is presented as an autonomous feature. The remainder of the face was decomposed into the lower facial area (quadrant 1, starting at segment 4) and upper facial area (quadrant 2, starting at segment 5), also promoted by the co-heritability maps in Figures 2A,H. Each segment was repeatedly partitioned into two toward the next level, increasingly focusing on local shape variations. The structural modules are depicted in Supplementary Figure S2 and a substantial overlap between the two alternative segmentations was proven by the high NMI scores (NMIL0 = 1, NMIL1 = 0.76, NMIL2 = 0.84, NMIL3 = 0.77, NMIL4 = 0.76, NMIL5 = 0.73).

FIGURE 2.

FIGURE 2

Facial maps of co-heritability. 3D pairwise landmark co-heritability (%) of the (A) orbital region, (B) forehead, (C) nasion, (D) zygomas, (E) nasal tip (F) upper lip, (G) chin and (H) cheeks. Landmarks of interest are indicated by a black dot, each representing the quasi-landmark in fathers that was used to predict facial variation in children for the corresponding as well as all other quasi-landmarks. The red-blue spectrum represents regions of high and low co-heritability, respectively. The maximum value was set to 70% for visualization purposes.

FIGURE 3.

FIGURE 3

Modules of co-inheritance. Hierarchical facial segmentation of the study cohort, resulting from the grouping of quasi-landmarks with strong co-inheritance (N = 762 father-offspring pairs). Segments are colored in blue. Facial shape variation is covered at five different levels of detail, with global shape variations located in the center (L0) and local shape variations located towards the outer circle (L5).

Modular Heritability

Multivariate estimates of heritability were obtained for all 63 modules in sons and daughters separately (Figure 4). Nearly all modules reached statistical significance (Figure 4 and Supplementary Figure S3), displaying low (<35%) to high (>65%) heritability estimates. The corresponding MSE values are listed in Supplementary Table S2 and stabilization of the estimates is demonstrated in Supplementary Figure S4. Modules of high heritability covered well known facial areas in both cohorts, including the nose, orbital area and upper facial parts. However, higher estimates were obtained in sons compared with daughters. The lower part of the face only showed low to moderate levels of heritability, whether or not reaching the significance threshold (quadrants 1, 3). The highest level of heritability was found for the global face (segment 1), followed by the facial area encompassing the nasion, zygomas and forehead (segment 5). Heritability of the nose was also high, with the nasal bridge being more heritable than the nasal tip and alae nasi. Similar to the landmark-based approach (Figure 1), heritability of the philtrum (segments 6, 12, 24) was lower in daughters compared with sons, whereas modular estimates of the chin (segments 33, 34) were roughly the same in both cohorts.

FIGURE 4.

FIGURE 4

Heritability of different global-to-local parts in the face. Modular heritability estimates (%) for sons and daughters, as obtained from the regression on fathers. Each node corresponds to the facial segments depicted in Figure 3. The red-blue spectrum represents levels of high and low heritability, respectively, and the corresponding values are plotted on top of each node. Black-encircled facial segments had p-values below the significance threshold correcting for the multiple-testing burden (α = 1.3889 × 10-3). All significance tests were based on 1,000,000 permutations.

The first three pairs of latent variables were visualized at the global level in sons (Figure 5), ordered according to the percentage covariation explained. Additional figures representing the corresponding directions in daughters and fathers can be found in Supplementary Figure S5. The first pair included aspects related to facial roundness in both fathers and children (Figure 5A and Supplementary Figure S5A), e.g., a short and round face versus an elongated face. The second and third latent pairs represented variation in the prominence of the midfacial area (Figure 5B and Supplementary Figure S5B), e.g., protrusion versus retrusion, and the angle of the nasal tip and prominence of the chin (Figure 5C and Supplementary Figure S5C), respectively.

FIGURE 5.

FIGURE 5

Global shape variations in offspring. Visualizations of the first three extracted latent variables at the global level in sons. Shape variations in daughters and fathers can be found in Supplementary Figure S5. (A) PLS component 1, (B) PLS component 2, (C) PLS component 3. In gray, illustrations of shape transformation or morph images (±4 standard deviations of the median), representing the direction in shape space encoded by the latent variables. In color, the normal displacement in each quasi-landmark, going from the lower (top) to the upper (bottom) extreme. Blue, inward repression; red, outward protrusion.

Discussion

In this study, we present a novel approach to determine facial heritability and co-heritability in fathers and offspring, starting from 3D spatially dense descriptions of the human face. By combining the co-heritability of neighboring quasi-landmarks, novel phenotypes with particular heritabilities were generated based on the biological mechanisms of inheritance. In addition to the analysis of co-inheritance, a second novel aspect of this work is the multivariate investigation of facial shape at multiple levels of organization. In general, similar patterns of facial features were inherited from the fathers to their 15-year-old offspring, including the global face, upper facial parts (forehead, orbital region, nasion) and the nose. Interestingly, sons showed stronger heritability to their fathers than daughters did at the age of 15.

Multivariate Analysis of Facial Shape

In contrast to previous work where facial shape is only represented using a sparse set of anatomical landmarks (Tables 13), spatially dense representations provide detailed information on the entire geometry of the facial surface (Claes et al., 2012a; Tsagkrasoulis et al., 2017). Moreover, the modularization approach allows focusing on more local shape variations in PCA space. Some previous studies also used PCA (Tables 13), but heritability scores were computed for each of these PCs separately. However, orthogonality in the PCs typically overlooks the underlying covariance structure. By contrast, the full-range of facial variations was retained throughout our analyses by considering all PCs simultaneously in a multivariate regression approach. Furthermore, preselection of facial traits as univariate measurements may introduce artifacts and results need to be interpreted with care. Analysis of heritability in the x-dimension consistently showed higher values of heritability at the midline (Supplementary Figure S6), which were erroneously introduced by aligning the faces at x = 0 during GPA, leading to an almost absence of variation in this region (i.e., order of 10-22). The same can be observed in the work of Tsagkrasoulis et al. (2017), where high levels of heritability seem to overlap with regions of zero-curvature, for example at the nasolabial folds.

Novel Facial Phenotypes Determined From the Patterns of Inheritance

A limitation of the AM mapping in combination with GPA to establish spatially dense configurations, is that quasi-landmarks are positioned in the context of all other quasi-landmarks. Facial areas comprising of densely sampled points, e.g., the cheeks, will hence drive the superimposition. This will also affect the heritability estimates obtained in Figure 1, although highlighted regions in our study still coincided with genetically determined facial traits as previously reported in literature (Tables 13). The modularization approach surpasses this limitation because all modules are subjected to a separate GPA. In this way, only biological shape was captured, independent from its integration within the full face. Yet, information on the integration of facial parts at higher levels was preserved through the hierarchical construction.

The hierarchical clustering approach is data-driven and segmentation of the face was performed on data of sons and daughters combined to increase the number of individuals (N = 762), although differences in patterns of inheritance were observed between the two cohorts. As expected, the degree of overlap between the co-heritability-based and structural segmentation was high because genetic and functional mechanisms are likely to be linked. Interestingly, modules of co-inheritance seem to reflect differences in tissue types. To illustrate, segment 9 coincided with the maxillary bone (Figure 3), whereas the same segment represented the mouth in the structural approach (Supplementary Figure S2). The nasal tip and bridge (segments 57 and 58, respectively) also formed distinct modules, reflecting the underlying bone-cartilage framework. This result is consistent with the one depicted in Figure 2E and imposes well-known difficulties in forensic applications such as craniofacial reconstruction (Claes et al., 2010).

Estimating Different Components of Variance

It is important to realize that heritability is only a descriptive statistic, referring to a particular population under particular conditions (Falconer and Mackay, 1996; AlKhudhairi and AlKofide, 2010). The reported heritability estimates were based on a European cohort and may not generalize to other populations. Various methods have been used to quantify facial heritability and as with other designs based on relatives, genetic contributions computed here are likely to be biased upward due to the environmental sources of covariance, i.e., common environment, providing an upper bound for the heritability. In essence, heritability is always a variable and never an absolute value, hence findings across studies should be interpreted in terms of low, moderate and high heritability only.

Mixed models provide an alternative method to estimate variance components, allowing for interactions between genotype and environment (Visscher et al., 2008; Lange et al., 2016; Mayhew and Meyre, 2017). Moreover, they can handle different types of (genetic) relationships as well. Moving toward the post-GWAS era, heritability studies will likely shift from the classical twin and family designs toward approaches estimating phenotypic variance from genome-wide SNP (i.e., single nucleotide polymorphism) data, as in the recent population study of Cole et al. (2017). In the context of ‘global’ shape heritability, Klingenberg (2003) states that only the GP-1 matrix (where G and P are the genetic and phenotypic covariance matrices, respectively) can be regarded as the multivariate equivalent of the otherwise univariate heritability measure because the spatial structure of variation is ignored when Procrustes distances are used (cf. Monteiro et al., 2002). The latter is only justified if the assumption of model isotropy holds or if the P and G matrices are proportional (Klingenberg and Monteiro, 2005). However, variance component analyses using mixed models are currently difficult to implement for high-dimensional data due to the computational burden. Moreover, patterns of shape variations in fathers correlated well with those predicted in children (Figure 5 and Supplementary Figure S5).

Facial Heritability and Co-heritability of Different Global-to-Local Segments

Low (<35%) to high (>65%) heritability estimates were obtained for different global-to-local parts in the face, ranging from 32 to 82%. As it is difficult to compute the appropriate sample size in our multivariate approach, we ran an analysis to experimentally determine the effect of sample size on the estimates. From Supplementary Figure S4 we can conclude that, for all segments, the heritability converges toward the tail end of the curve and stable estimates are generated.

In contrast to previous parent-offspring studies on craniofacial heritability, higher estimates were obtained in sons compared with daughters for the majority of the facial segments (Table 2). Heritability of the global face was maximal and equal to 82 and 72% in sons and daughters, respectively, higher than the global estimate observed by Cole et al. (2017) (Table 3). Given the remarkable facial similarity between first-degree relatives, such high values are expected. Consistent with the literature, genetic determination was found for midfacial parameters (Tables 13). High heritability was observed for nasal structures in both 3D landmark- and modular-based approaches (Figures 1, 4), as confirmed by previous heritability studies (Tables 13). In particular, there is general agreement on the high heritability of the position of the nasion, which is strongly linked to the PAX3 gene (Liu et al., 2012; Paternoster et al., 2012; Adhikari et al., 2016; Claes et al., 2018). Similar to previous studies who reported high heritability of intercanthal width and other traits related to the orbital region (Tables 13), we also observed high heritability of the corresponding segments (segments 21, 43). Strong genetic control was found for the upper facial part in general (segment 5), encompassing the zygomas and forehead in addition to the nasion and orbital structures. There have been very few studies investigating the heritability of the forehead, mainly because measurements in this region are lacking due to the definition of anatomical landmarks in the midface only. Surface shape analysis in twins revealed intrapair similarities in the region of the forehead (Naini and Moss, 2004; Djordjevic et al., 2013), consistent with the present results and the study of Tsagkrasoulis et al. (2017) (Table 1).

As expected, heritability of the lower parts of the face only ranged from low to moderate. The effect of BMI or facial fatness is mainly located in the areas around the cheek, chin and neck, reflecting the greater environmental component (Shrimpton et al., 2014). Moreover, there is a greater chance of movement of the jaw as well as a greater risk of trauma. The mandible is also influenced by function, e.g., breathing and eating habits (Al Ali et al., 2014a,b, 2015), and possible middle ear infections might interfere with the growth of the mandible (Kaneyama et al., 2008). In this study, low heritability was specifically observed in the small segments around the philtrum (segments 48–55) and cheeks (segment 32), whether or not significant. A number of genes associated with lip morphology have previously been identified (Wilson-Nagrani, 2016), yet differing levels of heritability were reported in literature (Tables 13). Moderate heritability of the mouth (segment 38) was found in this study. Facial segments around the chin area displayed similar levels of moderate heritability in both cohorts (cf. Lahoti et al., 2013; Tsagkrasoulis et al., 2017; Tables 1, 2), whereas 3D landmark heritability in the same region was higher in daughters compared with sons. This inconsistency between the two approaches may be related to the superimposition step as mentioned before, because integration-effects may still be present in the landmark-based approach. In addition, differences in our 15-year-old study cohort can partly be explained by gender-related differences in facial maturation. It is acknowledged that facial maturity develops in women between 12 and 14 years and 2 years later for men, hence male subjects may still be in puberty (Amini and Borzabadi-Farahani, 2009; Šidlauskas et al., 2016). Similarly, facial shape in boys continues to change between ages 12 and 16, mainly involving changes in the area of the chin, nose and supraorbital ridges (Kau and Richmond, 2008; Matthews et al., 2018). Furthermore, contributions of the mothers to the facial features of the offspring would also yield additional information enabling relative parental contributions to the facial shape in their offspring.

The regression approach of Monteiro et al. (2002) is limited in that comparison between modules of different dimensions is not straightforward, even though variation in parental and offspring phenotypes is corrected for. Therefore, multivariate correlation coefficients instead of regression coefficients were used to compute modular co-heritability (Supplementary Figure S7). Given a one-parent one-offspring design, the correlation is the same as the regression when variances in parental and offspring values are equal (Falconer and Mackay, 1996). However, this assumption is often not met. In our example, the moderate to high levels of correlation between the four quadrants (Supplementary Figure S7, level 2) further supported the high level of heritability of the global face.

Heritability Perspective on 3D Facial Shape in Practice

In sharp contrast with the high heritability, little is known on the genetic determinants of particular facial features (Roosenboom et al., 2016). Knowledge of which part of the facial surface is under strong genetic control and which part is mostly influenced by other factors like environmental influences or gene-environment interactions is useful in genetic association studies and allows focusing on those facial parameters displaying a strong genetic component. This can be confirmed by the association study of Claes et al. (2018), where global and local facial patterns of the discovered loci involved modules that are reported here as moderate to highly heritable, e.g., the nose, chin and forehead. Information on modular co-heritability is also useful in the definition of facial phenotypes. The hierarchical clustering approach forces the decomposition of modules into two more localized segments, but preservation of the original segment might be favorable when co-heritability is high. In addition to the use in association studies, the heritability perspective on 3D facial shape is also relevant for a variety of other scientific disciplines, such as anthropology, dysmorphology, ophthalmology, otolaryngology (ENT), orthodontics, craniofacial surgery and forensics.

Conclusion

In conclusion, we here propose a multivariate framework to explore genetic and environmental contributions to facial shape in families (grandparents, parents, and their offspring), which is of interest in a number of fields that deal with craniofacial morphology. Segmentation of the face into modules of co-inheritance allows focusing on global and local aspects of facial variation, demonstrating evidence of high heritability for the global face and for midfacial structures, such as the nasal and orbital region, in both sons and daughters.

Data Availability Statement

The dataset analyzed for this study (B2409: “Exploring the heritability of facial features in fathers and offspring using spatially-dense geometric morphometrics”) can be requested through the ALSPAC website. Please refer to the ALSPAC access policy for further details: https://www.bristol.ac.uk/alspac/researchers/data-access/.

Author Contributions

HH drafted the manuscript and performed all analyses under the supervision of PC, HP, and GH. PC and SR were involved in the initial project design and SR with AZ coordinated the collection of the children’s images. PC, JL, and KI provided input on the application of the analyses. DG conceptualized and implemented the connectome plot containing modular heritability and co-heritability. PC, HP, GH, SR, and ML aided in the interpretation of the data and revised the manuscript. All authors contributed to read and approved the final manuscript.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

We are extremely grateful to all the families who took part in this study, the midwives for their help in recruiting them, and the whole ALSPAC team, which includes interviewers, computer and laboratory technicians, clerical workers, research scientists, volunteers, managers, receptionists, and nurses.

Funding. This work was supported by the Research Program of the Fund for Scientific Research – Flanders (Belgium) (FWO, G078518N), the National Institutes of Health (1-RO1-DE027023), and the Research Fund KU Leuven (BOF-C1, C14/15/081). The UK Medical Research Council (MRC) and Wellcome Trust (102215/2/13/2) and the University of Bristol provide core support for ALSPAC. A comprehensive list of grants funding is available on the ALSPAC website. Funding for the collection of 3D face shape scans was specifically provided by the MRC and Wellcome Trust (092731) and the University of Cardiff. HP is a Senior Clinical Investigator of The Research Foundation – Flanders (FWO). This publication is the work of the authors and they will serve as guarantors for the contents of this paper.

2

The facial mapping software is available at https://github.com/TheWebMonks/meshmonk, free to use for academic purposes.

Supplementary Material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fgene.2018.00554/full#supplementary-material

References

  1. Adhikari K., Fuentes-Guajardo M., Quinto-Sánchez M., Mendoza-Revilla J., Camilo Chacón-Duque J., Acuña-Alonzo V., et al. (2016). A genome-wide association scan implicates DCHS2, RUNX2, GLI3, PAX1 and EDAR in human facial variation. Nat. Commun. 7:11616. 10.1038/ncomms11616 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Al Ali A., Richmond S., Popat H., Playle R., Pickles T., Zhurov A. I., et al. (2015). The influence of snoring, mouth breathing and apnoea on facial morphology in late childhood: a three-dimensional study. BMJ Open 5:e009027. 10.1136/bmjopen-2015-009027 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Al Ali A., Richmond S., Popat H., Toma A. M., Playle R., Pickles T., et al. (2014a). A three-dimensional analysis of the effect of atopy on face shape. Eur. J. Orthod. 36 506–511. 10.1093/ejo/cjs107 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Al Ali A., Richmond S., Popat H., Toma A. M., Playle R., Zhurov A. I., et al. (2014b). The influence of asthma on face shape: a three-dimensional study. Eur. J. Orthod. 36 373–380. 10.1093/ejo/cjs067 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. AlKhudhairi T. D., AlKofide E. A. (2010). Cephalometric craniofacial features in Saudi parents and their offspring. Angle Orthod. 80 1010–1017. 10.2319/050410-66 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Amini F., Borzabadi-Farahani A. (2009). Heritability of dental and skeletal cephalometric variables in monozygous and dizygous Iranian twins. Orthod. Waves 68 72–79. 10.1016/j.odw.2009.01.001 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  7. Arya R., Duggirala R., Comuzzie A. G., Puppala S., Modem S., Busi B. R., et al. (2002). Heritability of anthropometric phenotypes in caste populations of visakhapatnam, India. Hum. Biol. 74 325–344. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Baydaş B., Erdem A., Yavuz İ., Ceylan İ. (2007). Heritability of facial proportions and soft-tissue profile characteristics in Turkish Anatolian siblings. Am. J. Orthod. Dentofacial Orthop. 131 504–509. 10.1016/j.ajodo.2005.05.055 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Baynam G., Walters M., Claes P., Kung S., LeSouef P., Dawkins H., et al. (2013). The facial evolution: looking backward and moving forward. Hum. Mutat. 34 14–22. 10.1002/humu.22219 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Boyd A., Golding J., Macleod J., Lawlor D. A., Fraser A., Henderson J., et al. (2013). Cohort profile: the ‘children of the 90s’—the index offspring of the avon longitudinal study of parents and children. Int. J. Epidemiol. 42 111–127. 10.1093/ije/dys064 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Carels C., Van Cauwenberghe N., Savoye I., Willems G., Loos R., Derom C., et al. (2001). A quantitative genetic study of cephalometric variables in twins. Clin. Orthod. Res. 4 130–140. 10.1034/j.1600-0544.2001.040303.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Carson E. A. (2006). Maximum likelihood estimation of human craniometric heritabilities. Am. J. Phys. Anthropol. 131 169–180. 10.1002/ajpa.20424 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Claes P., Roosenboom J., White J. D., Swigut T., Sero D., Li J., et al. (2018). Genome-wide mapping of global-to-local genetic effects on human facial shape. Nat. Genet. 50 414–423. 10.1038/s41588-018-0057-4 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Claes P., Vandermeulen D., De Greef S., Willems G., Clement J. G., Suetens P. (2010). Bayesian estimation of optimal craniofacial reconstructions. Forensic Sci. Int. 201 146–152. 10.1016/j.forsciint.2010.03.009 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Claes P., Walters M., Clement J. (2012a). Improved facial outcome assessment using a 3D anthropometric mask. Int. J. Oral Maxillofac. Surg. 41 324–330. 10.1016/j.ijom.2011.10.019 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Claes P., Walters M., Shriver M. D., Puts D., Gibson G., Clement J., et al. (2012b). Sexual dimorphism in multiple aspects of 3D facial symmetry and asymmetry defined by spatially dense geometric morphometrics. J. Anat. 221 97–114. 10.1111/j.1469-7580.2012.01528.x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Claes P., Walters M., Vandermeulen D., Clement J. G. (2011). Spatially-dense 3D facial asymmetry assessment in both typical and disordered growth. J. Anat. 219 444–455. 10.1111/j.1469-7580.2011.01411.x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Cole J. B., Manyama M., Larson J. R., Liberton D. K., Ferrara T. M., Riccardi S. L., et al. (2017). Human facial shape and size heritability and genetic correlations. Genetics 205 967–978. 10.1534/genetics.116.193185 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Crouch D. J. M., Winney B., Koppen W. P., Christmas W. J., Hutnik K., Day T., et al. (2018). Genetics of the human face: identification of large-effect single gene variants. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 115 E676–E685. 10.1073/pnas.1708207114 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. de Jong M. A., Hysi P., Spector T., Niessen W., Koudstaal M. J., Wolvius E. B., et al. (2018). Ensemble landmarking of 3D facial surface scans. Sci. Rep. 8:12. 10.1038/s41598-017-18294-x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. de Jong M. A., Wollstein A., Ruff C., Dunaway D., Hysi P., Spector T., et al. (2016). An automatic 3d facial landmarking algorithm using 2d gabor wavelets. IEEE Trans. Image Process. 25 580–588. 10.1109/TIP.2015.2496183 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Djordjevic J., Jadallah M., Zhurov A. I., Toma A. M., Richmond S. (2013). Three-dimensional analysis of facial shape and symmetry in twins using laser surface scanning. Orthod. Craniofac. Res. 16 146–160. 10.1111/ocr.12012 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Djordjevic J., Zhurov A. I., Richmond S. (2016). Genetic and environmental contributions to facial morphological variation: a 3d population-based twin study. PLoS One 11:e0162250 10.1371/journal.pone.016225 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Ermakov S., Kobyliansky E., Livshits G. (2005). Quantitative genetic study of head size related phenotypes in ethnically homogeneous Chuvasha pedigrees. Ann. Hum. Biol. 32 585–598. 10.1080/03014460500247972 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Falconer D. S., Mackay T. F. C. (1996). Introduction to Quantitative Genetics 4th Edn. Harlow: Longman Group Ltd. [Google Scholar]
  26. Fraser A., Macdonald-Wallis C., Tilling K., Boyd A., Golding J., Davey Smith G., et al. (2013). Cohort profile: the avon longitudinal study of parents and children: ALSPAC mothers cohort. Int. J. Epidemiol. 42 97–110. 10.1093/ije/dys066 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Hammond P. (2007). The use of 3D face shape modelling in dysmorphology. Arch. Dis. Child. 92 1120–1126. 10.1136/adc.2006.103507 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Hopman S. M. J., Merks J. H. M., Suttie M., Hennekam R. C. M., Hammond P. (2014). Face shape differs in phylogenetically related populations. Eur. J. Hum. Genet. 22 1268–1271. 10.1038/ejhg.2013.289 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Jelenkovic A., Poveda A., Susanne C., Rebato E. (2008). Contribution of genetics and environment to craniofacial anthropometric phenotypes in belgian nuclear families. Hum. Biol. 80 637–654. 10.3378/1534-6617-80.6.637 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Jelenkovic A., Poveda A., Susanne C., Rebato E. (2010). Common genetic and environmental factors among craniofacial traits in Belgian nuclear families: comparing skeletal and soft-tissue related phenotypes. J. Comp. Hum. Biol. 61 191–203. 10.1016/j.jchb.2009.10.003 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Johannsdottir B., Thorarinsson F., Thordarson A., Magnusson T. E. (2005). Heritability of craniofacial characteristics between parents and offspring estimated from lateral cephalograms. Am. J. Orthod. Dentofacial Orthop. 127 200–207. 10.1016/j.ajodo.2004.07.033 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Kaneyama K., Segami N., Hatta T. (2008). Congenital deformities and developmental abnormalities of the mandibular condyle in the temporomandibular joint. Congenit. Anom. 48 118–125. 10.1111/j.1741-4520.2008.00191.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Karmakar B., Ermakov S., Yakovenko K., Kobyliansky E. (2007). Genetic determination of head-size-related anthropometric traits in an ethnically homogeneous sample of 373 indian pedigrees of West Bengal. Hum. Biol. 79 501–514. 10.1353/hub.2008.0007 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Kau C. H., Richmond S. (2008). Three-dimensional analysis of facial morphology surface changes in untreated children from 12 to 14 years of age. Am. J. Orthod. Dentofacial Orthop. 134 751–760. 10.1016/j.ajodo.2007.01.037 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Kim E., Sung J., Song Y.-M., Chae H. S., Mo S.-S., Kim Y. H., et al. (2018). Heritability of facial skeletal and dental characteristics of monozygotic and dizygotic twins using cephalometric analysis and falconer’s method. J. Craniofac. Surg. 29 e274–e279. 10.1097/SCS.0000000000004313 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Kim H.-J., Im S.-W., Jargal G., Lee S., Yi J.-H., Park J.-Y., et al. (2013). Heritabilities of facial measurements and their latent factors in korean families. Genomics Inform. 11:83. 10.5808/GI.2013.11.2.83 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Klingenberg C. P. (2003). Quantitative genetics of geometric shape: heritability and the pitfalls of the univariate approach. Evolution 57 191–195. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Klingenberg C. P., Monteiro L. R. (2005). Distances and directions in multidimensional shape spaces: implications for morphometric applications. Syst. Biol. 54 678–688. 10.1080/10635150590947258 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Kohn L. A. P. (1991). The role of genetics in craniofacial morphology and growth. Annu. Rev. Anthropol. 20 261–278. [Google Scholar]
  40. Lahoti S. K., Karia A. M., Lahoti K. B. (2013). Heritability of facial characteristics between parents and offsprings: a photographic study. J. Indian Orthod. Soc. 47 419–425. 10.5005/jp-journals-10021-1199 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  41. Lange K., Cantor R., Horvath S., Papp J. C., Sabatti C., Sinsheimer J. S., et al. (2016). Analysis option 19: variance components (Polygenic and QTL Mapping) in Mendel 16.0 Documentation. 209–218. [Google Scholar]
  42. Li J., Ji L. (2005). Adjusting multiple testing in multilocus analyses using the eigenvalues of a correlation matrix. Heredity 95 221–227. 10.1038/sj.hdy.6800717 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Liu F., Lijn F., van der, Schurmann C., Zhu G., Chakravarty M. M., Hysi P. G.et al. (2012). A genome-wide association study identifies five loci influencing facial morphology in Europeans. PLoS Genet 8:e1002932. 10.1371/journal.pgen.1002932 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Martínez-Abadías N., Esparza M., Sjøvold T., González-José R., Santos M., Hernández M. (2009). Heritability of human cranial dimensions: comparing the evolvability of different cranial regions. J. Anat. 214 19–35. 10.1111/j.1469-7580.2008.01015.x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Matthews H. S., Penington A. J., Hardiman R., Fan Y., Clement J. G., Kilpatrick N. M., et al. (2018). Modelling 3D craniofacial growth trajectories for population comparison and classification illustrated using sex-differences. Sci. Rep. 8:4771. 10.1038/s41598-018-22752-5 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Mayhew A. J., Meyre D. (2017). Assessing the heritability of complex traits in humans: methodological challenges and opportunities. Curr. Genomics 18 332–340. 10.2174/1389202918666170307161450 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Monteiro L. R., Diniz-Filho J. A., dos Reis S. F., Araújo E. D. (2002). Geometric estimates of heritability in biological shape. Evolution 56 563–572. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Moss J. (2006). The use of three-dimensional imaging in orthodontics. Eur. J. Orthod. 28 416–425. 10.1093/ejo/cjl025 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Naini F. B., Moss J. P. (2004). Three-dimensional assessment of the relative contribution of genetics and environment to various facial parameters with the twin method. Am. J. Orthod. Dentofacial Orthop. 126 655–665. 10.1016/j.ajodo.2003.08.034 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Paternoster L., Zhurov I., Toma A. M., Kemp J. P., St. Pourcain B., Timpson N. J., et al. (2012). Genome-wide association study of three-dimensional facial morphology identifies a variant in PAX3 associated with nasion position. Am. J. Hum. Genet 90 478–485. 10.1016/j.ajhg.2011.12.021 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Roosenboom J., Hens G., Mattern B. C., Shriver M. D., Claes P. (2016). Exploring the underlying genetics of craniofacial morphology through various sources of knowledge. Biomed. Res. Int. 2016:3054578. 10.1155/2016/3054578 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Šešelj M., Duren D. L., Sherwood R. J. (2015). Heritability of the human craniofacial complex. Anat. Rec. 298 1535–1547. 10.1002/ar.23186 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Sherwood R. J., Duren D. L., Demerath E. W., Czerwinski S. A., Siervogel R. M., Towne B. (2008). Quantitative genetics of modern human cranial variation. J. Hum. Evol. 54 909–914. 10.1016/j.jhevol.2008.02.009 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Shrimpton S., Daniels K., de Greef S., Tilotta F., Willems G., Vandermeulen D., et al. (2014). A spatially-dense regression study of facial form and tissue depth: towards an interactive tool for craniofacial reconstruction. Forensic Sci. Int. 234 103–110. 10.1016/j.forsciint.2013.10.021 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Šidlauskas M., Šalomskienė L., Andriuškevičiûtė I., Šidlauskienė M., Labanauskas Ž, Vasiliauskas A.et al. (2016). Heritability of mandibular cephalometric variables in twins with completed craniofacial growth. Eur. J. Orthod. 38 493–502. 10.1093/ejo/cjv062 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Snyders J., Claes P., Vandermeulen D., Suetens P. (2014). Development and Comparison of Non-Rigid Surface Registration Algorithms and Extensions. Leuven: ESAT. [Google Scholar]
  57. Toma A. M., Zhurov A., Playle R., Richmond S. (2008). A three-dimensional look for facial differences between males and females in a British-Caucasian sample aged 15½years old. Orthod. Craniofac. Res. 11 180–185. 10.1111/j.1601-6343.2008.00428.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Tøndel K., Indahl U. G., Gjuvsland A. B., Vik J., Hunter P., Omholt S. W., et al. (2011). Hierarchical Cluster-based Partial Least Squares Regression (HC-PLSR) is an efficient tool for metamodelling of nonlinear dynamic models. BMC Syst. Biol. 5:90. 10.1186/1752-0509-5-90 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Tsagkrasoulis D., Hysi P., Spector T., Montana G. (2017). Heritability maps of human face morphology through large-scale automated three-dimensional phenotyping. Sci. Rep. 7:45885. 10.1038/srep45885 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Visscher P. M., Hill W. G., Wray N. R. (2008). Heritability in the genomics era – concepts and misconceptions. Nat. Rev. Genet. 9 255–266. 10.1038/nrg2322 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Weinberg S. M., Parsons T. E., Marazita M. L., Maher B. S. (2013). Heritability of face shape in twins: a preliminary study using 3D stereophotogrammetry and geometric morphometrics. Dent 3000 7–11. 10.5195/D3000.2013.14 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Wilson-Nagrani C. E. (2016). Matching Genotype to Phenotype in a Detailed Assessment of Lip Morphology. Ph.D. thesis, Cardiff University; Wales [Google Scholar]
  63. Zelditch M. L., Swiderski D. L., Sheets H. D., Fink W. L. (eds) (2004). Geometric Morphometrics for Biologists: A Primer. London: Elsevier Academic Press. [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

Data Availability Statement

The dataset analyzed for this study (B2409: “Exploring the heritability of facial features in fathers and offspring using spatially-dense geometric morphometrics”) can be requested through the ALSPAC website. Please refer to the ALSPAC access policy for further details: https://www.bristol.ac.uk/alspac/researchers/data-access/.


Articles from Frontiers in Genetics are provided here courtesy of Frontiers Media SA

RESOURCES