Abstract
Background:
Stress poses a major issue in our modern society, making restoration an important research focus. Restoration likelihood has mostly been observed in nature, which was compared with urban environments that have little restorative potential, eg, industrial areas. However, many people reside in and need to find restoration in cities. The main aim of this review is to summarize research that has focused on investigating restoration possibilities in urban environments and the environmental elements interacting with the restoration likelihood of an urban environment.
Method:
This review focuses on studies addressing the topic of restoration possibilities in urban settings in built and human-made natural urban environments. The studies were searched via Google Scholar, PsycINFO, PsycARTICLES, and PSYNDEX. All studies concerned with restoration in urban environments were included. However, studies concerned with nonoriginal data, solely investigating effects of natural environments or treating urban environments as a control for restoration in nature, were excluded from the review. Overall, 39 studies corresponded to the criteria and were included.
Results:
Natural elements in urban environments have a restorative potential and can increase the restorativeness of urban settings. Furthermore, built urban environments vary in their restorative potential, but promising results have been uncovered as well. Architectural elements, cultural, and leisure areas had a restorative value, whereas the findings on streets and residential areas differ. In sum, many urban locations can have restorative effects, but these effects may be influenced by factors such as cultural background, age, social components, and individual dispositions.
Discussion:
Certain urban environments hold a restorative potential. However, the literature on restoration in urban environments is still quite scarce and therefore has been of little practical use. Even though applying the findings to real-life environments is desirable, it might prove difficult, considering the overall sparse evidence. More research on the predictors of restoration likelihood (eg, social factors), generational and cultural differences, and comparisons between natural and urban environments is recommended.
Keywords: Restoration, perceived restorativeness, urban environments, built environments, urban nature
Introduction
Stress has been identified as a major health risk in industrialized societies and is directly and indirectly responsible for immense costs to health systems and the economy.1 Consequently, strategies for avoiding stress—eg, improved working conditions and promoting a healthier lifestyle—have received increasing interest in many countries and companies. However, given the many potentially competing interests—eg, short-term vs long-term profits, public health expenditure vs taxes, working more for higher income vs leisure activities—it remains unclear to what extent and when such prevention-focused efforts will be successful. For the time being, identifying effective ways to recover from stress remains at least an equally important task.
On one hand, environmental factors such as noise can have a massively negative impact on physiological and psychological well-being.2 On the other hand, there is widespread agreement that certain features of the environment may also increase resilience or foster restoration.
Joye and van den Berg3(p58) define restoration as “the experience of a psychological and/or physiological recovery process that is triggered by particular environments and environmental configurations.” Most research on restoration likelihood suggests that natural environments hold a much higher restoration potential than urban environments.4,5 Explanations for these findings typically take an evolutionary point of view and range from the absence of information-processing in nature and a reduction in (physiological) arousal to being away from one’s everyday life and the recovery of attention.6,7
Urbanization poses new challenges: currently, more people reside in cities than in rural regions, and many lead stressful lives.8 Natural, rural environments are often not feasible options for city dwellers seeking recovery. Therefore, it is necessary to identify restoration possibilities in their everyday lives, eg, after work or school. In this review, we address the question, if and where restoration can be found in urban environments by summarizing the results of studies concerned with this question.
Concepts and Theories
Although this article focuses on the empirical evidence regarding urban restoration, some theoretical background is needed in order to understand the approaches taken in some of the studies. There are 2 major theories in restoration research: Attention Restoration Theory and Psycho-Evolutionary Theory of Stress Reduction.5,7 Attention Restoration Theory conceptualizes restoration as a recovery from mental fatigue fostered by an absence of directed attention. According to Kaplan and Kaplan, 4 environmental qualities can facilitate involuntary attention: (1) fascination, describing the environment’s ability to hold involuntary attention; (2) being away, referring to the feeling of being freed of directed attention and everyday life; (3) extent, as in a rich and coherent environment that creates a world of its own; and (4) compatibility, referring to the fit between the environment and a person’s intentions.7,9
In contrast, Ulrich6 and Ulrich et al5 conceptualized stress, not attention fatigue, as the source for need for restoration. The Psycho-Evolutionary Theory of Stress Reduction claims that emotions, rather than cognitions, have an impact on restoration.5,6
Both theoretical approaches, however, have certain limitations and are disputed in the field of environmental psychology.10 For example, Ulrich et al5 criticized the lack of emotional components in Attention Restoration Theory and its strong focus on rather complex cognitive appraisal mechanisms. In a way, Stress Reduction Theory mirrors Attention Restoration Theory’s deficit: the latter’s focus on emotional states and stress relief goes along only marginally with the cognitive processes being addressed. Moreover, the theories’ stances on basic psychological insights, such as the distinction between cognitive, behavioral, emotional and physiological levels of consideration, and their possible decoupling remain rather ambiguous. Most importantly, concerning the scope of this review, both Attention Restoration Theory and Stress Reduction Theory largely disregard and partly challenge the restorative potential of urban environments.5,9 Despite their different perspectives, both Attention Restoration Theory and Stress Reduction Theory presume that humans have been shaped by their interaction with natural environments in the course of evolution. This idea is accompanied by a general skepticism concerning the restorative value of nonnatural, human-made environments. This conceptual fixation on natural environments may have partly inhibited unbiased and rigorous comparisons to urban environments.
For a detailed description of the major theories, namely, Attention Restoration Theory7,9 and Psycho-Evolutionary Theory of Stress Reduction,5,6 please refer to the original articles or to the work by Staats11 for an overview.
Survey Methodology
Research questions
In this review, we aim to answer the following research questions:
Do urban environments have a restorative potential?
Which urban environments are perceived as restorative?
Which (environmental) elements interact with the restorative potential of a place?
Literature search
To answer these questions, we searched for articles concerned with restoration in urban environments. (Unfortunately, no review protocol is available.). The articles reviewed were researched via Google Scholar, PsycINFO, PsycARTICLES, and PSYNDEX. The following terms were used for this search: “architecture,” “built environment,” “environmental planning,” “health,” “perceived restorativeness,” “recreation areas,” “restoration,” “restoration likelihood,” “restoration possibilities,” and “urban environment.” The Journal of Environmental Psychology as well as Environment and Behavior were investigated issue by issue for additional articles concerning the topics reviewed individually. Furthermore, we visited the websites of those researchers, whose studies had been of interest for the review to find further studies, conference presentations, etc. If we found a review concerned with restoration in general, we checked which articles the authors had reviewed and if any of them met our inclusion criteria. Because we found few studies concerned with the relationship between restoration likelihood and built urban features, we expanded our search to human-made natural elements in urban contexts. Therefore, we included the terms “urban greening,” “urban green environments,” “urban green spaces,” “urban forestry,” “urban parks” and the terms for the 4 restorative qualities proposed by Kaplan9 “being away,” “compatibility,” “extent,” and “fascination”. The literature search ended in September 2018.
Inclusion and exclusion criteria
All studies on restoration possibilities in urban environments and, in particular, urban features holding a restoration potential that were found during the search were included in this review, regardless of method, publication status, and sample size. Reason for this decision was the overall sparse literature on restoration in urban environments. Studies concerning nature in urban environments were included if the environment was human-made, including city parks and city greening. For a study to be considered, the outcome criterium had to be the (perceived) restorativeness of a place in an urban environment. Unfortunately, only studies published in English could be considered. Therefore, it is possible that studies published in other languages were missed. Furthermore, the threat of publication bias is also present. However, we made an effort to find unpublished manuscripts or conference papers by scanning publication lists of authors we cited. Studies using a different restoration concept than psychological restoration, such as restoration of fish or plant population, were excluded after initial screening (21 studies). Studies that only addressed the restorative effects of nature, comparing nature to urban settings or solely investigating preferences for urban locations, were not taken into consideration. Articles focusing on rural environments, forests, green areas near cities, or other nonurban components or articles concerned with nonoriginal data (eg, reviews), or methodological papers were excluded after further reading as well (87 studies). We provide a table of the reviewed studies with each study’s topic, the sample size, the study’s design, the results, and country of conduction in Appendix 1. In total, 39 studies were included in the review. For a detailed breakdown of the literature research and the review criteria, see the PRISMA flow diagram in Figure 1 and the PRISMA checklist provided in the supplementary materials.12
Empirical Research on Urban Components
In the studies identified for this review, 2 broad categories of urban components with restorative value can be differentiated: natural elements in urban environments and aspects of the built environment itself.
The restorative effects of natural elements in the urban environment
Research on psychological restoration has focused on specific aspects of nature as opportunities for restoration and, as far as empirical findings are available; they suggest that natural environments provide better restoration than urban environments. Thus, it is not surprising that most studies agree that natural and naturalistic elements in an urban context—such as trees in gardens, grass patches and flowers growing along streets, or parks—exert restorative effects, as well.8,13–34
Perceived restoration in parks and botanical gardens is high, as stated for example by Staats et al8 and Carrus et al,13 and the restorative effect of playgrounds and school campuses can be increased by the presence of natural elements, as was found in studies by Bagot et al32 and Mejía-Castillo et al.27 Even relatively small patches of greenery make a difference: Hernández and Hidalgo14 showed that ratings on the restoration likelihood of various urban environments, eg, streets, buildings, or even industrial zones, increased if natural elements were present. Similar findings were reported by Lindal and Hartig15 using computer-generated streetscapes. Even flowering meadow green roofs,16 views of nature from windows, indoor plants, gardens, and proximity to green spaces in an urban neighborhood can have positive effects on cognitive and affective components of restorative experiences.17–19 A qualitative study from Norway by Nordh et al20 indicated that cemeteries can have restorative effects as well, possibly due to the fact that cemeteries in Northern Europe are usually well-maintained and park like green areas in the cities.
In line with Attention Restoration Theory7,9 and Psycho-Evolutionary Theory of Stress Reduction,5,6 the above findings point to a general restorative effect of natural and naturalistic elements in urban environments. However, many details remain unclear. For instance, Tenngart Ivarsson and Hagerhall19 found that 2 well-tended, attractive gardens can be rated quite differently in respect to their perceived restorativeness. There is also one study by Galindo and Hidalgo35 in which natural elements did not contribute to restoration likelihood in urban spaces. Consequently, in recent years, several studies have focused on identifying the specific factors underlying the restorative effect.
Nordh et al21 discovered that the restorative potential of parks was mediated by 2 of the 4 restorative qualities postulated by the Attention Restoration Theory,9 being away and fascination. Grass, amount of trees and bushes, as well as park size predicted restoration likelihood. In the already mentioned study by Lindal and Hartig,15 similar effects were found with computer-generated images: being away mediated the effect of grass and number of flowers on restoration likelihood, and fascination mediated the effect of tree arrangement, and presence of flowers on restoration likelihood. A study using an eye-tracking paradigm by Nordh et al22 showed which features in photos of urban parks participants focused on for rating restoration likelihood. The participants looked at trees, benches, and bushes. However, no relationship between the natural elements of the park and restoration likelihood could be found, except for a correlation with grass. In another study by Tyrväinen et al23 participants took a walk in 3 different urban locations, a constructed park, an urban woodland, and a built urban environment (city center) in Helsinki, Finland. They rated the perceived restorativeness highest for the woodland, but physiological measures of stress relief showed no difference between the 3 settings.
Nordh and Østby24 asked participants to rate the restoration potential of parks with different features. Participants tended to rate parks with many natural features like grass and water as most restorative and parks with many disturbances (eg, traffic) and little vegetation as least restorative. Participants used parks to philosophize and for social activities. Similar results with a Chinese sample were contributed by Wang et al.25
Nordh33 compared 3 different ways of measuring restoration in urban parks: ratings of environments based on quantified photos, eye-tracking, and choice-based conjoint analysis. With all 3 methods, she found that participants were able to distinguish between different elements of parks or were able to imagine the park setting. All 3 methods were able to provide insights into the restorative potential of parks.
Hipp et al26 conducted an online study on campus green spaces. Perceived restorativeness of the campus correlated with perceived campus greenness. Furthermore, perceived restorativeness mediated the relationship between perceived greenness and perceived quality of life, lending support to the restoration concept. However, the researchers themselves state that their findings might be hard to generalize to nonstudent populations. Similar results with school green spaces were contributed by Mejía-Castillo et al.27 They found schools with a medium to large amount of green spaces to be more restorative, more fascinating, more likely to have extent and to create a sense of being away than schools without green spaces.
Jiang et al30 found tree cover density in an urban neighborhood to have an effect on stress recovery for male participants, but not for female participants. For the male participants, recovery increased with tree cover density in the lower density range, then stagnated with medium levels, and increased again but slower with high density levels. In a second study, Jiang et al31 found the relationship between self-reported stress recovery and tree cover density to be linear, regardless of age, gender, and stress level.
Grahn and Stigsdotter34 asked participants in their study, which elements of nature they preferred and found that a combination of different aspects—refuge, nature, and diversity of species—could increase restoration in urban settings. Furthermore, they found that a low presence of social stimuli could also contribute to a restorative experience. Scopelliti et al28 came to similar results, as they found that people engaging in environmental in contrast to social activities experienced more restoration. Moreover, in their study, the urban green area with the highest biodiversity received the highest rating on perceived restorativeness. In another study from that group, Carrus et al29 asked residents of 4 Italian cities about their restorative experiences in 4 different urban locations, which varied in location (urban vs peri-urban) and biodiversity (low vs high): an urban square with trees, an urban park, a pinewood forest plantation, and a protected reserve. The peri-urban locations as well as the locations higher in biodiversity were perceived as more restorative than the urban locations or the locations with lower biodiversity, respectively. Furthermore, high biodiversity showed a stronger link to perceived restorativeness for urban compared with peri-urban environments.
Taken together, even little amounts of vegetation in urban environments seem to be sufficient for a restorative experience, with only 1 study producing negative results. Trees, flowers, and grass patches could be integrated into cities easily to enhance their restorativeness. However, the factors underlying the observed restorative effects are far from clear. Although theoretical approaches emphasize a special role of “naturalness” from an evolutionary perspective, this claim has not been substantiated empirically yet. Especially, if adding greenery is not a feasible option or limited by practical considerations, which elements of the built environment may serve the restoration process?
Built urban environments can be restorative as well
With the strong theoretical focus on natural environments (see above), the restorative potential of everyday urban environments has seldom been taken into account. In many studies, built environments mainly served as a baseline or control condition (see previous section), but there is also some evidence that at least certain types of built environments can be restorative themselves.
Scopelliti and Giuliani36 asked participants to name places that they visit to have a restorative experience. Surprisingly, they did not find a difference between natural and urban environments. Rather, the participants tended to name places associated with certain leisure activities, such as listening to music at home, going to the movies, a park, or a museum, or spending an evening at a restaurant. The participants’ answers highlight the notion that activities have a very high restorative potential, with the environment and the activity probably interacting. For example, social activities were rated to be restorative and easier to achieve in urban environments.
This already demonstrates 2 main findings present in most of the studies discussed below: (1) in principle, built environments can have restorative effects similar to those of natural environments, and (2) social aspects play an important role.
Hernández and Hidalgo14 asked participants to judge the perceived restorativeness of photos showing streetscapes, buildings, and industrial zones. Streetscapes yielded the highest scores, especially those with natural elements present. In 2 other studies with an explorative approach, Galindo and Hidalgo35 and Hidalgo et al37 asked participants to name places that they found most and least attractive in their hometowns and categorized them into 4 categories: cultural-historical places, recreational places, panoramic places, and housing areas. Participants liked cultural-historical places, recreational areas, and panoramic places, ie, places in the city with an expansive view, best, rated them as the most restorative, and restoration significantly predicted perceived attractiveness, creating a link between a place’s restorative and aesthetic value. The cultural-historical and the recreational places were rated as most restorative. These results for cultural-historical environments were replicated in another study.38 Interestingly, participants judged residential areas to be among the least liked and least restorative, but, as the authors noted themselves, this may have been partly due to the high crime rate in some of the neighborhoods.35,37
Fornara and Troffa38 recruited participants as they were spending leisure time in 4 different urban settings: an urban park, a shopping mall, a historical site, and a panoramic site. Participants were asked to rate the sites according to the environmental qualities proposed in Attention Restoration Theory. The researchers concluded that the historical site was perceived to be as restorative as the urban park on all restorative qualities and can therefore be considered a restorative urban environment. Furthermore, they found the panoramic promenade in their study to be less restorative compared with the cultural-historical site and the urban park. The panoramic promenade in their study did not hold any historical elements, differing from the panoramic view used in the study by Galindo and Hidalgo,35 possibly explaining the result.
Staats et al8 investigated the restorative value of parks, shopping malls, busy streets, and cafés by asking participants to imagine being in one of those places. The participants rated being in parks to be more restorative than sitting in a café, which was rated to be more restorative than shopping at a mall. However, all 3 settings were rated as rather restorative, whereas walking along a busy street was evaluated to be the least restorative.
Mejía-Castillo et al27 asked students to name their favorite places for restoration in their school. In addition to the schools’ green spaces, students rated cafeterias as a possible place for restoration. Cafeterias scored highest for facilitating a feeling of being away and green spaces for the other environmental qualities proposed by Kaplan.9
In line with the above findings on cultural-historical places, museums and churches provide restoration as well, at least for some people. In a study by Kaplan et al,39 experienced visitors found the visit more restorative compared with individuals who had not visited a museum since their childhood. Interestingly, inexperienced visitors found the art on display to be even more fascinating than did frequent visitors, but they did not show as much cognitive involvement. In a study by Packer and Bond,40 natural environments received the highest restoration ratings, but people who had previously visited art galleries rated their restorative potential higher than people, who had previously visited a botanic garden, a history museum, or an aquarium; furthermore, frequent visitors found the art gallery even more restorative than occasional visitors. Moreover, when asked concerning their motives for visiting churches, people also mentioned their restorative aspects: In line with Attention Restoration Theory, Herzog et al41 identified, among others, the factors beauty—summarizing extent and fascination—and being away. Similar results were reported by Ouellette et al42 for visits to monasteries.
Some studies had a closer look at age and gender differences: As would be expected, Bagot et al32 found that playgrounds have a higher restorative value for younger than for older children. Two studies by Rosenbaum and colleagues43,44 indicate that adolescents valued the restorative potential of servicescapes, such as video arcades and coffee shops, whereas elderly participants found restoration in a special café offering senior activities in their neighborhood. Age differences were also present in the study by Fornara and Troffa,38 already mentioned above. Participants age 50 and over rated the restorative qualities consistently higher than younger participants; however, in the study by Scopelliti and Giuliani,36 only minor age differences were present. In the study by Mejía-Castillo et al,27 males rated fascination, being away and overall restorativeness higher than females, suggesting possible gender differences. Furthermore, age correlated positively with fascination, being away, and extent, suggesting age effects and raising the question of developmental patterns in restorative experiences.
The studies having a closer look at age and gender differences show that noteworthy interindividual differences exist, and that effects of the environment potentially interact with personality traits, emotional states, expectations, and intentions. For instance, research by Finlay et al45,46 showed that even casinos hold restorative potential, especially for nonrisk gamblers, but these findings can probably not be transferred to the general public. In one of the studies by Staats et al,47 participants were asked to imagine being either attentionally fatigued or not, being in social company or alone, and further being in an urban park, at home, in the city center, or transit. Participants rated their preference for being in an urban park higher than being in any other of the 3 urban settings when in a state of attentional fatigue. Moreover, their preference for parks increased when fatigued compared with not fatigued, and it decreased for all other settings. In addition, their preference for social company decreased with attentional fatigue and increased when the participants imagined being not fatigued in all 4 settings. In another study by Staats et al,8 interestingly, restoration need did not have an effect on the rated restorativeness of the settings under investigation, but the authors found differences between their Dutch, Swedish, and American samples, which warrant a closer look in future studies. Another interesting result was that a “sense of belonging” to a city, as investigated in the study by Hidalgo et al,37 influenced the rated restorative value, whereas the years of living in a city did not. Furthermore, Galindo and Hidalgo35 found that the presence of leisure facilities as well as the attribution of being a “meeting place” correlated with restoration likelihood.
The results presented above indicate that the social, recreational, cultural, and spiritual functions of urban environments strongly determine their potential for restorativeness. However, by strongly focusing on these social aspects, these studies tell us little about the effects of the built environment itself. But some studies have focused on the effects of architectural details.
Using sets of computer-generated images, Lindal and Hartig48 showed that higher levels of architectural variation and lower building height were judged to provide a higher restoration likelihood, which was highly correlated with a general preference rating. Furthermore, the effects of architectural features on restoration likelihood were partly mediated by the qualities of fascination and being away proposed by Kaplan.9 By contrast, increased building height led to higher ratings of perceived enclosure, which in turn was negatively correlated with restoration likelihood. These results indicate that architectural features of buildings and streetscapes have effects on restoration likelihood. In another study, Lindal and Hartig15 used those pictures which had the highest and the lowest restoration potential and included trees, grass, and flowers. The restoration likelihood was low to moderate for all pictures, but tended to be higher if trees and flowers were present. Moreover, being away as well as fascination mediated the effect of number of trees on restoration likelihood. However, only being away mediated the effect of grass, and number of flowers on restoration likelihood, and only fascination mediated the effect of tree arrangement, and presence of flowers on restoration likelihood. Architecture and vegetation had a significant influence on restoration likelihood, their interaction, however, was not significant. Surprisingly, the effect size for architecture was much larger than for vegetation indicating that architecture had a bigger influence than vegetation.
Tabrizian et al49 examined how aspects such as permeability and spatial arrangement interacted with the environmental setting. An increase in permeability increased perceived restoration of an urban plaza, whereas in a park an increase in enclosure decreased perceived restoration. Furthermore, the effect of enclosure on restoration was mediated by safety. In another study, Galindo and Hidalgo35 found luminosity, openness, and congruence to correlate with perceived restorativeness.
San Juan et al50 compared 2 open public squares, which were either high in natural elements and high in enclosure or low in natural elements and low in enclosure. They found that both squares decreased tension-anxiety, anger-hostility, fatigue, vigor, and stress, and increased happiness from pre- to posttest. However, the square high in natural elements and high in enclosure showed higher values for restoration compared with the square low in natural elements and low in enclosure, but visitors to the latter showed a higher decrease in stress.
Summary and Discussion of the Findings
Summary of the results
We collected and compared studies concerned with urban environments in the previous section. Summing up the results and answering research question 1, we found that different types of urban environments may indeed have a restorative value.
To answer research question 2, we took a closer look into the types of urban environments that are perceived as restorative. We found that natural urban as well as built urban spaces can have a restorative value. Most of the studies reviewed were concerned with restoration in urban nature, all of which yielded positive results suggesting that green spaces in cities provide a good possibility for restorative experiences. Hernández and Hidalgo14 even discovered that the mean value for restoration likelihood increased if natural elements were present, independent of urban scene evaluated. Restorative potential has been uncovered for parks,21,22,24,25 botanical gardens,13 cemeteries,20 and urban woodland.23 But even smaller green spaces such as trees, grass, flowering meadow green roofs, or flowers growing along streets or in a residential area can help to relieve mental fatigue.15-18 Furthermore, green spaces in schools or on campuses are perceived as restorative.26,27
Built urban spaces also posed a restorative capacity. All built urban environments reviewed here had a restorative value. However, there were differences in their restorativeness with historical places showing the highest restorativeness.35,37 Places associated with recreational activities such as cafés and museums showed less restorative potential according to some studies,8,35,37,40 but were also visited for restorative purposes by some individuals.36
Research question 3 was concerned with the environmental elements interacting with the restoration likelihood of a place. Lindal and Hartig15 found that architectural elements had an effect on restoration as well, which was even bigger than the effect of vegetation on restoration. This finding suggests that, at least in urban settings dominated by constructed elements (in contrast to natural elements), architectural characteristics such as lower building height and higher levels of variation in the building façade play an important part in the restoration process. The finding of architectural variation as an influence factor of a place’s restoration likelihood might partly explain other findings showing that historical buildings, often rich in façade elements, are perceived as restorative.35,37,38
Other environmental attributes that had an effect on restoration potential were enclosure,48,49 biodiversity,28,29 aesthetic value attributed to the place by the participant,35 the individual’s sense of belonging to a city,37 amount of disturbances such as traffic,24 openness, luminosity, congruence,35 and state of mental fatigue,47 all of which moderated the restorative value of a place. Moreover, recreational areas offer a variety of leisure activities and studies on different leisure areas have produced promising results. These include, among others, cafés, restaurants, playgrounds, museums, spiritual places, and even casinos.8,32,36,39,40-43,45
In conclusion, there is a growing body of research on restoration possibilities in urban environments, which provides insights into the restorative value of different urban places.
Discussion of the empirical research on restoration
There are some limitations regarding the available empirical research concerning restoration possibilities in urban environments. First, despite its relevance, the total number of studies concerned with this research topic is relatively small. This is surprising because one might assume that restoration likelihood in urban areas should be an interesting and valuable subject for researchers from many different disciplines. Unfortunately, the findings so far have been of little practical use. Their application to concrete environmental settings is difficult, as research has generally focused on identifying already present urban environments with restorative value rather than on finding out how to construct them.
One important point of criticism is that the existing studies often conceptualize urban settings in strong contrast to natural environments.4,5,51,52 The urban environments used in some studies have mainly consisted of industrial and residential areas as well as streets in business districts, ie, particularly those with the lowest restoration likelihood found in other studies.8,35,37 A confounding variable may be the cultural expectation we have regarding cities and nature. Cities are assumed to be hectic and crowded, nature to be calm and friendly. However, just as there are restorative urban areas, some natural environments are probably nonrestorative. In fact, some studies provide evidence that urban places can be equally or even more restorative than natural environments.36,37,53–55 Aesthetic attributes affect the restorative quality of a place, and the restorative value of attractive urban areas is quite high.37
From a methodological point of view, most studies that we reviewed conducted group comparisons or were concerned with correlative relations. They may offer interesting insights and are an excellent starting point, but further statistical analyses dealing with causal relations were only performed in a few studies.48 Nevertheless, most of the existing studies are of high quality and allow for the generalization of their findings, even if an insight into causal, moderating, or mediating relationships is desirable in future research.
Recommendations for future research
The research on restoration in general and on restoration likelihood in urban environments in particular offers many prospects for further research drawing on those mechanisms already uncovered. For an overview of the recommendations, see Table 1.
Table 1.
Predictors | Favorite places/personal meaning of places Social factors Sensory impressions |
Cultural and generational differences | Cultural differences in restoration
likelihood Restoration in children, adolescents and elders Generational differences Development of restoration over the life span |
Comparisons between natural and urban environments | Comparison of restorative urban with natural settings |
Predictors of restoration likelihood
Influences on restoration have not yet been fully investigated. For example, which psychological constructs (eg, personality traits) predict restoration likelihood? What sensory impressions influence the restoration process? And what is the role of social components for restoration?
Some possible influences on restorative experiences come to mind, and some researchers have already begun investigating them. For example, Ratcliffe and Korpela56 found that place attachment and place memory can impart high restorative value. Scopelliti and Giuliani36 state more specifically that the restorative meaning of a place stems from the values a person attributes to experiences that he or she had there, what he or she did there, with whom, and how much the activity was enjoyed. They place a high value on the social component of restoration, and the results of their study indicate that social factors are indeed of importance, especially for adults. Activities with a partner or with family are more restorative for adults than they are for elderly people or young adults, whereas younger people profit most from spending time with friends. This highlights possible age differences in restoration likelihood. Indeed, social factors other than spending time with friends or family come to mind. Crowding is one of them and might even be a contributing factor as to why some perceive cities as being less restorative than nature. Noise produced by humans leads to depletion of cognitive abilities through the use of selective attention and thus hinders restoration.57
Furthermore, the time a person is able to spend on restoration is of importance as well. Scopelliti and Giuliani36 discovered that people tend to spend more time at home on single week days and more time in built or natural environments on weekends and vacations. Age is also predictive of the restoration likelihood of a place. Younger people tend to search out built or natural environments when they are in need for restoration, adults natural environments, and elderly people home environments. This could be confounded with the difficulty of reaching a natural environment for impaired elderly urban citizens.
The restoration concept is primarily based on visual aspects; the theories and studies have had little to say about other sensory impressions. Factors contributing to the restorative value of an environment may include singing birds,58 the smell of flowers, or even the rather unpleasant smell of some cities if a person connects those with nice memories, building on Scopelliti and Giuliani.36 Very little work has been conducted on this topic, and we recommend future research on it.
Relaxation techniques used in clinical psychology—including mindfulness—and restoration, have stress-reducing and recovery effects, respectively. Mindfulness is a stress-reducing concept used in psychotherapy and may also influence restoration likelihood.59 Mindfulness describes an effort to capture the present moment and cultivate awareness of oneself and one’s surroundings and consists of active coping mechanisms that help to generate relief from stress. Kabat-Zinn60,61 specifically states that mindfulness can help us experience our environment with all of our senses. From what we have discussed so far, environments can contribute to a restorative experience and may interplay with relaxation techniques and mindfulness.
Cultural and generational differences
Research on restoration has concentrated on Western cultures, specifically European and US-American citizens, with only a few recent studies conducted with East Asian participants.25 Although many different populations have been studied, including Icelandic, Swedish, and Spanish samples, among others, the absence of samples from many other cultures is striking. Staats et al8 compared Swedish, Dutch, and American citizens regarding their estimated restoration likelihood in the 4 different aforementioned settings. Although the 3 cultures were all Western societies, the researchers found major differences, which, according to the researchers, could be attributable to numerous factors, eg, differences in population density (Netherlands: high, Sweden: medium, United States: low). Small differences were also present in the Italian and Spanish sample in the study by Hidalgo et al.37 Staats et al8 found that country of residence moderated the evaluation of the 4 settings but did not influence restoration likelihood itself. Furthermore, the interaction effect of social company and country of residence was significant for the park and the street setting. Moreover, the interaction between mental fatigue and country of residence also had an intermediate effect. However, it is unclear, as the researchers stated themselves, whether the cultural differences found may be attributable to country of residence or rather to city size. Studies on other cultures may produce relevant results as well.
Furthermore, few studies have addressed restoration in children and adolescents. However, as far as we know from the existing studies, children find different environments to be restorative than do adults.32 Research on restorative environments for children is an important field. Adults can search out restorative places according to their own will, whereas children do not have this freedom and move in a much smaller circle predefined by their parents, teachers, or other adults. More research is desirable to highlight the development of preferred restorative environments over the life span.62
We also need research on the restorative value of environments for the elderly. This population group faces a similar problem as children. Due to physical impairments, the mobility of many elderly decreases. However, it is also important for them to have restoration possibilities nearby. Differences between this age group and adults have already been uncovered.36
Urban and natural environments compared
Much potential lies in the investigation of existing urban environments to find more restorative locations. Some have already been uncovered, but more studies are desirable to replicate and extend these findings. Researchers may like to determine more about what cultural and panoramic places are especially restorative and under which conditions. It is likely that the Colosseum in Rome is more recreational on a calm summer evening than on a chilly winter day or more recuperative with fewer tourists than with crowds of noisy visitors. Assumptions such as these have to be examined, however. Research on buildings and residential streets as potential restorative environments has produced mixed results.37,48 Further research is needed to discover what other influences predict the restorativeness of these areas. Confounding variables may be crime rate, maintenance, complexity, and novelty of building form and façade, as well as building height.63
Finally, many studies have compared natural environments to the least restorative urban locations, namely, streets and buildings in business districts and industrial zones.64 Studies comparing restorative urban settings, such as cultural areas, to natural ones still need to be conducted, as this has seldom been investigated. Stigsdotter et al55 took a first step in this direction by comparing the restorative effect of a walk in the historical part of Copenhagen to a walk in a forest in an arboretum. They discovered that there were no differences on psychophysiological measures, such as heart rate variability and blood pressure, after the walk in the 2 environments. However, they uncovered differences in perceived restorativeness between the 2 environments advantaging the forest. Karmanov and Hamel54 also compared a restorative natural environment (Amstelland) to a restorative urban environment (Eastern Docklands in Amsterdam). Perceived restoration did not differ after watching videos of the 2 settings. However, the natural environment showed a higher restorative potential for depression but not for anger and anxiety. These interesting results warrant a closer look into the differences between restorative urban and natural environments. For example, are natural environments more restorative under every condition? What about the prospect of meeting dangerous animals, which may not be present in most parts of Europe but certainly are in many other parts of the world?
Many open questions remain. Research on restoration and especially restoration in cities is a promising field with yet many more interesting phenomena to be discovered.
Conclusions
Urban environments can have a restorative potential. However, much research has concentrated on urban locations with very little restorative value. Nonetheless and especially during the past 2 decades, many researchers have reported promising findings. Certain urban locations, including cafés, museums, cultural places, and even some residential streets, have a high potential for restoration. Whether further research on cities’ restoration potential can change the negative connotations of cities as noisy, hazardous environments remains to be seen.
Studies on this reviewed topic are few, but they provide generalizable results, which can help to generate new research questions. Many questions are still unanswered, and it will be a long time before we will be able to respond to them adequately. What is certain is that restoration is not only limited to nature but also includes some urban environments, irrespective of these being located in a small town or a large city.
Appendix 1
Table A1.
Article | Topic | Location | Sample | Design and questionnaire | Result |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Bagot et al32 | Vegetation and restoration in urban settings | Australia | 550 children | Correlational PRCS-Ca |
Vegetation predicts perceived restoration likelihood of a playground |
Baron and Rosenbaum43 | Restoration in video arcades and coffee shops | USA | Study 1: 172 teenagers Study 2: 437 undergraduates |
Correlational PRSb |
Video arcades as well as coffee shops are restorative for adolescents |
Carrus et al13 | Restoration in botanical gardens | Italy | 127 from 4 Italian cities | Field experiment PRS, Italian versionc |
Perceived restoration in botanical gardens was high |
Carrus et al29 | Restoration in urban green spaces | Italy | 568 from 4 Italian cities | Field experiment PRS, Italian version |
Biodiversity has a stronger link to perceived restoration in urban compared to peri-urban environments |
Finlay et al45 | Restoration in casinos | USA | 22 people, who had gambled in 6 casinos | Natural experiment McKechnie65 and Kaplan and Kaplan7 scales |
Casinos have restorative value, especially for nonrisk gamblers |
Finlay et al46 | Restoration in casinos | USA | 484 people, who like to gamble | Correlational McKechnie65 scale |
Casinos have a restorative value |
Fornara and Troffa38 | Restoration potential of different urban environments | Italy | 197 | Correlational PRS, Italian version |
Historical environments have the same restorative potential as urban parks |
Galindo and Hidalgo35 | Restoration likelihood in different urban environments | Spain | 132 | Correlational PRS, Spanish versiond |
Aesthetic value of an urban place correlates with restoration likelihood |
Grahn and Stigsdotter | Restorative aspects of nature | Sweden | 953 | Correlational Self-estimations of health |
Refuge, diversity in species, nature, and absence of social stimuli could increase restoration likelihood of urban environments |
Hernández and Hidalgo14 | Vegetation and restoration in urban settings | Spain | 214 university students | Correlational PRS, Spanish version |
Natural elements in urban environments have a restorative value |
Herzog et al41 | Restoration in spiritual places | USA | 781 undergraduates | Correlational Adaptation of Ouellette et al42 scale |
Christian spiritual places have a restorative value |
Hipp et al26 | Vegetation and restoration in urban settings | USA and Scotland | 570 students from 3 universities | Field experiment PRS |
Vegetation on campus increases restorative value |
Hidalgo et al37 | Restoration and preference | Spain Italy |
Study 1: 58 Study 2: 98 |
Correlational PRS, Spanish and Italian versions |
Preferred places are rated to be more restorative, and restorative places are preferred |
Jiang et al30 | Effect of tree cover density on stress recovery | USA | 158 | Experiment Cortisol and skin conductance levels |
Tree cover density increased stress recovery for male participants in a U-shaped curve, but not for female participants |
Jiang et al31 | Effect of tree cover density on stress recovery | USA | 158 | Experiment VAS66 |
The association between tree cover density and stress recovery is linear |
Kaplan et al39 | Restoration likelihood in cultural places | USA | Study 1: focus groups with 8 to 12 members Study 2: 124 |
Natural experiment A survey |
Museums hold ART’s 4 restorative qualities and are restorative |
Kuo17 | Green areas in poverty stricken neighborhoods | USA | 145 female residents | Field experiment Digit Span Backwards test67 |
Residents of buildings with nearby green areas showed less mental fatigue compared with residents of buildings with no nearby green areas |
Kuo and Sullivan18 | Green areas in poverty stricken neighborhoods | USA | 145 female residents | Field experiment Digit Span Backwards test67 |
Residents of buildings with nearby green areas showed less mental fatigue and aggression compared with residents of buildings with no nearby green areas |
Lee et al16 | Vegetation and restoration in urban settings | Australia | 150 | Experiment PRS |
Flowering meadow green roofs are restorative |
Lindal and Hartig48 | Restoration likelihood of residential areas | Iceland | 263 | Experiment Some items of the PRS |
Being away and fascination enhance restoration likelihood in residential streetscapes |
Lindal and Hartig15 | Restoration likelihood of residential areas | Iceland | 188 | Experiment Some items of the PRS |
Vegetation on residential streetscapes enhances restoration likelihood. However, architectural elements had a larger effect on restoration than vegetation did |
Mejía-Castillo et al27 | Restoration likelihood in schools | Mexico | 706 high school students | Correlational EPREEe |
Leisure areas with green spaces are suited best for a restorative experience in schools |
Nordh33 | Methods to measure restoration | Study 1 and 2: Sweden Study 3: Norway |
Study 1: 51 university students Study 2: 33 university students Study 3: 154 |
Correlational Study 1: PRS Study 2: One question assessing restoration Study 3: One question assessing restoration |
All 3 methods are able to provide insights into the restorative value of parks |
Nordh et al20 | Restoration in cemeteries | Norway | 59 visitors to a cemetery in Oslo | Qualitative Semistandardized interview |
Visitors’ description of their use of the cemetery corresponded with a restorative environment |
Nordh et al22 | Restoration likelihood in urban spaces | Sweden | 33 | Correlational One question assessing restoration |
Grass and restoration likelihood have a positive relation. The relations to the other components (eg, benches) were nonsignificant |
Nordh et al21 | Restoration in parks | Sweden | 52 | Correlational, PRS |
Fascination and being away mediate restoration likelihood in parks. Grass, trees, bushes, and park size predicted restorativeness |
Nordh and Østby24 | Vegetation and restoration in urban settings | Norway | 58 university students | Correlational One question assessing restoration |
Green spaces in parks benefit restorative experiences |
Ouellette et al42 | Restoration in monasteries | Canada | 521 visitors to a Benedictine monastery | Correlational Survey developed for the study |
Monasteries hold restorative potential, but more experienced visitors name beauty and spirituality as their reasons for their visit |
Packer and Bond40 | Restorative values of different urban environments | Australia | 596 visitors to one of 4 urban sites: an arts museum, a history museum, an aquarium, or a botanic garden | Field experiment Restorative Components Scale68 Modified version of the Attention Recovery and Reflection Scale69 Restored Mental State Scale40 |
Visitors to an art gallery find it restorating. However, the national parks and beaches received highest restoration rating from the overall sample |
Rosenbaum and Sweeney44 | Restorative value of a café | USA | 90 senior citizens | Correlational SRSSf |
Cafés are restorative for elderly citizens |
San Juan et al50 | Restorative potential of urban squares | Spain | 46 | Experiment Spanish adaptation of PRS |
The urban square higher in vegetation was perceived as more restorative, but the urban square lower in vegetation decreased stress further than the other square |
Scopelliti et al28 | Biodiversity, affective, and social components | Italy | 124 | Field experiment, PRS |
High levels of biodiversity correlate with restoration potential of a place. Affective components mediated this relationship. |
Scopelliti and Giuliani36 | Restoration likelihood in natural vs urban environments | Italy | 67 | Correlational PRS |
Urban and natural environments are equally restorative. |
Staats et al8 | Restoration likelihood in different Western societies | USA Netherlands Sweden |
USA: 316 Netherlands: 80 Sweden: 100 |
Field experiment PRS |
Cultural differences in restorative urban environments |
Staats et al47 | Restoration in different urban environments | Netherlands | 70 | Correlational PRS |
Attentional fatigue moderates preference for a more restorative environment, here an urban park |
Tabrizian et al49 | Green space enclosure and restorativeness | USA | 87 | Experiment Modified version of PRS |
Enclosure positively affects perceived restorativeness of an urban plaza |
Tenngart Ivarsson and Hagerhall19 | Restorativeness of gardens, usefulness of PRS | Sweden | 74 | Field experiment Swedish version of PRS by Hartig |
Scenes can include elements that differ in perceived restorativeness. The PRS is a useful tool for measuring perceived restorativeness |
Tyrväinen et al23 | Restoration in different urban settings | Finland | 77 | Field experiment PRS ROSg |
Perceived restorativeness was rated highest for the woodland, but physiological measures of stress relief showed no difference between the settings |
Wang et al25 | Vegetation and restoration in urban settings | China | 140 | Experiment PRS |
Parks are restorative, which is supported by psychophysiological measures |
Footnotes
Funding:The author(s) received no financial support for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article.
Declaration of Conflicting Interests:The author(s) declared no potential conflicts of interest with respect to the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article.
Author Contributions: AMW and JT conceived of the presented idea. AMW conducted the literature research and selection, and drafted the manuscript. AMW and JT commented on and revised the manuscript. Both authors read and approved the final version of the manuscript.
References
- 1. Martin S. Our health at risk. Monitor Staff. 2012;43:18. [Google Scholar]
- 2. Basner M, Babisch W, Davis A, et al. Auditory and non-auditory effects of noise on health. Lancet. 2014;383:1325–1332. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(13)61613-X. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3. Joye Y, van den Berg AE. Restorative environments. In: Steg L, van den Berg AE, de Groot JIM, eds. Environmental Psychology. West Sussex, UK: Blackwell; 2013:57–66. [Google Scholar]
- 4. Herzog TR, Black AM, Fountaine KA, Knotts DJ. Reflection and attentional recovery as distinctive benefits of restorative environments. J Environ Psychol. 1997;17:165–170. doi: 10.1006/jevp.1997.0051. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 5. Ulrich RS, Simons RF, Losito BD, Fiorito E, Miles MA, Zelson M. Stress recovery during exposure to natural and urban environments. J Environ Psychol. 1991;11:201–230. doi: 10.1016/S0272-4944(05)80184-7. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 6. Ulrich RS. Aesthetic and affective response to natural environment. In: Altman I, Wohlwill JF, eds. Behavior and the Natural Environment (Human Behavior and Environment). Vol 6 New York, NY: Plenum Press; 1983:85–125. [Google Scholar]
- 7. Kaplan R, Kaplan S. The Experience of Nature: A Psychological Perspective. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press; 1989. [Google Scholar]
- 8. Staats H, Jahncke H, Herzog TR, Hartig T. Urban options for psychological restoration: common strategies in everyday situations. PLoS ONE. 2016;11:e0146213. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0146213. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9. Kaplan S. The restorative benefits of nature: toward an integrative framework. J Environ Psychol. 1995;15:169–182. doi: 10.1016/0272-4944(95)90001-2. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 10. Joye Y, Dewitte S. Nature’s broken path to restoration. A critical look at attention restoration theory. J Environ Psychol. 2018;59:1–8. [Google Scholar]
- 11. Staats H. Restorative environments. In: Clayton SD, ed. The Oxford Handbook of Environmental and Conservation Psychology. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press; 2012, pp. 445–458. [Google Scholar]
- 12. Moher D, Liberati A, Tetzlaff J, Altman DG; The PRISMA Group. Preferred reporting items for systematic reviews and meta-analyses: the PRISMA statement. PLoS Med. 2009;6:e1000097. doi: 10.1371/journal.pmed1000097. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13. Carrus G, Scopelliti M, Panno A, et al. A different way to stay in touch with “urban nature”: the perceived restorative qualities of botanical gardens. Front Psychol. 2017;8:1–9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14. Hernández B, Hidalgo MC. Effect of urban vegetation on psychological restorativeness. Psychol Rep. 2005;96:1025–1028. doi: 10.2466/pr0.96.3c.1025-1028. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15. Lindal PJ, Hartig T. Effects of urban street vegetation on judgments of restoration likelihood. Urban For Urban Gree. 2015;14:200–209. doi: 10.1016/j.ufug.2015.02.001. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 16. Lee KE, Williams KJH, Sargent LD, Williams NSG, Johnson KA. 40-second green roof views sustain attention: the role of micro-breaks in attention restoration. J Environ Psychol. 2015;42:182–189. doi: 10.1016/j.jenvp.2015.04.003. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 17. Kuo FE. Coping with poverty: impacts of environment and attention in the inner city. Environ Behav. 2001;33:5–34. [Google Scholar]
- 18. Kuo FE, Sullivan WC. Aggression and violence in the inner city: effects of environment via mental fatigue. Environ Behav. 2001;33:543–571. [Google Scholar]
- 19. Tenngart Ivarsson C, Hagerhall CM. The perceived restorativeness of gardens—assessing the restorativeness of a mixed built and natural scene type. Urban For Urban Gree. 2008;7:107–118. doi: 10.1016/j.ufug.2008.01.001. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 20. Nordh H, Evensen KH, Skår M. A peaceful place in the city—a qualitative study of restorative components of the cemetery. Landscape Urban Plan. 2017;167:108–117. doi: 10.1016/j.landurbplan.2017.06.004. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 21. Nordh H, Hartig T, Hagerhall CM, Fry G. Components of small urban parks that predict the possibility for restoration. Urban For Urban Gree. 2009;8:225–235. doi: 10.1016/j.ufug.2009.06.003. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 22. Nordh H, Hagerhall CM, Holmqvist K. Tracking restorative components: patterns in eye movements as a consequence of a restorative rating task. Landscape Res. 2013;38:101–116. doi: 10.1080/01426397.2012.691468. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 23. Tyrväinen L, Ojala A, Korpela K, Lanki T, Tsunetsugu Y, Kagawa T. The influence of urban green environments on stress relief measures: a field experiment. J Environ Psychol. 2014;38:1–9. doi: 10.1016/j.jenvp.2013.12.005. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 24. Nordh H, Østby K. Pocket parks for people—a study of park design and use. Urban For Urban Gree. 2013;12:12–17. doi: 10.1016/j.ufug.2012.11.003. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 25. Wang X, Rodiek S, Wu C, Chen Y, Li Y. Stress recovery and restorative effects of viewing different urban park scenes in Shanghai, China. Urban For Urban Gree. 2016;15:112–122. doi: 10.1016/j.ufug.2015.12.003. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 26. Hipp JA, Gulwadi GB, Alves S, Sequeira S. The relationship between perceived greenness and perceived restorativeness of university campuses and student-reported quality of life. Environ Behav. 2016;48:1292–1308. doi: 10.1177/0013916515598200. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 27. Mejía-Castillo AJ, López-Suárez AD, Estrada Rodríguez C, Lagunes-Córdoba R. Perceived restorativeness in school spaces of high schools in Xalapa City, México. Act Colom Psicol. 2016:199–209. doi: 10.14718/ACP.2016.19.2.9. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 28. Scopelliti M, Carrus G, Cini F, et al. Biodiversity, perceived restorativeness, and benefits of nature: a study on the psychological processes and outcomes of on-site experiences in urban and peri-urban green areas in Italy. In: Kabisch S, Kunath A, Schweizer-ries P, Steinführer A, eds. Vulnerability, Risks, and Complexity: Impact of Global Change on Human Habitats. Göttingen, Germany: Hogrefe; 2012:255–269. [Google Scholar]
- 29. Carrus G, Scopelliti M, Lafortezza R, et al. Go greener, feel better? the positive effects of biodiversity on the well-being of individuals visiting urban and peri-urban green areas. Landscape Urban Plan. 2015;134:221–228. doi: 10.1016/j.landurbplan.2014.10.022. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 30. Jiang B, Chang C-Y, Sullivan WC. A dose of nature: tree cover, stress reduction, and gender differences. Landscape Urban Plan. 2014;132:26–36. [Google Scholar]
- 31. Jiang B, Li D, Larsen L, Sullivan WC. A dose-response curve describing the relationship between urban tree cover density and self-reported stress recovery. Environ Behav. 2016;48:607–629. doi: 10.1177/0013916514552321. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 32. Bagot KL, Allen FCL, Toukhsati S. Perceived restorativeness of children’s school playground environments: nature, playground features and play period experiences. J Environ Psychol. 2015;41:1–9. doi: 10.1016/j.jenvp.2014.11.005. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 33. Nordh H. Quantitative methods of measuring restorative components in urban public parks. J Landsc Archit. 2012;7:46–53. doi: 10.1080/18626033.2012.693780. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 34. Grahn P, Stigsdotter UK. The relation between perceived sensory dimensions of urban green space and stress restoration. Landscape Urban Plan. 2010;94:264-275. doi: 10.1016/j.landurbplan.2009.10.012. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 35. Galindo MP, Hidalgo MC. Aesthetic preferences and the attribution of meaning: environmental categorization processes in the evaluation of urban scenes. Int J Psychol. 2005;40:19–26. [Google Scholar]
- 36. Scopelliti M, Giuliani MV. Choosing restorative environments across the lifespan: a matter of place experience. J Environ Psychol. 2004;24:423–437. doi: 10.1016/j.jenvp.2004.11.002. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 37. Hidalgo MC, Berto R, Galindo MP, Getrevi A. Identifying attractive and unattractive urban places: categories, restorativeness and aesthetic attributes. Medioambient Comportamient. 2006;7:115–133. [Google Scholar]
- 38. Fornara F, Troffa R. Restorative experiences and perceived affective qualities in different built and natural urban places. In: Yildiz HT, Guney YI, eds. Revitalising Built Environments: Requalifying Old Places for New Uses. Istanbul, Turkey: Istanbul Technical University; 2009:1–10. [Google Scholar]
- 39. Kaplan S, Bardwell LV, Slakter DB. The museum as a restorative environment. Environ Behav. 1993;25:725–742. doi: 10.1177/0013916593256004. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 40. Packer J, Bond N. Museums as restorative environments. Curator. 2010;53:421–436. doi: 10.1111/j.2151-6952.2010.00044.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 41. Herzog TR, Ouellette P, Rolens JR, Koenigs AM. Houses of worship as restorative environments. Environ Behav. 2010;42:395–419. doi: 10.1177/0013916508328610. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 42. Ouellette P, Kaplan R, Kaplan S. The monastery as a restorative environment. J Environ Psychol. 2005;25:175–188. doi: 10.1016/j.jenvp.2005.06.001. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 43. Baron S, Rosenbaum MS. Restorative servicescapes: restoring directed attention in third places. J Serv Manage. 2009;20:173–191. doi: 10.1108/09564230910952762. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 44. Rosenbaum MS, Sweeney JC. The restorative qualities of an activity-based, third place café for seniors: restoration, social support, and place attachment at Mather’s—more than a café. Senior Housing Care J. 2009;17:38–54. [Google Scholar]
- 45. Finlay K, Kanetkar V, Londerville J, Marmurek HHC. The physical and psychological measurement of gambling environments. Environ Behav. 2016;38:570–581. doi: 10.1177/0013916505283419. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 46. Finlay K, Marmurek HHC, Kanetkar V, Londerville J. Trait and state emotion congruence in simulated casinos: effects on at-risk gambling intention and restoration. J Environ Psychol. 2007;27:166–175. doi: 10.1016/j.jenvp.2007.03.002. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 47. Staats H, van Gemerden E, Hartig T. Preference for restorative situations: interactive effects of attentional state, activity-in-environment, and social context. Leisure Sci. 2010;32:401–417. doi: 10.1080/01490400.2010.510990. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 48. Lindal PJ, Hartig T. Architectural variation, building height, and the restorative quality of urban residential streetscapes. J Environ Psychol. 2013;33:26–36. 10.1016/j.jenvp.2012.09.003. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 49. Tabrizian P, Baran PK, Smith WR, Meentemeyer RK. Exploring perceived restoration potential of urban green enclosure through immersive virtual environments. J Environ Psychol. 2018;55:99–109. doi: 10.1016/j.jenvp.2018.01.001. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 50. San Juan C, Subiza-Pérez M, Vozmediano L. Restoration and the city: the role of public urban squares. Front Psychol. 2017;8:2093. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.02093. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51. Hartig T, Evans GW, Jammer LD, Davis DS, Gärling T. Tracking restoration in natural and urban field settings. J Environ Psychol. 2003;23:109–123. [Google Scholar]
- 52. Parsons R, Tassinary LG, Ulrich RS, Hebl MR, Grossman-Alexander M. The view from the road: implications for stress recovery and immunization. J Environ Psychol. 1998;18:113–140. doi: 10.1006/jevp.1998.0086. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 53. Herzog TR, Colleen P, Maguire CP, Nebel MB. Assessing the restorative components of environments. J Environ Psychol. 2003;23:159–170. [Google Scholar]
- 54. Karmanov D, Hamel R. Assessing the restorative potential of contemporary urban environment(s): beyond the nature versus urban dichotomy. Landscape Urban Plan. 2008;86:115–125. doi: 10.1016/j.landurbplan.2008.01.004. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 55. Stigsdotter UK, Corazon SS, Sidenius U, Kristiansen J, Grahn P. It is not all bad for the grey city—a crossover study on physiological and psychological restoration in a forest and an urban environment. Health Place. 2017;46:145–154. doi: 10.1016/j.healthplace.2017.05.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56. Ratcliffe E, Korpela KM. Memory and place attachment as predictors of imagined restorative perceptions of favourite places. J Environ Psychol. 2016;48:120–130. doi: 10.1016/j.jenvp.2016.09.005. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 57. Benfield JA, Bell PA, Troup LJ, Soderstrom N. Does anthropogenic noise in national parks impair memory? Environ Behav. 2010;42:693–706. doi: 10.1177/0013916509351219. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 58. Ratcliffe E, Gatersleben B, Sowden PT. Bird sounds and their contributions to perceived attention restoration and stress recovery. J Environ Psychol. 2013;36:221–228. doi: 10.1016/j.jenvp.2013.08.004. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 59. Baer RA. Mindfulness training as a clinical intervention: a conceptual and empirical review. Clin Psychol: Sci Pr. 2003;10:125–143. [Google Scholar]
- 60. Kabat-Zinn J. Awareness has no center and no periphery. Mindfulness. 2016;7:1241–1242. doi: 10.1007/s12671-016-0610-0. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 61. Kabat-Zinn J. The “sense” of time passing. Mindfulness. 2016;7:1238–1240. doi: 10.1007/s12671-016-0609-6. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 62. Korpela K. Children’s environment. In: Bechtel RB, Churchman A, eds. Handbook of Environmental Psychology. New York, NY: John Wiley & Sons; 2002:363–373. [Google Scholar]
- 63. van den Berg AE, Hartig T, Staats H. Preference for nature in urbanized societies: stress, restoration, and the pursuit of sustainability. J Soc Issues. 2007;63:79–96. doi: 10.1111/j.1540-4560.2007.00497.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 64. Bodin M, Hartig T. Does the outdoor environment matter for psychological restoration gained through running? Psychol Sport Exerc. 2003;4:141–153. doi: 10.1016/S1469-0292(01)00038-3. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 65. McKechnie GE. The environmental response inventory in application. Environ Behav. 1977;9:225-276. [Google Scholar]
- 66. Marsh-Richard DM, Hatzis ES, Mathias CW, Venditti N, Dougherty DM. Adaptive Visual Analog Scales (AVAS): a modifiable software program for the creation, administration, and scoring of visual analog scales. Behav Res Methods. 2009;41:99–106. doi: 10.3758/BRM.41.1.99. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 67. Hilbert S, Nakagawa TT, Puci P, Zech A, Bühner M. The digit span backwards task. Eur J Psychol Assess. 2015;31:174–180. doi: 10.1027/1015-5759/a000223. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 68. Laumann K, Gärling T, Stormark KM. Rating scale measures of restorative components of environments. J Environ Psychol. 2001;21:31–44. [Google Scholar]
- 69. Staats H, Kieviet A, Hartig T. Where to recover from attentional fatigue: an expectancy-value analysis of environmental preference. J Environ Psychol. 2003;23:147–157. [Google Scholar]
- 70. Bagot KL. Perceived restorative components: a scale for children. Child Youth Environ. 2004;14:107–129. [Google Scholar]
- 71. Bagot KL, Kuo FE, Allen FCL. Amendments to the Perceived Restorative Components Scale for Children (PRCS-C II). Child Youth Environ. 2007;17:124–127. [Google Scholar]
- 72. Hartig T, Korpela K, Evans GW, Garling T. A measure of restorative quality in environments. Scand J Psychol. 1997;37:378–393. [Google Scholar]
- 73. Pasini M, Berto R, Scopelliti M, Carrus G. Measuring the restorative value of the environment: contribution to the validation of the Italian version of the Perceived Restorativeness Scale. Bollet Psicol Appl. 2009;257:3–11. [Google Scholar]
- 74. Hidalgo MC, Hernández B. Si te sientes bien te recuperarás mejor: La evaluación de los lugares restauradores. In: San Juan C, Rodríguez B, Vergara A, eds. Monografías Socioambientales: VII Congreso De Psicología Ambiental. Human Habitats-XXI. Barcelona, Spain: Publications Universitat De Barcelona; 2001. [Google Scholar]
- 75. Han K-T. A reliable and valid self-rating measure of the restorative quality of natural environments. Landscape Urban Plan. 2003;64:209–232. doi: 10.1016/S0169-2046(02)00241-4. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 76. Han K-T. Responses to six major terrestrial biomes in terms of scenic beauty, preference, and restorativeness. Environ Behav. 2007;39:529–556. doi: 10.1177/0013916506292016. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 77. Korpela K, Ylén M, Tyrväinen L, Silvennoinen H. Determinants of restorative experiences in everyday favourite places. Health Place. 2008;14:636–652. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]