Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2018 Nov 28.
Published in final edited form as: Lang Cogn Neurosci. 2018 May 22;33(10):1275–1295. doi: 10.1080/23273798.2018.1476727

Individual differences in decoding skill, print exposure, and cortical structure in young adults

Clinton L Johns a, Andrew A Jahn a, Hannah R Jones b, Dave Kush a,c, Peter J Molfese a,d, Julie A Van Dyke a,g, James S Magnuson a,e,f,g, Whitney Tabor a,e,g, W Einar Mencl a, Donald P Shankweiler a,e, David Braze a,g
PMCID: PMC6258201  NIHMSID: NIHMS971944  PMID: 30505876

Abstract

This exploratory study investigated relations between individual differences in cortical grey matter structure and young adult readers’ cognitive profiles. Whole-brain analyses revealed neuroanatomical correlations with word and nonword reading ability (decoding), and experience with printed matter. Decoding was positively correlated with grey matter volume (GMV) in left superior temporal sulcus, and thickness (GMT) in right superior temporal gyrus. Print exposure was negatively correlated with GMT in left inferior frontal gyrus (pars opercularis) and left fusiform gyrus (including the visual word form area). Both measures also correlated with supramarginal gyrus (SMG), but in spatially distinct subregions: decoding was positively associated with GMV in left anterior SMG, and print exposure was negatively associated with GMT in left posterior SMG. Our comprehensive approach to assessment both confirms and refines our understanding of the novel relation between the structure of pSMG and proficient reading, and unifies previous research relating cortical structure and reading skill.

Keywords: individual differences, reading skills, neuroimaging, grey matter structure, phonological decoding, print exposure, supramarginal gyrus

1. Introduction

Proficient reading depends upon the efficient coordination of both language-specific processes (e.g., phonological, lexical, syntactic, semantic) and domain general processes (e.g., working memory, reasoning). Becoming literate entails changes to the brain’s cortical structure, both in grey and white matter (Carreiras et al., 2009; Castro-Caldas et al., 1999; Petersson, Silva, Castro-Caldas, Ingvar, & Reis, 2007). There is substantial evidence for individual behavioral differences in reading comprehension and its components (for reviews see Long, Johns, & Morris, 2006; van den Broek, Mouw, & Kraal, 2015; Wagner, Piasta, & Torgesen, 2006), and that these differences often correlate with differences in functional activity in task-relevant brain regions (e.g., Clements-Stephens et al., 2012; Meyler et al., 2008; Shankweiler et al., 2008; Welcome & Joanisse, 2012). However, although it might be assumed that cortical structure and function may be similarly related to such behavioral differences, there is relatively little structural evidence available to support this hypothesis (for review see Richardson & Price, 2009). Structural imaging may provide critical complementary information about the neural substrates underlying reading behavior. Consequently, the goal of the current study is to explore potential relations between cortical grey matter structure and performance measures related to reading comprehension.

There are few studies that have directly assessed whether literacy-related skills correlate with indices of grey matter structure. In a recent example (Jednoróg et al., 2015), two such skills were assessed: rapid automatized naming (RAN) and decoding. The goal was to assess potential differences in grey matter volume (GMV) between two large groups of children, one with developmental dyslexia (n=130), and the other an age-matched control group of unimpaired readers (n=106). Performance on the RAN task – a speeded response task in which readers name characters that appear in a visual array – is often associated with fluent and efficient processing speed (although this is the subject of ongoing debate; for review see Norton & Wolf, 2012). Decoding refers to the ability to accurately map orthographic information – i.e., printed words in alphabetic languages – to a corresponding phonological representation. Both RAN and decoding ability are strong predictors of reading comprehension (Gough & Tunmer, 1986; Hoover & Gough, 1990; Norton & Wolf, 2012; Shankweiler et al., 1999). Jednoróg and colleagues found no evidence for between-group differences in cortical grey matter. There was also no evidence that either decoding or RAN were related to grey matter differences in dyslexic readers. However, they did observe individual differences within the control group: in unimpaired readers, decoding and GMV were correlated such that higher word reading accuracy corresponded to greater GMV in left supramarginal gyrus (SMG). Given its power, this study provides robust evidence that a skill that is critical to reading comprehension can also be directly related to variation in the structure of cortical grey matter in non-dyslexic readers.

The approach adopted by Jednoróg and colleagues (2015) is in some respects emblematic of most investigations of grey matter morphology and reading ability. Such studies are often primarily concerned with describing some specific group of interest (e.g., dyslexic or bilingual readers). Consistent with this, most studies of linguistic relations to cortical grey matter emphasize group-level comparisons with “typical” (i.e., non-dyslexic, or monolingual) readers, rather than specific measures of the participants’ reading-related skills. Furthermore, even when such measures are included, their scope is usually quite limited: it is most common to obtain only a single skill measure (or perhaps two). One consequence of these methodological emphases is that our ability to assess potential relations between distinct components of reading skill and neuroanatomical structure is limited by the reliance on only narrow information about readers’ literacy skills. Another is that we know comparatively little about the possible neurostructural correlates of literacy in so-called “typical” readers. Thus, we conducted an exploratory study of cortical grey matter structure in monolingual young adults without diagnosed reading disabilities. Further, in order to more fully characterize both the processes supporting reading comprehension in our participants and their neuroanatomic correlates, we administered a battery of behavioral tests indexing a wide range of reading-related skills.

There is some evidence that the structural relation to decoding skill that was observed in children (Jednoróg et al., 2015) may also be present in young adults. In one study, brain morphology was related to group differences in college students (Welcome, Chiarello, Thompson, & Sowell, 2011). Group membership was determined by testing not only decoding efficiency, but also participants’ reading comprehension ability (i.e., their global understanding of a text). There were three groups: proficient readers (n=22), whose scores indicated that their performance on both tests was commensurate with age; poor readers (n=12), whose performance was below age-based norms; and “resilient” readers (n=21), who had impaired decoding, but nonetheless exhibited age-appropriate reading comprehension. Decoding ability was related to hemispheric asymmetry in temporo-parietal regions (including SMG): grey matter thickness (GMT) in left hemisphere regions was greater relative to their homologues in the right hemisphere in proficient readers. However, this asymmetry was reduced in both groups with poor decoding skills. Although reductions in the typical leftward asymmetry are known to be associated with specific reading disability (Eckert, 2004; Heim & Keil, 2004; see also Chiarello, Lombardino, Kacinik, Otto, & Leonard, 2006), this was the first report of such a relation in individuals without a history of reading disability. In contrast to decoding, reading comprehension ability was not related to the structure of the left temporo-parietal region. Instead, it correlated only with structural aspects of right hemisphere brain regions: specifically, radial expansion – a measure of “local brain shape” related to cortical surface area – was smaller in frontal and parietal regions for poor comprehenders relative to the other groups. Overall, these findings provide important neurostructural information about the components of literacy skill: they corroborate the importance of assessing decoding ability; indicate a discrete role for measures of reading comprehension ability; and suggest that the relation between efficient decoding and the structure of cortical regions including SMG may be consistent across developmentally distinct age groups.

Another study of college students without histories of reading difficulty (N=28) also assessed both decoding ability and reading comprehension skill, and additionally included measures of experience with printed material (Goldman & Manis, 2013). Significant correlations among these three literacy-related skills are well established. Skilled readers typically exhibit greater decoding ability and more extensive print exposure; conversely, decoding difficulty is associated with less print exposure and poorer reading comprehension (Cunningham & Stanovich, 1991, 1997; Shankweiler et al., 1999; Share, 1995; Stanovich, 1986; Stanovich & Cunningham, 1992; for review see Mol & Bus, 2011). Goldman & Manis defined regions of interest in the left hemisphere reading network (e.g., Bolger, Perfetti, & Schneider, 2005; Pugh, 2006; Pugh et al., 2013). Decoding ability was not related to GMT in any of these regions in this sample. However, both print exposure and reading comprehension were positively correlated with GMT. In separate analyses, print exposure was related to GMT in left SMG, left fusiform gyrus, both pars opercularis and pars triangularis in inferior frontal gyrus (IFG), and angular gyrus (AG); reading comprehension ability, by contrast, was related to GMT only in the latter two regions. A subsequent analysis explored whether the overlapping correlations in IFG and AG indicated that each literacy skill was a unique predictor of GMT, or whether the correlations in the initial analyses might instead be based upon variance shared between the two measures. Although the latter was the case in IFG, print exposure emerged as a unique predictor of GMT in AG. These results corroborate the importance of assessing reading comprehension ability; suggest experience with printed material as a potentially important structural correlate; and highlight the importance of accounting for shared variance among multiple measures of literacy skills in analyses of individual differences and brain morphology.

Two additional studies report significant correlations between cortical grey matter and literacy skills in non-clinical populations. The principal findings concern vocabulary knowledge, which is well-known to be strongly correlated with reading comprehension (Anderson, Wilson, & Fielding, 1988; Joshi, 2005; Perfetti, 2007; Stanovich, 1986). Measures of vocabulary knowledge often emerge as unique predictors that capture reading-related variance beyond that of other cognitive assessments (for reviews see Braze et al., 2016; Protopapas, Mouzaki, Sideridis, Kotsolakou, & Simos, 2013; Tunmer & Chapman, 2012). Lee and colleagues (2007) found that greater vocabulary knowledge was positively related to grey matter density in bilateral SMG in adolescents (N=34). Richardson and colleagues (2010) confirmed this finding in adolescents, but not in either young children or adults. Moreover, in the later study, vocabulary knowledge was also positively associated with grey matter density in two other brain regions: left superior temporal sulcus (STS) and, in adults and adolescents only, in left posterior temporo-parietal cortex. Finally, both studies included some additional indices of individual differences, none of which correlated with cortical structure: Lee et al. included measures of verbal fluency and both verbal and performance IQ, and Richardson et al. included a measure of matrix reasoning ability. These results demonstrate the importance of assessing readers’ word knowledge. In addition, both studies used their measures of general reasoning as nuisance variables in their regression analyses, so that any observed effects could be attributed specifically to linguistic factors, rather than to general cognitive ability.

Taken together, these studies provide evidence that at least a small set of cognitive skills related to reading comprehension may be correlated with cortical grey matter structure. However, it is equally clear that direct comparison of their results is not straightforward. For example, the studies of collegiate young adults included no measure of vocabulary knowledge, obviating a possible extension of the vocabulary findings. Both studies of college students (Goldman & Manis, 2013; Welcome et al., 2011) assessed decoding ability, but comparing their results is complicated: although one tested whether decoding related to cortical structure, the other employed it to differentiate groups of participants (and analyzed broadly defined cortical regions rather than specific areas of the brain). Both assessed reading comprehension, but Goldman and Manis did not examine the right hemisphere, precluding confirmation of differences in hemispheric asymmetry found by Welcome and colleagues. The populations tested differ in each study, with two using convenience samples of college students (age 18-24, Goldman & Manis; age 18-34, Welcome et al.), one using pre-collegiate adolescents (age 12-16, Lee et al., 2007), one using children from three different nations (age 8-13, Jednoróg et al., 2015), and one using a wide spectrum of ages (age 7-11, n=9; age 12-17, n=17; age 21-72, n=22; Richardson et al., 2010). These sampling differences are non-trivial for comparing the results of these studies, as there is ample evidence for neurodevelopmental structural changes across these age groups (Giedd et al., 1999; Gogtay et al, 2004; Lu et al, 2009; Paus, 2005; Salat et al., 2004). The consequence of these methodological differences – assessing different indices of grey matter structure, and different measures of reading-related abilities, in different populations – is that each study presents a relatively narrow account of potential neurostructural links to the components of literacy skill. That is, although the available evidence does not contain clear contradictions, it also does not admit clear conclusions, either about the literacy skills that might be related to cortical structure, or the cortical structures to which they might relate.

Despite these inconsistencies, there is one region whose grey matter structure appears to be consistently linked to components of reading comprehension: supramarginal gyrus. Furthermore, SMG’s structural relations to literacy skills may provide unique information about the neural substrates of reading comprehension. Specifically, both studies of vocabulary knowledge localized the correlation with grey matter density to posterior SMG (pSMG), rather than anterior SMG (aSMG). This distinction is important since, as noted by both Lee and colleagues (2007) and Richardson and colleagues (2010), pSMG has not been functionally related to language-specific processes. Rather, functional relations to language are typically reported in neighboring areas such as aSMG and AG. These regions are associated with functional activity during phonological (aSMG) and semantic (AG) processing (Booth et al., 2002; Démonet et al., 1992; Devlin, Matthews, & Rushworth, 2003; Gathercole, Hitch, Service, & Martin, 1997; Price, More, Humphreys, & Wise, 1997; Tan, Laird, Li, & Fox, 2005). Additional support for this structural dissociation comes from a complementary analysis of white matter tractography (Lee et al., 2007), which revealed that pSMG has direct connections with both aSMG and AG, but that aSMG and AG are not themselves directly connected. Although Goldman & Manis (2013) did not specify MNI coordinates for their findings (making it unclear whether print exposure was correlated with a specific subregion of SMG), the other studies reporting links to SMG are broadly consistent with this pattern. The area of SMG in which GMT related to decoding in children was anteriorly located (Jednoróg et al., 2015); and although Welcome and colleagues (2011) also did not discriminate subregions of SMG, their figures suggest that the effects driven by group differences in decoding were in anterior SMG (see Figures 2 and 3 of Welcome et al., pp. 1199 & 1201). Importantly, this fine-grained subdivision of the structure of SMG according to specific components of literacy is not possible on the basis of functional associations alone.

This study advances our knowledge of possible links between brain morphology and skills related to literacy achievement in young adult readers. It is notable for at least three reasons. First, we assessed literacy skills using a large battery of cognitive assessments. This battery included measures assessed in previous studies, such as decoding ability, reading comprehension, vocabulary knowledge, and print exposure, as well as additional cognitive abilities that were not. This study simultaneously assessed the unique contributions of a broad range of specific cognitive measures, and is a necessary step for reconciling the diverse findings from previous research. Second, we assessed two indices of cortical structure: grey matter thickness and grey matter volume. Most previous research examining relations between cortical structure and reading ability (especially in clinical populations – see the General Discussion) has focused on only one of these, typically the latter, which is intuitive: GMV is derived from GMT and cortical surface area, and it might therefore be expected that differences observed in GMV would be reflected in its component measures. Yet recent evidence shows that this is only true for cortical surface area; in contrast, differences that manifest in GMT may not be reflected in GMV, and vice versa (Frye et al., 2010; Greve et al., 2013), making it important to assess both in order to clearly characterize grey matter variation (cf. Winkler et al., 2010). Finally, although most previous work has recruited students of one kind or another, we chose to recruit a community-based sample of young adults who were not university students. The neurobiological bases of literacy skill have not been as extensively studied in young adults as in early language learners (Curtis, 2002), and even less is known about young adult readers who are not enrolled in (and may not plan to obtain) post-secondary education. Based on our previous work with this population, we expected a broad range in literacy-related skills across participants (Braze et al., 2007, 2011, 2016; Johns, Matsuki, & Van Dyke, 2015; Kukona et al., 2016; Kuperman & Van Dyke, 2011; Li et al., 2017; Magnuson et al., 2011; Shankweiler et al., 2008; Van Dyke et al., 2014), which confers an advantage in our power to detect individual differences (for discussion see Peterson, 2001).

Given the small number of studies that constitute the current state of the field, we consider this an exploratory investigation. The scarcity of previous research, as well as its methodological heterogeneity and diverse, non-overlapping patterns of results, make it difficult to propose specific hypotheses about potential links between cortical structure and behavioral measures of literacy-related skills. Thus, although we were particularly interested in SMG (see above), we did not define any hypothesis-driven regions of interest a priori. Rather, we conducted a naïve whole brain analysis without assumptions or restrictions based on the size, location, or direction of potential neurostructural correlations with the behavioral battery measures.

2. Method

2.1 Participants

We obtained informed consent from 39 young people recruited from the local community. We recruited participants in several ways, including presentations at adult education centers, advertisements in local newspapers, flyers placed on adult school campuses, community centers, public transportation hubs, local retail and laundry facilities. Of the 39 participants, four were left-handed, and their data were excluded from further analysis. The remaining 35 participants (ages 16-24 years, mean 20.44; 17 female) were right-handed native English speakers with normal or corrected-to-normal vision. Participants reported no history of psychiatric or neurological disorder, no active use of psychoactive medications, and no diagnosed reading or learning disability. Based on the Fast Reading subtest of the Stanford Diagnostic Reading Test (Karlson & Gardner, 1995), all participants demonstrated the ability to read well enough to comprehend basic texts (70% accuracy on attempted items). Each participant underwent two experimental sessions, each on a separate day. Participants received $80 for one scanning session, which lasted no longer than 60 minutes, together with one behavioral testing session lasting no longer than three hours. Behavioral testing was completed prior to the MRI scan. The Yale University Human Investigation Committee approved this protocol.

2.2 Literacy-related Cognitive Assessments

We administered a battery of behavioral tests of literacy-related skills and abilities. Standardized instruments were chosen to optimize construct validity and test-retest reliability. The standardized measures are widely used for clinical assessment and diagnosis, and were administered individually during individual test sessions. Two skills – working memory and print exposure – were not derived from standardized assessments; in these cases, we employed test instruments identical in format to those that are commonly used in experimental research. The skills we examined, and the tests associated with them, included:

  • Print exposure: Magazine Recognition Test (Acheson, Wells, & MacDonald, 2008; Cunningham & Stanovich, 1990), in which participants identify real magazine titles from a list that includes real and foil titles. We retained the original format of this test, but updated test items by replacing out-of-print titles with the names of current publications.

  • Vocabulary knowledge: Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test-Revised (PPVT-R; Dunn & Dunn, 1997), a test of receptive vocabulary knowledge, in which participants hear a target word and select a picture (from a group of four possibilities) that best depicts its definition.

  • Working memory capacity (WMC): assessed with the Sentence Span task (Daneman & Carpenter, 1980). Following the original format of this complex span assessment, participants heard sets of 2 – 6 sentences (number per set increases linearly), judging each as true or false; after each series, all sentence-final words must be recalled (in any order). We used an auditory variant of the task, permitting us to measure verbal working memory independent of the need to decode printed stimuli; moreover, we modified the sentence materials in order to make them more amenable for use with our community-based sample (for details, see Clark, McRoberts, Van Dyke, Shankweiler, & Braze, 2012).

  • Non-linguistic reasoning ability: assessed using the Weschler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence (WASI; Psychological Corp., 1999). We used the Matrix Reasoning subtest, in which participants completed visual analogical reasoning tasks, as a measure of general cognitive ability.

  • Phonological awareness: Comprehensive Test of Phonological Processing (CTOPP; Wagner, Torgesen, & Rashotte, 1999). We used the composite phonological awareness measure, derived from the Elision core subtest (forming words by eliding a phonological segment from spoken word prompts) and the Blending Words core subtest (spoken sounds are combined to form words).

  • Rapid naming: CTOPP (Wagner, Torgesen, & Rashotte, 1999), Rapid Letter Naming core subtest. Scores on this test reflect time to name letters presented in a grid-like array. Because RAN scores are naming times, lower scores indicate better performance.

  • Reading comprehension: Peabody Individual Achievement Test-Revised (PIAT-R; Markwardt, 1998). Participants read a series of sentences of increasing difficulty, choosing a picture (from a group of four possibilities) corresponding to the meaning of each. We administered odd numbered items to measure reading comprehension, reserving the even numbered items for a measure of speech sentence comprehension, described below (Leach, Scarborough, & Rescorla, 2003; Spring & French, 1990).

  • Listening comprehension: we created a listening comprehension assessment by splitting the PIAT-R (Markwardt, 1998), such that even numbered items were recorded and presented aurally in order to assess listening comprehension (Leach et al., 2003; Spring & French, 1990). The characteristics of the sentences, and the behavioral response task (i.e., selecting a picture from an array) are therefore identical to our reading comprehension measure.

  • Reading Fluency: indexed using the WJ-III silent reading fluency subtest, from the reading and oral comprehension area subtests (WJ-III; Woodcock, McGrew, & Mather, 2001). This test measures the speed of reading sentences silently and answering yes/no questions about each. We also measured oral reading fluency through a subset of the Gray Oral Reading Test, fourth edition (GORT, passages 5, 7, and 9; Wiederholt & Bryant, 2001). Reading time for each passage was converted to a rate using the published tables; these were summed to yield a single score.

  • Decoding skill: assessed using both the Woodcock-Johnson-III Tests of Achievement, reading and oral comprehension area subtests (WJ-III; Woodcock, McGrew, & Mather, 2001), Word Attack (reading a list of pseudowords aloud) and Letter-Word Identification (naming words from a list); and the Test of Word Reading Efficiency (TOWRE; Torgesen, Wagner, & Rashotte, 1999), sight word efficiency subtest (indexes the number of words that can be named in 45 seconds) and phonemic decoding efficiency subtests (indexes the number of pronounceable nonwords that can be named in 45 seconds).

2.3 Data Preparation

We inspected the distribution of the raw scores for each measure using density and quantile-quantile plots in order to assess univariate normality. We examined this via the Shapiro-Wilk test, which assesses both skewness and kurtosis (Shapiro & Wilk, 1965; implemented in the stats R package, R Core Team, 2016). Some measures showed significantly skewed distributions, which can inflate the influence of non-normal data. In order to correct this, we applied the Box-Cox transformation where appropriate (Box & Cox, 1964). The transformation equation is y(λ) = (yλ – 1) / λ, given λ ≠ 0; if λ = 0, then log(y). Using the caret R package (Kuhn, 2016), we calculated optimal lambda values for each measure. Lambda values close to 1.00 indicate that no transformation is necessary, since the distribution of the transformed data will be identical to the original data.

After addressing the distributional characteristics of the measures, all predictor variables were standardized: each was first mean-centered (i.e., the mean of each measure is subtracted from its value, setting its mean to zero, but leaving the standard deviation unchanged) and then scaled (i.e., the resulting values were converted to z-scores). Standardization has several well-established analytic benefits (as described in, e.g., McElreath, 2016), such as allowing straightforward comparison of the relative influence of predictor variables, (which might not be possible with unstandardized data due to, for example, differences in measurement and/or scale), and reducing potential problems related to multicollinearity among predictor variables (see below).

Some measures in our test battery target the same theoretical constructs. For these, we built composite variables to improve reliability and to more robustly represent the underlying constructs. We created two composite variables: Reading Fluency (comprised of the WJ-III Reading Fluency subtest and the GORT, r = .372, p = .028) and Word Decoding (comprised of the WJ-III Word Attack and Letter-Word Identification subtests, and the TOWRE subtests, all rs > .45, all ps < .01). Composites were derived by averaging component measures after they were first standardized, and then rescaling the resulting composite values. This approach is common in clinical and psycholinguistic studies of reading and reading-related skills (e.g., Braze et al., 2007; Guo, Roehrig, & Williams, 2011; Hua & Keenan, 2014; Kukona et al., 2016; Pugh et al., 2008; Sabatini, Sawaki, Shore, & Scarborough, 2010; Shankweiler et al., 2008; Van Dyke et al., 2014).

Finally, it is well known that performance on individual difference measures tends to be correlated, sometimes highly, making it difficult to uniquely relate specific constructs to dependent variables (for discussion see Freed, Hamilton, & Long, 2017). However, neither the number nor the magnitude of bivariate correlations is an unambiguous indicator of troublesome multicollinearity: strongly correlated measures may not induce problematic multicollinearity, while high multicollinearity can occur even when all bivariate correlations in a set of variables are quite low (Belsley, 1991a; Flom, 1999). Thus, although standardizing the predictor variables is known to reduce such dependencies (for discussion see McElreath, 2016), we nonetheless assessed the potential for problematic multicollinearity among our individual differences assessments. Using the perturb R package (Hendrickx, 2012), we calculated each predictor’s condition number (κ), which provides information about how much the variance associated with an estimated regression coefficient is increased because of overlap with other predictors (Belsley, Kuh, & Welsch, 1980). Condition numbers are considered to be both more informative and precise than other estimates of multicollinearity, such as the more commonly used variance inflation factor (Belsley, 1991a). This is because condition numbers, unlike variance inflation factors, provide not only estimates of shared variance but information about the ensembles of variables which may be sharing variance. By contrast, variance inflation factors are relatively uninformative, because they cannot account for connections among variables (Harrell, 2001). For condition numbers that are “absolutely small, for example, 5 or 10 … collinearity is not really a major problem” (Belsley, 1991b, p. 42), whereas κ ≥ 30 suggests problematic multicollinearity (Belsley, 1980; Belsley, 1991a,b; Faraway, 2014). Similarly, Baayen notes that for κ values “between 0 and 6, there is no collinearity to speak of. Medium collinearity is indicated by condition numbers around 15, and conditions numbers of 30 or more indicate potentially harmful collinearity” (Baayen, 2008, p. 182).

2.4 Structural Imaging

2.4.1 Image Acquisition and Processing

We collected volumetric data from high-resolution 3D MPRAGE anatomical images, acquired on a Siemens 1.5T Sonata MR system (192 sagittal slices; TE = 4.66 ms, TR = 2530 ms; FOV = 256 × 256 voxel matrix; resolution = 1.33 × 1.33 × 1.30 mm3). One whole-head, high resolution T1-weighted MPRAGE anatomical volume was acquired per participant. We used the FreeSurfer image analysis suite to perform cortical reconstruction and volumetric segmentation (Dale, Fischl, & Sereno, 1999; Dale & Sereno, 1993; Fischl & Dale, 2000; Fischl et al., 2001; http://surfer.nmr.mgh.harvard.edu, version 5.3). Prior to segmentation and classification, all images were visually inspected to identify potentially problematic motion artifacts. Subsequently, each segmentation was visually inspected to ensure 1) accurate skull stripping, 2) correct classification of grey/white matter boundaries, 3) appropriate separation of brain/non-brain matter. Minor adjustments to the automated segmentation and parcellation routines were made when necessary (e.g., adding control points to facilitate grey/white matter classification), but no major alterations were necessary. Non-brain tissue was removed using a hybrid watershed/surface deformation procedure (Ségonne et al., 2004). The resulting skull-stripped brain was processed using an automated Talairach transformation, segmentation of the subcortical white matter and deep gray matter volumetric structures (Fischl et al., 2002; Fischl et al., 2004), intensity normalization (Sled, Zijdenbos, & Evans, 1998), tessellation of the gray/white matter boundary, automated topology correction (Fischl et al., 2001; Ségonne, Pacheco, & Fischl, 2007), and surface deformation following intensity gradients to optimally place the gray/cerebrospinal and gray/white fluid borders at the location where the greatest shift in intensity defines the transition to the other tissue class (Dale et al., 1999; Dale & Sereno, 1993; Fischl & Dale, 2000). Both intensity and continuity information from the entire three-dimensional MR volume are used to produce representations of cortical thickness, calculated as the closest distance from the gray/white boundary to the gray/CSF boundary at each vertex on the tessellated surface (Fischl and Dale, 2000). The maps are not restricted to the voxel resolution of the original data, and are created using spatial intensity gradients across tissue classes (i.e., they are not simply reliant on absolute signal intensity). These procedures for the measurement of cortical thickness have been validated against histological analysis (Rosas et al., 2002) and manual measurements (Kuperberg et al., 2003; Salat et al., 2004).

The left and right hemispheres of all 35 participants were registered to the fsaverage atlas (common surface space) templates included in FreeSurfer, and smoothed with a Gaussian kernel of FWHM 10 mm. Each hemisphere was modeled separately. In contrast to non-surface based volumetric smoothing, surface-based smoothing only averages data from nearby vertices on the cortical mantle. This prevents the mixing of signal from nearby ridges and different tissue types and increases the spatial specificity of the averaged signal.

2.4.2 Spatial Analysis

Differences in the measurements of grey matter volume and thickness were examined for both the left and right cerebral hemispheres with a vertex by vertex general linear model (GLM). Statistical analysis was performed at each vertex to test the significance of the correlation between the individual difference measurements and these structural measurements. The model included 11 regressors: 10 corresponding to the behavioral individual difference assessments, and an additional covariate of no interest – participant age (mean centered) – which was modeled to reduce error variance. Total intracranial volume (TIV) is often included as a covariate for between-group designs and regions-of-interest analyses, with the goal of normalizing the data so that group-level effects are not confounded with individual differences in GMV. However, since we are specifically concerned with modeling correlations with individual differences in GMV, including TIV would explicitly remove variance relevant to this research question, making it an inappropriate covariate for our within-subjects design. In addition, there is broad agreement that GMT should not be normalized in any case (e.g., Fjell et al., 2009; Westman, Aguilar, Muehlboeck, & Simmons, 2013), and previous individual difference studies of GMT have thus not done so (e.g., Goldman & Manis, 2013; He et al., 2013).

Separate GLMs were constructed for GMV and GMT to estimate parameters for the covariates. These parameter estimates were then submitted to a group-level analysis and converted to p-value maps. Given the exploratory nature of this study, all vertex-wise results were thresholded at an individual vertex level of p < 0.05, and cluster extent thresholds corrected for multiple comparisons (p < 0.05) were calculated through Monte Carlo simulations of white noise on the cortical surface (Hagler, Saygin, & Sereno, 2006). These analyses permit the evaluation of the unique contributions of the regressors, in that they assess the proportion of variance in the dependent variables (GMT and GMV) that is associated with one predictor but not any of the other predictors (i.e., their squared semi-partial correlations; see, e.g., Keith, 2014; Tabachnick & Fidell, 2012). Finally, it is worth noting that the appropriate parameters for the analysis of neuroimaging data is a topic of active, ongoing discussion. For example, although Eklund, Nichols, & Knutsson (2016) advocate more stringent cluster-forming thresholds for functional data, their results were not subsequently replicated (Cox, Chen, Glen, Reynolds, & Taylor, 2017a, 2017b), and may not account for elevated Type II error (Lohmann et al., 2017). The implications of this debate for the analysis of structural MRI data have not been tested. Therefore, our cluster-forming threshold was selected to be consistent with both the exploratory nature of our study and the current state-of-the-science threshold used in other recent studies examining structural imaging data (e.g., Bizzo et al., 2017; Gardumi et al., 2017; Jasińska et al., 2017).

3. Results

3.1 Literacy-related Cognitive Assessments

All battery measures were analyzed with the R statistical software, version 3.3.1 (R Core Team, 2016). Descriptive information, including range, mean, standard deviation, are shown in Table 1. To aid interpretability and ease comparison with other studies, we include grade or age equivalents where possible. CTOPP scores do not have age or grade equivalents, but do include age-leveled percentile ranks. All analyses are based on raw scores except for the two TOWRE subtests and the WJ-III reading fluency subtest: because some participants completed these timed tests in less than the maximum time allotted, we converted scores on these measures into rates that index items-per-minute.

Table 1.

Descriptive statistics for all battery measures.

Measure Range M SD Max. possible
1. Magazine recognition test 0 23 10.31 5.66 40
2. Vocabulary knowledge 132 192 169.80 16.84 204
Age equivalent 10.4 23.1 18.93 4.51 > 23.1
3. Working memory capacity 28 60 46.37 7.26 60
4. Matrix Reasoning 9 31 23.89 4.61 48
Test Age equivalent 6.5 25-29 13.93 4.67 25-29
5. Phonological awareness 61 118 93.23 15.66 150
Percentile equivalent 1 89 38.86 29.42 99
6. Rapid letter naming (in seconds) 16 34 23.91 4.18 n/a
Percentile equivalent 5 99 52.34 28.45 99
7. Listening comprehension 14 40 33.97 4.79 41
Grade equivalent 3 13 9.80 2.31 13
8. Reading comprehension 20 40 32.74 5.60 41
Grade equivalent 3.8 13 9.26 2.9 13
9. Oral reading fluency 12 30 23.46 4.69 30
10. Silent reading fluency 51 98 74.69 15.04 98
Grade equivalent 5.8 19 12.65 6.60 > 18
Rate (items/min., max. 180 s) 17 43 25.68 6.68 n/a
11. Word identification 59 76 68.49 4.37 76
Grade equivalent 6.7 19 13.65 4.16 > 18
12. Letter-word identification 20 31 27.23 2.91 32
Grade equivalent 4.3 19 10.67 4.26 > 18
13. Sight word efficiency 75 104 91.71 8.92 104
Grade equivalent 5.8 12.6 10.68 2.12 > 12.6
Rate (items/min., max. 45 s) 100 156 123.90 14.66 n/a
14. Phonemic decoding efficiency 22 62 52.23 8.73 63
Grade equivalent 2.8 12.6 10.63 2.76 > 12.6
Rate (items/min., max. 45 s) 29 116 72.34 15.50 n/a

Note: 1: Print Exposure (adapted from Cunningham & Stanovich, 1990); 2: Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test-Revised (Dunn & Dunn, 1997; 3: Listening span (Daneman & Carpenter, 1980); 4: Weschler Abbreviated Scales of Intelligence (Psychological Corp., 1999); 5-6: Comprehensive Test of Phonological Awareness (Wagner et al., 1999); 7-8: Peabody Individual Achievement Test-Revised (Markwardt, 1998); 9: Gray Oral Reading Test (Wiederholt & Bryant, 2001); 10: Woodcock-Johnson-III Tests of Achievement (Woodcock et al., 2001) silent reading fluency.; 11-12: Woodcock-Johnson-III Tests of Achievement (Woodcock et al., 2001); 13-14: Test of Word Reading Efficiency (Torgesen et al., 1999).

We next examined the distribution of the raw scores for each measure for normality and potential outliers. Density and quantile-quantile plots suggested that some battery data were non-normal. The results of the Shapiro-Wilks normality test indicate that the data from seven battery measures deviated from normality. After applying the Box-Cox transformation to these measures (as described above), all distributions but one no longer deviated from normality. The remaining measure, WJ-III (silent) reading fluency, had a lambda of 1.04, indicating no advantage to transformation. The details of the tests of normality and of the Box-Cox data transformations appear in Table 2.

Table 2.

Results of the Shapiro-Wilk (W) test of normality before and after data transformation (and associated lambda (λ) values, where appropriate) for all battery and composite measures.

Measure W λ Box-Cox W
1. Magazine recognition test 0.972
2. Vocabulary knowledge 0.930* 4.36 0.956
3. Working memory capacity 0.978
4. Matrix Reasoning 0.869*** 2.93 0.974
5. Phonological awareness 0.965
6. Rapid letter naming 0.978
7. Listening comprehension 0.767**** 4.81 0.970
8. Reading comprehension 0.890** 3.49 0.946
9. Oral reading fluency 0.902** 2.94 0.957
10. Silent reading fluency 0.900** 1.04
11. Word identification 0.976
12. Letter-word identification 0.916* 4.80 0.958
13. Sight word efficiency 0.965
14. Phonemic decoding efficiency 0.967
9-10. Reading Fluency Composite 0.980
11-14. Decoding Composite 0.975
*

p < .05.

**

p < .01.

***

p < .001.

****

p < .0001.

transform unnecessary

Correlations among the 10 battery regressors are shown in Table 3. We observed a range of correlation strength, which we characterize according to the finer gradation proposed in Evans (1996). The correlations ranged from very weak (|r| ≤ .19), weak (.20 ≥ |r| ≤ .39), and moderate (.40 ≥ |r| ≤ .59) to strong (.60 ≥ |r| ≤ .79) and very strong (|r| ≥ .80). We observed moderate to strong correlations between those measures previously included in studies of cortical grey matter in non-dyslexic populations. For example, vocabulary knowledge was strongly correlated to decoding (r = .684, p < .0001), and moderately to print exposure (r = .552, p < .001); the latter measures were themselves also moderately correlated (r = .408, p = .015). Overall, the observed correlations are consistent with many other studies that have measured a broad range of literacy skills (e.g., Braze et al., 2007, 2016; Cromley, Snyder-Hogan, & Luciw-Dubas, 2010; Kukona et al., 2016; Li et al., 2017; Long et al., 2017; Long, Prat, Johns, Morris, & Jonathan, 2008; Macaruso & Shankweiler, 2010; Van Dyke et al., 2014).

Table 3.

Condition numbers (κ) for and correlations (r) among the individual differences regressors.

Measure κ 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10
1. Print Exposure 2.29 .001 .314 .728 .047 .574 .066 .099 .009 .015
2. Vocabulary 4.64 .552 .002 .057 .000 .884 .000 .000 .000 .000
3. Working Memory 3.98 .175 .506 .089 .006 .933 .002 .022 .046 .006
4. Reasoning 3.16 .061 .324 .291 .002 .049 .004 .000 .257 .117
5. Phonological Awareness 2.85 .339 .640 .456 .508 .178 .000 .000 .012 .000
6. Rapid Naming 3.68 −.098 −.026 −.015 .335 .233 .481 .123 .173 .028
7. Reading Comprehension 6.87 .315 .813 .506 .470 .630 .123 .000 .006 .003
8. Listening Comprehension 5.01 .283 .747 .385 .569 .605 .266 .778 .000 .000
9. Reading Fluency Composite 8.26 .435 .581 .339 .197 .420 −.236 .560 .454 .000
10. Decoding Composite 10.18 .408 .684 .452 .270 .624 −.372 .669 .483 .654

r| ≥ .335, p < .05, |r| ≥ .435, p < .01, |r| ≥ .552, p < .001, |r| ≥ .624, p < .0001

Note: Correlations appear below the diagonal; their associated p-values appear above the diagonal.

Finally, we assessed multicollinearity among the predictors by calculating their condition numbers (κ). Our analysis showed that multicollinearity among our predictors is not problematic for our subsequent analyses: all κs were substantially below 30, with all but three measures below 6. The condition number for each predictor appears in Table 3.1

3.2 Structural Imaging

Our whole brain analysis revealed numerous correlations between literacy skills and cortical structure in both hemispheres2. Significant correlations between our behavioral measures and grey matter structure appear in Table 4 (GMT; see Figure 1) and Table 5 (GMV; see Figure 2). Most these correlations were negative, indicating that more effective performance on a given measure was correlated with thinner, rather than thicker, GMT and/or reduced, rather than increased, GMV. The exception to this was the decoding composite, for which increased decoding efficiency corresponded to grey matter increases. The only predictors that were uncorrelated with any aspect of cortical structure in our analysis were participant age, vocabulary knowledge and rapid naming.

Table 4.

Grey matter thickness: individual difference measures, peak t-values, and centroid coordinates.

Measure Cluster Area
(mm2)
t p MNI Coordinates
x y z
Print Exposure LH ITG 1617 −4.01 .0001 −40.8 −59.4 −6.2
LH IFG (pars opercularis) 1202 −5.75 .0037 −54.3 19.4 16.4
LH SMG 1150 −2.53 .0051 −38.8 −44.8 35.5
LH caudal middle frontal 901 −2.20 .0251 −38.3 0.2 46.5
RH rostral middle frontal 3280 −3.83 .0001 42.3 28.4 21.2
RH MTG 1229 −3.66 .0040 47.8 −61.1 3.9
Working Memory RH precentral 1581 −3.15 .0004 34.8 −10.0 56.0
Phonological Awareness RH MTG 1129 −3.07 .0069 60.8 −40.5 −5.3
Listening Comprehension RH lingual 1420 3.58 .0008 32.8 −50.5 −6.1
Reading LH rostral middle frontal 2325 −2.92 .0001 −19.3 40.8 33.7
Comprehension LH ITG 1401 −3.39 .0007 −40.8 −59.4 −6.2
LH superior frontal 860 −3.27 .0333 −9.3 57.5 10.3
RH transverse temporal 3093 −5.97 .0001 39.2 −29.9 11.2
RH rostral middle frontal 1380 −4.52 .0010 41.9 26.1 21.6
RH inferior parietal 849 −3.54 .0470 52.0 −48.1 24.8
RH caudal middle frontal 841 −2.79 .0492 39.3 9.7 47.0
Decoding Composite RH STG 1720 4.23 .0001 43.8 −32.3 9.5
RH precentral 1357 3.76 .0012 23.1 −28.4 51.2
RH lateral occipital 1191 4.60 .0047 29.4 −88.0 11.7

Figure 1.

Figure 1

Cluster-corrected results for individual difference measures correlated with GMT projected onto the fsaverage template in FreeSurfer. All cluster-corrected results depicted at p < 0.05. (A) Results projected onto the left hemisphere; (B) results projected onto the right hemisphere. From top to bottom: (1) lateral view, (2) medial view, (3) ventral view. Note: yellow indicates overlap between reading comprehension skill and the decoding composite.

Table 5.

Grey matter volume: individual difference measures, peak t-values, and centroid coordinates.

Measure Cluster Area
(mm2)
t p MNI Coordinates
x y z
Print Exposure RH superior frontal 1133 −4.84 .0130 22.0 1.3 56.1
Working Memory LH superior frontal 905 −3.03 .0432 −17.2 16.1 57.7
Reasoning LH postcentral 895 −2.05 .0470 −53.3 −20.7 33.1
Reading Fluency Composite LH lateral occipital 1261 −2.29 .0048 −22.8 −94.0 7.0
Decoding Composite LH lateral occipital 1308 2.54 .0036 −21.5 −94.7 5.3
LH SMG 1257 3.03 .0048 −52.3 −26.4 20.2
LH lateral orbitofrontal 1244 2.89 .0058 −27.2 25.6 0.2
LH medial orbitofrontal 1223 3.27 .0061 −7.7 41.7 −16.6
LH superior frontal 1103 2.75 .0118 −8.7 11.5 54.4
LH STS 920 3.23 .0394 −50.2 −45.4 8.2
RH postcentral 3448 3.90 .0001 37.7 −7.1 17.7
RH superior frontal 2889 5.05 .0001 7.9 50.5 17.6
RH MTG 1148 3.01 .0121 57.6 −53.4 7.1
RH lingual 1030 3.46 .0246 22.4 −53.6 6.6
RH superior parietal 1002 2.81 .0291 22.0 −61.3 37.8

Figure 2.

Figure 2

Cluster-corrected results for individual difference measures correlated with GMV projected onto the fsaverage template in FreeSurfer. All cluster-corrected results depicted at p < 0.05. (A) Results projected onto the left hemisphere; (B) results projected onto the right hemisphere. From top to bottom: (1) lateral view, (2) medial view. Note: purple indicates overlap between matrix reasoning ability and the decoding composite; yellow indicates overlap between the reading fluency and decoding composite measures.

4. Discussion

Our discussion focuses on the subset of the overall findings that converge with previous studies. This encompasses SMG, left IFG, left STS, and areas in right frontal and parietal areas. Given SMG’s prominence in previous studies, we highlight significant correlations between the grey matter structure of this region and literacy skills in Figure 3.

Figure 3.

Figure 3

Left supramarginal gyrus: the bounded area is the Desikan-Killiany parcellation of SMG in MNI space. Positive correlations with GMV are depicted on a red-yellow scale, and negative correlations with GMT are depicted on a blue-white scale. Results depicted at an uncorrected vertex-wise threshold of p < 0.05.

4.1 Supramarginal Gyrus

Our analysis confirms and extends several previous findings in this region, as well as producing significant novel results. In our study, decoding ability was positively correlated with GMV in SMG. This confirms a previous finding in children (Jednoróg et al., 2015), and is also in line with our interpretation of the group differences observed between good and both poor and resilient readers (Welcome et al., 2011). In addition, we observed a significant negative correlation between print exposure and GMT in left SMG, extending posteriorly to AG, indicating that greater experience with printed material was associated with thinner grey matter in our sample. This confirms the previous report of a relation between print exposure and GMT in SMG, although the correlation in that study was positive (Goldman & Manis, 2013). This discrepancy may stem from methodological differences, e.g., our use of a broad, community-based sample of participants, rather than a relatively skilled subgroup (i.e., college students) of the population. Furthermore, our finding is also consistent with a hypothesis initially proposed by Goldman and Manis: that a negative correlation between cortical thickness and print exposure is compatible with longitudinal evidence that cortical thinning is a byproduct of maturation, possibly related to skill consolidation (as proposed in, e.g., Lu et al., 2007; see also Sowell et al., 2001, 2004). Finally, we found no relation between vocabulary knowledge and the structure of SMG in our young adult readers. This is consistent with previous findings indicated that such a relation was only present in adolescents between 12 and 17 years of age (Lee et al., 2007; Richardson et al., 2010), a range with which our sample barely overlaps.

Our results also dovetail with previous studies indicating that SMG can be bisected into distinct anterior and posterior regions. Here, the correlation with decoding efficiency was centered in anterior SMG, and the centroid of the correlation with print exposure was in the posterior region of SMG. Although ours is the first study to directly relate decoding to the structure of aSMG in monolingual young adult readers, this result is analogous to previous findings in children (Jednoróg et al., 2015) and bilingual young adults (L2 English; He et al., 2013). In the latter case, decoding was positively correlated with GMV in aSMG in a large sample (N = 253) of native Chinese speakers. Our study is also the first to directly relate readers’ experience with printed material to structural aspects of pSMG. Furthermore, print exposure is known to be correlated with vocabulary knowledge (Acheson et al., 2008; Long et al., 2008; Stanovich, 1993; Stanovich & West, 1989; Stanovich, West, & Harrison, 1995), as indeed it is in our data (r = .552, p < .001). Reading facilitates acquisition of novel word forms and the development of skilled spelling (Mol & Bus, 2011). Indeed, readers with greater print experience are more likely to encounter rare words in print than in spoken language (e.g., Carroll, Davies, & Richman, 1971), and potentially see ten times as many words as readers with more limited exposure (Nagy & Anderson, 1984). It is therefore no surprise that experience with printed matter is a significant vehicle by which new words are acquired (Cunningham, Stanovich, & West, 1994; Stanovich, 1986). Thus, the relation between grey matter in pSMG and print exposure that we report here is clearly not inconsistent with previously reported structural correlations between pSMG and vocabulary knowledge (Lee et al., 2007; Richardson et al., 2010).

4.2 Left Inferior Frontal Gyrus

Our whole-brain analysis also found a relation between literacy skills and pars opercularis in left IFG. Pars opercularis – as part of Broca’s Area – has long been associated with many language-related processes (for review, see Friederici, 2011). As in SMG, print exposure was negatively correlated with GMT in this area, indicating that greater experience with printed matter was associated with thinner cortex (see Figure 1). This confirms the previously reported link between print exposure and left IFG but, again, the direction of the correlation in our study differs from the first report (Goldman & Manis, 2013). As previously noted, we suspect that the results diverge in this way due to methodological and analytic differences between the two studies.

Our findings align with those of Goldman and Manis (2013) in one regard: neither study obtained evidence for a correlation between decoding ability and GMT in IFG. Such a relation might have been expected because of two prior findings. First, longitudinal research suggests that the cortex thickens in IFG (and bilateral STG) over time in young children (Sowell et al., 2004). Second, thickening in pars opercularis during normal development in childhood has been related to increasingly proficient phonological awareness (Lu et al., 2007). Phonemic awareness is a necessary precursor to skilled decoding ability, and corresponds to the capacity to exploit knowledge about individual phonemes in a language (Scarborough & Brady, 2002). However, as with decoding ability, we found no evidence that phonological awareness correlated with either GMT or GMV in IFG, suggesting that the correlation observed in children reflects active neural development that may not be present in our more developmentally mature sample. Instead, in our participants, phonological awareness was negatively correlated with GMT in right middle temporal gyrus (see Figure 1), a region that is functionally important for discriminating sublexical speech sounds (Boets et al., 2013).

The absence of a relation between decoding skill and cortical structure in left IFG in our participants may also be related to the fact that decoding skill – which is strongly correlated with phonological awareness (in this study, r = .624, p < .0001) – decreases in importance as children grow into competent comprehenders, explaining progressively less variance related to comprehension (Wagner et al. 1997; Goldman & Manis, 2013). In contrast, the importance of experience with printed matter increases throughout maturation, from explaining 12% of the variance in oral language comprehension in kindergarten-age children to 34% in college-aged young adults (Mol & Bus, 2011). The pattern of relations that we observed for both decoding and print exposure in this region is consistent with these developmental shifts.

4.3 Left Superior Temporal Sulcus

We observed a positive correlation between decoding ability and GMV in left STS (Figure 2). There is no analog for this finding in the previous literature investigating cortical structure in non-dyslexic readers (but see our discussion of clinical similarities below). Rather, this region was previously related to vocabulary knowledge in this population (Richardson et al., 2010), such that greater vocabulary knowledge was associated with greater grey matter density. In addition, because the correlation was consistent across age groups, Richardson and colleagues suggested that this region might be related to processes of vocabulary acquisition that are independent of formal instruction (as opposed to their proposed relation between pSMG and vocabulary, to which we return below). We obtained no evidence that vocabulary was related to grey matter structure in this region. However, vocabulary knowledge in our sample was strongly correlated with decoding (r = .684, p < .0001), and this may be a case in which variance that might otherwise have been assigned to vocabulary knowledge may instead have been attributed to other measures that were not modeled in Richardson et al.

4.4 Right Frontal/Parietal Regions

Several correlations emerged between cortical structure in right frontal and parietal areas and measures in our individual differences battery. As shown in Table 4, these include relations between GMT and print exposure, working memory capacity, phonological awareness, comprehension, and decoding ability; and per Table 5, between GMV and both print exposure and decoding ability. We know of no previous reports of structural relations between these brain regions and behavioral assessments of these literacy-related skills. Nevertheless, Welcome and colleagues (2011) observed group differences in radial expansion, a measure related to cortical surface area, in broadly-defined right frontal/parietal regions. Specifically, poor comprehenders showed smaller radial expansion in these areas than both proficient and resilient readers. To the extent that either GMV or GMT are related to radial expansion, some of our results loosely correspond with those of Welcome and colleagues. However, whereas Welcome and colleagues reported that radial expansion was relatively small in poor readers, some of our findings are in the opposite direction, with thinner GMT and/or smaller GMV associated with better performance on individual difference measures. In light of the unclear relation between radial expansion and more traditional indices of cortical grey matter structure (discussed in greater detail by Welcome et al., p. 1203), and the associated difficulty in interpreting the group differences in Welcome and colleagues’ study relative to our continuous approach, we refrain from offering any interpretation of this difference.

5. General Discussion

The goal of this study was to assess potential relations between a broad array of the components of literacy and cortical grey matter structure in young adult readers. Although there are few previous research reports addressing this question, many aspects of our study make contact with earlier work, while also offering some methodological or analytic extensions. This study clearly demonstrates that mastery of fundamental skills related to proficient reading comprehension can be reflected in neurostructural characteristics of an array of language-relevant brain regions. The breadth of our cognitive assessments allowed us to both identify correlations between cortical structure and literacy skills that are consistent across previous studies in spite of methodological differences, and to assess whether individual literacy skills contributed uniquely to any structural variance observed in our imaging results. In addition, by measuring both GMV and GMT, we were able to make a detailed assessment of neuroanatomical relations to literacy skills – one which potentially accounts for differing patterns of results in these two measures. Finally, our use of a community-based sample confers several advantages to our study. This sample is more representative of the population at large than a convenience sample of undergraduates, and consequently the range of literacy and literacy-related abilities is greater than is typical in most neuroimaging studies of adult readers, which increases our ability to detect meaningful relationships between our assessments and grey matter structure.

On balance, our findings indicate that two measures – print exposure and decoding ability – are associated with grey matter structure in regions known to be related to language comprehension in general and, as we will describe below, to reading-specific behaviors in particular. This latter point is not trivial, since both print exposure and decoding skill reflect a reader’s interaction with orthographic input. Both are particularly important to the development of orthographic processing skill (i.e., the ability to construct, encode, and retrieve orthographic representations), which is thought to explain unique variance in word recognition (Cunningham & Stanovich, 1990, 1997; Stanovich, 1986; Stanovich & West, 1989). Skilled orthographic processing strengthens readers’ awareness of the phonological parameters of their language (Ehri, 1984; Nation & Hulme, 2011), enabling a tight link between words’ orthographic and phonological representations (Barron, 1986; Ehri, 1980, 1987) and, ultimately, facilitating efficient lexical access by means of these representations.

It is therefore notable that many of the cortical regions that we have discussed thus far have been functionally linked to establishing relations between orthographic and phonological information. For example, aSMG (but not pSMG) is active during the conversion of orthographic input to licit phonological information (e.g., Booth et al., 2002; Hartwigsen et al., 2010; Richlan et al., 2009; Tan et al., 2005; see also Heim et al., 2010, discussed below). In IFG, indices of print exposure have been functionally related to this area’s role in print-speech convergence, such that greater print exposure was related to greater functional overlap during the processing of spoken and written sentences (Shankweiler et al., 2008). Left STS has been implicated in processes of orthography-to-phonology conversion (van Atteveldt, Formisano, Goebel, & Blomert, 2004), with dysfunction in this region indicating impaired translation of novel graphemes to licit phonemes. (Perhaps unsurprisingly, left STS is also proximate to heteromodal language processing regions associated with print-speech convergence; Braze et al., 2011; Frost et al., 2009; Shankweiler et al., 2008.) And the importance of left fusiform gyrus (and the VWFA) to word reading is of course well known (McCandliss, Cohen, & Dehaene, 2003).

Further, many of the right hemisphere regions in which we observed structural correlations with literacy skills have been functionally implicated in studies of semantic and syntactic relations, sentence comprehension, and discourse processing (e.g., Kuperberg, Lakshmanan, Caplan, & Holcomb, 2006; Petersson, Folia, & Hagoort, 2012; Prat & Just, 2011; Prat, Mason, & Just, 2011; Robertson et al., 2000; St. George, Kutas, Martinez, & Sereno, 1999; Snijders et al., 2009). Right hemisphere homologs of “classical” left hemisphere regions associated with language processing are often concurrently activated during processing, and as such they might best be considered as part of an extended cortical network related to language comprehension in general (Hagoort, 2009).

In addition to the correspondence between the structural relations to literacy-related skills that we have observed and previous functional research, our results also support previous reports of a unique structural relation between pSMG and reading-related skills that has no functional analog. In previous research, grey matter density in this region was correlated with vocabulary, with greater density indicative of a more extensive vocabulary knowledge (Lee et al., 2007; Richardson et al., 2010; see also Mechelli et al., 2004). Given its direct connections to both aSMG and AG, these studies concluded that pSMG is likely a binding site for orthographic, phonological, and semantic information. In addition, they proposed that because this relation was obtained only in adolescents, it might be a cortical reflection of formal instruction in the service of vocabulary acquisition (see also Richardson & Price, 2009).

We concur with the broader conclusion of these studies regarding SMG’s potential relevance to the assembly of high-dimensional lexical representations. However, our observation of correlations between decoding ability and aSMG, and print exposure and pSMG, suggest a revised account of the link between pSMG and vocabulary. We agree that aSMG may be chiefly concerned with phonological aspects of grapheme-to-phoneme translation. We suggest that pSMG, in contrast, may be primarily concerned with orthographic aspects of this process; that is, the structure of pSMG may not reflect the number of lexical forms added to a reader’s vocabulary, or the way in which these forms were acquired (Lee et al., 2007; Richardson et al., 2009, 2010), but the amount of reading that a person actually does3. This explanation is more parsimonious than the proposal that pSMG is related to vocabulary instruction – an explanation that was always somewhat problematic given that the relation with vocabulary knowledge was not present in children, in whom increasing vocabulary knowledge has instead been related to cortical thinning in left parietal regions (Sowell et al., 2004; see also Linkersdörfer et al., 2014). This explanation is also consistent with SMG’s well-established functional role in linking graphemes to their phonemic equivalents during reading.

It is worth noting that our study reports different cortical grey matter measurements than those studies examining the pSMG and vocabulary knowledge. We report correlations with GMT and GMV, whereas Lee et al. (2007) and Richardson et al. (2010) assessed relations to grey matter density, an index of the ratio of grey matter voxels to those of other types of brain tissue in a particular cortical region (for a detailed discussion, see Mechelli, Price, Friston, & Ashburner, 2005). Because grey matter density, GMT, and GMV are partially independent measures, it is reasonable to expect them to show different patterns of results. Further, as we discussed in section 4.1 above, experience with printed material is an established precursor to vocabulary knowledge (see also Cunningham & Stanovich, 1991; Stanovich & Cunningham, 1992; Stanovich & West, 1989). By assessing print exposure and vocabulary together, our study examined whether these measures explained unique variance in our indices of cortical structure. Our findings in this region (and others, such as left STS) suggests that it is important to assess skills linked to multiple components of reading ability, rather than a single measure (or a small set of measures), in order to determine whether effects are related to common variance shared among the measures, or specific contributions of particular cognitive abilities.

Finally, there is also substantial agreement between our findings and the results of neuropsychological studies of group differences between dyslexic and typical readers. Although there is considerable heterogeneity among the results of such studies, two recent meta-analyses clearly identify several points of convergence. For example, Linkersdörfer, Lonnemann, Lindberg, Hasselhorn, & Fiebach, (2012, N=277) identified four cortical areas in which dyslexic readers consistently exhibit smaller GMV: the bilateral supramarginal gyrus (SMG), left fusiform gyrus (including VWFA) and the right superior temporal gyrus (STG). In contrast, Richlan, Kronbichler, & Wimmer, (2013, N=266) found only two: right STG and left superior temporal sulcus (STS). In our study, we observed significant relations between literacy skills and all of these regions that are consistent with this literature. Decoding ability was positively correlated with GMV in left SMG and left STS, and with GMT in right STG in our community-based sample, such that poor decoders also had smaller GMV/thinner cortex than those with more efficient decoding ability. Given the obvious relation between dyslexia and decoding ability, the parallel between our results and the clinical pattern is clear, with some functional imaging research explicitly suggesting that these group differences are related to impaired grapheme-to-phoneme conversion (e.g., Heim et al., 2010). Furthermore, we also observed a relation between GMT and both print exposure and reading comprehension in an area of left inferior temporal gyrus (ITG) corresponding to the fusiform gyrus. Here the correlation was negative, with greater experience with printed matter and greater reading comprehension ability coinciding with thinner grey matter. However, we do not believe this difference suggests any meaningful conflict between ours and the neuropsychological studies. Rather, the relation of GMV to group differences related to dyslexia and, in our study, of GMT and the development of skilled reading comprehension are parallel, rather than contradictory, findings.

6. Conclusions and Future Directions

Skilled reading is the result of the fluent orchestration of numerous component processes, some of which are thought to be linguistically-based, whereas others are domain-general. The results of this exploratory study confirm, extend, and refine many of the diverse findings of previously published work. In addition, this study clearly demonstrates the importance of taking a multifaceted approach to investigating the neurostructural correlates of skilled reading comprehension. This is true not just for future investigations of cortical grey matter, but for investigations of other aspects of brain morphology as well. For example, there are a small number of studies in which literacy skills have been correlated with white matter structure. We have mentioned one such study, in which vocabulary knowledge informed our understanding of the structural significance of pSMG (Lee et al., 2007). There are others exploring relations between the connectivity of regions in the left-hemisphere reading network and, for example, decoding ability (Welcome & Joanisse, 2014; Zhang et al., 2014; in dyslexic readers, see Pugh et al., 2000; Steinbrink et al., 2008). Taking white matter structure into account could be particularly important since increased white matter myelination decreases the space between the cortex and the skull, potentially contributing to grey matter thinning (Lu et al., 2007; Sowell et al., 2004). However, as in previous investigations of cortical grey matter, most such studies have been narrowly construed, suggesting that a broad-based approach to assessing participants’ literacy skills could be similarly applied, with the same potential benefits to analysis and interpretation (e.g., Van Dyke et al., 2015).

Indeed, we view this approach to assessing individual cognitive differences as essential to future research into the neural architecture supporting reading comprehension. This is consistent with recent behavioral research on the relation between individual cognitive variation and skilled reading. Although our own battery of assessments is more extensive than previous work, it is hardly exhaustive; large-scale behavioral studies typically administer many more assessments than were included in our project. For example, studies such as Van Dyke, Johns, & Kukona (2014) and Freed and colleagues (2017) both assessed performance on more than two dozen measures. The benefits offered by such an approach are twofold. First, expanding the battery of assessments allows the testing of theoretically important constructs for which previous research (including our own) does not account. As noted by Freed and colleagues, individual differences in both perceptual speed and suppression/inhibition ability, neither of which have been assessed in studies such as ours, are both likely contributors to comprehension performance. Second, more extensive test batteries allow theoretical constructs to be measured with multiple assessments, which would permit the construction of more stable composite predictors (cf. Braze et al., 2016). This is particularly important for non-standardized instruments, such as the complex span tasks that are in common use to assess working memory capacity: the test-retest reliability of these measures is known to be relatively low, and a composite based on two or three assessments (e.g., sentence span, operation span, symmetry span, etc.) has much greater predictive stability (Waters & Caplan, 2003). Finally, as noted in our introduction, most of this research has been conducted with adult readers. However, studies such as Lee and colleagues (2007) and Richardson and colleagues (2010) make it clear that assessing developmentally distinct groups of readers is important for understanding neurostructural correlates of individual cognitive abilities. For example, as discussed above, experience with printed material increases in importance as readers age, while decoding ability experiences a corresponding decrease in importance (Mol & Bus, 2011); this yields testable predictions about both when and where these abilities should have correlates in the brain. In light of evidence that both grey and white matter are sensitive to efforts at remediation in poor readers (e.g., Keller & Just, 2009; Krafnick, Flowers, Napoliello, & Eden, 2011), it seems clear that a comprehensive assessment of both cortical structure and the precise cognitive capacities that support the achievement of literacy is essential for our understanding of the neural architecture supporting skilled reading comprehension.

Acknowledgments

This research was supported by the following NIH grants to Haskins Laboratories: P01 HD-01994 (Jay G. Rueckl, PI), R01 HD-040353 (Donald P. Shankweiler, PI), R01 HD-071988 (David Braze, PI), R01 HD-073288 (Julie A. Van Dyke, PI), and NRSA HD-080331 (Dave Kush, PI). The authors are grateful to Jessica Grittner for assistance with data collection, to Morgan L. Bontrager, Alexis R. Johns, Leonard Katz, Victor Kuperman, and Ashley G. Lewis for insightful comments and advice on earlier drafts of this paper, and to P. R. Nelson for support and inspiration.

Footnotes

1

Although, as noted in the Method, we have reservations about using variance inflation factors (VIFs) to index multicollinearity, we nonetheless calculated them for our predictors. The VIFs converged with the results of the condition numbers: their range (1.43 – 6.08) was not only below the commonly recommended threshold of 10 (e.g., Cohen, Cohen, West, & Aiken, 2003; James et al., 2013; Kutner, Nachtsheim, Neter, & Li, 2005), but also below a more conservative recent recommendation of 7 (Keith, 2014).

2

In line with recent recommendations (e.g., Eklund, Nichols, & Knutsson, 2017), all structural MRI data and analysis scripts will be made available, at OpenNeuro (http://www.openneuro.org), a free online repository for neuroimaging data.

3

Similarly, Goldman & Manis (2013) observed a relation between SMG and the amount of pleasure reading their participants engaged in outside of instructional settings. Although the absence of MNI coordinates in this study does not permit assessment of the locus of their SMG correlations, we mention it here as weak converging evidence for our account of pSMG’s structural relation to reader characteristics.

References

  1. Acheson DJ, Wells JB, MacDonald MC. New and updated tests of print exposure and reading abilities in college students. Behavior Research Methods. 2008;40(1):278–289. doi: 10.3758/brm.40.1.278. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Anderson RC, Wilson PT, Fielding LG. Growth in reading and how children spend their time outside of school. Reading Research Quarterly. 1988;23(3):285–303. [Google Scholar]
  3. Baayen RH. Analyzing linguistic data: A practical introduction to statistics using R. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press; 2008. [Google Scholar]
  4. Barron RW. Word recognition in early reading: A review of the direct and indirect access hypotheses. Cognition. 1986;24(1-2):93–119. doi: 10.1016/0010-0277(86)90006-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Belsley DA. Conditioning diagnostics: Collinearity and weak data in regression. New York, NY: John Wiley & Sons, Inc; 1991a. [Google Scholar]
  6. Belsley DA. A guide to using the collinearity diagnostics. Computational Economics. 1991b;4(1):33–50. [Google Scholar]
  7. Belsley DA, Kuh E, Welsch RE. Regression diagnostics: Identifying influential data and sources of collinearity. Vol. 571. New York, NY: John Wiley & Sons; 1980. [Google Scholar]
  8. Boets B, de Beeck HPO, Vandermosten M, Scott SK, Gillebert CR, Mantini D, Ghesquière P. Intact but less accessible phonetic representations in adults with dyslexia. Science. 2013;342:1251–1254. doi: 10.1126/science.1244333. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Bizzo BC, Sanchez TA, Tukamoto G, Zimmermann N, Netto TM, Gasparetto EL. Cortical Thickness and Episodic Memory Impairment in Systemic Lupus Erythematosus. Journal of Neuroimaging. 2017;27(1):122–127. doi: 10.1111/jon.12394. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Bolger DJ, Perfetti CA, Schneider W. Cross-cultural effect on the brain revisited: Universal structures plus writing system variation. Human Brain Mapping. 2005;25(1):92–104. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20124. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Booth JR, Burman DD, Meyer JR, Gitelman DR, Parrish TB, Mesulam MM. Functional anatomy of intra-and cross-modal lexical tasks. NeuroImage. 2002;16(1):7–22. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2002.1081. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Box GE, Cox DR. An analysis of transformations. Journal of the Royal Statistical Society Series B (Methodological) 1964:211–252. [Google Scholar]
  13. Braze D, Katz L, Magnuson JS, Mencl WE, Tabor W, Van Dyke JA, Gong T, Johns CL, Shankweiler DP. Vocabulary does not complicate the simple view of reading. Reading and Writing. 2016;29(3):435–451. doi: 10.1007/s11145-015-9608-6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Braze D, Mencl WE, Tabor W, Pugh KR, Constable RT, Fulbright RK, Shankweiler DP. Unification of sentence processing via ear and eye: an fMRI study. Cortex. 2011;47(4):416–431. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2009.11.005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Braze D, Tabor W, Shankweiler DP, Mencl WE. Speaking up for vocabulary reading skill differences in young adults. Journal of Learning Disabilities. 2007;40(3):226–243. doi: 10.1177/00222194070400030401. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Carreiras M, Seghier ML, Baquero S, Estévez A, Lozano A, Devlin JT, Price CJ. An anatomical signature for literacy. Nature. 2009;461(7266):983–986. doi: 10.1038/nature08461. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Carroll JB, Davies P, Richman B. The American Heritage word frequency book. Boston: Houghton Mifflin; 1971. [Google Scholar]
  18. Castro-Caldas A, Miranda PC, Carmo I, Reis A, Leote F, Ribeiro C, Ducla-Soares E. Influence of learning to read and write on the morphology of the corpus callosum. European Journal of Neurology. 1999;6(1):23–28. doi: 10.1046/j.1468-1331.1999.610023.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Chiarello C, Lombardino LJ, Kacinik NA, Otto R, Leonard CM. Neuroanatomical and behavioral asymmetry in an adult compensated dyslexic. Brain and Language. 2006;98(2):169–181. doi: 10.1016/j.bandl.2006.04.012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Clark NB, McRoberts GW, Van Dyke JA, Shankweiler DP, Braze D. Immediate memory for pseudowords and phonological awareness are associated in adults and pre-reading children. Clinical Linguistics & Phonetics. 2012;26(7):577–596. doi: 10.3109/02699206.2012.673045. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Clements-Stephens AM, Materek AD, Eason SH, Scarborough HS, Pugh KR, Rimrodt S, Pekar JJ, Cutting LE. Neural circuitry associated with two different approaches to novel word learning. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience. 2012;2:S99–S113. doi: 10.1016/j.dcn.2011.06.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Cohen J, Cohen P, West SG, Aiken LS. Applied multiple regression/correlation analysis for the behavioral sciences. 3rd. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates; 2013. [Google Scholar]
  23. Cox RW, Chen G, Glen DR, Reynolds RC, Taylor PA. FMRI Clustering in AFNI: False-Positive Rates Redux. Brain Connectivity. 2017a;7(3):152–171. doi: 10.1089/brain.2016.0475. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Cox RW, Chen G, Glen DR, Reynolds RC, Taylor PA. fMRI clustering and false-positive rates. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 2017b;114(17):E3374–E3375. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1614961114. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Cromley JG, Snyder-Hogan LE, Luciw-Dubas UA. Reading comprehension of scientific text: A domain-specific test of the direct and inferential mediation model of reading comprehension. Journal of Educational Psychology. 2010;102(3):687–700. [Google Scholar]
  26. Cunningham AE, Stanovich KE. Assessing print exposure and orthographic processing skill in children: A quick measure of reading experience. Journal of Educational Psychology. 1990;82(4):733–740. [Google Scholar]
  27. Cunningham AE, Stanovich KE. Tracking the unique effects of print exposure in children: Associations with vocabulary, general knowledge, and spelling. Journal of Educational Psychology. 1991;83(2):264–274. [Google Scholar]
  28. Cunningham AE, Stanovich KE. Early reading acquisition and its relation to reading experience and ability 10 years later. Developmental Psychology. 1997;33(6):934–945. doi: 10.1037//0012-1649.33.6.934. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Cunningham AE, Stanovich KE, West RF. Literacy environment and the development of children’s cognitive skills. In: Assink EMH, editor. Literacy acquisition and social context: Approaches, emphases, and questions. London: Harvester-Wheatsheaf; 1994. pp. 70–90. [Google Scholar]
  30. Curtis ME. Adolescent reading: A synthesis of research. Paper presented as US Department of Education and National Institute of Child Health and Human Development Conference on Adolescent Literacy. 2002 Retrieved 24 June 2004 from http://216.26.160.105/conf/nichd/synthesis.asp.
  31. Dale AM, Fischl B, Sereno MI. Cortical surface-based analysis: I. Segmentation and surface reconstruction. NeuroImage. 1999;9(2):179–194. doi: 10.1006/nimg.1998.0395. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Dale AM, Sereno MI. Improved localization of cortical activity by combining EEG and MEG with MRI cortical surface reconstruction: a linear approach. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 1993;5(2):162–176. doi: 10.1162/jocn.1993.5.2.162. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Daneman M, Carpenter PA. Individual differences in working memory and reading. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior. 1980;19(4):450–466. [Google Scholar]
  34. Démonet JF, Chollet F, Ramsay S, Cardebat D, Nespoulous JL, Wise R, Frackowiak R. The anatomy of phonological and semantic processing in normal subjects. Brain. 1992;115(6):1753–1768. doi: 10.1093/brain/115.6.1753. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Devlin JT, Matthews PM, Rushworth MF. Semantic processing in the left inferior prefrontal cortex: a combined functional magnetic resonance imaging and transcranial magnetic stimulation study. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 2003;15(1):71–84. doi: 10.1162/089892903321107837. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Dunn LM, Dunn LM. Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test. 3rd. American Guidance Services; Circle Pines, MN: 1997. [Google Scholar]
  37. Eckert M. Neuroanatomical markers for dyslexia: a review of dyslexia structural imaging studies. The Neuroscientist. 2004;10(4):362–371. doi: 10.1177/1073858404263596. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Ehri LC. The development of orthographic images. In: Frith U, editor. Cognitive processes in spelling. London: Academic Press; 1980. pp. 311–338. [Google Scholar]
  39. Ehri LC. How orthography alters spoken language competencies in children learning to read and spell. In: Downing J, Valtin R, editors. Language awareness and learning to read. New York: Springer; 1984. pp. 119–147. [Google Scholar]
  40. Ehri LC. Learning to read and spell words. Journal of Literacy Research. 1987;19(1):5–31. [Google Scholar]
  41. Eklund A, Nichols TE, Knutsson H. Cluster failure: why fMRI inferences for spatial extent have inflated false-positive rates. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 2016;113(28):7900–7905. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1602413113. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Eklund A, Nichols TE, Knutsson H. Reply to Brown and Behrmann, Cox, et al. and Kessler et al.: Data and code sharing is the way forward for fMRI. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 2017;114(17):E3374–E3375. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1620285114. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Evans JD. Straightforward statistics for the behavioral sciences. Pacific Grove, CA: Brooks/Cole Publishing Company; 1996. [Google Scholar]
  44. Faraway JJ. Extending the linear model with R: generalized linear, mixed effects and nonparametric regression models. 2nd. New York, NY: CRC press; 2014. [Google Scholar]
  45. Fischl B, Dale AM. Measuring the thickness of the human cerebral cortex from magnetic resonance images. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 2000;97(20):11050–11055. doi: 10.1073/pnas.200033797. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Fischl B, Salat DH, Busa E, Albert M, Dieterich M, Haselgrove C, Montillo A. Whole brain segmentation: automated labeling of neuroanatomical structures in the human brain. Neuron. 2002;33(3):341–355. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(02)00569-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Fischl B, Salat D, Kennedy D, Makris N, Albert M, Killiany R, Dale A. Automatic segmentation of the structures in the human brain. NeuroImage. 2001;13(6):118. [Google Scholar]
  48. Fischl B, Salat DH, van der Kouwe AJ, Makris N, Ségonne F, Quinn BT, Dale AM. Sequence-independent segmentation of magnetic resonance images. NeuroImage. 2004;23:S69–S84. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.07.016. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Fjell AM, Westlye LT, Amlien I, Espeseth T, Reinvang I, Raz N, Dale AM. High consistency of regional cortical thinning in aging across multiple samples. Cerebral Cortex. 2009;19(9):2001–2012. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhn232. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Flom P. Unpublished doctoral dissertation. New York University; NYC: 1999. Multicollinearity diagnostics for multiple regression: A Monte Carlo study. [Google Scholar]
  51. Freed EM, Hamilton ST, Long DL. Comprehension in proficient readers: The nature of individual variation. Journal of Memory and Language. 2017;97:135–153. doi: 10.1016/j.jml.2017.07.008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Friederici AD. The brain basis of language processing: from structure to function. Physiological Reviews. 2011;91(4):1357–1392. doi: 10.1152/physrev.00006.2011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Frost SJ, Landi N, Mencl WE, Sandak R, Fulbright RK, Tejada ET, Pugh KR. Phonological awareness predicts activation patterns for print and speech. Annals of Dyslexia. 2009;59(1):78–97. doi: 10.1007/s11881-009-0024-y. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Frye RE, Liederman J, Malmberg B, McLean J, Strickland D, Beauchamp MS. Surface area accounts for the relation of gray matter volume to reading-related skills and history of dyslexia. Cerebral Cortex. 2010;20(11):2625–2635. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhq010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Gardumi A, Ivanov D, Havlicek M, Formisano E, Uludağ K. Tonotopic maps in human auditory cortex using arterial spin labeling. Human Brain Mapping. 2017;38(3):1140–1154. doi: 10.1002/hbm.23444. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Gathercole SE, Hitch GJ, Service E, Martin AJ. Phonological short-term memory and vocabulary development: further evidence on the nature of the relationship. Applied Cognitive Psychology. 1997;13(1):65–77. [Google Scholar]
  57. Giedd JN, Blumenthal J, Jeffries NO, Castellanos FX, Liu H, Zijdenbos A, Rapoport JL. Brain development during childhood and adolescence: a longitudinal MRI study. Nature Neuroscience. 1999;2(10):861–863. doi: 10.1038/13158. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Gogtay N, Giedd JN, Lusk L, Hayashi KM, Greenstein D, Vaituzis AC, Rapoport JL. Dynamic mapping of human cortical development during childhood through early adulthood. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 2004;101(21):8174–8179. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0402680101. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Goldman JG, Manis FR. Relationships among cortical thickness, reading skill, and print exposure in adults. Scientific Studies of Reading. 2013;17(3):163–176. [Google Scholar]
  60. Golestani N. Brain structural correlates of individual differences at low-to high-levels of the language processing hierarchy: A review of new approaches to imaging research. International Journal of Bilingualism. 2014;18(1):6–34. [Google Scholar]
  61. Gough PB, Tunmer WE. Decoding, reading, and reading disability. Remedial and Special Education. 1986;7(1):6–10. [Google Scholar]
  62. Greve DN, Van der Haegen L, Cai Q, Stufflebeam S, Sabuncu MR, Fischl B, Brysbaert M. A surface-based analysis of language lateralization and cortical asymmetry. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 2013;25(9):1477–1492. doi: 10.1162/jocn_a_00405. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  63. Guo Y, Roehrig AD, Williams RS. The relation of morphological awareness and syntactic awareness to adults’ reading comprehension: Is vocabulary knowledge a mediating variable? Journal of Literacy Research. 2011;43(2):159–183. [Google Scholar]
  64. Hagler DJ, Saygin AP, Sereno MI. Smoothing and cluster thresholding for cortical surface-based group analysis of fMRI data. NeuroImage. 2006;33(4):1093–1103. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.07.036. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  65. Hagoort P. Reflections on the neurobiology of syntax. In: Bickerton D, Szathmáry E, editors. Biological Foundations and Origin of Syntax. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press; 2009. pp. 279–296. [Google Scholar]
  66. Hartwigsen G, Baumgaertner A, Price CJ, Koehnke M, Ulmer S, Siebner HR. Phonological decisions require both the left and right supramarginal gyri. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 2010;107(38):16494–16499. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1008121107. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  67. He Q, Xue G, Chen C, Chen C, Lu ZL, Dong Q. Decoding the neuroanatomical basis of reading ability: a multivoxel morphometric study. The Journal of Neuroscience. 2013;33(31):12835–12843. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0449-13.2013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  68. Heim S, Grande M, Meffert E, Eickhoff SB, Schreiber H, Kukolja J, Amunts K. Cognitive levels of performance account for hemispheric lateralisation effects in dyslexic and normally reading children. Neuroimage. 2010;53(4):1346–1358. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.07.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  69. Heim S, Keil A. Large-scale neural correlates of developmental dyslexia. European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry. 2004;13(3):125–140. doi: 10.1007/s00787-004-0361-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  70. Hendrickx J. perturb: Tools for evaluating collinearity [Software] 2012 R package version 2.05. URL https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=perturb.
  71. Hoover WA, Gough PB. The simple view of reading. Reading and Writing. 1990;2(2):127–160. [Google Scholar]
  72. Hua AN, Keenan JM. The role of text memory in inferencing and in comprehension deficits. Scientific Studies of Reading. 2014;18(6):415–431. doi: 10.1080/10888438.2014.926906. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  73. James G, Witten D, Hastie T, Tibshirani R. An introduction to statistical learning. New York: Springer; 2013. [Google Scholar]
  74. Jasińska KK, Molfese PJ, Kornilov SA, Mencl WE, Frost SJ, Lee M, Landi N. The BDNF Val 66 Met polymorphism is associated with structural neuroanatomical differences in young children. Behavioural Brain Research. 2017;328:48–56. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2017.03.014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  75. Jednoróg K, Marchewka A, Altarelli I, Monzalvo Lopez AK, van Ermingen-Marbach M, Grande M, Ramus F. How reliable are gray matter disruptions in specific reading disability across multiple countries and languages? insights from a large-scale voxel-based morphometry study. Human Brain Mapping. 2015;36(5):1741–1754. doi: 10.1002/hbm.22734. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  76. Johns CL, Matsuki K, Van Dyke JA. Poor readers’ retrieval mechanism: efficient access is not dependent on reading skill. Frontiers in Psychology. 2015;6:1552. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01552. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  77. Joshi RM. Vocabulary: A critical component of comprehension. Reading & Writing Quarterly. 2005;21(3):209–219. [Google Scholar]
  78. Karlson B, Gardner E. Stanford diagnostic reading test. 4th. San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corp; 1995. [Google Scholar]
  79. Keith TZ. Multiple regression and beyond: An introduction to multiple regression and structural equation modeling. 2nd. New York, NY: Routledge; 2014. [Google Scholar]
  80. Keller TA, Just MA. Altering cortical connectivity: remediation-induced changes in the white matter of poor readers. Neuron. 2009;64(5):624–631. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2009.10.018. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  81. Krafnick AJ, Flowers DL, Napoliello EM, Eden GF. Gray matter volume changes following reading intervention in dyslexic children. NeuroImage. 2011;57(3):733–741. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.10.062. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  82. Kukona A, Braze D, Johns CL, Mencl WE, Van Dyke JA, Magnuson JS, Pugh KR, Shankweiler DP, Tabor W. The real-time prediction and inhibition of linguistic outcomes: Effects of language and literacy skill. Acta Psychologia. 2016;171:72–84. doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2016.09.009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  83. Kuhn M. caret: Classification and Regression Training [Software] 2016 R package version 6.0-71. URL https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=caret.
  84. Kutner MH, Nachtsheim C, Neter J, Li W. Applied linear regression models. 5th. McGraw-Hill/Irwin; 2005. [Google Scholar]
  85. Kuperberg GR, Broome MR, McGuire PK, David AS, Eddy M, Ozawa F, Salat DH. Regionally localized thinning of the cerebral cortex in schizophrenia. Archives of General Psychiatry. 2003;60(9):878–888. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.60.9.878. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  86. Kuperberg GR, Lakshmanan BM, Caplan DN, Holcomb PJ. Making sense of discourse: An fMRI study of causal inferencing across sentences. NeuroImage. 2006;33(1):343–361. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.06.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  87. Kuperman V, Van Dyke JA. Effects of individual differences in verbal skills on eye-movement patterns during sentence reading. Journal of Memory and Language. 2011;65(1):42–73. doi: 10.1016/j.jml.2011.03.002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  88. Leach JM, Scarborough HS, Rescorla L. Late-emerging reading disabilities. Journal of Educational Psychology. 2003;95(2):211–224. [Google Scholar]
  89. Lee H, Devlin JT, Shakeshaft C, Stewart LH, Brennan A, Glensman J, Price CJ. Anatomical traces of vocabulary acquisition in the adolescent brain. Journal of Neuroscience. 2007;27(5):1184–1189. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4442-06.2007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  90. Li M, Braze D, Kukona A, Johns CL, Tabor W, Van Dyke JA, Mencl WE, Shankweiler DP, Pugh KR, Magnuson JS. Individual differences in subphonemic sensitivity and reading-related abilities. Haskins Laboratories; 2017. Manuscript under review. http://doi.org/10.17605/OSF.IO/3FNCY. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  91. Linkersdörfer J, Jurcoane A, Lindberg S, Kaiser J, Hasselhorn M, Fiebach CJ, Lonnemann J. The association between gray matter volume and reading proficiency: A longitudinal study of beginning readers. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 2014;27(2):308–318. doi: 10.1162/jocn_a_00710. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  92. Linkersdörfer J, Lonnemann J, Lindberg S, Hasselhorn M, Fiebach CJ. Grey matter alterations co-localize with functional abnormalities in developmental dyslexia: an ALE meta-analysis. PloS one. 2012;7(8):e43122. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0043122. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  93. Lohmann G, Stelzer J, Mueller K, Lacosse E, Buschmann T, Kumar VJ, Scheffler K. Inflated false negative rates undermine reproducibility in task-based fMRI. bioRxiv. 2017;122788 doi: 10.1101/122788. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  94. Long DL, Johns CL, Morris PE. Comprehension ability in mature readers. In: Traxler M, Gernsbacher M, editors. Handbook of Psycholinguistics. 2nd. Burlington, MA: Academic Press; 2006. pp. 801–833. [Google Scholar]
  95. Long DL, Prat C, Johns C, Morris P, Jonathan E. The importance of knowledge in vivid text memory: An individual-differences investigation of recollection and familiarity. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review. 2008;15(3):604–609. doi: 10.3758/PBR.15.3.604. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  96. Lu LH, Dapretto M, O’Hare ED, Kan E, McCourt ST, Thompson PM, Sowell ER. Relationships between brain activation and brain structure in normally developing children. Cerebral Cortex. 2009;19(11):2595–2604. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhp011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  97. Lu LH, Leonard CM, Thompson PM, Kan E, Jolley J, Welcome SE, Sowell ER. Normal developmental changes in inferior frontal gray matter are associated with improvement in phonological processing: a longitudinal MRI analysis. Cerebral Cortex. 2007;17(5):1092–1099. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhl019. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  98. Macaruso P, Shankweiler D. Expanding the simple view of reading in accounting for reading skills in community college students. Reading Psychology. 2010;31(5):454–471. [Google Scholar]
  99. Magnuson J, Kukona A, Braze D, Johns CL, Van Dyke JA, Tabor W, Mencl WE, Shankweiler D. Phonological instability in young adult poor readers: Time course measures and computational modeling. In: McCardle P, Miller B, Lee JR, Tzeng OJL, editors. Dyslexia Across Languages. Baltimore, MD: Paul H. Brookes Publishing Co; 2011. pp. 184–201. [Google Scholar]
  100. Markwardt FC., Jr . Peabody individual achievement test–Revised. Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service; 1998. [Google Scholar]
  101. McCandliss BD, Cohen L, Dehaene S. The visual word form area: expertise for reading in the fusiform gyrus. Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 2003;7(7):293–299. doi: 10.1016/s1364-6613(03)00134-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  102. McElreath R. Statistical rethinking: A Bayesian course with examples in R and Stan. 2nd. New York, NY: CRC Press; 2016. [Google Scholar]
  103. Mechelli A, Crinion JT, Noppeney U, O’Doherty J, Ashburner J, Frackowiak RS, Price CJ. Neurolinguistics: structural plasticity in the bilingual brain. Nature. 2004;431:757. doi: 10.1038/431757a. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  104. Mechelli A, Price CJ, Friston KJ, Ashburner J. Voxel-based morphometry of the human brain: methods and applications. Current medical imaging reviews. 2005;1(2):105–113. [Google Scholar]
  105. Meyler A, Keller TA, Cherkassky VL, Gabrieli JD, Just MA. Modifying the brain activation of poor readers during sentence comprehension with extended remedial instruction: A longitudinal study of neuroplasticity. Neuropsychologia. 2008;46(10):2580–2592. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2008.03.012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  106. Mol SE, Bus AG. To read or not to read: a meta-analysis of print exposure from infancy to early adulthood. Psychological Bulletin. 2011;137(2):267–296. doi: 10.1037/a0021890. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  107. Nagy WE, Anderson RC. How many words are there in printed school English? Reading Research Quarterly. 1984;19(3):304–330. [Google Scholar]
  108. Nation K, Hulme C. Learning to read changes children’s phonological skills: Evidence from a latent variable longitudinal study of reading and nonword repetition. Developmental Science. 2011;14(4):649–659. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2010.01008.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  109. Norton ES, Wolf M. Rapid automatized naming (RAN) and reading fluency: Implications for understanding and treatment of reading disabilities. Annual Review of Psychology. 2012;63:427–452. doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-120710-100431. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  110. Paus T. Mapping brain maturation and cognitive development during adolescence. Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 2005;9(2):60–68. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2004.12.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  111. Perfetti C. Reading ability: Lexical quality to comprehension. Scientific Studies of Reading. 2007;11(4):357–383. [Google Scholar]
  112. Peterson RA. On the use of college students in social science research: Insights from a second-order meta-analysis. Journal of Consumer Research. 2001;28:450–461. [Google Scholar]
  113. Petersson KM, Folia V, Hagoort P. What artificial grammar learning reveals about the neurobiology of syntax. Brain and Language. 2012;120(2):83–95. doi: 10.1016/j.bandl.2010.08.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  114. Petersson KM, Silva C, Castro-Caldas A, Ingvar M, Reis A. Literacy: a cultural influence on functional left–right differences in the inferior parietal cortex. European Journal of Neuroscience. 2007;26(3):791–799. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2007.05701.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  115. Prat CS, Just MA. Exploring the neural dynamics underpinning individual differences in sentence comprehension. Cerebral Cortex. 2011;21(8):1747–1760. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhq241. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  116. Prat CS, Mason RA, Just MA. Individual differences in the neural basis of causal inferencing. Brain and Language. 2011;116(1):1–13. doi: 10.1016/j.bandl.2010.08.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  117. Price CJ, Moore CJ, Humphreys GW, Wise RJ. Segregating semantic from phonological processes during reading. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 1997;9(6):727–733. doi: 10.1162/jocn.1997.9.6.727. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  118. Protopapas A, Mouzaki A, Sideridis GD, Kotsolakou A, Simos PG. The role of vocabulary in the context of the simple view of reading. Reading & Writing Quarterly. 2013;29(2):168–202. [Google Scholar]
  119. Psychological Corp. WASI: Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence. San Antonio, TX: 1999. [Google Scholar]
  120. Pugh K. A neurocognitive overview of reading acquisition and dyslexia across languages. Developmental Science. 2006;9(5):448–450. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2006.00528.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  121. Pugh KR, Frost SJ, Sandak R, Landi N, Rueckl JG, Constable RT, Mencl WE. Effects of stimulus difficulty and repetition on printed word identification: An fMRI comparison of nonimpaired and reading-disabled adolescent cohorts. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 2008;20(7):1146–1160. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2008.20079. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  122. Pugh KR, Landi N, Preston JL, Mencl WE, Austin AC, Sibley D, Frost SJ. The relationship between phonological and auditory processing and brain organization in beginning readers. Brain and Language. 2013;125(2):173–183. doi: 10.1016/j.bandl.2012.04.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  123. Pugh KR, Mencl WE, Shaywitz BA, Shaywitz SE, Fulbright RK, Constable RT, Liberman AM. The angular gyrus in developmental dyslexia: task-specific differences in functional connectivity within posterior cortex. Psychological Science. 2000;11(1):51–56. doi: 10.1111/1467-9280.00214. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  124. Core Team R. R: A language and environment for statistical computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing; Vienna, Austria: 2016. URL http://www.R-project.org/ [Google Scholar]
  125. Richardson FM, Price CJ. Structural MRI studies of language function in the undamaged brain. Brain Structure and Function. 2009;213(6):511–523. doi: 10.1007/s00429-009-0211-y. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  126. Richardson FM, Thomas MS, Filippi R, Harth H, Price CJ. Contrasting effects of vocabulary knowledge on temporal and parietal brain structure across lifespan. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 2010;22(5):943–954. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2009.21238. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  127. Richlan F, Kronbichler M, Wimmer H. Functional abnormalities in the dyslexic brain: A quantitative meta-analysis of neuroimaging studies. Human Brain Mapping. 2009;30(10):3299–3308. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20752. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  128. Richlan F, Kronbichler M, Wimmer H. Structural abnormalities in the dyslexic brain: a meta-analysis of voxel-based morphometry studies. Human Brain Mapping. 2013;34(11):3055–3065. doi: 10.1002/hbm.22127. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  129. Robertson DA, Gernsbacher MA, Guidotti SJ, Robertson RR, Irwin W, Mock BJ, Campana ME. Functional neuroanatomy of the cognitive process of mapping during discourse comprehension. Psychological Science. 2000;11(3):255–260. doi: 10.1111/1467-9280.00251. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  130. Rosas J, Ramos-Casals M, Ena J, García-Carrasco M, Verdu J, Cervera R, Pascual E. Usefulness of basal and pilocarpine-stimulated salivary flow in primary Sjögren’s syndrome. Correlation with clinical, immunological and histological features. Rheumatology. 2002;41(6):670–675. doi: 10.1093/rheumatology/41.6.670. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  131. Sabatini JP, Sawaki Y, Shore JR, Scarborough HS. Relationships among reading skills of adults with low literacy. Journal of Learning Disabilities. 2010;43(2):122–138. doi: 10.1177/0022219409359343. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  132. Salat DH, Buckner RL, Snyder AZ, Greve DN, Desikan RS, Busa E, Fischl B. Thinning of the cerebral cortex in aging. Cerebral Cortex. 2004;14(7):721–730. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhh032. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  133. Scarborough HS, Brady SA. Toward a common terminology for talking about speech and reading: A glossary of the “phon” words and some related terms. Journal of Literacy Research. 2002;34(3):299–336. [Google Scholar]
  134. Ségonne F, Dale AM, Busa E, Glessner M, Salat D, Hahn HK, Fischl B. A hybrid approach to the skull stripping problem in MRI. NeuroImage. 2004;22(3):1060–1075. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.03.032. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  135. Ségonne F, Pacheco J, Fischl B. Geometrically accurate topology-correction of cortical surfaces using nonseparating loops. IEEE Transactions on Medical Imaging. 2007;26(4):518–529. doi: 10.1109/TMI.2006.887364. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  136. Shankweiler D, Lundquist E, Katz L, Stuebing KK, Fletcher JM, Brady S, Shaywitz BA. Comprehension and decoding: Patterns of association in children with reading difficulties. Scientific Studies of Reading. 1999;3(1):69–94. [Google Scholar]
  137. Shankweiler D, Mencl WE, Braze D, Tabor W, Pugh KR, Fulbright RK. Reading differences and brain: Cortical integration of speech and print in sentence processing varies with reader skill. Developmental Neuropsychology. 2008;33(6):745–775. doi: 10.1080/87565640802418688. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  138. Shapiro SS, Wilk MB. An analysis of variance test for normality (complete samples) Biometrika. 1965;52(3/4):591–611. [Google Scholar]
  139. Share DL. Phonological recoding and self-teaching: sine qua non of reading acquisition. Cognition. 1995;55(2):151–218. doi: 10.1016/0010-0277(94)00645-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  140. Sled JG, Zijdenbos AP, Evans AC. A nonparametric method for automatic correction of intensity nonuniformity in MRI data. IEEE Transactions on Medical Imaging. 1998;17(1):87–97. doi: 10.1109/42.668698. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  141. Snijders TM, Vosse T, Kempen G, Van Berkum JJ, Petersson KM, Hagoort P. Retrieval and unification of syntactic structure in sentence comprehension: an fMRI study using word-category ambiguity. Cerebral Cortex. 2009;19(7):1493–1503. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhn187. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  142. Sowell ER, Thompson PM, Leonard CM, Welcome SE, Kan E, Toga AW. Longitudinal mapping of cortical thickness and brain growth in normal children. The Journal of Neuroscience. 2004;24(38):8223–8231. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1798-04.2004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  143. Spring C, French L. Identifying children with specific reading disabilities from listening and reading discrepancy scores. Journal of Learning Disabilities. 1990;23(1):53–58. doi: 10.1177/002221949002300112. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  144. St George M, Kutas M, Martinez A, Sereno MI. Semantic integration in reading: engagement of the right hemisphere during discourse processing. Brain. 1999;122(7):1317–1325. doi: 10.1093/brain/122.7.1317. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  145. Stanovich KE. Matthew effects in reading: Some consequences of individual differences in the acquisition of literacy. Reading Research Quarterly. 1986;11(4):360–407. [Google Scholar]
  146. Stanovich KE. Does reading make you smarter? Literacy and the development of verbal intelligence. Advances in child development and behavior. 1993;24:133–180. doi: 10.1016/s0065-2407(08)60302-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  147. Stanovich KE, Cunningham AE. Studying the consequences of literacy within a literate society: The cognitive correlates of print exposure. Memory & Cognition. 1992;20(1):51–68. doi: 10.3758/bf03208254. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  148. Stanovich KE, West RF. Exposure to print and orthographic processing. Reading Research Quarterly. 1989;24(4):402–433. [Google Scholar]
  149. Stanovich KE, West RF, Harrison MR. Knowledge growth and maintenance across the life span: The role of print exposure. Developmental Psychology. 1995;31(5):811–826. [Google Scholar]
  150. Steinbrink C, Vogt K, Kastrup A, Müller HP, Juengling FD, Kassubek J, Riecker A. The contribution of white and gray matter differences to developmental dyslexia: insights from DTI and VBM at 3.0 T. Neuropsychologia. 2008;46(13):3170–3178. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2008.07.015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  151. Tabachnick BG, Fidell LS. Using multivariate statistics. 6th. Boston, MA: Pearson; 2012. [Google Scholar]
  152. Tan LH, Laird AR, Li K, Fox PT. Neuroanatomical correlates of phonological processing of Chinese characters and alphabetic words: A meta-analysis. Human Brain Mapping. 2005;25(1):83–91. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20134. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  153. Torgesen JK, Wagner RK, Rashotte CA. Test of word reading efficiency. Austin, TX: Pro-Ed; 1999. [Google Scholar]
  154. Tunmer WE, Chapman JW. The simple view of reading redux: Vocabulary knowledge and the independent components hypothesis. Journal of Learning Disabilities. 2012;45(5):453–466. doi: 10.1177/0022219411432685. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  155. van Atteveldt N, Formisano E, Goebel R, Blomert L. Integration of letters and speech sounds in the human brain. Neuron. 2004;43(2):271–282. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2004.06.025. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  156. van den Broek P, Mouw JM, Kraal A. Individual differences in reading comprehension. In: Afflerbach P, editor. Handbook of Individual Differences in Reading: Reader, Text, and Context. Routledge; New York: 2015. pp. 138–150. [Google Scholar]
  157. Van Dyke JA, Johns CL, Kukona A. Low working memory capacity is only spuriously related to poor reading comprehension. Cognition. 2014;131(3):373–403. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2014.01.007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  158. Van Dyke JA, Matsuki K, Jones HR, Molfese PJ, Jahn AA, Johns CL, Kush D, Bontrager ML. A random forests analysis of adding-related skills and white matter tractography. Poster presented at the 7th annual Society for the Neurobiology of Language Conference, 15 October 2015 [Google Scholar]
  159. Wagner RK, Piasta SB, Torgesen JK. Learning to read. In: Traxler M, Gernsbacher M, editors. Handbook of Psycholinguistics. 2nd. Burlington, MA: Academic Press; 2006. pp. 1111–1142. [Google Scholar]
  160. Wagner RK, Torgesen JK, Rashotte CA. Comprehensive test of phonological processing. Austin, TX: Pro-Ed; 1999. [Google Scholar]
  161. Wagner RK, Torgesen JK, Rashotte CA, Hecht SA, Barker TA, Burgess SR, Garon T. Changing relations between phonological processing abilities and word-level reading as children develop from beginning to skilled readers: a 5-year longitudinal study. Developmental Psychology. 1997;33(3):468–479. doi: 10.1037//0012-1649.33.3.468. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  162. Waters GS, Caplan D. The reliability and stability of verbal working memory measures. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers. 2003;35(4):550–564. doi: 10.3758/bf03195534. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  163. Welcome SE, Chiarello C, Thompson PM, Sowell ER. Reading skill is related to individual differences in brain structure in college students. Human Brain Mapping. 2011;32(8):1194–1205. doi: 10.1002/hbm.21101. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  164. Welcome SE, Joanisse MF. Individual differences in skilled adult readers reveal dissociable patterns of neural activity associated with component processes of reading. Brain and Language. 2012;120(3):360–371. doi: 10.1016/j.bandl.2011.12.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  165. Welcome SE, Joanisse MF. Individual differences in white matter anatomy predict dissociable components of reading skill in adults. NeuroImage. 2014;96:261–275. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.03.069. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  166. Westman E, Aguilar C, Muehlboeck JS, Simmons A. Regional magnetic resonance imaging measures for multivariate analysis in Alzheimer’s disease and mild cognitive impairment. Brain Topography. 2013;26(1):9–23. doi: 10.1007/s10548-012-0246-x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  167. Wiederholt JL, Bryant BR. Gray oral reading tests (GORT-4) Austin, TX: Pro-Ed; 2001. [Google Scholar]
  168. Winkler AM, Kochunov P, Blangero J, Almasy L, Zilles K, Fox PT, Glahn DC. Cortical thickness or grey matter volume? The importance of selecting the phenotype for imaging genetics studies. NeuroImage. 2010;53(3):1135–1146. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.12.028. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  169. Woodcock RW, McGrew KS, Mather N. Woodcock-Johnson III Tests of Achievement. Itasca, IL: Riverside; 2001. [Google Scholar]
  170. Zhang M, Chen C, Xue G, Lu ZL, Mei L, Xue H, Dong Q. Language-general and-specific white matter microstructural bases for reading. NeuroImage. 2014;98:435–441. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.04.080. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES