Abstract
Episodic memory, the conscious recollection of past events, is typically experienced from a first-person (egocentric) perspective. The hippocampus plays an essential role in episodic memory and spatial cognition. Although the allocentric nature of hippocampal spatial coding is well understood, little is known about whether the hippocampus receives egocentric information about external items. We recorded single units of rats from the lateral (LEC) and medial (MEC) entorhinal cortex, the two major inputs to the hippocampus. Many LEC neurons showed tuning for egocentric bearing of external items, whereas MEC cells tended to represent allocentric bearing. These results demonstrate a fundamental dissociation between the reference frames of LEC and MEC neural representations.
One Sentence Summary:
This study demonstrates that the lateral entorhinal cortex represents salient items in the environment in an egocentric framework.
The hippocampus is critical for the formation and retrieval of episodic memories (1–3). Episodic memories preserve a person’s original, first-person (i.e., egocentric) perspective (4). Cognitive map theory proposes that the hippocampus provides an objective spatial framework that is used to organize the various components of an experience, binding them so that they can be stored and later retrieved as a re-experience of the original event (5). Current theories propose that the various allocentric spatial correlates of cells in the hippocampus and MEC (6) provide the objective spatial framework of an experience. In contrast, representations of the specific components of an experience (i.e., the content) have been hypothesized to be provided by the LEC (7, 8), in an egocentric framework (9).
We thus examined the behavior of 275 LEC cells while 7 rats performed a foraging task in 2D open arenas in which the walls or boundaries were the most prominent features of the local environment (Fig. 1A and Fig. S1A). Although the firing rate maps of LEC cells showed weak spatial selectivity (10, 11), some LEC cells showed activity patterns that suggested an egocentric representation of the nearby boundaries (12) or, equivalently, the center of the apparatus (Fig. 1, B to E, and Figs. S2 and S3). We constructed tuning curves of the egocentric bearing (the angle of the vector from the rat to an external item, referenced from the allocentric head direction of the rat) of the nearest boundary (BearingBoundary, Fig. 1B) (13, 14) and compared them with classic allocentric head direction tuning curves (Fig. 1, C and E, right curves). LEC cells tended to have more sharply tuned egocentric bearing selectivity than head direction selectivity (Fig. 1F, Wilcoxon signed rank test, z = 8.46, P = 2.56×10−17, n = 275). Across the population with significant tuning, the preferred BearingBoundary covered all angles, albeit nonuniformly, with an underrepresentation of 180° (Fig. S4A). From these cells, it was possible to decode the BearingBoundary of the rats throughout the session (Fig. S4 and (15)). Egocentric bearing tuning was not an artifact of directional sampling biases, body-centered self-motion, or movement direction (Fig. S5) and it was stable within a session (Fig. S6). Some LEC cells showed tuning for distance of the animal to the boundary, and 23% of these cells showed BearingBoundary selectivity as well (Fig. S7 and Table S1).
Fig. 1. LEC but not MEC cells show egocentric bearing tuning of nearby boundaries in open arenas.
(A) Experimental setups. In some experiments (small-large box), rats foraged first in a small box, and then the inner walls were removed to allow the rat to forage in the large box. In others, rats foraged either in a square box or a circular platform. (B) Definition of egocentric bearing, the angle of the vector (black) from the rat to an external item (cyan), referenced from the rat’s allocentric head direction (orange). (C) An example LEC cell. Left, spatial rate map (number on top denotes the peak firing rate). Middle, head direction tuning field plot showing preferred head direction (arrow direction) at different locations. Arrow size denotes firing rate. Color saturation denotes mean vector length (MVL) of tuning curve. Number on top, maximum MVL. Right, tuning curves of egocentric bearing of the boundary (top) and allocentric bearing (i.e., classic head direction) (bottom). Number on top, MVL. (D) Same cell as in (C). Left four panels, the trajectory (gray line) and position of the rat when the cell fired (black dots) separated based on the current head direction. Right, overall trajectory; dot color signifies head direction. (E) A pair of simultaneously recorded cells showing opposite preferred BearingBoundary. (F) Scatter plot of MVLs for allocentric head direction tuning vs. BearingBoundary tuning. (G) Two example MEC cells, see additional description in the caption for Fig. S8B. (H) Distributions of MVLs of BearingBoundary tuning curves of LEC and MEC cells. (I) Cumulative distributions of ΔBICBoundary for LEC and MEC cells. (J) Left, schematic of MVL map construction. Tuning curves of egocentric bearing of each location (coordinates i, j) were constructed; the values of the MVL for each location were plotted. Right, an example MVL map with the location of the peak (MVLMax location) denoted with a black dot. (K) 2D histograms of the distributions of MVLMax locations for LEC and MEC cells in the small-large box task. Overrepresentation of the arena center corresponds to overrepresentation of the environmental boundaries (Fig. S2A). (L) Distributions of MVLMax of LEC and MEC cells.
In contrast, BearingBoundary tuning of MEC neurons recorded in the small-large box task (Fig. S1B) was significantly weaker than that of LEC cells, as quantified by the mean vector length of the tuning curve (MVLBoundary) (Fig. 1, G and H, Fig. S8; LEC median = 0.31, MEC median = 0.11; Wilcoxon rank-sum test, z = 6.84, P = 8.0×10−12; LEC, n = 72, MEC, n = 117). Because some MEC neurons show conjunctive tuning for location and head direction (6), which could be confounded with BearingBoundary tuning, we applied a generalized linear model (GLM) framework (15) (Fig. S9) to dissociate between an allocentric model (with a spatial location component conjunctive with allocentric bearing, i.e., classic head direction) and an egocentric model (with a spatial location component conjunctive with egocentric bearing). The goodness of fit was assessed with a Bayesian information criterion (ΔBICBoundary, (15)) measure. A negative value of the ΔBICBoundary indicated that the egocentric model was a better fit than the allocentric model, and a positive value indicated the opposite. LEC and MEC cells were significantly better fit by the egocentric and the allocentric models, respectively (Fig. 1I, Fig. S10, and Tables S1 to S3; LEC median = −0.0076, MEC median = 0.0016; Wilcoxon signed rank test; LEC: z = −6.44, P = 1.22×10−10, n = 72; MEC: z = 5.68, P = 1.34×10−8, n = 117; LEC vs. MEC: Wilcoxon rank-sum test, z = −8.88, P = 6.46×10−19), revealing a double dissociation in the preferred reference frames for angular coordinates of LEC and MEC representations.
We constructed MVL spatial maps to search for the location that generated the sharpest egocentric tuning curve (i.e., the largest MVL, called MVLMax; Fig. 1J). For cells with tuning for the egocentric bearing of the boundary, the location that produces the MVLMax will be at the center of the arena (Fig. S2). The MVLMax locations of LEC but not of MEC cells were significantly clustered around the center (Monte Carlo test for nonrandom distribution; LEC: P < 0.001, n = 72; MEC: P = 0.16, n = 117), with those of MEC cells significantly more scattered than LEC (bootstrap test, P < 0.001) (Fig. 1K, Figs. S3 and S8). Moreover, the values of MVLMax were significantly larger for LEC than MEC (Fig. 1L; LEC median = 0.35, MEC median = 0.22; Wilcoxon rank-sum test, z = 4.73, P = 2.21×10−6, LEC, n = 72, MEC, n = 117).
We next investigated whether LEC cells could encode the egocentric bearing of discrete, external items when the rat foraged in an arena that contained 4 objects (Fig. 2A; Fig. S1, C and D) (16). We first determined which one of the 4 objects for each cell generated the strongest egocentric bearing tuning (largest MVL). LEC cells had a significant preference for egocentric representation of those objects (ΔBICObject < 0), whereas MEC cells preferred the allocentric representation (ΔBICObject > 0) (Fig. 2, B and C, Figs. S11 and S12, Table S4; LEC median = −0.0026; MEC median = 0.0011; Wilcoxon signed rank test; LEC: z = −8.13, P = 4.41×10−16, n = 140; MEC: z = 3.97, P = 7.08×10−5, n = 88; LEC vs. MEC: Wilcoxon rank-sum test, z = −8.35, P = 6.85×10−17).
Fig. 2. LEC but not MEC cells show egocentric bearing tuning of 3D objects.
(A) The multiple-object arena. (B) Example cells from LEC (left column) and MEC (right column). Top to bottom, spatial rate map (circles denote the objects), head direction tuning field plot (red circle denotes the object with the highest MVLObject), egocentric bearing tuning curve for the object with the highest MVLObject, head direction tuning curve. (C) Cumulative distributions of best ΔBICObject for LEC and MEC cells. (D) Schematic showing construction of MVL map with data in a local partition. (E) Example cells from LEC (left) and MEC (right) with multiple object-representing partitions. Top to bottom, spatial rate map, head direction tuning field plot, and MVL maps together with tuning curves of egocentric bearing of the local object for four partitions. Colored tuning curves were from object-representing partitions (the partitions with red circles denoting the object), which are the ones with a sharp peak near the local object on the MVL map. Note that the preferred bearings (shaded regions) of the LEC cell are consistent, whereas the MEC cell has different preferred bearings for each object. (F) Distributions of the differences in preferred BearingObject between object-representing partition pairs for LEC and MEC cells with more than one object-representing partition.
To investigate whether LEC cells represented the locations of each object in the rat’s immediate vicinity as it explored, we partitioned the box into sections based on the closest object to each location in the box (Fig. 2D and Fig. S11B). Using the data in each partition, we constructed the MVL map to search for the MVLMax location in the entire arena (Fig. 2D) and computed the egocentric bearing tuning of the object in that partition (BearingObject) (Fig. 2E and Fig. S13). An object-representing partition was defined as a partition in which the MVL of BearingObject > 0.25 and the MVL map showed a small, localized peak (less than 20% of the arena with values > 70% of the peak value) close to the local object (< 25 cm). For the cells with two or more object-representing partitions, the preferred BearingObject values across the partitions were significantly more similar to each other for LEC cells than for MEC cells (Fig. 2F; Rayleigh test: LEC, z = 12.15, P = 1.42×10−7, n = 15; MEC, z = 0.51, P = 0.62, n = 8. Absolute value: LEC median = 18.26°, MEC median = 114.35°; LEC vs. MEC, Wilcoxon rank-sum test, z = −3.58, P = 3.40×10−4), indicating a more reliable egocentric code for objects in LEC than MEC (Figs. S11D and S12B).
To determine whether behaviorally salient locations were represented in an egocentric framework, we recorded LEC cells in a goal-oriented task (Fig. 3A and Fig. S1E), in which the goal (a food well) was shifted between sessions. Food pellets were thrown sporadically into the food well and other places in the box, encouraging both exploration of the entire box and frequent visits to the goal (Fig. 3B). Some cells encoded the egocentric bearing of the goal (Fig. 3C, current goal locations, and Fig. 3D, standard goal location; Fig. S14). The standard goal quadrant (southeast) was over-represented compared to the northeast quadrant, which never contained a goal location (Fig. 3E; Monte Carlo test against a uniformly distributed random distribution, P < 0.002 for all three sessions). Between sessions, there was a significant change in the number of cells that represented the standard versus displaced goal locations (Fig. 3E; bootstrap test, P < 0.01 for both S1 vs. S2 and S2 vs. S3), which could not be explained by behavioral confounds at the goal location (Fig. S15). As in the first experiment, some cells also showed distance selectivity to the MVLMax location (Fig. S16 and Table S5).
Fig. 3. LEC cells show goal-related egocentric tuning.
(A) The goal-oriented task. A single food well (red circle) was shifted from the standard goal location (STD) in session 1 to one of two displaced locations in session 2, and back to the original standard location in session 3. (B) Left, typical trajectory of rats in this task for 1 min. Right, typical occupancy map in an entire session. The goal location is over-occupied. (C) An example cell in three sessions (columns). From top to bottom, spatial rate maps, head direction tuning field plots, MVL maps (red circle, food well), egocentric bearing tuning curves for MVLMax locations. The MVLMax locations for this neuron coincided with the current goal location in each session as shown in the MVL maps. (D) Another example cell. The standard goal location was represented on the MVL maps regardless of the current goal locations. (E) 2D histograms of the distributions of MVLMax locations in three sessions.
These results demonstrate that LEC represents the bearing of external items in an egocentric framework. They confirm a key prediction of the hypothesis that the LEC provides the hippocampus with the sensory input that constitutes the content of an episodic memory (7, 8, 16, 17). They are also consistent with an earlier hypothesis that, in contrast to the MEC representation of “self” location, LEC mainly represents information of “other” items in an egocentric framework (9). Findings in rats, bats and primates have highlighted the involvement of the hippocampus in such representations of “other” (14, 18–22). We suggest that the LEC provides information about items in the environment, which is used for building comprehensive maps of what is “out there” (9, 21, 22). This functional segregation of “self” information in MEC and “other” information in LEC may reflect a fundamental organizing principle of the medial temporal lobe memory system (7, 9).
Spatial information in the hippocampal formation is generally considered to be represented in an allocentric framework. However, the present findings revealed that LEC cells display a robust spatial representation in an egocentric framework. Thus, LEC provides an integration of spatial and nonspatial information, in the form of a spatial relationship between the observer and specific items in the world, which deviates from the conventional view that spatial (“where”) information is conveyed only through the postrhinal (parahippocampal)-MEC pathway (7, 8). These results also clarify the interpretation of recent lesion studies of entorhinal cortex. First, LEC is critical for performance of an object-place association task that promotes the use of an egocentric strategy (23); second, hippocampal cells in rats with complete lesion of MEC still had place fields, which suggests that input from LEC alone can support hippocampal spatial selectivity (24) through transformation of egocentric direction and distance information, as proposed in some of the earliest models of place cells (25, 26). It is thus likely that LEC contributes to the process of abstraction of egocentric information into an allocentric framework for memory storage (5, 12). The parietal cortex is often viewed as an essential node in providing limbic system structures with egocentric representations of cues (13) and actions (27, 28) for memory and navigation (29), but it is assumed that the information must undergo a transformation from egocentric to allocentric coordinates (29) before reaching the hippocampus. However, at least in rats, the LEC receives direct projections from parietal cortex (30), thus allowing the hippocampus to combine directly these egocentric representations from parietal cortex with the MEC allocentric representations of self-position.
Supplementary Material
Acknowledgments:
The authors thank G. Rao, A. Smolinsky and M. Lee for technical assistance and A. Tsao for critical reading of the manuscript.
Funding: This work was supported by NIH Grant R01 MH094146, NIH Grant R01 NS039456, and the International Human Frontier Science Program Organization LT00683/2006C (F.S.).
Footnotes
This manuscript has been accepted for publication in Science. This version has not undergone final editing. Please refer to the complete version of record at http://www.sciencemag.org/. The manuscript may not be reproduced or used in any manner that does not fall within the fair use provisions of the Copyright Act without the prior, written permission of AAAS.
Competing interests: None declared.
Data and materials availability: Data and code are accessible at https://sites.krieger.jhu.edu/knierim-lab/resources/data-for-wang-chen-et-al/.
References and Notes:
- 1.Scoville WB, Milner B, Loss of recent memory after bilateral hippocampal lesions. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 20, 11–21 (1957). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Squire LR, Memory systems of the brain: A brief history and current perspective. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory. 82, 171–177 (2004). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Vargha-Khadem F et al. , Differential effects of early hippocampal pathology on episodic and semantic memory. Science. 277, 376–380 (1997). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Conway MA, Episodic memories. Neuropsychologia. 47, 2305–2313 (2009). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.O’Keefe J, Nadel L, The Hippocampus as a Cognitive Map (Oxford: Clarendon Press, 1978). [Google Scholar]
- 6.Moser EI, Moser M-B, McNaughton BL, Spatial representation in the hippocampal formation: a history. Nat. Neurosci 20, 1448–1464 (2017). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Knierim JJ, Neunuebel JP, Deshmukh SS, Functional correlates of the lateral and medial entorhinal cortex: objects, path integration and local-global reference frames. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond., B, Biol. Sci 369, 20130369 (2014). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Eichenbaum H, Yonelinas AP, Ranganath C, The medial temporal lobe and recognition memory. Annu. Rev. Neurosci 30, 123–152 (2007). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Lisman JE, Role of the dual entorhinal inputs to hippocampus: a hypothesis based on cue/action (non-self/self) couplets. Prog. Brain Res. 163, 615–625 (2007). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Hargreaves EL, Rao G, Lee I, Knierim JJ, Major dissociation between medial and lateral entorhinal input to dorsal hippocampus. Science. 308, 1792–1794 (2005). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Yoganarasimha D, Rao G, Knierim JJ, Lateral entorhinal neurons are not spatially selective in cue-rich environments. Hippocampus. 21, 1363–1374 (2011). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Peyrache A, Schieferstein N, Buzsáki G, Transformation of the head-direction signal into a spatial code. Nat Commun. 8, 1752 (2017). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Wilber AA, Clark BJ, Forster TC, Tatsuno M, McNaughton BL, Interaction of egocentric and world-centered reference frames in the rat posterior parietal cortex. J. Neurosci 34, 5431–5446 (2014). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Sarel A, Finkelstein A, Las L, Ulanovsky N, Vectorial representation of spatial goals in the hippocampus of bats. Science. 355, 176–180 (2017). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Materials and methods are available as supplementary materials.
- 16.Deshmukh SS, Knierim JJ, Representation of non-spatial and spatial information in the lateral entorhinal cortex. Front Behav Neurosci. 5, 69 (2011). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Tsao A, Moser M-B, Moser EI, Traces of Experience in the Lateral Entorhinal Cortex. Current Biology. 23, 399–405 (2013). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Rolls ET, O’Mara SM, View-responsive neurons in the primate hippocampal complex. Hippocampus. 5, 409–424 (1995). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Killian NJ, Jutras MJ, Buffalo EA, A map of visual space in the primate entorhinal cortex. Nature. 491, 761–764 (2012). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Acharya L, Aghajan ZM, Vuong C, Moore JJ, Mehta MR, Causal Influence of Visual Cues on Hippocampal Directional Selectivity. Cell. 164, 197–207 (2016). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Danjo T, Toyoizumi T, Fujisawa S, Spatial representations of self and other in the hippocampus. Science. 359, 213–218 (2018). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Omer DB, Maimon SR, Las L, Ulanovsky N, Social place-cells in the bat hippocampus. Science. 359, 218–224 (2018). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Wilson DIG, Watanabe S, Milner H, Ainge JA, Lateral entorhinal cortex is necessary for associative but not nonassociative recognition memory. Hippocampus. 23, 1280–1290 (2013). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Hales JB et al. , Medial entorhinal cortex lesions only partially disrupt hippocampal place cells and hippocampus-dependent place memory. Cell Rep. 9, 893–901 (2014). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.O’Keefe J, An allocentric spatial model for the hippocampal cognitive map. Hippocampus. 1, 230–235 (1991). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Sharp PE, Computer simulation of hippocampal place cells. Psychobiology. 19, 103–115 (1991). [Google Scholar]
- 27.McNaughton BL et al. , Cortical representation of motion during unrestrained spatial navigation in the rat. Cereb. Cortex 4, 27–39 (1994). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Whitlock JR, Pfuhl G, Dagslott N, Moser M-B, Moser EI, Functional split between parietal and entorhinal cortices in the rat. Neuron. 73, 789–802 (2012). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Byrne P, Becker S, Burgess N, Remembering the past and imagining the future: a neural model of spatial memory and imagery. Psychol Rev. 114, 340–375 (2007). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Olsen GM, Ohara S, Iijima T, Witter MP, Parahippocampal and retrosplenial connections of rat posterior parietal cortex. Hippocampus. 27, 335–358 (2017). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Savelli F, Yoganarasimha D, Knierim JJ, Influence of boundary removal on the spatial representations of the medial entorhinal cortex. Hippocampus. 18, 1270–1282 (2008). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Klatzky RL, in Spatial Cognition (Springer, Berlin, Heidelberg, 1998), pp. 1–17. [Google Scholar]
- 33.Heeger D, Poisson Model of Spike Generation (Technical Report, 2000). [Google Scholar]
- 34.Davidson TJ, Kloosterman F, Wilson MA, Hippocampal replay of extended experience. Neuron. 63, 497–507 (2009). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Kropff E, Carmichael JE, Moser M-B, Moser EI, Speed cells in the medial entorhinal cortex. Nature. 523, 419–424 (2015). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Hardcastle K, Maheswaranathan N, Ganguli S, Giocomo LM, A Multiplexed, Heterogeneous, and Adaptive Code for Navigation in Medial Entorhinal Cortex. Neuron. 94, 375–387.e7 (2017). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Truccolo W, Eden UT, Fellows MR, Donoghue JP, Brown EN, A point process framework for relating neural spiking activity to spiking history, neural ensemble, and extrinsic covariate effects. Journal of Neurophysiology. 93, 1074–1089 (2005). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Insausti R, Herrero MT, Witter MP, Entorhinal cortex of the rat: cytoarchitectonic subdivisions and the origin and distribution of cortical efferents. Hippocampus. 7, 146–183 (1997). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Dolorfo CL, Amaral DG, Entorhinal cortex of the rat: topographic organization of the cells of origin of the perforant path projection to the dentate gyrus. J. Comp. Neurol 398, 25–48 (1998). [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.