Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2019 Nov 1.
Published in final edited form as: Depress Anxiety. 2018 Sep 26;35(11):1018–1029. doi: 10.1002/da.22833

Smaller Hippocampal CA1 Subfield Volume in Posttraumatic Stress Disorder

Lyon W Chen a,b,#, Delin Sun a,b,#, Sarah L Davis a,b, Courtney C Haswell a,b, Emily L Dennis c, Chelsea A Swanson a,b, Christopher D Whelan c, Boris Gutman c, Neda Jahanshad c, Juan Eugenio Iglesias d, Paul Thompson c; Mid-Atlantic MIRECC Workgroupa,, H Ryan Wagner a, Philipp Saemann e, Kevin S LaBar a,b, Rajendra A Morey a,b
PMCID: PMC6261348  NIHMSID: NIHMS985602  PMID: 30256497

Abstract

Background

Smaller hippocampal volume in patients with PTSD represents the most consistently reported structural alteration in the brain. Subfields of the hippocampus play distinct roles in encoding and processing of memories, which are disrupted in PTSD. We examined PTSD-associated alterations in 12 hippocampal subfields in relation to global hippocampal shape, and clinical features.

Methods

Case-control cross-sectional study of US military veterans (n=282) from the Iraq and Afghanistan era were grouped into PTSD (n=142) and trauma-exposed controls (n=140). Participants underwent clinical evaluation for PTSD and associated clinical parameters followed by MRI at 3-Tesla. Segmentation with FreeSurfer v6.0 produced hippocampal subfield volumes for the left and right CA1, CA3, CA4, dentate gyrus, fimbria, fissure, hippocampus-amygdala transition area, molecular layer, parasubiculum, presubiculum, subiculum, and tail, as well as hippocampal meshes. Covariates included age, gender, trauma exposure, alcohol use, depressive symptoms, antidepressant medication use, total hippocampal volume, and MRI scanner model.

Results

Significantly lower subfield volumes were associated with PTSD in left CA1 (p=.01; d=.21; uncorrected), CA3 (p=.04; d=.08; uncorrected), and right CA3 (p=.02; d=.07; uncorrected) only if ipsilateral whole hippocampal volume was included as a covariate.

Conclusions

The present finding of smaller hippocampal CA1 in PTSD is consistent with model systems in rodents that exhibit increased anxiety-like behavior from repeated exposure to acute stress. Behavioral correlations with hippocampal subfield volume differences in PTSD will elucidate their relevance to PTSD, particularly behaviors of associative fear learning, extinction training, and formation of false memories.

INTRODUCTION

Individuals with posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) may experience deficits in declarative memory such as remembering events, facts or lists, fragmentation of autobiographical or trauma-related memories, and trauma-related amnesia [1]. The hippocampus plays an important role in memory formation and retrieval that has long been implicated in the clinical presentation of PTSD [2]. Indeed, lower hippocampal volume in PTSD has been a reliably reported structural alteration for over two decades [34]. We sought to attain improved spatial and functional characterization of this structural alteration via two complimentary approaches, (1) quantify the volume of 12 hippocampal subfields, and (2) conduct 3-D vertex-based shape analysis of the hippocampus to identify localized surface features associated with PTSD.

Careful investigation of PTSD-associated alterations in hippocampal subfields and their relationship to differences in hippocampal shape, and clinical features is scant, has met with inconsistent results, and has been beset by limitations [57]. Employing manual subfield segmentation, Wang et al reported lower volume in CA3 associated with PTSD, Hayes et al found smaller dentate gyrus (DG), whereas Mueller et al found PTSD was associated with no subfield differences. The primary limitation of previous studies was a small sample size of n=36 given the expected range of effect sizes. Hayes et al studied the largest sample to date (n=97), but used subfield segmentation with FreeSurfer v5.3 that suffered from three major shortcomings [8]. First, the resolution of the in vivo training data was insufficient for the human raters to accurately distinguish subregions, forcing excessive reliance on geometric boundary criteria for delineating subfields, but on the other hand may be able to overcome image artifacts sometimes overlooked by automated techniques. The second issue was that the segmentation protocol was designed for the hippocampal body, which translated poorly to the hippocampal head and tail. The resultant third problem was that the volumes of the subregions did not agree well with histological studies. Therefore, FreeSurfer v5.3 is based on an anatomically incorrect atlas [9], notably for CA1. These shortcomings are addressed in the present study by a completely new atlas in FreeSurfer v6.0 that was built with a novel atlasing algorithm and ex vivo MRI data acquired from post mortem brains [10]. Thus, FreeSurfer v6.0 combines ex-vivo and in-vivo scans, with the former acquired on 15 ex-vivo brains scanned at 7-Tesla to attain extremely high signal-to-noise ratio and 130-μm3 isotropic resolution, whereas v5.3 used in vivo atlas from five cases acquired at nearly 9-fold lower resolution of 380-μm3 and the manual segmentation studies were acquired at 4T with 180-fold lower resolution of 400 × 500 μm in-plane and 2000-μm through-plane. These enhancements enabled segmentation of 12 subfields with FreeSurfer v6.0 as compared to five subfields with v5.3 or manual segmentation methods.

Subfields of the hippocampus are involved in discrete aspects of memory encoding and consolidation. For instance, the dentate gyrus (DG) is important in distinguishing features that are different from other memories in order to store similar memories as discrete events – a phenomenon called pattern separation [1112]. Pattern separation deficits may underlie fear generalization [13], a process that occurs in anxiety and stress based disorders including PTSD [14]. By contrast, the entorhinal cortex (EC) and cornu ammonis subfield-3 (CA3) are crucial in recognizing different events with overlapping features – a phenomenon called pattern completion that has important implications in contextual fear conditioning [15] and is a widely investigated model of PTSD [16]. Chronic stress in rats produces atrophy and debranching of dendrites in pyramidal neurons of the CA3 [17] and decreased neurogenesis in the DG [18], which appear to be reversible in these models when stress is alleviated. Preclinical research in rats has shown that the CA1 subfield is involved in context-specific memory retrieval after extinction [19]. These and other findings clearly point to a major influence of hippocampal CA1 neurons in conditioned fear and its extinction [20]. Extinction learning, which relies critically on intact CA1 function [20; 2324], is impaired in widely adopted experimental models of PTSD and consistent with re-experiencing symptoms of PTSD.

The capability for automated, in vivo segmentation of the human hippocampal subfields is now imperative for achieving replicability and reproducibility with efficiency in large multi-site initiatives [2526]. The recently released FreeSurfer v6.0 makes it possible to estimate the hippocampal subfields from 1-mm T1-weighted MRI. Despite the fact that historically, segmentation of subfields was based on higher resolution images (0.2–0.7 mm) that may include T2-weighting, and limited contrast between some of the subfields at 1-mm resolution, it has been shown that segmentations estimated from 1-mm scan resolution nevertheless carry useful information on subfield volumes [10]. The segmentation algorithm was validated on three publicly available datasets with varying MRI contrast and resolution [27].

Our goal was to investigate the association between PTSD and the volume of 12 hippocampal subfield, as well as the relationship of subfield volume to hippocampal shape among a large cohort of younger US military veterans. Based on the foregoing evidence from animal and human research, we hypothesized the PTSD group would have smaller CA1, CA3, and DG volumes. Given the application of an anatomically incorrect atlas for CA1 in FreeSurfer v5.3 that was corrected in FreeSurfer v6.0, the unique role of CA1 in context-specific memory retrieval, and a strongly implicated behavioral deficit in PTSD, we elevated our prediction probability of smaller CA-1 volume relative to CA-3 and DG. Furthermore, a complimentary analysis of hippocampal shape was expected to reveal differences for surfaces corresponding to the affected subfields.

METHODS

Participants

We enrolled a total of 290 Iraq and Afghanistan era military service veterans, which were recruited from our repository [28]. Among these participants, 282 were selected for analysis following quality control (QC) procedures that consisted of 142 individuals with PTSD and 140 trauma-exposed controls. Eight scans failed segmentation QC (non-hippocampal tissue assigned to CA1, contrast insufficient to identify the hippocampal fissure, presence of holes) resulting in participant exclusion. Participants were screened for inclusion/exclusion criteria based on information available in the repository. Important exclusions included major Axis I diagnosis (other than MDD or PTSD), contraindication to MRI, moderate/severe traumatic brain injury, substance dependence, neurological disorders, and age over 65 years. All participants provided written informed consent to participate in procedures reviewed and approved by the Institutional Review Boards at Duke University and the Durham VA Medical Center. Participants’ age, sex, and other demographic and clinical information are summarized in Table 1 (see Supplement).

Table 1.

Clinical and Demographic Features of Sample

Group Control (n=140) mean±SD PTSD (n=142) mean±SD statistic p-value
Age 39.2 ± 10.1 40.1 ± 10.2 t = −.806 .421
Race (AA), n [%] 61 [43.6] 61 [43.0] Χ2 =.69 .71
female gender; n [%] 28 [24.3] 25 [21.0] Χ2 =.26 .63
CAPS - Life 17.2 ± 11.5 40.2 ± 10.2 t = −3.17 .002
BDI-II 5.13 ± 7.96 17.32 ± 11.51 t = −10.39 < .0001
TLEQ 1.94 ± 2.51 4.73 ± 3.28 t = −8.05 <.0001
Combat Exposure 6.29 ± 8.13 14.70 ± 11.03 t = −7.53 < .0001
Child Trauma (TLEQ) .39 ± .78 .85 ± 1.05 t = −4.15 < .0001
AUDIT 2.80 ± 3.32 4.32 ± 4.67 t = −3.18 .002
Serotonergic n [%] 4 [2.9] 52 [36.6] Χ2 = −7.97 < .0001
Antipsychotic n [%] 0 [0] 4 [2.6] Χ2 = −2.01 .05
Mood Stabilizer n [%] 0 [0] 2 [1.4] Χ2 = −.57 .57
TIV 1508089 ± 166866 1511375 ± 165735 t = −.17 .87
GE 3-T MR 750 77 (27.3) 86 (30.5) Χ2 = 5.56 .06
GE 3-T EXCITE 42 (14.9) 47 (16.7) NA NA
Philips 3-T Ingenia 21 (7.4) 9 (3.2) NA NA
total hippocampus 3537 ± 375 3480 ± 327 F=1.96 .16

Abbreviations: SD= Standard Deviation, AA=African American, CAPS = Clinician Administered PTSD Scale, BDI=Beck Depression Inventory, TLEQ=Trauma Life Events Questionnaire, AUDIT = Alcohol Use Disorders Test, TIV=Total Intracranial Volume.

MRI acquisition

All images were acquired on a 3-Tesla scanner equipped with an 8-channel head coil. All scans were acquired as high-resolution T1-weighted whole-brain axial images with 1-mm isotropic voxels on three different scanners: [1] GE Discovery MR750 (n=163, 57.8%; Control n=77, PTSD n=86) [2] GE Signa EXCITE (n=89, 31.5%; Control n=42, PTSD n=47), and [3] Philips Ingenia scanner (n=30, 10.6%; Control n=21, PTSD n=9). See Online Supplement for ‘Image Acquisition Parameters’.

Hippocampal Subfield Volume Analysis

Automated segmentation and labeling of subcortical volumes and estimation of total intracranial volume (TIV) from T1 images was performed using the FreeSurfer image analysis suite[29] (v5.3.0; http://surfer.nmr.mgh.harvard.edu/) and its library tool, recon-all. Hippocampal subfield segmentation was performed using FreeSurfer v6.0 and its library function, hippocampal-subfields-T1. Hippocampal subfield volumes for the left and right hemispheres were generated in each subject for the CA1, CA3, CA4, DG, fimbria, fissure, hippocampus-amygdala transition area (HATA), molecular layer, parasubiculum, presubiculum, subiculum, and tail (Figure 1). We applied standardized protocols for image analysis for subcortical segmentation developed by the Consortium for ENhancing Imaging Genetics through Meta-Analysis (ENIGMA; see Supplement), (http://enigma.ini.usc.edu/protocols/imaging-protocols/). We have previously analyzed FreeSurfer output from multiple scanners with evidence that results are robust to this heterogeneity. In Logue et al we used 16 different scanners and found phet=0.74 and I2=0 for hippocampal volume. In Whelan et al [25], we demonstrated high intraclass correlation (ICC) across 1.5T and 3.0-T scanners with FreeSurfer v6.0 for hippocampal subfields including CA-1 (ICC = 0.915) and CA3 (ICC=0.827).

Figure 1. Hippocampal Subfield Segmentation.

Figure 1.

Automated segmentation of the hippocampus into 12 subfields in each hemisphere of the brain was performed with FreeSurfer v6.0. Subfield images of CA1, CA2/3, CA4, dentate gyrus, hippocampal-amygdala transition area (HATA), subiculum, tail, fissure, presubiculum, parasubiculum, molecular layer, and fimbria are shown in magnified (a) sagittal, (b) coronal, and (c) sagittal planes.

Quality Control Procedures

We applied quality assurance for hippocampal subfield segmentations using protocols developed by the ENIGMA-MDD Consortium and the ENIGMA-MDD hippocampal subfields project (see https://pgc-ptsd.com/wp-content/uploads/2017/08/PTSD_Instructions_Subfields_part_IR_II.pdf ).

Hippocampal Shape Analysis

We applied a standard analysis pipeline for subcortical shape developed by ENIGMA [30]. FreeSurfer segmentation and labels created from the volumetric analysis described above were used to generate meshes and shape data for the hippocampus. Vertex information from each subject was extracted to carry out between group analyses with regressors. We applied vertex-wide FDR correction based on 2,502 vertices (see Supplement).

Statistical Analysis

The subfield volumes obtained from FreeSurfer was the dependent variable in an ordinary least square (OLS) regression model run separately for each subfield. Bonferroni correction for multiple testing was applied to the volumetric analyses given that 12 regions from 2 hemispheres were assessed. We included a covariate for ipsilateral whole hippocampal volume because CA1 volume is highly correlated with whole hippocampal volume, i.e. individuals with smaller hippocampi will have smaller CA1 in much the same way that individuals with a small brain (TIV) will have a small hippocampus. Further details of the regression model and regressors are in the Supplement. Results for the CA1, CA3, and DG were not Bonferroni corrected because of prior evidence of volume reduction reported in adults exposed to childhood maltreatment [31] and adult PTSD [67]. Regressors in the initial analysis were selected based on established associations with hippocampal volume, but subsequent re-analysis included only regressors with p < 0.15 (see Supplement).

The preexisting group difference in the level of trauma exposure between the PTSD and Control groups meant that group differences could be attributable to either PTSD or trauma exposure. Inclusion of trauma exposure as covariate could lead to inconclusive findings because removing the variance associated with trauma exposure will also alter variance in the dependent variable associated with diagnostic groups as explained by Miller and Chapman [32]. The role of trauma exposure on hippocampal volume was further scrutinized vis-à-vis PTSD with correlations between trauma exposure and each of the significant subfields results in PTSD and Control groups. The difference in correlations between the PTSD and Control group was compared with Fisher’s r-to-z test [33].

RESULTS

Demographic and clinical characteristics

In broad terms, exposure to combat trauma and lifetime trauma, as well as symptoms of depression and alcohol use were significantly greater in the PTSD group than the trauma-exposed control group. Detailed clinical and demographic information is reported by diagnostic group in Table 1.

Association of subfield volumes with PTSD

We found PTSD was associated with significantly lower volume in L-CA1 (p=.01; Cohen’s d=.21), L-CA3 (p=.04; Cohen’s d=.08), and R-CA3 (p=.02; Cohen’s d=.07). The data was reanalyzed including only regressors with p < 0.15 in the initial univariate model. These results were fully consistent with the initial analyses with all regressors as listed in Table 2 and the Supplement. The largest effect size among regions with significant between-group differences was in L-CA1 (Cohen’s d = .21), whereas the effect size was small for L-CA3 and R-CA3.

Table 2.

Hippocampal Subfield Volume Association with PTSD Including Ipsilateral Whole Hippocampal Volume Covariate

Group statistics Regressor p-values
Hippocampal Subfield Control Mean ± SD PTSD Mean ± SD F-Stat p-value Cohen’s d age gender Child trauma TLEQ CES BDI AUDIT 5-HT Scanner TIV Hippo volume
a priori hypothesized regions
L CA1 627.5±78.7 612.2±66.1 6.24 .01 .21 .45 .47 .43 .51 .31 .31 .04 .77 .02 .15 .0001
R CA1 649.2±85.1 639.0±72.5 0.77 .38 .07 .65 .69 .76 .04 .15 .94 .88 .33 .05 .61 .0001
L CA3 217.5± 31.6 214.8± 33.1 4.00 .04 .08 .74 .55 .36 .87 .91 .003 .30 .84 .31 .96 .0001
R CA3 235.6±31.5 233.5±30.8 5.24 .02 .07 .98 .64 .99 .96 .11 .02 .32 .87 .95 .63 .0001
L DG 304.4± 33.3 299.9± 33.0 2.4 .12 .13 .30 .74 .89 .55 .95 .02 .54 .90 .43 .42 .0001
R DG 317.7±35.9 313.7±33.1 2.12 .15 .11 .81 .12 .51 .72 .62 .08 .87 .35 .35 .72 .0001
non-hypothesized regions
L tail 558.5±70.8 555.5±64.4 .152 .70 .04 .67 .48 .88 .02 .11 .28 .19 .46 .02 .10 0001
R tail 563.2± 76.5 557.8± 69.4 .01 .98 .07 .76 .24 .30 .23 .27 .34 .06 .50 .02 .22 .0001
L subiculum 431.8±54.7 426.7±46.0 .02 .88 .10 .30 .32 .02 .78 .95 .55 .54 .55 .82 .21 .0001
R subiculum 429.1± 49.8 421.9± 41.5 .82 .37 .15 .07 .44 .57 .33 .79 1.00 .15 .58 .47 .42 .0001
L fissure 149.5±28.7 148.1± 24.4 .05 .83 .05 .06 .43 .18 .48 .50 .71 .63 .85 .30 .36 .0001
R fissure 154.6±27.2 155.4±24.9 2.4 .12 −.03 .01 .96 .96 .75 .08 .81 .62 .86 .04 .03 .0001
L presubiculum 314.6± 40.0 310.4± 37.6 .07 .78 .11 .68 .96 .49 .66 .57 .36 .51 .98 .47 .11 .0001
R presubiculum 297.9± 35.5 290.9± 33.7 4.26 .04 .20 .94 .74 .94 .19 .01 .94 .20 .69 .09 .07 .0001
L parasubiculum 66.4± 11.26 63.4± 10.6 3.29 .07 .27 .32 .92 .35 .89 .81 .91 .78 .97 .88 .005 .0001
R parasubiculum 63.2± 10.3 60.6± 10.6 2.97 .09 .25 .03 .73 .72 .36 .51 .21 .56 .38 .21 .02 .0001
L molecular layer 576.1± 66.6 564.0± 57.1 3.83 .05 .19 .97 .34 .20 .17 .27 .88 .58 .65 .78 .45 .0001
R molecular layer 589.4± 65.2 578.3± 55.1 1.8 .18 .18 .83 .01 .50 .51 .30 .60 .37 .76 .10 .32 .0001
L CA4 261.9± 29.0 258.7± 28.7 3.08 .08 .11 .34 .92 .95 .87 .86 .09 .28 .82 .30 .59 .0001
R CA4 274.6±30.8 271.7± 29.2 3.2 .08 .10 .99 .04 .56 .77 .80 .08 .90 .41 .63 .81 .0001
L fimbria 80.1± 19.5 78.1± 20.3 1.40 .24 .10 .80 .83 .46 .29 .57 .78 .08 .71 .0001 .05 .0001
R fimbria 78.3 ±19.3 79.3±19.5 .17 .68 −.05 .03 .84 .96 .23 .21 .04 .70 .41 .0001 .22 .0001
L HATA 64.0± 9.2 63.6±10.5 .13 .72 .04 .19 .91 .62 27 .52 .92 .51 .39 .0001 .28 .0001
R HATA 62.5±10.2 62.2±10.3 .13 .72 .03 .84 .55 .69 .98 .27 .51 .80 .88 .0001 .88 .0001

Subfield volumes for each subject are normalized based on total intracranial volume from FreeSurfer 5.3. The F-statistic and p-value for main effect of diagnosis was first calculated with all covariates. The F-statistic and p-value reported in the table was re-estimated using only the covariates with p < 0.15. Significant (p < .05) main effects of group are highlighted in pink and trending main effects of group (p < 0.1) are highlighted in blue. All p-values reported are uncorrected.

There were no other subfields with significant between-group differences after correction for multiple comparisons. Detailed results of descriptive and inferential statistics including the role of each regressor are provided in Table 2. Serotonergic medication usage, which was a regressor in the initial analyses, was non-significant for all subfields. Results of secondary analysis that omitted data of subjects on antipsychotic medication (n=4) yielded results that were consistent with the main analyses, L-CA1 [F4, 273=5.76, p=0.017], L-CA3 [F4, 273=4.27, p=0.04], and at trend level for R-CA3 [F4, 273=3.80, p=.052]. To address the possible role of SSRI medication, we repeated main analyses by excluding subjects taking SSRI medication (n=56). Our results were consistent with our main findings in the L-CA1 [F4, 220=4.76, p=0.03], L-CA3 [F4, 220 = 3.80, p=0.052], and at trend level for R-CA3 [F4, 220=3.33, p=.07]. Among regressors, the whole hippocampal volume regressor (ipsilateral) showed the most consistent results. Subfields with very small volumes (≤ 100 mm3) consistently demonstrated significant effect of scanner type. In addition, the gender regressor for R-CA4, combat exposure for R-presubiculum, depression symptoms regressor for L-DG, L-CA3, R-CA3, and R-fimbria were nominally significant (Table 2).

Excluding ipsilateral whole hippocampal volume as a covariate resulted in a trend level association of L-CA1 with PTSD [F4, 221=3.32, p = 0.07] and non-significant results for the L-CA3 [F4, 221=0.07, p = 0.79] and R-CA3 [F4, 221=0.23, p = 0.63] (Table 3). To understand the role of including ipsilateral whole hippocampal volume as a covariate, we plotted the residualized L-CA1, L-CA3, and R-CA3 values with the covariates included in the regression model, importantly the ipsilateral whole hippocampal volume (Figure 2).

Table 3.

Hippocampal Subfield Volume Association with PTSD Excluding Ipsilateral Whole Hippocampal Volume Covariate

Hippocampal Subfield Control Mean ± SD PTSD Mean ± SD F-Stat p-value age gender Child trauma TLEQ CES BDI AUDIT 5-HT Scanner TIV
a priori hypotdesized regions
L CA1 627.5±78.7 612.2±66.1 3.32 .07 ns .001 ns ns ns ns ns ns .04 .0001
R CA1 649.2± 85.1 639.0± 72.5 .36 .55 ns .08 ns ns ns ns ns ns .05 ns
L CA3 217.5± 31.6 214.8± 33.1 .07 .79 ns .11 ns ns ns ns ns ns .03 .0001
R CA3 235.6±31.5 233.5±30.8 .23 .63 ns ns ns ns ns ns ns ns .06 .0001
L DG 304.4± 33.3 299.9± 33.0 .96 .33 ns .004 ns ns ns ns ns ns .02 .0001
R DG 317.7±35.9 313.7±33.1 .01 .91 ns ns ns ns ns ns ns ns .002 .0001
non-hypothesized regions
L tail 558.5±70.8 555.5±64.4 .54 .46 ns .05 ns ns ns ns ns ns .01 .0001
R tail 563.2± 76.5 557.8± 69.4 .38 .54 ns .02 ns ns ns ns ns ns .03 .08
L subiculum 431.8±54.7 426.7±46.0 .19 .66 ns ns ns ns ns ns ns ns ns .0001
R subiculum 429.1± 49.8 421.9± 41.5 1.29 .26 ns ns ns ns ns ns ns ns .01 .0001
L fissure 149.5±28.7 148.1± 24.4 .10 .76 .07 ns ns ns ns ns ns ns .05 0001
R fissure 154.6±27.2 155.4±24.9 .37 .55 .004 ns ns ns ns ns ns ns .04 .0001
L presubiculum 314.6± 40.0 310.4± 37.6 .36 .55 ns .02 ns ns ns ns ns ns .01 .0001
R presubiculum 297.9± 35.5 290.9± 33.7 1.68 .20 ns .01 ns ns ns ns ns ns .001 .0001
L parasubiculum 66.4± 11.26 63.4± 10.6 4.50 .04 .08 .10 ns ns ns ns ns ns ns .0001
R parasubiculum 63.2± 10.3 60.6± 10.6 5.16 .02 .05 ns ns ns ns ns ns ns ns .0001
L molecular layer 576.1± 66.6 564.0± 57.1 2.10 .15 ns .01 ns ns ns ns ns ns .01 .0001
R molecular layer 589.4± 65.2 578.3± 55.1 .95 .33 ns .13 ns ns ns ns ns ns .01 .0001
L CA4 261.9± 29.0 258.7± 28.7 .42 .52 ns .01 ns ns ns ns ns ns .06 .0001
R CA4 274.6±30.8 271.7± 29.2 .00 .99 ns ns ns ns ns ns ns ns .002 .0001
L fimbria 80.1± 19.5 78.1± 20.3 .60 .44 .01 .001 ns ns ns ns ns ns .0001 .0001
R fimbria 78.3 ±19.3 79.3±19.5 .08 .78 .001 .01 ns ns ns ns ns ns .0001 ns
L HATA 64.0± 9.2 63.6±10.5 .13 .72 .06 .005 ns ns ns ns ns ns .001 .0001
R HATA 62.5±10.2 62.2±10.3 .04 .84 .07 .05 ns ns ns ns ns ns .0001 .0001

Figure 2.

Figure 2.

The L-CA1, L-CA3, and R-CA3 residualized values for the covariates included in the regression model, which includes the ipsilateral whole hippocampal volume covariate.

Association of subfield volume with trauma exposure

The association between trauma exposure and subfield volumes for L-CA1, L-CA3, and R-CA3 was examined because trauma exposure was collinear with diagnostic groups, i.e. significantly greater in the PTSD than Control group [t281=−8.05; p < .0001). This raised the possibility that subfield volumes were related to trauma exposure rather than PTSD. Correlations between trauma exposure and subfield volumes were weak (r’s < .15). Furthermore, the correlation strength between trauma exposure and subfield volumes was not significantly different in PTSD than in Control groups based on Fisher’s r-to-z for L-CA1 (z=−.055; p=0.95), L-CA3 (z=1.27; p=0.20), and R-CA3 (z=1.15; p=0.25) (Figure 3).

Figure 3.

Figure 3.

Results of the association between lifetime trauma exposure (TLEQ score) and subfield volumes for (A) L-CA1, (B) L-CA3, and (C) R-CA3, were weak and were not significantly different in the PTSD group (red trend line) compared to the trauma-exposed Control group (blue trend line).

The analysis that resulted in a significant association between R-CA3 and PTSD included a regressor for combat exposure based on a significant result for CES from the initial analysis, which included all regressors (Table 2). The CES score was significantly higher in the PTSD group than the control group [t281=−7.53; p<0.0001), which meant that the CES regressor was correlated with diagnostic grouping, again raising the possibility that the group-difference in R-CA3 was associated with combat exposure rather than PTSD. The correlation strengths between combat exposure and R-CA3 subfield volumes were weak (r’s < .06) and not significantly different in PTSD than in Control groups using Fisher’s r-to-z (z=.79; p=0.43) [33].

Effect of Age in PTSD

We found trend-level correlations between age and volume for bilateral CA1, CA2, and DG, but the interaction of age by PTSD diagnosis was non-significant (correlations did not differ significantly in the PTSD group relative to the trauma-exposed control group; Table 3) [6].

Shape Results

The vertex-based analyses revealed that shape differences between the PTSD group and the control group were non-significant after vertex-wide FDR correction for multiple testing (p < .05). Results for trend-level significance with FDR correction (p < 0.2) are provided in Figure 4 for β-map and p-map visualization of shape differences based on the Jacobian determinant.

Figure 4.

Figure 4.

The results of 3D shape analysis at 2,502 vertices for PTSD < Control and PTSD > Control at the FDR corrected significance level of p < 0.2 display of (A) β-map (B) p-map.

Effect of Scanner

There was no systematic difference in the number of cases and controls across scanner [X2=4.48; p = .11]. Subfield findings between scanners did not differ significantly based on PTSD diagnosis when controlling for age. The interaction of PTSD and scanner type was non-significant for L-CA1 [F278,2=1.29, p=0.28], L-CA3 [F278,2=1.40, p=0.25], and R_CA3 [F278,2=1.58, p=0.21].

DISCUSSION

In this study, we examine PTSD-associated differences in the volume of hippocampal subfields and the shape of the whole hippocampus. Our study demonstrates smaller volume in the L-CA1, L-CA3, and R-CA3 in patients with PTSD. Among the present findings, only L-CA1 had a non-trivial effect size, whereas L-CA3 and R-CA3 had very small effect sizes. While trauma-exposure was significantly higher in the PTSD group than the trauma-exposed control group, we found no evidence to support that between-group differences in subfield volumes were correlated to severity of trauma exposure. We found an inverse correlation of the L-DG subfield with age in the combined group, and with L-CA1, and L-DG in the PTSD group, but this relationship was unaffected by PTSD diagnosis. No PTSD-associated differences in hippocampal shape were detected with vertex-based morphometry. Compared to previously published reports of hippocampal subfield volume in PTSD [7], our study used a sample size that was over 2.5 times larger and employed more advanced methods for segmentation of 12 hippocampal subfields based on an atlas constructed from ultra-high resolution MRI scans of postmortem brains.

Lesion studies in model animal systems that introduced neurotoxic lesions in either CA1 or CA3 prior to extinction training eliminated the context dependence of extinguished fear. However lesions of CA1 or CA3 placed after extinction training found that only CA1 lesions impaired context dependence of extinction [19]. Thus, hippocampal CA1 neurons may play an important role in conditioned fear and extinction [20]. Insofar as contextual fear extinction is a realistic model of PTSD and the assumption that reduced CA1 volume implies a corresponding function in humans, our finding is consistent with the clinical features of PTSD, specifically the presence of prominent re-experiencing symptoms that persist in contexts without threat.

Hippocampal subfields CA1–3 are altered by experimental stress in animal studies [34] and are consistent with human behavioral findings of the preferential impact of early-life maltreatment stress in at-risk families [35]. One consequence of this type of stress is corticosteroid-mediated impairment of learning and memory including the formation of false memories [36]. The basolateral amygdala, which is critical to associative emotional learning, projects directly and indirectly through the entorhinal cortex to CA1 [37]. Pavlovian fear conditioning increases synchronization of the basolateral amygdala with CA1 and facilitates consolidation of declarative memory. Whereas repeated exposure to acute restraint stress produces hypertrophy of spine density in the basolateral amygdala and increase in anxiety-like behavior in the mice, it results in long term depression and atrophy in CA1 [38; 39]. Preclinical research in rats shows that the CA1 subfield is involved in context-specific memory retrieval after extinction [19]. Ventral CA1 contains enriched levels of anxiety cells that are activated by anxiogenic environments and required for avoidance behavior in mice [40]. Amygdala-hippocampal interactions are important for both non-declarative and declarative forms of emotional memory. Thus, our finding of lower CA1 volume in PTSD is consistent with the well-designed carefully controlled experiments in animals. In PTSD, the basolateral amygdala and the hippocampus function to control effects of emotion and arousal on consolidation of memories that include spatial or contextual cues about the environment [41].

To interpret the left lateralization of the present CA1 findings in PTSD, we turn to evidence from humans so we may adequately consider the unique role of language and verbal memory, which are lateralized to the left hippocampus [22]. Kerchner and colleagues [42] report that neuronal volume in CA1 alone is associated with episodic memory for verbal, visuospatial, and logical information. This association of CA1 with episodic memory is left lateralized and not found in relation to CA3 or DG. Disruption in verbal memory is a prominent feature of PTSD, particularly the inability to recall granular details of traumatic experiences, which is consistently observed in clinical encounters [36; 43; 44]. These memory disruptions are correlated with left lateralized decrements in hippocampal activation [36; 45]. Thus, converging evidence from behavioral, neuropsychological, functional imaging, volumetric, and lesion studies supports a compelling rationale for left-lateralized CA1 alterations.

Teicher and collegues reported lower volume of CA1, CA2/CA3, fimbria, presubiculum, subiculum, and CA4/DG in adults who experienced maltreatment as children, with 7% of the sample diagnosed with PTSD. However, there are a number of methodologic differences between the present study and Teicher et al. FreeSurfer 5.3 is based on an anatomically incorrect atlas [9], notably for CA1. In studies of PTSD that employed manual subfield segmentation, Wang et al report lower volume in CA3, whereas Mueller et al show no subfield differences associated with PTSD. Several factors for divergent findings in previous studies may include lack of power, differences in military service, education, age, race, and socioeconomic status between samples. Small effect sizes calculated for the present results show that previous studies conducted with a sample sizes of n=36 [6] and n=85 [5] were underpowered. While our findings recapitulate the age associated volume decrease in CA1, we did not find a significant interaction between age and PTSD that explained CA3 volume as reported by Wang et al., [6].

As expected, directly comparing results across studies poses several challenges because of methodologic heterogeneity. The present segmentation with FreeSurfer v6.0 was fully automated, with the exception of quality checks performed on 12 segmented labels. However, Wang et al. and Mueller et al. employed manual segmentation, which may be able to overcome image artifacts sometimes overlooked by automated techniques. However, recent developments in automated methods take advantage of generative models for multi-atlas image segmentation that do not rely on the intensity of the training images. The, generative approaches used in FreeSurfer v6.0 and SPM have bias field estimation integrated into the model and are therefore very robust to bias fields. However, we actually applied the FreeSurfer segmentation with the bias field correction turned off because the norm.mgz is already bias field corrected and moreover the bias field around the hippocampus is negligible. Noise is also integrated into the framework by modeling Gaussian distributions. An explicit model for motion is challenging to implement because it needs to contend with a variety of pulse sequences. Thus, severe motion can indeed derail the algorithm, but in practice it works very well [46]. Typically, the spatially varying tissue priors used by automated segmentations are produced by humans and training sets used by machines are also initially classified by humans [47]. FreeSurfer v6.0 combines ex-vivo and in-vivo scans with the former acquired on 15 postmortem brains scanned at 7-Tesla with 130-μm isotropic resolution, whereas v5.3 used in vivo atlas from five cases acquired at 9-fold lower resolution of 380-μm3 resolution and manual segmentation studies were acquired at 4T with 180-fold lower resolution of 400 × 500 μm in-plane and 2000-μm through-plane resolution. Although the landmarks chosen to delineate the subfield boundaries on the high resolution 7T ex vivo images are defined based on knowledge derived from histological exams, the accuracy of the resulting ex vivo labels are not confirmed by a histological exam in these specimens.

We conducted analyses with and without the inclusion of ipsilateral whole hippocampal volume as a covariate. Our reasoning for including ipsilateral whole hippocampal volume covariate is based on the fact that CA1 volume is highly correlated with whole hippocampal volume, i.e. individuals with smaller hippocampi will have smaller CA1 in much the same way that individuals with a smaller brains (TIV) will have a small hippocampi. This correlation is strong between L-CA1 and L-whole hippocampal volume in the control group (R2=0.8647) and in the PTSD group (R2=.8679). Thus, including the covariate for whole hippocampal volume is more effective at accounting for sources of variance, in particular for individual differences in brain size for both control (R2=0.3565) and PTSD (R2=0.3197) groups. While it is possible that smaller overall hippocampal volume in PTSD might partially mask the association between PTSD and subfield volume, in fact, it appears that this source of variance contributes more to the model than diagnostic status. Additional arguments for including ipsilateral whole hippocampal volume over TIV include (1) TIV does not account for laterality effects since it encompasses both hemispheres unlike ipsilateral whole hippocampal volume which is hemisphere-specific, and (2) the subfield volume is highly correlated with ipsilateral whole hippocampal volume because the FS v6.0 subfield segmentation relies heavily on atlas priors as compared to specific anatomical landmarks. By contrast, not including ipsilateral whole hippocampal volume answers a different but interesting question, which is whether subfield volume is associated with PTSD but without regard to the volume of the whole hippocampus. The association of the ‘absolute subfield volume’ to PTSD is also important because it does not presuppose the role of whole hippocampal volume or the association between PTSD and whole hippocampal volume. A thorough and thoughtful treatment of multiple regression modeling with multiple collinear variables is provided by Friedman and Wall [48] and also by Wurm and Fisicaro [49].

An estimation of statistical power for the largest published study (n=97) [7] to detect group differences in CA1 volume based on the effect size we observed (Cohen’s d=.21), reveals only 55.3% power to reject the null hypothesis or a 44.7% chance of a false positive. By comparison, the present sample size (n=282) based on the same effects size (0.21) attains 94.9% power to reject the null or a 5.1% chance of a false positive. Clearly the results of previously published studies are concerning for reproducibility with a false positive chance approaching 50%.

Limitations

An important limitation in interpreting our finding of diminished CA1 volume is the lack of correlative behavioral measures of memory. Lacking such data, only indirect and tenuous associations can be made between PTSD diagnosis and memory disturbance, which can be inferred from symptoms of recurrent intrusive trauma memories and trauma reliving, or by invoking prior reports that demonstrate PTSD is associated with disrupted fear memory [50]. Nonetheless, we feel that the outcome of the present study could generate future work to test these predictions.

FreeSurfer v6.0 segmentation was qualitatively compared with manual segmentation on a publicly available dataset of high resolution T1/T2 images [27] and quantitatively compared with the ADNI dataset, which involved discriminating MCI and normally aging elderly subjects. FreeSurfer compares favorably to manual segmentation based on extensive testing of FreeSurfer v6.0 as described by Iglesias et al [10].

A notable limitation of the FreeSurfer 6.0 scheme for parcellation is that it depends heavily on a shape prior from an atlas composed of 15 ex vivo ultra-high resolution images acquired at 7T and hence delineates structures that cannot be discerned visually on in vivo 1-mm isotropic images with sufficient clarity to accurately parcellate 12 subfields. This difference of approach is a concern because the algorithm sometimes relies on the shape of priors for labeling rather than contrast information detected in the image being segmented [10]. The lack of sufficient contrast features from the in vivo data makes robust quality control a challenge even by the most expert manual rater.

Our results lacked significant shape findings related to PTSD after applying the correction for multiple comparisons. While the corrections are applied to control Type 1 error, the procedure invariably comes at the cost of inflating Type II error. On the other hand, we imposed no correction on the volumetry results for CA1 because of a priori evidence implicating CA1 in PTSD, which eliminated any risk of Type II error. Thus, the first possible explanation relates to the application of multiple comparison correction in the shape analysis but no correction in the volumetry analysis of CA1. It does appear that the uncorrected shape results show differences in the area of CA1. The second explanation is that shape differences are just below the threshold that is deemed significant (e.g. p > 0.05). However, it is possible that the subthreshold vertex-based differences accumulate to produce a volume difference that is significantly different between groups. This is all the more likely given that the effect size of volume differences was small even for the CA1, which was the largest effect size among the subfields. The third possibility is simply some combination of the first two.

A limitation is our use of three different scanners (two GE 3T and one Philips 3T). We have previously analyzed FreeSurfer v6.0 output from different scanners with evidence that scanner heterogeneity provides robust and consistent results for hippocampal segmentation [4] and hippocampal subfield segmentation [25].

Conclusions

Our results provide robust evidence of an association between smaller hippocampal L-CA1 volume and PTSD. Lower CA1 volume in PTSD is consistent with established research in mice that demonstrates atrophy in CA1 and increased anxiety-like behavior results from repeated exposure to acute stress. Further work on hippocampal subfield plasticity in PTSD with longitudinal studies will be important to elucidate the role of subfield alterations on learning and memory impairments in PTSD, particularly associative fear learning, extinction training, and the formation of false memories.

Supplementary Material

Supp FigS2a
Supp FigS3
Supp figS1
Supp figS2b
Supp info

ACKNOWLEDGMENTS

This research was supported by the U.S. Department of Veterans Affairs (VA) Mid-Atlantic Mental Illness Research, Education, and Clinical Center (MIRECC) core funds. Dr. Morey also received financial support from the VA Office of Research and Development (5I01CX000748–01, 5I01CX000120–02). Additional financial support was provided by the National Institute for Neurological Disorders and Stroke (R01NS086885–01A1). Juan Eugenio Iglesias is supported by a Starting Grant from the European Research Council (project no. 677697: “BUNGEE-TOOLS”). Emily Dennis is supported by a grant from the NINDS (K99 NS096116). Emily Dennis, Christopher Whelan, Boris Gutman, Neda Jahanshad, and Paul Thompson are supported by NIH grants to Paul Thompson: U54 EB020403 (BD2K), R01 EB008432, R01 AG040060, and R01 NS080655. Finally, we could not have conducted this study without the willing participation of Veterans.

Abbreviations:

SD

Standard Deviation

AA

African American

CAPS

Clinician Administered PTSD Scale

BDI

Beck Depression Inventory

TLEQ

Trauma Life Events Questionnaire

AUDIT

Alcohol Use Disorders Test

TIV

Total Intracranial Volume

REFERENCES:

  • 1.Elzinga BM, Bremner JD. Are the neural substrates of memory the final common pathway in posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD)? Journal of Affective Disorders 2002;70(1):1–17. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Bremner JD, Randall P, Vermetten E et al. Magnetic resonance imaging-based measurement of hippocampal volume in posttraumatic stress disorder related to childhood physical and sexual abuse--a preliminary report. Biological Psychiatry 1997;41(1):23–32. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Morey RA, Gold AL, LaBar KS et al. Amygdala volume changes in posstraumatic stress disorder in a large case-controlled veteran group. Archives of General Psychiatry 2012;69(11):1–10. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Logue MW, van Rooij SJH, Dennis EL et al. Smaller Hippocampal Volume in Posttraumatic Stress Disorder: A Multi-Site ENIGMA-PGC Study. Biological Psychiatry 2017;In Press. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Mueller SG, Ng P, Neylan T et al. Evidence for disrupted gray matter structural connectivity in posttraumatic stress disorder. Psychiatry Research: Neuroimaging 2015;234(2):194–201. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Wang Z, Neylan TC, Mueller SG et al. Magnetic resonance imaging of hippocampal subfields in posttraumatic stress disorder. Arch Gen Psychiatry 2010;67(3):296–303. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Hayes JP, Hayes S, Miller DR et al. Automated measurement of hippocampal subfields in PTSD: Evidence for smaller dentate gyrus volume. Journal of Psychiatric Research 2017. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Van Leemput K, Bakkour A, Benner T et al. Automated segmentation of hippocampal subfields from ultra-high resolution in vivo MRI. Hippocampus 2009;19(6):549–57. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Wisse LE, Biessels GJ, Geerlings MI. A critical appraisal of the hippocampal subfield segmentation package in FreeSurfer. Frontiers in aging neuroscience 2014;6:261. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Iglesias JE, Augustinack JC, Nguyen K et al. A computational atlas of the hippocampal formation using ex vivo, ultra-high resolution MRI: Application to adaptive segmentation of in vivo MRI. NeuroImage 2015;115:117–137. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Schmidt B, Marrone DF, Markus EJ. Disambiguating the similar: the dentate gyrus and pattern separation. Behavioural brain research 2012;226(1):56–65. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Yassa MA, Stark CE. Pattern separation in the hippocampus. Trends in neurosciences 2011;34(10):515–525. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Kheirbek MA, Klemenhagen KC, Sahay A, Hen R. Neurogenesis and generalization: a new approach to stratify and treat anxiety disorders. Nature neuroscience 2012;15(12):1613. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Morey R, Dunsmoor J, Haswell C et al. Fear learning circuitry is biased toward generalization of fear associations in posttraumatic stress disorder. Translational psychiatry 2015;5(12):e700. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.O’Reilly RC, Norman KA. Hippocampal and neocortical contributions to memory: Advances in the complementary learning systems framework. Trends in cognitive sciences 2002;6(12):505–510. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Parsons RG, Ressler KJ. Implications of memory modulation for post-traumatic stress and fear disorders. Nat Neurosci 2013;16(2):146–53. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Vyas A, Mitra R, Shankaranarayana Rao BS, Chattarji S. Chronic stress induces contrasting patterns of dendritic remodeling in hippocampal and amygdaloid neurons. J Neurosci 2002;22(15):6810–8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Gould E, Tanapat P, McEwen BS et al. Proliferation of granule cell precursors in the dentate gyrus of adult monkeys is diminished by stress. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 1998;95(6):3168–3171. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Ji J, Maren S. Differential roles for hippocampal areas CA1 and CA3 in the contextual encoding and retrieval of extinguished fear. Learning & Memory 2008;15(4):244–251. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Furini C, Myskiw J, Izquierdo I. The learning of fear extinction. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews 2014;47:670–683. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Sass KJ, Spencer D, Kim J et al. Verbal memory impairment correlates with hippocampal pyramidal cell density. Neurology 1990;40(11):1694–1694. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Papanicolaou AC, Simos PG, Castillo EM et al. The hippocampus and memory of verbal and pictorial material. Learning & Memory 2002;9(3):99–104. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Tronson NC, Schrick C, Guzman YF et al. Segregated populations of hippocampal principal CA1 neurons mediating conditioning and extinction of contextual fear. Journal of Neuroscience 2009;29(11):3387–3394. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Dillon GM, Qu X, Marcus JN, Dodart J-C. Excitotoxic lesions restricted to the dorsal CA1 field of the hippocampus impair spatial memory and extinction learning in C57BL/6 mice. Neurobiology of learning and memory 2008;90(2):426–433. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Whelan CD, Hibar DP, van Velzen LS et al. Heritability and reliability of automatically segmented human hippocampal formation subregions. NeuroImage 2015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Logue MW, Amstadter AB, Baker DG et al. The Psychiatric Genomics Consortium Posttraumatic Stress Disorder Workgroup: Posttraumatic stress disorder enters the age of large-scale genomic collaboration. Neuropsychopharmacology 2015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Winterburn JL, Pruessner JC, Chavez S et al. A novel in vivo atlas of human hippocampal subfields using high-resolution 3T magnetic resonance imaging. Neuroimage 2013;74:254–265. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Brancu M, Wagner HR, Morey RA et al. The Post‐Deployment Mental Health (PDMH) study and repository: A multi‐site study of US Afghanistan and Iraq era veterans. International Journal of Methods in Psychiatric Research 2017;26(3). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Fischl B, Salat DH, Busa E et al. Whole brain segmentation: automated labeling of neuroanatomical structures in the human brain. Neuron 2002;33(3):341–55. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Gutman B, Wang Y, Morra J et al. Disease classification with hippocampal shape invariants. Hippocampus 2009;19(6):572. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Teicher MH, Anderson CM, Polcari A. Childhood maltreatment is associated with reduced volume in the hippocampal subfields CA3, dentate gyrus, and subiculum. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 2012;109(9):E563–E572. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Miller GA, Chapman JP. Misunderstanding analysis of covariance. Journal of Abnormal Psychology 2001;110(1):40–48. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Diedenhofen B, Musch J. cocor: A comprehensive solution for the statistical comparison of correlations. PloS one 2015;10(4):e0121945. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Sapolsky RM. Atrophy of the hippocampus in posttraumatic stress disorder: how and when? Hippocampus 2001;11(2):90–1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Morey RA, Haswell CC, Hooper SR, De Bellis MD. Amygdala, Hippocampus, and Ventral Medial Prefrontal Cortex Volumes Differ in Maltreated Youth with and without Chronic Posttraumatic Stress Disorder. Neuropsychopharmacology 2015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Hayes JP, LaBar KS, McCarthy G et al. Reduced hippocampal and amygdala activity predicts memory distortions for trauma reminders in combat-related PTSD. J Psychiatr Res 2011;45(5):660–9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Pikkarainen M, Rönkkö S, Savander V et al. Projections from the lateral, basal, and accessory basal nuclei of the amygdala to the hippocampal formation in rat. Journal of Comparative Neurology 1999;403(2):229–260. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Zhou Q, Homma KJ, Poo M-m. Shrinkage of dendritic spines associated with long-term depression of hippocampal synapses. Neuron 2004;44(5):749–757. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Oh WC, Hill TC, Zito K. Synapse-specific and size-dependent mechanisms of spine structural plasticity accompanying synaptic weakening. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 2013;110(4):E305–E312. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Jimenez J, Su K, Goldberg A et al. Anxiety cells in a hippocampal hypothalamic circuit. Neuron 2018;97:1–14. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Roozendaal B, McEwen BS, Chattarji S. Stress, memory and the amygdala. Nature Reviews Neuroscience 2009;10(6):423–433. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Kerchner GA, Deutsch GK, Zeineh M et al. Hippocampal CA1 apical neuropil atrophy and memory performance in Alzheimer’s disease. Neuroimage 2012;63(1):194–202. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.van der Kolk BA, Fisler R. Dissociation and the fragmentary nature of traumatic memories: overview and exploratory study. Journal of Traumatic Stress 1995;8(4):505–25. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Besnard A, Sahay A. Adult hippocampal neurogenesis, fear generalization, and stress. Neuropsychopharmacology 2016;41(1):24–44. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Bremner JD, Vythilingam M, Vermetten E et al. MRI and PET study of deficits in hippocampal structure and function in women with childhood sexual abuse and posttraumatic stress disorder. American Journal of Psychiatry 2003;160(5):924–32. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Iglesias JE, Sabuncu MR, Van Leemput K. A generative model for multi-atlas segmentation across modalities; 2012. City: IEEE, p 888–891. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Fischl B FreeSurfer. Neuroimage 2012;62(2):774–781. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Friedman L, Wall M. Graphical views of suppression and multicollinearity in multiple linear regression. The American Statistician 2005;59(2):127–136. [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Wurm LH, Fisicaro SA. What residualizing predictors in regression analyses does (and what it does not do). Journal of Memory and Language 2014;72:37–48. [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Garfinkel SN, Abelson JL, King AP et al. Impaired contextual modulation of memories in PTSD: an fMRI and psychophysiological study of extinction retention and fear renewal. The Journal of Neuroscience 2014;34(40):13435–13443. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

Supp FigS2a
Supp FigS3
Supp figS1
Supp figS2b
Supp info

RESOURCES