Skip to main content
Data in Brief logoLink to Data in Brief
. 2018 Nov 14;21:2129–2133. doi: 10.1016/j.dib.2018.11.040

Feature coding dataset for trained and untrained working memory tasks in randomized controlled trials of working memory training

Susan E Gathercole 1,, Darren L Dunning 1, Joni Holmes 1, Dennis G Norris 1
PMCID: PMC6262199  PMID: 30533463

Abstract

The data presented in this article are produced as part of the original research article entitled “Working memory training involves learning new skills” (Gathercole, Dunning, Holmes & Norris, in press). This article presents a dataset of coded features for pairs of trained and untrained working memory (WM) tasks from randomized controlled trials of WM training with active control groups. Feature coding is provided for 113 untrained WM tasks each paired with the most similar task in the training program, taken from 23 training studies. A spreadsheet provides summary information for each task pair, its transfer effect size, and coding of the following features for each task: stimulus category, stimulus domain, stimulus modality, response modality, and recall paradigm.


Specifications table

Subject area Psychology
More specific subject area Cognitive psychology
Type of data Excel spreadsheet
How data were acquired Taken from published reports and where necessary supplied by authors on request
Data format Raw
Experimental factors None
Experimental features Pairs of trained and untrained working memory tasks were coded according to a novel feature coding protocol
Data source location Data are held in the home institutions of the 23 original articles listed inTable 1
Data accessibility Data supplied with the article
Related research article Gathercole SE, Dunning DL, Holmes J, Norris DG. Working memory training involves learning new skills. J Mem & Lang. in press. [1]

Value of the data

  • This assembly of effect sizes for transfer following working memory (WM) to other WM tasks provides a resource that will enable other researchers to analyze the factors associated with transfer.

  • The specification of coded features will facilitate the development of an expanded protocol to guide understanding of the cognitive mechanisms underpinning transfer following WM training.

  • This illustration of the feature coding protocol could support its application to other studies and areas of cognitive training.

  • The transfer effect size data will aid the calculations of statistical power in future studies of WM training.

1. Data

The data consist of 113 pairs of trained and untrained tasks derived from 23 published randomized controlled trials (RCTs) of transfer following working memory training that included an active control group. the spreadsheet supplies the following information about each pair of tasks: a brief task summary, details of the participants, the effect size for transfer, and coding of the following features – stimulus category, stimulus domain, stimulus modality, response modality, and recall paradigm.

2. Experimental design, materials and methods

The criteria for selection of the randomized controlled trials of WM training are described in Gathercole et al. (2018) [1] (YJMLA3988). Details of the studies are provided in Table 1.

Table 1.

Characteristics of the selected studies.

Study Sample Selection criteria N experimental group N control group
Ang et al. [2] School-age children Low working memory 32 (updating), 25 (Cogmed) 28
Bergman Nutley et al. [3] Preschool children none 24 26
Bigorra et al. [4] School-age children ADHD 30 31
Brehmer et al. [5] Adults 20–30 years & 60–70 years 54 45
Chacko et al. [6] School-age children ADHD 44 41
Chooi & Thompson [7] Adults None 15 26
Dentz et al. [8] 18–63 years ADHD 23 21
Dunning & Holmes [9] 18–21 years None 15 15
Foster et al. [10] 18–35 years Low and high memory span 40 (complex span), 39( running span) 39
Gray et al. [11] Adolescents Learning difficulties & ADHD 32 20
Harrison et al. [12] Adults None 21 (complex span), 17 (simple span) 17
Henry et al. [13] School-age children None 18 18
Hitchcock, Westwell [14] School-age children None 50 44
Karbach et al. [15] School-age children None 14 14
Kundu et al. [16] Adults None 15 15
Lawlor-Savage, Goghari [17] Adults None 27 30
Metzler-Baddeley et al [18] Adults None 20 20
Minear et al. [19] Adults None 31 (n-back1), 32 ( complex span) 26
Passolunghi & Costa [20] Preschool children None 15 15
Redick et al. [21] Adults None 24 29
Thompson et al. [22] Adults None 20 19
Van der Molen et al. [23] Adolescents Learning difficulties 41 26
von Bastian et al. [24] Adults 18–35 years & 61–77 years 61 62

Task pairing and feature coding were conducted as follows. Each untrained WM task was matched with a single WM task in the training program and both tasks were then coded according to five categories of feature: stimulus type (digits, letters, words, objects, spatial locations), stimulus domain (verbal, visuo-spatial), stimulus modality (auditory, visual), response modality (spoken, manual), and recall paradigm (serial recall, complex span, backward span, running span and N-back). Coding of the ‘serial recall’ feature was restricted simple serial recall tasks and not to the other complex WM paradigms which also require the recall or serial order. Feature coding was conducted independently by SG and DD/ JH, with differences resolved by discussion. The procedure for matching the trained task with each untrained task within each study was as follows.

  • (i)

    Match on both paradigm and stimulus domain (e.g., verbal & complex span).

  • (ii)

    If 1 is not possible, match on paradigm alone (e.g., complex memory, or serial recall).

  • (iii)

    If 2 is not possible or there are multiple trained tasks for 2, match on the trained task with the greatest total number of other matched features.

  • (iv)

    If two or more training activities are equivalently matched according to the above criteria, select a single representative trained task for matching.

For some tasks, it was necessary to code multiple features within a single category. For example, each stimulus item in a dual n-back task consists of both a verbal and visuo-spatial stimulus and was coded as having both features. In total, 113 pairs of trained (T) and untrained (UT) WM tasks met the task selection criteria. For each task pair, each feature was coded as either not present (empty cell), present in the trained task only (T), present in the untrained task only (UT), or present in both tasks (T&UT). In the four studies in which different groups performed different WM training programs, each untrained task was matched with the closest task from each of the different training programs, generating multiple task pairs for the same untrained task. The full feature coding matrix is provided in Table S2.

Cohen׳s d was employed as an index of the effect size for transfer following adaptive training for each pairs of tasks. This is calculated as the difference in the performance gains on the untrained task (post- vs pre-training scores) between groups (adaptive group gain score – control group gain) divided by the pooled SD of the gains scores from both groups.

Acknowledgments

This research was supported by the Medical Research Council of the UK, the University of Cambridge, the Economic and Social Research Council, UK (RES-000-23-0979), and the Leverhulme Trust, UK (F00/224/AI).

Footnotes

Transparency document

Transparency document associated with this article can be found in the online version at https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dib.2018.11.040.

Appendix A

Supplementary data associated with this article can be found in the online version at https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dib.2018.11.040.

Transparency document. Supplementary material

Supplementary material

mmc1.docx (12.4KB, docx)

.

Appendix A. Supplementary material

Supplementary material

mmc2.xlsx (41.2KB, xlsx)

.

References

  • 1.Gathercole S., Dunning D., Holmes J., Norris D. Working memory training involves learning new skills. J. Mem. Lang. 2018 doi: 10.1016/j.jml.2018.10.003. (In press) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Ang S.Y., Lee K., Cheam F., Poon K., Koh J. Updating and working memory training: immediate improvement, long-term maintenance, and generalisability to non-trained tasks. J. Appl. Res. Mem. Cognit. 2015;4:121–128. [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Bergman Nutley S., Söderqvist S., Bryde S., Thorell L.B., Humphreys K., Klingberg T. Gains in fluid intelligence after training non‐verbal reasoning in 4‐year‐old children: a controlled, randomized study. Dev. Sci. 2011;14:591–601. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2010.01022.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Bigorra A., Garolera M., Guijarro S., Hervás A. Long-term far-transfer effects of working memory training in children with ADHD: a randomized controlled trial. Eur. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry. 2016;25:853–867. doi: 10.1007/s00787-015-0804-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Brehmer Y., Westerberg H., Bäckman L. Working-memory training in younger and older adults: training gains, transfer, and maintenance. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 2012;6:63. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2012.00063. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Chacko A., Bedard A.C., Marks D.J., Feirsen N., Uderman J.Z., Chimiklis A., Rajwan E., Cornwell M., Anderson L., Zwilling A., Ramon M. A randomized clinical trial of Cogmed working memory training in school‐age children with ADHD: a replication in a diverse sample using a control condition. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry. 2014;55:247–255. doi: 10.1111/jcpp.12146. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Chooi W.T., Thompson L.A. Working memory training does not improve intelligence in healthy young adults. Intelligence. 2012;40:531–542. [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Dentz A., Guay M.C., Parent V., Romo L. Working memory training for adults with ADHD. J. Atten. Disord. 2017 doi: 10.1177/1087054717723987. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Dunning D.L., Holmes J. Does working memory training promote the use of strategies on untrained working memory tasks? Mem. Cognit. 2014;42:854–862. doi: 10.3758/s13421-014-0410-5. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Foster J.L., Harrison T.L., Hicks K.L., Draheim C., Redick T.S., Engle R.W. Do the effects of working memory training depend on baseline ability level? J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cognit. 2017;43(11):1677. doi: 10.1037/xlm0000426. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Gray S.A., Chaban P., Martinussen R., Goldberg R., Gotlieb H., Kronitz R., Hockenberry M., Tannock R. Effects of a computerized working memory training program on working memory, attention, and academics in adolescents with severe LD and comorbid ADHD: a randomized controlled trial. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry. 2012;53:1277–1284. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2012.02592.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Harrison T.L., Shipstead Z., Hicks K.L., Hambrick D.Z., Redick T.S., Engle R.W. Working memory training may increase working memory capacity but not fluid intelligence. Psychol. Sci. 2013;24:2409–2419. doi: 10.1177/0956797613492984. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Henry L.A., Messer D.J., Nash G. Testing for near and far transfer effects with a short, face‐to‐face adaptive working memory training intervention in typical children. Infant Child Dev. 2014;23:84–103. [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Hitchcock C., Westwell M.S. A cluster‐randomised, controlled trial of the impact of Cogmed working memory training on both academic performance and regulation of social, emotional and behavioural challenges. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry. 2017;58:140–150. doi: 10.1111/jcpp.12638. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Karbach J., Strobach T., Schubert T. Adaptive working-memory training benefits reading, but not mathematics in middle childhood. Child Neuropsychol. 2015;21:285–301. doi: 10.1080/09297049.2014.899336. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Kundu B., Sutterer D.W., Emrich S.M., Postle B.R. Strengthened effective connectivity underlies transfer of working memory training to tests of short-term memory and attention. J. Neurosci. 2013;33:8705–8715. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5565-12.2013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Lawlor-Savage L., Goghari V.M. Dual N-back working memory training in healthy adults: a randomized comparison to processing speed training. PLoS One. 2016;11:e0151817. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0151817. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Metzler-Baddeley C., Caeyenberghs K., Foley S., Jones D.K. Task complexity and location specific changes of cortical thickness in executive and salience networks after working memory training. Neuroimage. 2016;130:48–62. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.01.007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Minear M., Brasher F., Guerrero C.B., Brasher M., Moore A., Sukeena J. A simultaneous examination of two forms of working memory training: evidence for near transfer only. Mem. Cognit. 2016;44:1014–1037. doi: 10.3758/s13421-016-0616-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Passolunghi M.C., Costa H.M. Working memory and early numeracy training in preschool children. Child Neuropsychol. 2016;22:81–98. doi: 10.1080/09297049.2014.971726. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Redick T.S., Shipstead Z., Harrison T.L., Hicks K.L., Fried D.E., Hambrick D.Z., Kane M.J., Engle R.W. No evidence of intelligence improvement after working memory training: a randomized, placebo-controlled study. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 2013;142:359. doi: 10.1037/a0029082. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Thompson T.W., Waskom M.L., Garel K.L., Cardenas-Iniguez C., Reynolds G.O., Winter R., Chang P., Pollard K., Lala N., Alvarez G.A., Gabrieli J.D. Failure of working memory training to enhance cognition or intelligence. PLoS One. 2013;8:e63614. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0063614. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Van der Molen M., Van Luit J.E., Van der Molen M.W., Klugkist I., Jongmans M.J. Effectiveness of a computerised working memory training in adolescents with mild to borderline intellectual disabilities. J. Intellect. Disabil. Res. 2010;54:433–447. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2788.2010.01285.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.von Bastian C.C., Oberauer K. Distinct transfer effects of training different facets of working memory capacity. J. Mem. Lang. 2013;69:36–58. [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

Supplementary material

mmc1.docx (12.4KB, docx)

Supplementary material

mmc2.xlsx (41.2KB, xlsx)

Articles from Data in Brief are provided here courtesy of Elsevier

RESOURCES