Skip to main content
The Journal of Physiology logoLink to The Journal of Physiology
. 2018 Jun 2;596(23):5687–5708. doi: 10.1113/JP274950

Waking up too early – the consequences of preterm birth on sleep development

Laura Bennet 1,, David W Walker 2, Rosemary S C Horne 3
PMCID: PMC6265542  PMID: 29691876

Abstract

Good quality sleep of sufficient duration is vital for optimal physiological function and our health. Sleep deprivation is associated with impaired neurocognitive function and emotional control, and increases the risk for cardiometabolic diseases, obesity and cancer. Sleep develops during fetal life with the emergence of a recognisable pattern of sleep states in the preterm fetus associated with the development, maturation and connectivity within neural networks in the brain. Despite the physiological importance of sleep, surprisingly little is known about how sleep develops in individuals born preterm. Globally, an estimated 15 million babies are born preterm (<37 weeks gestation) each year, and these babies are at significant risk of neural injury and impaired brain development. This review discusses how sleep develops during fetal and neonatal life, how preterm birth impacts on sleep development to adulthood, and the factors which may contribute to impaired brain and sleep development, leading to altered neurocognitive, behavioural and motor capabilities in the infant and child. Going forward, the challenge is to identify specific risk factors for impaired sleep development in preterm babies to allow for the design of interventions that will improve the quality and quantity of sleep throughout life.

graphic file with name TJP-596-5687-g002.jpg

Keywords: perinatal, preterm, sleep, neurodevelopment

Introduction

As children we fight it, as adults we crave it – that mysterious period of relative unconsciousness we experience each day called sleep. After we are born, sleep is defined as a “reversible behavioural state of perceptual disengagement from and unresponsiveness to the environment” (Carskadon & Dement, 2011). Our brain is driven to go to sleep through two integrated processes; an increase in hypnogenic substances throughout the day which drive the homeostatic need for sleep (Process S) interacting with our internal biological circadian clock (Process C) (Schwartz & Kilduff, 2015). The purpose of sleep is multifactorial, but primarily it facilitates the encoding and consolidation of information (memory retention and forgetting) (Feld & Born, 2017), as well as the emotion we experience during the day and over time (Altena et al. 2016). Additionally, sleep appears essential for maintenance of the neural network, facilitating neuronal and glial connectivity and synaptic plasticity (Cirelli & Tononi, 2017; Feld & Born, 2017; Lowe et al. 2017).

Sleep is vital for our health and wellbeing. Short‐ and long‐term sleep deprivation, for example, negatively impacts on our neurocognitive capacities such as executive functioning, sustained attention, memory retention and retrieval of long‐term memory (Krause et al. 2017; Lowe et al. 2017). Impaired or inadequate sleep is also associated with an increased risk for cardio‐metabolic diseases, immune compromise, cancer and premature death, as well as fertility and pregnancy problems (Sen & Sellix, 2016; Cappuccio & Miller, 2017; Hatori et al. 2017). Indeed, active disruption of sleep and loss of the circadian drive for sleep through such factors as increased activity at night, and exposure to light‐emitting electronic devices and shift‐work, is now recognised as a significant public health risk (Hatori et al. 2017).

As this review will demonstrate, the foundational architecture of our sleep is laid down before we are born, and continues to evolve throughout our lives as the brain grows and changes its internal connectivity. This raises the question of what happens when we interrupt the normal ontogeny of sleep development through being born too early. Each year around 15 million babies are born preterm (<37 weeks of gestation), and preterm birth has an adverse impact on neurodevelopment (Kidokoro et al. 2014; Brumbaugh et al. 2016; Back, 2017), which persists into adult life (Allotey et al. 2018). A recent meta‐analysis of 74 studies (64,061 children) showed that prematurity at any age was associated with lower academic, cognitive and motor skills, and increased behavioural problems such as attention deficit hyperactivity disorder, with younger babies more at risk (Allotey et al. 2018). Structural brain abnormalities and altered functional connectivity of the neural network underpin these cognitive, motor and behavioural deficits (Kidokoro et al. 2014; Brumbaugh et al. 2016; Scheinost et al. 2016; Back, 2017). Thus, how does sleep develop in a brain that itself does not develop normally? The purpose of this review is to discuss the development of sleep before birth, the impact of preterm birth on sleep from infant to adult life, and the factors which may contribute to the development of sleep dysfunction.

The ontogeny of sleep from fetus to newborn

Development of the neural network

Sleep‐like activity evolves as the brain matures and the neural network becomes more coherent (Kostovic & Judas, 2010), and evaluation of sleep can be considered “a complex phenotype of developmental neural plasticity” (Scher & Loparo, 2009). By the time that embryonic neurulation is complete, the brain is producing cells at a rate of around 250,000 per minute (Stiles & Jernigan, 2010). By the end of the embryonic phase at the 12th week of gestational age (GA), the basic structures of the central and peripheral nervous systems are in place (Stiles & Jernigan, 2010). In humans, neuron production starts around embryonic day 42 and this process is largely complete by mid‐gestation, and neural migration effectively completed by 26–29 weeks GA (Bystron et al. 2008; Tymofiyeva et al. 2012). The earliest oligodendrocyte progenitors migrate from the ventricular and sub‐ventricular zones around 20–22 weeks GA (Rivkin et al. 1995), with peak production for these future myelin producing cells occurring between 23 and 32 weeks GA (van Tilborg et al. 2017). Thus, this preterm period of life is critical for white matter and subsequent cortical neuronal development, the latter itself requiring appropriate maturation of white matter (Back, 2017; van Tilborg et al. 2017). As discussed below, it is also a critical time for establishing functional connectivity of the neural network which is important for sleep regulation.

The ontogeny of the major neural networks is harder to determine, but some evidence suggests that circuit formation begins around 16–21 weeks GA in human development, with first synapses evident around 16 weeks GA (de Graaf‐Peters & Hadders‐Algra, 2006; Tymofiyeva et al. 2012). Functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) studies have been used to examine the development of neural networks in both fetuses (Jakab et al. 2014; Thomason et al. 2015; Friedrichs‐Maeder et al. 2017), and infants (Smyser et al. 2010; Fransson et al. 2011; Ball et al. 2012, 2014). The ability of the brain to functionally process complex signals requires the development of a structural, hierarchically connected network that allows for fast, short‐range (adjacent areas) and local processing of information within specific network modules (regional intra‐clusters and connector inter‐clusters), combined with global integration between modules or hubs through long‐range connections (e.g. thalamocortical), and so called ‘rich‐club’ hubs (densely connected network hub regions) (Ouyang et al. 2017).

fMRI studies have demonstrated the existence of primitive neural networks in human fetuses as young as 21–24 weeks GA (Jakab et al. 2014; Thomason et al. 2015). Networks expand in a region‐specific manner starting with the occipital region followed by network activity in temporal, frontal and then parietal regions, i.e. from primary sensory to association and higher order processing areas (Jakab et al. 2014; Thomason et al. 2015). There is increasing coherence between networks with age (Jakab et al. 2014; Thomason et al. 2015), and the expansion of the overall connectivity network, short‐range, and interhemispheric connections peaks at around 26–29 weeks GA (Jakab et al. 2014). These data suggest that the architectural foundation of local network hubs is established in the second and third trimester at the time of peak expansion of white matter (Jakab et al. 2014). Notably, short‐range structural connectivity plays a key role in how the brain develops and in mediating our neurocognitive function after we are born (Ouyang et al. 2017). In contrast, there is a linear development of long‐range connections throughout gestation (Jakab et al. 2014), and long‐range connectivity is greater at 30 weeks GA, consistent with white matter tract maturation. Magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) studies have also shown that the advancement of grey and white matter in the fetus is inter‐related, and highly dependent on the integrity and complexity of the underlying axonal circuitry within the white matter tracts (Friedrichs‐Maeder et al. 2017). As discussed below, preterm birth disrupts the establishment of the neural connectivity framework, and this impacts on the development and efficiency of sleep.

Fetal sleep

Sleep during fetal life is, by definition, difficult to determine given that the human fetus cannot be directly assessed. Animal studies allow more direct access, as discussed below, but in human studies the fetus is assessed by indirect measures such as ultrasound to evaluate behaviour (e.g. eye, body and breathing movements) and fetal heart rate recordings. In clinical obstetrics, fetal ultrasound studies show that preterm fetal behaviour consists of random movements, but from mid‐gestation defined rest and activity periods start to develop, with increasing prolongation of periods of quiescence (reduced body movements) with advancing gestational age (Ten Hof et al. 2002). These patterns are also seen in preterm infants after birth, and thus are not a consequence of reduced intrauterine space, but rather reflective of maturation of basic neuro‐inhibitory processes (Ten Hof et al. 2002). From at least 32 weeks GA, fetal behaviour can be categorised into four states based on fetal heart rate, and body and eye movements, viz. 1F, quiet sleep (F denotes fetus, and 1F consists of slow regular heart rate, infrequent body movements, mostly startles and no eye movements), 2F active sleep (regular heart rate, eye movements, frequently and periodic gross body movements, mostly stretches), 3F, quiet awake (fast regular heart rate, eye movements, no body movements), and 4F, active awake (fast irregular heart rate with prolonged periods of tachycardia, eye movements, continual body movements) (Nijhuis et al. 1982, 1999). While the terminology of awake is used as part of these definitions, there is limited if any evidence for actual wakefulness in fetal life, as discussed below.

In younger fetuses (<30 weeks GA), specific clusters of behaviour cannot be readily identified, and this is referred to as indeterminate sleep (Nijhuis et al. 1999; Mirmiran et al. 2003). A gradual co‐ordination between behaviours is seen from 25 to 30 weeks GA (Drogtrop et al. 1990; Nijhuis et al. 1999), with clear linkages seen after 32–34 weeks GA (Visser et al. 1987; Nijhuis et al. 1999), and accelerated maturation at 34–36 weeks GA (Nijhuis et al. 1999). With maturation, the fetus spends most time (∼90%) in 1F and 2F, with very little time spent in 3F and 4F (Pillai & James, 1990; Nijhuis et al. 1999).

In late gestation, fetuses switch between 1F and 2F in an ultradian cycle of 70–90 min (Visser et al. 1992), and the stability of concordant behaviours, along with organised short transition phases between states, are used as measures of normal neurodevelopment (Van den Bergh & Mulder, 2012). The maturation of fetal behaviour has been examined by determining the sequence of transitions with gestational age (Arduini et al. 1989; Nijhuis et al. 1999). Fetal heart rate is the first variable to change during transition between 1F to 2F between 28 and 39 weeks, and the last to change during 2F to 1F transitions between 32 and 39 weeks, but this is lost in growth retarded fetuses where there is no maturational order (Arduini et al. 1989; Nijhuis et al. 1999), presumably reflecting immature and/or impaired neural network development. More recently it has been shown in children at 8–9 and 14—15 years of age, independent of other socio‐economic factors, that the duration of their fetal state transitions in late gestation was significantly associated with their degree of self‐regulation or effortful control (defined as more effective emotional control and executive functioning) (Van den Bergh & Mulder, 2012). In particular, quick transitions from 1F to 2F were associated with higher levels of effortful control (Van den Bergh & Mulder, 2012). Thus, the association between quicker, synchronised sleep state switching and better behavioural control in postnatal life suggests better maturation of the brain in these individuals during fetal life. These data reinforce the concept that how sleep evolves before birth is important to postnatal neurocognitive outcomes in later life.

Behavioural patterns are influenced by a number of physiological and pathological factors, including fetal growth (Nijhuis et al. 2000), maternal glucocorticoid administration (Mulder et al. 2009), circadian rhythms (Patrick et al. 1982; Visser et al. 1982; de Vries et al. 1987), maternal alcohol consumption (Mulder et al. 1998), and medical conditions such as diabetes (Mulder et al. 1987). A recent study showed that maternal sleeping position can also alter the behavioural sleep states of late gestation fetuses, as determined by fetal heart rate variability (Stone et al. 2017). This study demonstrated that, compared to 2F, 4F activity was seen almost exclusively when the mother was in a left or right lateral position, while 1F was more common when the mother was supine (Stone et al. 2017). Further, 4F was more common between 21.00 and 01.00 h than between 01.00 and 07.00 h (Stone et al. 2017). These data are consistent with previous circadian studies showing increased fetal activity in the late evening in humans and animals (Dalton et al. 1977; Patrick et al. 1982; Visser et al. 1982; de Vries et al. 1987; Seron‐Ferre et al. 2012). The quality of maternal sleep itself is also important, because pregnant women suffering obstructive sleep apnoea (OSA) show evidence of established placental hypoxia (Ravishankar et al. 2015), and an increased incidence of preterm birth, low birth weight, poor Apgar scores and stillbirth (Pien et al. 2014; Bourjeily et al. 2017). It has been suggested that the incidence of OSA in pregnancy is dramatically under‐reported (Pien et al. 2014), and in industrialised countries OSA during pregnancy is increasing due to the obesity epidemic, particularly in younger women, and is associated with a number of pregnancy complications (Bourjeily et al. 2017).

The neonatal surrogate for fetal sleep

Supporting these in utero studies are observations made in infants born preterm, where direct measurements of electroencephalographic (EEG), electromyographic, and electrooculographic activity are added to behavioural and heart rate measures. However, the authors note the caveat that the in utero environment (including the placenta) plays a key regulatory role in fetal brain activity and behaviour, as discussed below. Fetal neural control is different, not only as a function of maturation, but also because of the in utero environment. For example, while the fetus does not breathe for oxygenation, it does make breathing movements to mature the lungs and respiratory musculature (Walker et al. 2000). In late gestation, fetal breathing movements (FBMs) are episodic in nature, with the apnoeic periods unique to the fetus regulated by nuclei in the lateral pons (Walker et al. 2000). Similarly, there is no respiratory chemoreflex in response to hypoxia, and the fetal response is characterised by apnoea and a switch to 1F, rather than the arousal and an increase in respiratory activity seen postnatally (Walker et al. 2000).

Sleep states after birth are generally defined as quiet sleep (QS, equivalent to 1F) and active sleep (AS, equivalent to 2F), and these states are the precursors to rapid eye movement (REM) and non‐rapid eye movement (NREM) sleep in adults. QS is characterised by high voltage low amplitude EEG activity, the absence of eye movements, higher muscle tone, and regular heart rate and respiration, while AS is characterised by low amplitude high frequency EEG activity, eye movements, reduced muscle tone and irregular heart rate and respiration (Curzi‐Dascalova et al. 1988). However, the distinction between sleep states in very premature infants is acknowledged as being difficult to objectively assess before 30 weeks GA, and prior to this sleep is largely characterised as indeterminate sleep (Curzi‐Dascalova, 2001; Mirmiran et al. 2003). EEG activity is characterised as being discontinuous in nature with bursts of variable amplitudes and frequencies, and quiescent/suppressed periods (interburst intervals) (Vanhatalo & Kaila, 2006; Tolonen et al. 2007; Andre et al. 2010; Omidvarnia et al. 2013; Koolen et al. 2017).

However, some studies suggest that a rudimentary sleep–wake cycle can be seen between 24 and 30 weeks GA. This is based on alternating periods with and without eye movements related to EEG discontinuity (Vecchierini et al. 2003; Scher et al. 2005). With maturity, the percentage of indeterminate sleep falls to <20% at term as AS and QS become established (Curzi‐Dascalova et al. 1988; Mirmiran et al. 2003). The preterm newborn spends proportionally more time in AS up to term equivalent age (Peirano et al. 2003), and AS is important for brain growth and development of complexity of the neural networks (Marks et al. 1995; Mirmiran et al. 2003; Cirelli & Tononi, 2017). QS is harder to determine in preterm infants as chin submental hypotonia is difficult to evaluate (Curzi‐Dascalova et al. 1988). QS becomes clearly identifiable around about 36 weeks GA, consistent with maturation of the thalamocortical and intracortical pathways and increased synaptogenesis (Peirano et al. 2003; Kostovic & Judas, 2010).

The proportion of time spent in QS and wakefulness increases with maturational age from term equivalent age (Holditch‐Davis & Edwards, 1998; Peirano et al. 2003). QS is also important for brain development and some preterm infants have been reported to experience longer sleep cycles and QS periods (Roffwarg et al. 1966; Scher et al. 1992, 2011; Peirano et al. 2003). This may be an adaptive response to the altered physiological and hormonal environment of extra‐uterine life, or accelerated maturation (Scher et al. 1992, 2011; Holditch‐Davis et al. 2004; Graven, 2006). QS may play a role in brain development by promoting synaptic remodelling through reactivation and processing of neural information encoded during wakefulness (Peirano et al. 2003). Quantitative analysis of neonatal EEG during sleep has demonstrated that even late preterm infants (34 0/7 and 36 6/7 weeks GA) have structurally different sleep, with differences observed in several indices such as arousals during QS, spectral beta/alpha power ratios during AS and QS, sleep cycle length, and spectral correlations between homologous centro‐temporal regions during QS (Scher et al. 2011). These changes demonstrate that being born too soon, at any age impacts on the development of the neural network that regulates sleep architecture (Scher et al. 2011). These data are consistent with the finding that moderate to late‐preterm and near‐term infants also have a higher risk of neurodevelopmental disability compared to their full‐term counterparts (Ortinau & Neil, 2015; Brumbaugh et al. 2016)

A key feature of the immature EEG that supports development of the neural network neural network/sleep‐like activity, is the occurrence of spontaneous activity transient (SAT) activity, which derives from brainstem or deep brain structures, and which occurs when long neural pathways are actively developing as discussed above (Ben‐Ari, 2001; Vanhatalo & Kaila, 2006; Omidvarnia et al. 2013). These intermittent events appear important for guiding activity‐dependent structural development of the neural network (Vanhatalo & Kaila, 2006; Omidvarnia et al. 2013). In preterm infants, total brain volume, and in particular subcortical grey matter volume, grew faster in infants with more SAT activity and with greater cortical activity (Benders et al. 2015). Dysregulation of preterm SAT activity leads to impaired cortico‐thalamic connectivity (Catalano & Shatz, 1998). The general fall in EEG amplitude with maturation in part reflects a reduction in SAT activity (Vanhatalo & Kaila, 2006), which may be mediated by a gradual shift from an excitatory role for the neurotransmitter gamma‐aminobutyric acid (GABA), when chloride efflux promotes depolarisation (Ben‐Ari, 2001), to the inhibitory, hyperpolarising activity of GABA seen in late gestation and after birth (Ben‐Ari, 2001; Vanhatalo et al. 2005).

Sleep ontogeny in pre‐clinical animal studies

The chronically instrumented fetal sheep is the primary experimental model for studying fetal physiology and behaviour, where the fetus can be studied growing and developing in utero over long periods without the confounding effects of anaesthesia (van den Heuij et al. 2016). The authors note the caveat that the precocial development of the fetal sheep brain is such that the brain may be considered equivalent to the human brain at around 125–130 days GA (term in the sheep is 145–150 days) (Barlow, 1969; McIntosh et al. 1979), and thus studies after this time reflect a post‐mature brain.

In preterm fetal sheep at 0.6–0.7 gestation (85–105 days sheep gestation, which is equivalent to human brain development around 25–32 weeks GA), electrocorticogram (ECOG) activity is discontinuous and comprises mixed amplitude and frequency, interburst intervals, and high amplitude transients (Clewlow et al. 1983; Bennet et al. 1999; Mellor et al. 2005; Davidson et al. 2011). Fetal behaviour (FBMs, eye and body movements) is not closely related to brain ECOG activity, but is rather continuous and appears to be coincidental (Clewlow et al. 1983; Mellor et al. 2005). The regular alternation between REM and NREM sleep‐state like cycling starts to emerge in ECOG recordings after 110–115 days, with clear evidence by 120 days (Dawes et al. 1972; Ruckebusch et al. 1977; Clewlow et al. 1983; Szeto & Hinman, 1985; Walker et al. 1990; Richardson et al. 1994; Keen et al. 2011). There is a clear diurnal rhythm in some fetal behaviours and endocrinology (Dalton et al. 1977; Zemdegs et al. 1988; McMillen et al. 1990; Houghton et al. 1993), with maternal entrainment of the fetal circadian clocks evident from many animal studies (for review, see Seron‐Ferre et al. 2012).

As fetal sleep state cycles develop, fetal behaviour becomes episodic in nature. REM‐like brain activity is associated with rapid eye movements and relative body muscle atonia; characteristic features of REM sleep after birth. Associated with fetal REM sleep is the periodic presence of FBMs and other behaviours such as licking and swallowing, but reduced or absent body movements (Dawes et al. 1972; Walker et al. 1990; Richardson et al. 1994), and blood pressure, cerebral blood flow and metabolism increase, while heart rate is lower (Clapp et al. 1980; Richardson et al. 1994; Akay & Szeto, 1995; Czikk et al. 2001). In contrast, NREM sleep‐like activity is associated with a reduction or cessation in FBMs and the presence of body movements (Dawes et al. 1972; Walker et al. 1990; Richardson et al. 1994), and blood pressure, cerebral blood flow and metabolism decrease, while heart rate is higher (Richardson et al. 1994; Akay & Szeto, 1995; Czikk et al. 2001). This compartmentalisation of behaviour may reflect the development of the cortico‐spinal pathways that are fundamentally involved in the regulation of sleep (Walker et al. 2000), and may have the important function of managing fetal energy expenditure to balance the demands of growth with the necessity of preparing the body (e.g. lungs, respiratory musculature) for the demands of postnatal life. Temporary paralysis of fetal lambs with anaesthesia or a neuromuscular blocking agent results in a fall of total fetal oxygen consumption of 15–20% (Rurak & Gruber, 1983; Rurak, 2017), indicating the energy expense of fetal activity, and suggesting that fetal sleep is a mechanism to reduce this ‘non‐purposeful’ use of energy. On the other hand, an abnormal reduction in FBMs is associated with lung hypoplasia (Harding & Hooper, 1996).

In the maturing sheep fetus, REM state predominates initially, occurring 40–60% of the time, but this percentage gradually falls with maturation, as is seen in the human fetus and neonate (Akay & Szeto, 1995; Rao et al. 2009; Keen et al. 2011). The duration of NREM is positively correlated with the duration of the previous REM phase (Rao et al. 2009), and the authors of this study suggested that this positive REM/NREM linkage supports the homeostatic model for sleep (Rao et al. 2009). Both REM and NREM support brain growth, as is observed postnatally (Cirelli & Tononi, 2017). Evidence from fetal sheep, for example, demonstrates that there is protein turnover and degradation during NREM (Czikk et al. 2003), suggesting that NREM activity may support new protein synthesis as part of supporting brain maturation. In addition to REM and NREM activity, a third behavioural state is observed, the incidence of which increases with maturation. This state consists of mixed ECOG frequency, with coincidental expression of FBMs, eye and body movements, and variable blood pressure and heart rate responses. These periods of fetal arousal are similar to newborn wakefulness, and thus it has been postulated that this state represents fetal wakefulness (for review, see Mellor et al. 2005). However, in reality, is the fetus ever truly awake?

Fetal sleep – if I sleep, do I wake? How different is in utero life?

Philosophically it is interesting to consider what benefits the fetus would gain in waking in such an enclosed environment; deprived as it is of the capacity to interact with its environment and care‐givers. Perhaps fleeting moments of drowsy wakefulness towards term are useful for practising for the moment of birth where being awake is part of our daily life cycles, just as FBMs prepare the fetus for the mechanics of breathing. Certainly it is easy to be convinced that the fetus must be awake when looking at a three dimensional ultrasound picture of a fetus displaying complex behaviours, some of which look like wakefulness in infants (e.g. eyes open and sucking thumbs). Further, in late‐gestation we know that the fetus can process auditory stimuli (i.e. the fetus can hear; Walker et al. 1971; Hepper, 1997), and data show that the fetus and newborn are physiologically responsive to the sound of their mother's voice (e.g. heart rate and respiration change when they hear her but not others; Hepper, 1997). This suggests that fetuses can form memories, which are retained by the neonate, albeit briefly as this recognition pattern is lost shortly after birth. Is this evidence of conscious awareness? Further, numerous studies show that the fetus physically responds to noise (such as vibroacoustic stimulation) and to touch (such as inadvertent contact with a needle during obstetric sampling procedures) with jerking, limb withdrawals and increased heart rate (Mellor et al. 2005), just as we would act if startled. Such stimuli generally cause us to wake up and if we wake then it may be reasoned that these fetal responses provide evidence that the fetus also wakes up. Supporting this perception is the widespread use in clinical and pre‐clinical studies of the terminologies of awake or wakefulness when describing fetal behaviour, as discussed in the previous sections.

It is critical, however, to put these potentially perceptual biases into a physiological framework of understanding. The clinical descriptions of fetal behaviour are derived from clusters of behaviour such as heart rate, eye and body movements observed in infants during sleep and wake periods (Nijhuis et al. 1982; Visser et al. 1987), not evidence of a fetal awake state or activation of the neurophysiological processes which ensure transition to an awake state (Mellor et al. 2005). Similarly, in fetal sheep studies, reference is also made to these clinical definitions. For example, nuchal muscle tone is used as a surrogate measure of wakefulness (high tone) and sleep (low tone), based on neonatal observations (Karlsson et al. 2011). Fetuses can indeed process auditory stimuli, but we know that wakefulness or consciousness is not necessary to entrain memory, as sleep is important for memory processing and we can create memories while asleep (e.g. dreams remembered when we wake). Further, surprisingly complex behaviours can be displayed when fully asleep, including opening of eyes and sucking of thumbs; witness the behaviour of adults and children who sleep and those who sleepwalk. Finally, fetal arousal to noise or touch could be considered as evidence of brainstem and subcortical reflex actions rather than prima facie evidence of activation of the cortical arousal to wakefulness.

Importantly, it should be appreciated that the fetus lives in a very different environment to that experienced after birth, an environment which includes the placenta (Mellor et al. 2005). Evidence strongly suggests that the fetal brain is actively inhibited from initiating cortical arousal to an awake state, even in late gestation, through significant release of inhibitory neurotransmitters and modulators from the placenta as well as the fetal brain itself, such as steroids, adenosine and prostaglandin D2 (Lee et al. 2002; Hunter et al. 2003; Nguyen et al. 2004). These inhibitors are further increased during hypoxia and other noxious stimuli such as hypercapnia and pain that would cause arousal to wakefulness after birth. In the fetus these stimuli induce transition to 1F or NREM activity, or even greater suppression of brain activity along with cessation of FBMs and body movements in term and preterm fetuses (Walker et al. 2000; Mellor et al. 2005; Bennet, 2017).

Further proof that the fetus may not be truly awake in utero comes from an intriguing study in fetal sheep by Rigatto and colleagues that allowed investigators to physically observe the developing fetus during different behavioural states (Rigatto et al. 1986). Despite many thousands of hours of observations, during all types of fetal behavioural states, the researchers did not find any evidence of wakefulness in terms of conscious awareness and interaction with the environment or the observers; the hallmarks of the awake state (Rigatto et al. 1986). Collectively, the evidence suggests that periods of fetal arousal represent another form of sleep, albeit brief, similar to subcortical sleep arousal in infants which promotes (IPWGA, 2005) cardiorespiratory and neural development, and which is mediated by subcortical and brainstem activity, not arousal to consciousness (McNamara et al. 2002).

This section provided a general overview of the ontogeny of sleep and how sleep reflects the maturing neural network. It emphasises the importance of the preterm period as a critical phase in the development of sleep state cycling. In the following section we examine the known impact of preterm birth on the ontogeny of sleep from infant to adult life. We then discuss the perinatal factors which may mediate altered sleep development.

The neural network and preterm birth

Sleep is vital to the development and function of an optimal neural network, and impaired sleep may disturb myelination of the maturing brain (Kurth et al. 2016). Individuals born preterm are at a significant risk for impaired neurodevelopment, this includes moderate to late‐preterm and near‐term infants who also have a higher risk of neurodevelopmental disability compared to their full‐term counterparts (Back, 2015; Brumbaugh et al. 2016; Allotey et al. 2018). Underpinning impaired neurodevelopment is an altered cerebral architecture characterised by reduced grey and white matter volumes, diffuse non‐cystic white matter loss, and reduced cortical folding and gyral complexity (Back, 2017). Reduced brain growth is associated with impaired cortical arborisation and synapse formation, synaptic pruning, and white matter dysmaturation, leading to compromised neuronal integrity and functional capacity (Back, 2017). In turn this leads to an altered development of the neural network and connectivity, characterised by fewer connections and disruption to local and short‐path connections and cortical to subcortical relays, i.e. to less complexity of the neural network and an altered connectome (Hagmann et al. 2010; Ball et al. 2015; Meng et al. 2016; Scheinost et al. 2016). Evidence suggests that the rich‐club hub network, which is established before birth as discussed earlier, is maintained at the expense of peripheral connections after preterm birth (Ball et al. 2014; Fischi‐Gomez et al. 2016).

A less complex, less well connected neural network, for example, reduces the efficiency of information processing, requiring more connections to achieve the same response, often with a loss of those vital short‐term and local connections we discussed earlier (Hagmann et al. 2010; Rathbone et al. 2011; Thompson et al. 2016; Cao et al. 2017). Reduced brain volumes and the aberrant intrinsic network connectivity seen in neonates persists into adult life (Meng et al. 2016), and is associated with adult neurocognitive dysfunction (Meng et al. 2016). Thomason and colleagues showed that impaired functional connectivity can also be observed in fetuses who go onto be born preterm (Thomason et al. 2017), and this is associated with both acute and chronic chorioamnionitis and funisitis, demonstrating a strong association between perinatal inflammation and impaired neurodevelopment, as discussed below (Thomason et al. 2017). These researchers also demonstrated disturbed brain connectivity, complexity and network structures in 7‐year‐olds who were born <30 weeks GA, and observed a strong correlation between perinatal infection and a lower density of white matter connections, and that bronchopulmonary dysplasia was associated with reduced connection strength within some sub‐networks (Thompson et al. 2016). We discuss the impact of such respiratory conditions on sleep further below.

The causes of impaired neurodevelopment and functional connectivity are multifactorial and for younger preterm infants are seldom isolated events, but rather the preterm brain can be subjected to multiple insults, combined and intermittently, and that combination insults can both sensitise the brain to greater injury or create tolerance to subsequent insults (Dammann & Leviton, 2014; Hagberg et al. 2015; Back, 2017). Challenges include exposure of both the fetus and newborn to acute and chronic infection and inflammation and perinatal hypoxia–ischaemia (HI), the adaptation of immature organs to the physiological demands of the ex utero environment and all the clinical interventions (e.g. cardio‐respiratory support, as well as pain and seizure medication) necessary to ensure survival (Bennet et al. 2013; Back, 2017; Yawno et al. 2017).

Effects of preterm birth on the development of sleep after term equivalent age

Sleep disruptions can occur from the first hours of life. The quality of sleep can be impaired by clinical treatments including the light and noise of neonatal intensive care units (NICU) (van den Hoogen et al. 2017) and respiratory support (Collins et al. 2015). Further, the development of sleep may be compromised by the many morbidities experienced by preterm infants.

The maturation of sleep is one of the most important physiological processes occurring during the first year of life and is particularly rapid during the first 6 months after birth, with term‐born newborns spending 70% of each 24 h asleep and preterm‐born newborns spending approximately 90% of their time asleep (Gaultier, 1995). As with the fetus, the natural emergence of coherent sleep states in the newborn is dependent on the maturation of the central nervous system and is a reliable indicator of the normal development of the brain (Curzi‐Dascalova, 2000). Indeed, the quality of sleep between 27 and 28 weeks GA appears to be an important determinant of functional brain development (Holditch‐Davis & Edwards, 1998).

Thus it is surprising how little research there is on the impact of preterm birth on the development of sleep. Such assessment is rarely included in the assessment of preterm newborns (Shellhaas et al. 2017). Polysomnography is the gold standard method for sleep analysis, and access polysomnography equipment or the degree of instrumentation required may be rate‐limiting steps to uptake in the neonatal intensive care unit. In this regard, we have recently demonstrated the utility for 2 lead aEEG recordings, which are increasingly used in neonatal intensive care units, for the analysis of sleep activity (Bennet et al. 2016). Hoppenbrouwers and colleagues reported that beyond the neonatal period preterm birth does not seem to affect the maturation of sleep states and sleep architecture, with the percentage of time spent in QS, AS, indeterminate sleep or wake being similar to that in age‐matched term infants between 38 and 55 weeks post‐conceptional age (Hoppenbrouwers et al. 2005). No sleep duration, night‐waking or sleep‐onset difficulties were observed in children from birth to 10 years of age, assessed by parental report, who were born at a median age of 34.1 weeks GA (range 27.1–36.8 weeks GA) compared to term born controls (Iglowstein et al. 2006). Similarly, no differences were found in night waking, sleep onset or co‐sleeping between preterm infants between 32 and 36 weeks GA and term infants at 20 months and 56 months of age, assessed by structure interview with parents (Wolke et al. 1998).

However, recent studies suggest that, for some infants, preterm birth affects sleep structure and efficiency, as discussed in detail below (Asaka & Takada, 2010; Perkinson‐Gloor et al. 2015; Shellhaas et al. 2017; Stangenes et al. 2017; Yiallourou et al. 2017). In some studies, sleep is associated with apparent neural injury, although the authors note that neural assessment for injury is not routinely assessed in sleep studies in infants and children. Sleep disturbances in children with cerebral palsy (CP) are well known and include difficulties initiating sleep and morning awakening, repeated night waking, nightmares, insomnia, and sleep anxiety (Lelis et al. 2016). However, it must be appreciated that sleep disturbances can occur in the absence of injury. As discussed previously, impaired brain maturation associated with prematurity can occur independently of injury, and this is associated with impaired neurodevelopment and functional connectivity of the brain (Back, 2017).

In 11‐year‐old children born very preterm (<28 weeks completed GA, or weighing <1000 g), who had neurodevelopmental disabilities (assessed at 5 years of age), there was a higher percentage of sleep problems compared to term‐matched control counterparts (Stangenes et al. 2017). The preterm‐born infants went to bed earlier, had longer sleep‐onset latency, increased night waking, and they slept longer. Sleep differences increased as a function of neurodevelopmental disabilities severity. Notably, however, preterm infants with no reported neurodevelopmental disabilities also had sleep difficulties, which were similar to those reported for their preterm counterparts with neurodevelopmental disabilities (Stangenes et al. 2017). Longer nocturnal sleep duration, more night awakenings, and longer daytime sleep duration was also observed in 6‐month‐old infants born at an older age (31.5 ± 3.2 weeks GA, 1467 ± 588.6 g, mean±SD) compared to term control infants (Huang et al. 2014). The authors of this study did not evaluate neurodevelopmental disabilities, but did note that sleep disturbances were associated with respiratory problems (Huang et al. 2014).

We have shown that sleep is disturbed in infants who do not have neurodevelopmental disabilities. We demonstrated that both prematurity and growth restriction (see below for further discussion on growth) are associated with altered micro‐ and macro‐sleep architecture changes indicative of reduced sleep quantity and quality in childhood (Yiallourou et al. 2017). Children were studied with overnight polysomnography and the children born preterm had less total sleep time (by 45 min), more wake after sleep onset and lower sleep efficiency, all indicating reduced and more fragmented sleep patterns. Furthermore, the preterm–born children had reduced amounts of NREM sleep (by 40–48 min), in particular N2 stage sleep was significantly shorter compared to children born at term and also those born preterm with fetal growth restriction and a higher proportion of N3 stage sleep. Consistent with these macro‐architecture findings, reduced delta and sigma EEG power were recorded, indicting less deep sleep and fewer sleep spindles. Shorter sleep duration and a higher proportion of N3 stage sleep, may reflect compensatory sleep recovery to repay sleep debt that occurs due to poor quality sleep or excessive daytime sleepiness (Yiallourou et al. 2017).

In contrast to studies reporting longer sleep durations, several studies report shorter day and night time sleep durations. At 1 year after preterm birth, very low birthweight infants (27.4 ± 2.2 weeks GA, 937 ± 202 g, mean±SD), with no evident neurological injury, slept less during the night compared to full‐term infants and had a greater number of night wakings compared to term infants (Asaka & Takada, 2010). In a subsequent study at 1 and 2 years of age that included older preterm infants (28.6 ± 4.0 weeks, 1042 ± 420, mean±SD) the authors only observed a reduction in total duration of sleep (Asaka & Takada, 2013), whether this reflects a greater degree of brain maturation is unclear. In a separate study, prematurely born children (31.1 ± 2.4 weeks GA, 1600.8 ± 501.2 g, mean±SD) studied at 2 years of age were shown to get to sleep more quickly and were earlier to rise, but had increased incidences of night waking and restlessness and breathing problems associated with poor attention, greater negative emotions, and hyperactivity (Caravale et al. 2017). In a similar cohort of children, a reduction in sleep duration and quality of sleep was associated with an increased risk of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (Gossel‐Symank et al. 2004).

In a combined Australian and Canadian study of older children (5–12 years of age) born preterm (∼27 weeks GA, 942–998 g), sleep measured by actigraphy and by parental assessment showed that these children also had a shorter sleep duration, sleeping on average around 8 h (Biggs et al. 2016), with earlier bed and wake times seen in the Australian children. This is in contrast to the recommended sleep durations of 10–13 h for 5‐year‐olds, 9–11 h for 6–13‐year‐olds (Biggs et al. 2016). Other studies have shown that preterm birth (29.7 ± 1.9 weeks GA, 1302 ± 408.7 g), is associated reduced sleep efficiency with more nocturnal awakenings, more stage 2 sleep, and less NREM sleep (less restorative sleep) in children between 6 and 10 years of age when compared to term‐born children (Perkinson‐Gloor et al. 2015).

In this study, sleep dysfunction was associated with behavioural and emotional problems and lower morning and evening cortisol. An associated study on this cohort of children demonstrated that sleep dysfunction related to impaired cognitive function (Hagmann‐von Arx et al. 2014). Decreased verbal working memory performance was observed in children 6–9 years of age born preterm (median 28 (range 23–32) weeks GA, 915 (range 470–2360) g (McCann et al. 2018). This did not correlate with day time sleepiness, but rather the cognitive restraints of slower processing speed and reduced short‐term memory storage capacity. The quality of night‐time sleep was also associated with reduced executive function in preterm, but not term children who slept poorly (McCann et al. 2018).

Inefficient or shorter sleep is also seen in unwell near‐term infants. In term infants (>35 weeks GA) assessed at 18 months of age, who had evidence of hypoxic–ischaemic encephalopathy at birth, sleep was characterised by increased QS with lower EEG delta power (Shellhaas et al. 2017). This study also demonstrated that children born preterm had attenuated changes in cerebral fractional tissue oxygen extraction between QS and wakefulness, as measured by near‐infrared spectroscopy (Shellhaas et al. 2017). The study demonstrated that attenuated differences in fractional tissue oxygen extraction between wake and QS predicted worse cognitive, language, and motor scores at 18 months of age (Shellhaas et al. 2017).

It was notable that many studies (from infants to adults) report that individuals born preterm, have phase shifts in their sleep with significantly earlier bed and wake times (Bjorkqvist et al. 2014; Hibbs et al. 2014; Schwichtenberg et al. 2016; Stangenes et al. 2017). These studies suggest that chronotype (i.e. early vs. late rising) may be programmed early in life. Further, data suggest that preterm infants adopt their circadian 24 h cycle earlier than term counterparts (Guyer et al. 2015). These observations in young children are important, as they support the concept that chronotype, which typically manifests in adolescence, has its origins much earlier in life (Kuula et al. 2017). In the longer term, chronotype resetting to become an ‘early bird’ may be beneficial, as it is associated with better health outcomes, but this can be offset by circadian disruption, for example, shifting sleeping patterns, e.g. normally early risers staying up later, which often occurs in adolescence (Vetter et al. 2015).

It should be noted, however, that many behavioural factors can impact on how sleep develops, including the role of parents (Biggs et al. 2010). Parents of preterm babies can be stressed by the many health challenges their infants face and are often sleep‐deprived themselves (Blomqvist et al. 2017). Improved parental sleep and nutrition can improve sleep in their children (Nordheim et al. 2016), and their perception of the sleeping patterns of their infants (Blomqvist et al. 2017). Inadequate or inappropriate sleep training in infancy and childhood can increase sleep latency and prolonged night waking (sleep‐onset association disorder) in children and adolescents (Biggs et al. 2016; Owens et al. 2016). Sleep‐onset association disorder can occur, for example, if parents fail to be consistent with bed‐times and if children depend on parental attention to get to sleep, leading to a failure to learn to ‘self‐soothe’ and the requirement of parental attention to return to sleep (Biggs et al. 2016; Owens et al. 2016). Additionally, sleep impairment is also associated with parent–infant attachments, sleep location, socioeconomic status and cultural factors such as co‐sleeping (for review, see Schwichtenberg & Goodlin‐Jones, 2010), and with sleep position (as discussed further below in the section on postnatal hypoxia and cardiovascular instability).

In summary, for many infants being born too soon has significant effects on brain development, which is reflected in the development of sleep and in turn associated with neurocognition and behavioural difficulties. These differences persist throughout childhood and there is some evidence that they are still evident in adulthood. Why some preterm children go on to experience relatively normal sleep, while others have impaired sleep is unclear at this time. However, as discussed below, the development of sleep may be compromised by the many morbidities experienced by preterm infants.

Mediators of altered brain and sleep development

Fetal hypoxia

Many infants born preterm are at risk of exposure to general hypoxia, cerebral ischaemia or even both during the perinatal period. The incidence of hypoxic–ischaemic encephalopathy is significantly higher in preterm birth than in full‐term infants: 5–9/1000 live births versus 1–3/1000 live births at term (Salhab & Perlman, 2005; Logitharajah et al. 2009; Chalak et al. 2012; Gopagondanahalli et al. 2016). In term infants, the acute adaptation (3 days post insult) to a mild‐to‐moderate HI insult is an increased percentage of QS and IS at the expense of AS (Scher et al. 2002).

Complicating our understanding about the role of HI insults in mediating neural injury or impaired neurodevelopment is the fact that, for many infants, significant hypoxia may occur well before birth. Such antenatal insults may contribute to many cases of CP that have their origins in the antenatal not the intrapartum period (Tan, 2014; see also Ellery et al. 2018, in this issue). Further, it is important to appreciate that the preterm fetuses of many species, including humans, have cardiac glycogen stores which peak in preterm life, significantly increasing the anaerobic tolerance to HI in terms of survival (for review, see Bennet, 2017). Thus many infants may experience antenatal HI well before birth but survive with injury or impaired brain development to go on to be born at term without further complications. Importantly, current evidence suggests that mild HI may cause greater injury to the immature brain than it does to the term brain. For example, a mild HI insult in postnatal (P) 3 neonatal rats (24–28 weeks GA human brain maturation equivalent) causes compromised cortical growth and selective alteration of cortical myelinated axons with persistent gliosis (Sizonenko et al. 2003), and at P7 (30—34 weeks GA human brain equivalent) mild HI leads to delayed cerebral atrophy and infarction many weeks after the insult (Geddes et al. 2001). In neonatal mice, intermittent but mild non‐ischaemic hypoxia caused evolving non‐cystic white matter injury, hypomyelination and sensorimotor deficits (Juliano et al. 2015).

In preterm fetal sheep at 0.65–0.7 gestation (28–30 weeks human brain maturation equivalent), transient hypoxia can significantly disrupt maturation of the fetal sub‐plate neuron arborisation and activity, and the degree of compromise relates to the level of hypoxia (McClendon et al. 2017). Hypoxia and associated neuroinflammation may contribute to impaired brain and neural network development by impairing the maturation of white matter cells during preterm life (Back, 2017; Bennet et al. 2017). Consistent with this, acute lesions are not seen in many infants who develop CP, but there is delayed loss of white matter and hypo‐myelination (Woodward et al. 2006). Currently we lack reliable biomarkers (Bennet et al. 2010) to determine whether an insult has occurred before birth, and the nature of that insult, and thus the relative contribution of antenatal events in mediating impaired neurodevelopment.

Intrauterine growth restriction and chronic hypoxia

Intrauterine growth restriction (IUGR) is characterised by varying degrees of hypoxia and malnutrition, which can occur at any stage in pregnancy, and is a common precursor of preterm birth (Korzeniewski et al. 2017). IUGR is associated with poor brain growth (Korzeniewski et al. 2017) and a recent systematic review demonstrates that IUGR increases the risk of adverse neurodevelopmental outcomes and that this risk increased in children who had preferential redistribution of blood flow to central organs such as the brain (Murray et al. 2015). IUGR also impacts on the organisation of sleep states in both infants (Cohen et al. 2018) and children (Yiallourou et al. 2017). It is difficult to separate the contribution of preterm birth from the effects of impaired growth in this regard, but it is clear that IUGR fetuses have decreased amounts of AS and increased amounts of QS and indeterminate sleep compared to appropriately grown fetuses (Gazzolo et al. 1995). This may be an adaptive response by potentially reducing cerebral metabolic demand (Richardson & Bocking, 1998). In a growth restriction fetal sheep model, chronically hypoxic fetuses had a 30% reduction in REM ECOG activity (reflecting AS sleep) compared to age‐matched and appropriately grown fetuses (Keen et al. 2011).

A decrease in the time spent in AS could potentially have a significant effect on brain maturation, as AS may play a key role in stimulating central nervous system development in the fetus and the neonate (Cirelli & Tononi, 2017). For instance, AS provides endogenous stimulation to sensory processing areas in the central nervous system via fetal movements, breathing movements, sucking, swallowing, yawns, stretches, and eye movements which all characteristically occur in AS (Peirano et al. 2003). A rat model of IUGR produced by maternal under‐nutrition also found that the offspring, studied at P90–100 days, spent 20% more time in slow‐wave sleep (equivalent to N3 in humans) and 61% less time in REM sleep compared to control rats (Datta et al. 2000).

The incidence and duration of sleep state transitions was also altered in chronically hypoxic fetuses (Arduini et al. 1989; Keen et al. 2011). Notably, transition between sleep states takes longer in human IUGR fetuses (Arduini et al. 1989). Sleep–wake transitions in preterm infants born small for gestational age (SGA) at ∼31 weeks GA, have some predictive power for neural development (Weisman et al. 2011). For example, those infants whose sleep shifted between QS and wakefulness had better neural development and behavioural and cognitive outcomes at 5 years of age, compared to those who cycled between AS and high arousal or between short episodes of AS and QS (Weisman et al. 2011).

Only a limited number of studies have investigated the long‐term effects of preterm birth and IUGR on sleep in humans. In infants born IUGR, the development of sleep is altered during both the neonatal and post‐neonatal periods. Preterm infants born with IUGR have higher amounts of AS compared with age matched preterm infants with appropriate birth weights (Hoppenbrouwers et al. 2005). A study of 26 IUGR children and 47 control children aged between 4 and 7 years, using actigraphy to determine sleep–wake patterns, showed that the IUGR children have poorer sleep, defined as lower sleep efficiency, shorter sleep and more awakenings (Leitner et al. 2002). In polysomnographic studies, IUGR preterm‐born infants demonstrated altered EEG power spectrum maturation compared to age‐matched appropriately grown preterm and term infants, with the IUGR preterm infants having less delta power and increased theta, alpha and beta power, reflecting less deep sleep at 1 month term corrected age (Cohen et al. 2018). Recent studies in children (aged 7–12 years), which separated the effects of IUGR and preterm birth, found that preterm birth with appropriate‐for‐gestational‐age birth weight had reduced sleep quantity and quality, whereas children born preterm with IUGR had altered sleep micro‐architecture as assessed by spectral analysis of EEG patterns, potentially reflecting sleep disruption (Yiallourou et al. 2017).

Several recent studies have evaluated the development of neural networks in children (1, 6 and 10 years) born preterm (28–35 weeks GA) with either normal birthweight or IUGR (Fischi‐Gomez et al. 2016; Munoz‐Moreno et al. 2016). These studies have shown that children from both preterm groups significantly differ in terms of neural network organisation (as discussed above) to children born at term, but both had a similar brain trajectory, in terms of connectivity, to each other. However, IUGR was associated with a greater reduction in structure and organisation of neural connections between brain regions, and lower neural myelination (Munoz‐Moreno et al. 2016). This was associated with worse neurocognitive and behavioural outcomes compared to preterm birth alone (Fischi‐Gomez et al. 2016; Munoz‐Moreno et al. 2016).

The loss of the placenta and neurotrophic support

As discussed in the section on fetal sleep and wakefulness, the placenta plays a key role in modulating fetal behaviour, in particular by providing steroids that are further metabolised in the fetal brain to suppress activity and induce sleep (Crossley et al. 1997; Mellor et al. 2005; Brunton et al. 2014). By 7 weeks GA, the human placenta becomes the primary source of steroids during pregnancy, with progesterone‐dependent immunomodulation important for maintenance of pregnancy (Brunton et al. 2014). Placental steroids and those synthesised in the fetal brain are essential for brain development, promoting white matter development and myelination (Brunton et al. 2014), and in guinea‐pig studies, inhibition of neurosteroids in late gestation is associated with behavioural abnormalities in juveniles after birth (Brunton et al. 2014). Placental steroids play a key role in modulating fetal sleep‐related behaviour, both in normoxia (Nicol et al. 1997; Yawno et al. 2011) and hypoxia (Yawno et al. 2011). Neurosteroids are neuroprotective in a variety of species (Brunton et al. 2014). For example, in fetal sheep, suppression of the metabolite allopregnanolone, which is synthesised from progesterone, after an HI insult increased brain injury and seizures (Yawno et al. 2009). Allopregnanolone increases in the fetal sheep brain during the early phase of recovery from an HI insult which may facilitate endogenous neuroprotection by reducing neural excitation (Nguyen et al. 2004; Yawno et al. 2009).

Importantly, the expression of the key enzymes for allopregnanolone synthesis 5 α‐reductase type 1 and 2, which transforms progesterone into 5α‐dihydroprogesterone, increase in the human placenta during the third trimester, suggesting that placental neurosteroid production rises towards term (Brunton et al. 2014). This may, in part, help facilitate the gradual switch from an excitatory role for GABA, which promotes development of the neural network (Ben‐Ari, 2001), to an inhibitory one, as allopregnanolone has greater affinity for GABAA receptors containing subunits which permit tonic inhibition of excitation (Brunton et al. 2014). Supporting this supposition, a magnetic resonance spectroscopy and fMRI study in human preterm and term infants reports regional neural changes in GABA concentrations after preterm birth that correlated to resting state connectivity and which are consistent with delayed maturation of GABA (Kwon et al. 2014).

Thus, preterm birth is associated with a sudden loss of the supply of progesterone needed for allopregnanolone synthesis, which cannot be compensated for after preterm birth in contrast to term birth (Shaw et al. 2015). 5 α‐reductase type 2 expression is reduced in the brain of growth‐restricted fetuses, suggesting that IUGR further limits the capacity for neurosteroid synthesis in the brain following birth (Kelleher et al. 2011; for recent review, see Tolcos et al. 2017). This potentially leaves the brain vulnerable to injury and impaired neurodevelopment, with consequences for the development of the neural network. The role of in situ neurosteroid synthesis in the neonatal brain in determining sleep after preterm birth, with or without growth restriction, deserves further attention.

Postnatal hypoxia and cardiovascular instability

While it is difficult to determine whether mild or intermittent hypoxia has occurred antenatally, it can be clearly observed postnatally. The mild hypoxia seen with conditions such as the apnoea of prematurity and periodic breathing in children born preterm is clearly associated with impaired neurodevelopment (Schmidt et al. 2017). This has been confirmed in a recent study in neonatal rats, where intermittent mild hypoxia between P2 and P12 (P12, approximate brain maturation equivalent to neonatal human) caused reduced deposition of myelin and elevated cytokines between P13 and P22 (P22, approximate brain maturation equivalent to an infant around 2 years of age) (Darnall et al. 2017). Some preterm infants are at risk of significant cerebral oxygen desaturations as measured by near‐infrared spectroscopy, particularly during AS (Decima et al. 2015). A longitudinal study of ex‐preterm infants showed that short apnoeas and periodic breathing persists, and that falls in cerebral oxygenation were more marked at 2–3 months and 5–6 months than at 2–4 weeks of age (Horne et al. 2017a). These data suggest that the preterm infant remains at risk of repeated exposure to mild hypoxic events, which may contribute to adverse neurocognitive outcomes (Horne et al. 2017a).

Adults with OSA have impaired sleep (D'Rozario et al. 2017), and this is associated with altered functional connectivity and impaired neural network organisation (Park et al. 2016). Sleep patterns are less disrupted by OSA in children than in adults (Yang et al. 2010). However, OSA is nevertheless associated with neurocognitive deficits and behavioural difficulties in both pre‐school (Jackman et al. 2012) and school‐aged children (Bourke et al. 2011a, b ). Although neuroimaging studies in children with OSA are scarce, changes in brain metabolites, systemic inflammatory responses and reductions in grey matter have been reported (Halbower et al. 2006; Gozal et al. 2007; Philby et al. 2017). fMRI studies suggest that children with OSA had greater neuronal recruitment in brain regions associated with cognitive control, conflict monitoring, and attentional allocation (Kheirandish‐Gozal et al. 2014). Children with severe OSA, who exhibited impaired attention and visual‐fine motor coordination, showed reduced regional grey matter volumes (Chan et al. 2014; Philby et al. 2017). Diffusion tensor imaging studies have identified that both chronic and acute changes occur, indicating that both short‐term and long‐lasting processes are operating in children with OSA, probably resulting from a combination of ischaemic and hypoxic mechanisms associated with the syndrome (Horne et al. 2017b). Preterm‐born children are 3–5 times more likely to be diagnosed with OSA than those children born at term (Rosen et al. 2003). However, it is yet to be elucidated if the repetitive hypoxia and sleep disruption associated with persistent apnoea and OSA in children born preterm is associated with behavioural and neurocognitive deficits that are prevalent in this population.

In addition to respiratory complications, infants born preterm are at risk of cardiovascular instability (Bennet et al. 2012), which during infancy is most marked during sleep (Gaultier, 1995). Altered haemodynamic control has implications for brain oxygenation and metabolism, with consequent effects on brain maturation. Preterm infants have impaired autonomic control compared with term infants studied at or before term equivalent age (Reulecke et al. 2012; Thiriez et al. 2015; Yiallourou et al. 2017), which can be sustained in nature (Patural et al. 2008). Longitudinal studies after term equivalent age have identified that preterm infants exhibited lower blood pressure, delayed blood pressure recovery following head‐up tilting and impaired baroreflex control of blood pressure and heart rate across the first 6 months corrected age, when compared with age‐matched term infants (Witcombe et al. 2012; Yiallourou et al. 2013; Fyfe et al. 2015a, b ). Further, preterm infants (<32 weeks GA) have reduced baroreflex control of blood pressure compared to term infants (Fyfe et al. 2015c).

Sleep position has a significant impact on sleep in infants. In term infants, sleeping on the tummy or in the prone position decreases arousability from both AS and QS at both 2–3 weeks and 2–3 months of age (Horne et al. 2001). Furthermore, the nature of the arousal response is altered by sleep position, with more cortical arousals and fewer subcortical arousals when term infants slept prone at 2–3 months of age. This age coincides with the peak incidence for sudden infant death syndrome (SIDS) and prone sleeping is the major risk factor for SIDS. The authors suggested that increased cortical arousals in healthy term infants was a protective response which may be reduced in those infants who died from SIDS (Richardson et al. 2008). Preterm infants are at increased risk for SIDS and when slept prone had increased cortical arousals across the first 6 months after term equivalents age in both AS and QS (Richardson & Horne, 2013). When arousal responses were compared between term and preterm infants, preterm infants had significantly fewer cortical arousals in QS when prone at 2–3 months of age and more cortical arousals when prone at 2–4 weeks in AS. There were no differences in either sleep state when infants slept supine (Richardson & Horne, 2013). Thus, in preterm infants increased cortical arousals maybe protective against the prone sleeping position.

Recently, studies have identified that cerebral oxygenation is also lower in preterm compared to term infants across the first 6 months corrected age when infants slept prone (Fyfe et al. 2014), and that cerebrovascular control after a head‐up tilt was more variable (Fyfe et al. 2015c), indicating immature or impaired autonomic control. Thus, persistent immature cardiovascular and cerebrovascular control in infants born preterm may further compound the effects of any hypoxic insults that occur with the frequent respiratory disturbances in this population. This additive insult further contributes to impaired brain development and adverse neurodevelopmental outcomes.

Inflammation

Immune factors, such as interleukin‐1 beta (IL1) and tumour necrosis factor alpha (TNFα), play a regulatory role in sleep homeostasis (Clinton et al. 2011). Both central and peripheral immune cells express circadian clock genes which regulate the circadian oscillations in the synthesis and release of pro‐ and anti‐inflammatory factors (Curtis et al. 2014; Labrecque & Cermakian, 2015). The circadian sensitivity of our immune cells can be seen during exposure to the inflammatory mediator lipopolysaccharide, a component of the outer membrane of Gram‐negative bacteria. Alamili et al. (2014) demonstrated, for example, that lipopolysaccharide given during the day stimulated release of anti‐inflammatory cytokines, while night time exposure promoted release of pro‐inflammatory cytokines. Chronodisruption, such as sleep deprivation or broken sleep can cause immune compromise (Curtis et al. 2014; Labrecque & Cermakian, 2015). Inflammation can independently shift the timing of the circadian clock, alter clock gene expression and increases suprachiasmatic nucleus cytokine release, (Curtis et al. 2014; Labrecque & Cermakian, 2015). In adults, chronic inflammatory conditions such as diabetes are associated with white matter lesions, altered neural network functional connectivity, and altered sleep patterns including an increased propensity for QS (Mullington et al. 2010; Hoogenboom et al. 2014; Ramos et al. 2014).

The preterm fetus and newborn is at significant risk of exposure to inflammation. The inflammatory condition of chorioamnionitis, for example, complicates nearly all preterm births before 24 weeks and a quarter to a third of births before 34 weeks GA (Hagberg et al. 2015; Bennet et al. 2017), and inflammation may persist well beyond birth (for recent review, see Bennet et al. 2017). The Extremely Low Gestational Age Newborns study has shown that circulating levels of pro‐inflammatory cytokines can be elevated for the first month of life after preterm birth and this correlates with adverse neurological outcomes in children at 10 years of age (Kuban et al. 2017; Yanni et al. 2017). Inflammation, and hypersensitisation to inflammation, may persist for even longer as demonstrated by Lin and colleagues in preterm children with white matter‐induced CP (Lin et al. 2010).

Markers for inflammation, such as C‐reactive protein and cytokines, are elevated in children born preterm who have conditions such as OSA, particularly in children who go on to have neurocognitive disorders (Gozal et al. 2007). Similar to the cardiovascular and systemic inflammation seen in adults, a recent report in children studied at 9–10 years of age born full‐term has shown that primary snoring and sleep disordered breathing is associated with higher ascending aorta peak systolic blood flow velocity (indicating altered vascular function) and systemic levels of cytotoxic T cell lymphocytes producing tumour necrosis factor alpha and T cell interferon gamma compared to that seen in non‐snoring children (Kontos et al. 2017). Inflammation is likely to be a persistent occurrence in preterm children who have a greater risk of sleep‐disordered breathing, and potentially this is a significant contributor the increased risk for adverse neurodevelopmental outcomes.

Conclusions and perspectives

In summary, our review has demonstrated that many infants born preterm go on to have altered sleep patterns throughout life characterised by reduced sleep efficiency and quality of sleep. It is of note that there is variability between reports, for example, some studies suggest shorter sleep, while others have observed longer sleep durations. Further, not all studies found an effect of prematurity on the development or quality of sleep. What is clear is that considerable further work is needed in this area, with more consistent methodologies. We suggest that it would be of value for future studies to include assessment of the brain for injury, along with measures of neurodevelopmental disabilities. Standardised use of direct measures of sleep/behaviour in addition to parental reporting would help address potential issues related to the stress parents have when caring for preterm children that have been raised in some studies. For example, do preterm children go to sleep earlier (does prematurity set the chronotype of early rising?), or do parents who are sleep‐deprived themselves institute earlier bedtimes? Do preterm children wake frequently at night, or do parents of preterm children check up on them more frequently given the increased health issues these children have (e.g. respiratory complications)? It would also be of value to more tightly define the preterm age range studied to address the question of the impact of specific stages of brain maturation has on subsequent sleep development. There is also a need to consider the age of term control subjects, given that data now to show that very late‐preterms, i.e. those within a few weeks of normal GA birth, are also at risk of impaired neurodevelopment, which may affect sleep (Brumbaugh et al. 2016).

Our review has highlighted several mediators which may contribute to the variable findings on the effect of prematurity on sleep development, including the degree of prematurity, growth restriction, neural injury and/or impaired neural maturation, sleep disordered breathing, and systemic inflammation which may be chronic in nature. Many of these factors will occur in combination or be additive, and they will produce self‐reinforcing feedback loops. For example, perinatal inflammation leading to impaired neural maturation may predispose to impaired sleep function and sleep disordered breathing, which in turn mediates further inflammation, which then in turn impacts upon ongoing neural maturation. We recognise that we have not addressed many other influential factors such as the role of sexual dimorphism in sleep regulation (Mong & Cusmano, 2016). However, an important consideration arising from the material reviewed above is that preterm babies may need to be cared for until term‐equivalent age under conditions that reproduce, or at least endeavour to replace, the hormonal conditions present in utero (for reviews, see Brunton et al. 2014; Hirst et al. 2014).

As discussed at the start of this review, sleep is vital for optimal neurocognitive function, and impaired sleep is a risk factor for many diseases. Being born prematurely alters how the brain develops, and is associated with increased risks of significant health issues throughout life (Committee on Understanding Premature Birth and Assuring Healthy Outcomes, 2007). Our review demonstrates that impaired sleep from infancy to adulthood may be contributory to these risks. Significant research is now needed to determine specific factors related to prematurity, and their timing, which influence sleep development in order to facilitate the development of timely interventions and treatment strategies that will promote better sleep throughout life.

Additional information section

Competing interests

The authors declare no competing financial interests.

Author contributions

All authors conceptualized this Topical Review, and undertook the writing and editing of the manuscript. All authors reviewed and approved the final version as submitted to The Journal of Physiology.

Funding

L.B. acknowledges grant funding from the New Zealand Health Research Council (projects 12/613, 17/601). D.W.W. acknowledges support from National Health & Medical Research Council of Australia (projects APP1061291, APP1124493), Cerebral Palsy Alliance of Australia (PG3315, PG8816), and RMIT University funding to DWW to establish the Neurodevelopment in Health and Disease Program. R.S.C.H. acknowledges support from National Health & Medical Research Council of Australia (Projects APP1006647, APP1045559), The Heart Foundation of Australia (project HF G12M6564) and Victorian Government's Operational Infrastructure Support Programme.

Acknowledgments

This review is dedicated to the memory of the late Professor Julian (Bill) Parer, friend, mentor and clinician‐scientist whose perinatal research curiosity inspired us and many generations.

Biography

Laura Bennet is co‐director of the Fetal Physiology and Neuroscience Group in the department of Physiology, at the University of Auckland. She is a fetal systems physiologist currently leading a programme of research on the preterm fetal behavioural and physiological adaptations to hypoxia and infection and the development of perinatal neuroprotection strategies. David Walker is programme leader of Neurodevelopment in Health and Disease at the School of Health and Biomedical Sciences, Royal Melbourne Institute of Technology (RMIT) University, Melbourne, Australia. He is a perinatal physiologist with interests in fundamental aspects of the development of the brain and behaviour. He currently leads a research programme on the effects of hypoxia on creatine and mitochondrial function in the perinatal brain. Rosemary Horne is a Senior Principal Research Fellow and heads the Infant and Child Health research theme within the Ritchie Centre, Hudson Institute of Medical Research and Department of Paediatrics, Monash University, Australia. Her research interests focus on sleep in infants and children and current research projects focus on the development of cardio‐respiratory control in preterm infants and the effects of sleep disordered breathing on behaviour and the cardiovascular system in children.

graphic file with name TJP-596-5687-g001.gif

Edited by: Ole Petersen & Dino Giussani

References

  1. Akay M & Szeto HH (1995). Investigating the relationship between fetus EEG, respiratory, and blood pressure signals during maturation using wavelet transform. Ann Biomed Eng 23, 574–582. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Alamili M, Bendtzen K, Lykkesfeldt J, Rosenberg J & Gogenur I (2014). Pronounced inflammatory response to endotoxaemia during nighttime: a randomised cross‐over trial. PloS One 9, e87413. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Allotey J, Zamora J, Cheong‐See F, Kalidindi M, Arroyo‐Manzano D, Asztalos E, van der Post J, Mol BW, Moore D, Birtles D, Khan KS & Thangaratinam S (2018). Cognitive, motor, behavioural and academic performances of children born preterm: a meta‐analysis and systematic review involving 64 061 children. BJOG 125, 16–25. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Altena E, Micoulaud‐Franchi JA, Geoffroy PA, Sanz‐Arigita E, Bioulac S & Philip P (2016). The bidirectional relation between emotional reactivity and sleep: From disruption to recovery. Behav Neurosci 130, 336–350. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Andre M, Lamblin MD, d'Allest AM, Curzi‐Dascalova L, Moussalli‐Salefranque F, T SNT, Vecchierini‐Blineau MF, Wallois F, Walls‐Esquivel E & Plouin P (2010). Electroencephalography in premature and full‐term infants. Developmental features and glossary. Neurophysiol Clin 40, 59–124. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Arduini D, Rizzo G, Caforio L, Boccolini MR, Romanini C & Mancuso S (1989). Behavioural state transitions in healthy and growth retarded fetuses. Early Hum Dev 19, 155–165. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Asaka Y & Takada S (2010). Activity‐based assessment of the sleep behaviors of VLBW preterm infants and full‐term infants at around 12 months of age. Brain Dev 32, 150–155. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Asaka Y & Takada S (2013). Relation between sleep status of preterm infants aged 1–2 years and mothers' parenting stress. Pediatr Int 55, 416–421. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Back SA ( 2015). Brain injury in the preterm infant: New horizons for pathogenesis and prevention. Pediatr Neurol 53, 185–192. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Back SA ( 2017). White matter injury in the preterm infant: pathology and mechanisms. Acta Neuropatho 134, 331–349. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Ball G, Aljabar P, Zebari S, Tusor N, Arichi T, Merchant N, Robinson EC, Ogundipe E, Rueckert D, Edwards AD & Counsell SJ (2014). Rich‐club organization of the newborn human brain. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 111, 7456–7461. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Ball G, Boardman JP, Rueckert D, Aljabar P, Arichi T, Merchant N, Gousias IS, Edwards AD & Counsell SJ (2012). The effect of preterm birth on thalamic and cortical development. Cereb Cortex 22, 1016–1024. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Ball G, Pazderova L, Chew A, Tusor N, Merchant N, Arichi T, Allsop JM, Cowan FM, Edwards AD & Counsell SJ (2015). Thalamocortical connectivity predicts cognition in children born preterm. Cereb Cortex 25, 4310–4318. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Barlow RM ( 1969). The foetal sheep: morphogenesis of the nervous system and histochemical aspects of myelination. J Comp Neurol 135, 249–262. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Ben‐Ari Y ( 2001). Developing networks play a similar melody. Trends Neurosci 24, 353–360. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Benders MJ, Palmu K, Menache C, Borradori‐Tolsa C, Lazeyras F, Sizonenko S, Dubois J, Vanhatalo S & Huppi PS (2015). Early brain activity relates to subsequent brain growth in premature infants. Cereb Cortex 25, 3014–3024. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Bennet L ( 2017). Sex, drugs and rock and roll: tales from preterm fetal life. J Physiol 595, 1865–1881. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Bennet L, Booth L & Gunn AJ (2010). Potential biomarkers for hypoxic‐ischemic encephalopathy. Semin Fetal Neonatal Med 15, 253–260. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Bennet L, Booth LC, Drury PP, Quaedackers JS & Gunn AJ (2012). Preterm neonatal cardiovascular instability: does understanding the fetus help evaluate the newborn? Clin Exp Pharmacol Physiol 39, 965–972. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Bennet L, Dhillon S, Lear CA, van den Heuij L, King V, Dean JM, Wassink G, Davidson JO & Gunn AJ (2017). Chronic inflammation and impaired development of the preterm brain. J Reprod Immunol 125, 45–55. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Bennet L, Fyfe KL, Yiallourou SR, Merk H, Wong FY & Horne RS (2016). Discrimination of sleep states using continuous cerebral bedside monitoring (aEEG) compared to polysomnography in infants. Acta Paediatr 105, e582–e587. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Bennet L, Rossenrode S, Gunning MI, Gluckman PD & Gunn AJ (1999). The cardiovascular and cerebrovascular responses of the immature fetal sheep to acute umbilical cord occlusion. J Physiol 517, 247–257. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Bennet L, Van Den Heuij L, Dean JM , Drury P, Wassink G & Gunn AJ (2013). Neural plasticity and the Kennard Principle ‐ does it work for the preterm brain? Clin Exp Pharmacol Physiol 40, 774–784. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Biggs SN, Meltzer LJ, Tapia IE, Traylor J, Nixon GM, Horne RS, Doyle LW, Asztalos E, Mindell JA & Marcus CL (2016). Sleep/wake patterns and parental perceptions of sleep in children born preterm. J Clin Sleep Med 12, 711–717. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Biggs SN, Pizzorno VA, van den Heuvel CJ, Kennedy JD, Martin AJ & Lushington K (2010). Differences in parental attitudes towards sleep and associations with sleep‐wake patterns in Caucasian and Southeast Asian school‐aged children in Australia. Behav Sleep Med 8, 207–218. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Bjorkqvist J, Paavonen J, Andersson S, Pesonen AK, Lahti J, Heinonen K, Eriksson J, Raikkonen K, Hovi P, Kajantie E & Strang‐Karlsson S (2014). Advanced sleep‐wake rhythm in adults born prematurely: confirmation by actigraphy‐based assessment in the Helsinki Study of Very Low Birth Weight Adults. Sleep Med 15, 1101–1106. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Blomqvist YT, Nyqvist KH, Rubertsson C & Funkquist EL (2017). Parents need support to find ways to optimise their own sleep without seeing their preterm infant's sleeping patterns as a problem. Acta Paediatr 106, 223–228. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Bourjeily G, Danilack VA, Bublitz MH, Lipkind H, Muri J, Caldwell D, Tong I & Rosene‐Montella K (2017). Obstructive sleep apnea in pregnancy is associated with adverse maternal outcomes: a national cohort. Sleep Med 38, 50–57. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Bourke R, Anderson V, Yang JS, Jackman AR, Killedar A, Nixon GM, Davey MJ, Walker AM, Trinder J & Horne RS (2011a). Cognitive and academic functions are impaired in children with all severities of sleep‐disordered breathing. Sleep Med 12, 489–496. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Bourke RS, Anderson V, Yang JS, Jackman AR, Killedar A, Nixon GM, Davey MJ, Walker AM, Trinder J & Horne RS (2011b). Neurobehavioral function is impaired in children with all severities of sleep disordered breathing. Sleep Med 12, 222–229. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Brumbaugh JE, Conrad AL, Lee JK, DeVolder IJ, Zimmerman MB, Magnotta VA, Axelson ED & Nopoulos PC (2016). Altered brain function, structure, and developmental trajectory in children born late preterm. Pediatr Res 80, 197–203. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Brunton PJ, Russell JA & Hirst JJ (2014). Allopregnanolone in the brain: protecting pregnancy and birth outcomes. Prog Neurobiol 113, 106–136. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Bystron I, Blakemore C & Rakic P (2008). Development of the human cerebral cortex: Boulder Committee revisited. Nat Rev Neurosci 9, 110–122. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Cao M, He Y, Dai Z, Liao X, Jeon T, Ouyang M, Chalak L, Bi Y, Rollins N, Dong Q & Huang H (2017). Early development of functional network segregation revealed by connectomic analysis of the preterm human brain. Cereb Cortex 27, 1949–1963. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Cappuccio FP & Miller MA (2017). Sleep and Cardio‐metabolic disease. Curr Cardiol Rep 19, 110. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Caravale B, Sette S, Cannoni E, Marano A, Riolo E, Devescovi A, De Curtis M & Bruni O (2017). Sleep characteristics and temperament in preterm children at two years of age. J Clin Sleep Med 13, 1081–1088. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Carskadon MA & Dement WC (2011). Monitoring and staging human sleep In Principles and Practice of Sleep Medicine, eds Kryger MH, Roth T. & Dement WC, pp. 16–26. Elsevier Saunders, St Louis, USA. [Google Scholar]
  38. Catalano SM & Shatz CJ (1998). Activity‐dependent cortical target selection by thalamic axons. Science 281, 559–562. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Chalak LF, Rollins N, Morriss MC, Brion LP, Heyne R & Sanchez PJ (2012). Perinatal acidosis and hypoxic‐ischemic encephalopathy in preterm infants of 33 to 35 weeks' gestation. J Pediatr 160, 388–394. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Chan KC, Shi L, So HK, Wang D, Liew AW, Rasalkar DD, Chu CW, Wing YK & Li AM (2014). Neurocognitive dysfunction and grey matter density deficit in children with obstructive sleep apnoea. Sleep Med 15, 1055–1061. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Cirelli C & Tononi G (2017). The sleeping brain. Cerebrum 2017. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Clapp JF, Szeto HH, Abrams R, Larrow R & Mann LI (1980). Physiologic variability and fetal electrocortical activity. Am J Obstet Gynecol 136, 1045–1050. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Clewlow F, Dawes GS, Johnston BM & Walker DW (1983). Changes in breathing, electrocortical and muscle activity in unanaesthetized fetal lambs with age. J Physiol 341, 463–476. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Clinton JM, Davis CJ, Zielinski MR, Jewett KA & Krueger JM (2011). Biochemical regulation of sleep and sleep biomarkers. J Clin Sleep Med 7, S38–42. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Cohen E, Wong FY, Wallace EM, Mockler JC, Odoi A, Hollis S, Horne RSC & Yiallourou SR (2018). EEG power spectrum maturation in preterm fetal growth restricted infants. Brain Res 1678, 180–186. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Collins CL, Barfield C, Davis PG & Horne RS (2015). Randomized controlled trial to compare sleep and wake in preterm infants less than 32 weeks of gestation receiving two different modes of non‐invasive respiratory support. Early Hum Dev 91, 701–704. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Committee on Understanding Premature Birth and Assuring Healthy Outcomes (2007). Preterm Birth: Causes, Consequences, and Prevention. Institute of Medicine, The National Academies Press, Washington, DC. [Google Scholar]
  48. Crossley KJ, Nicol MB, Hirst JJ, Walker DW & Thorburn GD (1997). Suppression of arousal by progesterone in fetal sheep. Reprod Fertil Dev 9, 767–773. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Curtis AM, Bellet MM, Sassone‐Corsi P & O'Neill LA (2014). Circadian clock proteins and immunity. Immunity 40, 178–186. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Curzi‐Dascalova L (2001). Between‐sleep states transitions in premature babies. J Sleep Res 10, 153–158. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Curzi‐Dascalova L, Challamel Marie‐Josephe (2000). Neurophysiological basis of sleep development In Sleep and Breathing in Children: A Developmental Approach, eds Gerald M, Loughlin JLC. & Marcus CL, pp. 1–37. Marcel Dekker Inc, New York, USA. [Google Scholar]
  52. Curzi‐Dascalova L, Peirano P & Morel‐Kahn F (1988). Development of sleep states in normal premature and full‐term newborns. Dev Psychobiol 21, 431–444. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Czikk MJ, Sweeley JC, Homan JH, Milley JR & Richardson BS (2003). Cerebral leucine uptake and protein synthesis in the near‐term ovine fetus: relation to fetal behavioral state. Am J Physiol Regul Integr Comp Physiol 284, R200–R207. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Czikk MJ, Totten S, Homan JH, White SE & Richardson BS (2001). Sagittal sinus blood flow in the ovine fetus as a continuous measure of cerebral blood flow: relationship to behavioural state activity. Brain Res Dev Brain Res 131, 103–111. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. D'Rozario AL, Cross NE, Vakulin A, Bartlett DJ, Wong KKH, Wang D & Grunstein RR (2017). Quantitative electroencephalogram measures in adult obstructive sleep apnea ‐ Potential biomarkers of neurobehavioural functioning. Sleep Med Rev 36, 29–42. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Dalton KJ, Dawes GS & Patrick JE (1977). Diurnal, respiratory, and other rhythms of fetal heart rate in lambs. Am J Obstet Gynecol 127, 414–424. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Dammann O & Leviton A (2014). Intermittent or sustained systemic inflammation and the preterm brain. Pediatr Res 75, 376–380. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Darnall RA, Chen X, Nemani KV, Sirieix CM, Gimi B, Knoblach S, McEntire BL & Hunt CE (2017). Early postnatal exposure to intermittent hypoxia in rodents is proinflammatory, impairs white matter integrity, and alters brain metabolism. Pediatr Res 82, 164–172. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Datta S, Patterson EH, Vincitore M, Tonkiss J, Morgane PJ & Galler JR (2000). Prenatal protein malnourished rats show changes in sleep/wake behavior as adults. J Sleep Res 9, 71–79. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Davidson JO, Quaedackers JS, George SA, Gunn AJ & Bennet L (2011). Maternal dexamethasone and EEG hyperactivity in preterm fetal sheep. J Physiol 589, 3823–3835. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Dawes GS, Fox HE, Leduc BM, Liggins GC & Richards RT (1972). Respiratory movements and rapid eye movement sleep in the foetal lamb. J Physiol 220, 119–143. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. de Graaf‐Peters VB & Hadders‐Algra M (2006). Ontogeny of the human central nervous system: what is happening when? Early Hum Dev 82, 257–266. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  63. de Vries JI, Visser GH, Mulder EJ & Prechtl HF (1987). Diurnal and other variations in fetal movement and heart rate patterns at 20–22 weeks. Early Hum Dev 15, 333–348. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  64. Decima PF, Fyfe KL, Odoi A, Wong FY & Horne RS (2015). The longitudinal effects of persistent periodic breathing on cerebral oxygenation in preterm infants. Sleep Med 16, 729–735. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  65. Drogtrop AP, Ubels R & Nijhuis JG (1990). The association between fetal body movements, eye movements and heart rate patterns in pregnancies between 25 and 30 weeks of gestation. Early Hum Dev 23, 67–73. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  66. Ellery SJ, Kelleher M, Grigsby P, Burd I, Derks JB, Hirst JJ, Miller SL, Sherman LS, Tolcos M & Walker DW (2018). Antenatal prevention of cerebral palsy and childhood disability: is the impossible possible? J Physiol 596, 5593–5609. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  67. Feld GB & Born J (2017). Sculpting memory during sleep: concurrent consolidation and forgetting. Curr Opin Neurobiol 44, 20–27. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  68. Fischi‐Gomez E, Munoz‐Moreno E, Vasung L, Griffa A, Borradori‐Tolsa C, Monnier M, Lazeyras F, Thiran JP & Huppi PS (2016). Brain network characterization of high‐risk preterm‐born school‐age children. NeuroImage Clin 11, 195–209. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  69. Fransson P, Aden U, Blennow M & Lagercrantz H (2011). The functional architecture of the infant brain as revealed by resting‐state fMRI. Cereb Cortex 21, 145–154. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  70. Friedrichs‐Maeder CL, Griffa A, Schneider J, Huppi PS, Truttmann A & Hagmann P (2017). Exploring the role of white matter connectivity in cortex maturation. PloS One 12, e0177466. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  71. Fyfe KL, Odoi A, Yiallourou SR, Wong FY, Walker AM & Horne RS (2015a). Preterm infants exhibit greater variability in cerebrovascular control than term infants. Sleep 38, 1411–1421. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  72. Fyfe KL, Yiallourou SR, Wong FY, Odoi A, Walker AM & Horne RS (2014). Cerebral oxygenation in preterm infants. Pediatrics 134, 435–445. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  73. Fyfe KL, Yiallourou SR, Wong FY, Odoi A, Walker AM & Horne RS (2015b). The effect of gestational age at birth on post‐term maturation of heart rate variability. Sleep 38, 1635–1644. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  74. Fyfe KL, Yiallourou SR, Wong FY, Odoi A, Walker AM & Horne RS (2015c). Gestational age at birth affects maturation of baroreflex control. J Pediatr 166, 559–565. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  75. Gaultier C ( 1995). Cardiorespiratory adaptation during sleep in infants and children. Pediatr Pulmonol 19, 105–117. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  76. Gazzolo D, Visser GH, Santi F, Magliano CP, Scopesi F, Russo A, Pittaluga C, Nigro M, Camoriano R & Bruschettini PL (1995). Behavioural development and Doppler velocimetry in relation to perinatal outcome in small for dates fetuses. Early Hum Dev 43, 185–195. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  77. Geddes R, Vannucci RC & Vannucci SJ (2001). Delayed cerebral atrophy following moderate hypoxia‐ischemia in the immature rat. Dev Neurosci 23, 180–185. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  78. Gopagondanahalli KR, Li J, Fahey MC, Hunt RW, Jenkin G, Miller SL & Malhotra A (2016). Preterm hypoxic‐ischemic encephalopathy. Front Pediatr 4, 114. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  79. Gossel‐Symank R, Grimmer I, Korte J & Siegmund R (2004). Actigraphic monitoring of the activity‐rest behavior of preterm and full‐term infants at 20 months of age. Chronobiol Int 21, 661–671. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  80. Gozal D, Crabtree VM, Sans Capdevila O, Witcher LA & Kheirandish‐Gozal L (2007). C‐reactive protein, obstructive sleep apnea, and cognitive dysfunction in school‐aged children. Am J Respir Crit Care Med 176, 188–193. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  81. Graven S (2006). Sleep and brain development. Clin Perinatol 33, 693–706, vii. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  82. Guyer C, Huber R, Fontijn J, Bucher HU, Nicolai H, Werner H, Molinari L, Latal B & Jenni OG (2015). Very preterm infants show earlier emergence of 24‐hour sleep‐wake rhythms compared to term infants. Early Hum Dev 91, 37–42. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  83. Hagberg H, Mallard C, Ferriero DM, Vannucci SJ, Levison SW, Vexler ZS & Gressens P (2015). The role of inflammation in perinatal brain injury. Nat Rev Neurol 11, 192–208. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  84. Hagmann‐von Arx P, Perkinson‐Gloor N, Brand S, Albert D, Holsboer‐Trachsler E, Grob A, Weber P & Lemola S (2014). In school‐age children who were born very preterm sleep efficiency is associated with cognitive function. Neuropsychobiology 70, 244–252. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  85. Hagmann P, Sporns O, Madan N, Cammoun L, Pienaar R, Wedeen VJ, Meuli R, Thiran JP & Grant PE (2010). White matter maturation reshapes structural connectivity in the late developing human brain. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 107, 19067–19072. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  86. Halbower AC, Degaonkar M, Barker PB, Earley CJ, Marcus CL, Smith PL, Prahme MC & Mahone EM (2006). Childhood obstructive sleep apnea associates with neuropsychological deficits and neuronal brain injury. PLoS Med 3, e301. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  87. Harding R & Hooper SB (1996). Regulation of lung expansion and lung growth before birth. J Appl Physiol 81, 209–224. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  88. Hatori M, Gronfier C, Van Gelder RN, Bernstein PS , Carreras J, Panda S, Marks F, Sliney D, Hunt CE, Hirota T, Furukawa T & Tsubota K (2017). Global rise of potential health hazards caused by blue light‐induced circadian disruption in modern aging societies. NPJ Aging Mech Dis 3, 9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  89. Hepper PG (1997). Memory in utero? Dev Med Child Neurol 39, 343–346. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  90. Hibbs AM, Storfer‐Isser A, Rosen C, Ievers‐Landis CE, Taveras EM & Redline S (2014). Advanced sleep phase in adolescents born preterm. Behav Sleep Med 12, 412–424. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  91. Hirst JJ, Kelleher MA, Walker DW & Palliser HK (2014). Neuroactive steroids in pregnancy: key regulatory and protective roles in the foetal brain. J Steroid Biochem Mol Biol 139, 144–153. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  92. Holditch‐Davis D & Edwards LJ (1998). Temporal organization of sleep‐wake states in preterm infants. Dev Psychobiol 33, 257–269. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  93. Holditch‐Davis D, Scher M, Schwartz T & Hudson‐Barr D (2004). Sleeping and waking state development in preterm infants. Early Hum Dev 80, 43–64. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  94. Hoogenboom WS, Marder TJ, Flores VL, Huisman S, Eaton HP, Schneiderman JS, Bolo NR, Simonson DC, Jacobson AM, Kubicki M, Shenton ME & Musen G (2014). Cerebral white matter integrity and resting‐state functional connectivity in middle‐aged patients with type 2 diabetes. Diabetes 63, 728–738. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  95. Hoppenbrouwers T, Hodgman JE, Rybine D, Fabrikant G, Corwin M, Crowell D, Weese‐Mayer DE & Group CS (2005). Sleep architecture in term and preterm infants beyond the neonatal period: the influence of gestational age, steroids, and ventilatory support. Sleep 28, 1428–1436. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  96. Horne RS, Ferens D, Watts AM, Vitkovic J, Lacey B, Andrew S, Cranage SM, Chau B & Adamson TM (2001). The prone sleeping position impairs arousability in term infants. J Pediatr 138, 811–816. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  97. Horne RS, Fung AC, NcNeil S, Fyfe KL, Odoi A & Wong FY (2017a). The longitudinal effects of persistent apnea on cerebral oxygenation in infants born preterm. J Pediatr 182, 79–84. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  98. Horne RSC, Roy B, Walter LM, Biggs SN, Tamanyan K, Weichard A, Nixon GM, Davey MJ, Ditchfield M, Harper RM & Kumar R (2017b). Regional brain tissue changes and associations with disease severity in children with sleep disordered breathing. Sleep 41, zsx203. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  99. Houghton DC, Walker DW, Young IR & McMillen IC (1993). Melatonin and the light‐dark cycle separately influence daily behavioral and hormonal rhythms in the pregnant ewe and sheep fetus. Endocrinology 133, 90–98. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  100. Huang YS, Paiva T, Hsu JF, Kuo MC & Guilleminault C (2014). Sleep and breathing in premature infants at 6 months post‐natal age. BMC Pediatr 14, 303. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  101. Hunter CJ, Bennet L, Power GG, Roelfsema V, Blood AB, Quaedackers JS, George S, Guan J & Gunn AJ (2003). Key neuroprotective role for endogenous adenosine A1 receptor activation during asphyxia in the fetal sheep. Stroke 34, 2240–2245. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  102. Iglowstein I, Latal Hajnal B, Molinari L, Largo RH & Jenni OG (2006). Sleep behaviour in preterm children from birth to age 10 years: a longitudinal study. Acta Paediatr 95, 1691–1693. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  103. IPWGA (2005). The scoring of arousals in healthy term infants (between the ages of 1 and 6 months). J Sleep Res 14, 37–41. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  104. Jackman AR, Biggs SN, Walter LM, Embuldeniya US, Davey MJ, Nixon GM, Anderson V, Trinder J & Horne RS (2012). Sleep‐disordered breathing in preschool children is associated with behavioral, but not cognitive, impairments. Sleep Med 13, 621–631. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  105. Jakab A, Schwartz E, Kasprian G, Gruber GM, Prayer D, Schopf V & Langs G (2014). Fetal functional imaging portrays heterogeneous development of emerging human brain networks. Front Hum Neurosci 8, 852. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  106. Juliano C, Sosunov S, Niatsetskaya Z, Isler JA, Utkina‐Sosunova I, Jang I, Ratner V & Ten V (2015). Mild intermittent hypoxemia in neonatal mice causes permanent neurofunctional deficit and white matter hypomyelination. Exp Neurol 264, 33–42. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  107. Karlsson KA, Arnardottir H, Robinson SR & Blumberg MS (2011). Dynamics of sleep‐wake cyclicity across the fetal period in sheep (Ovis aries). Dev Psychobiol 53, 89–95. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  108. Keen AE, Frasch MG, Sheehan MA, Matushewski B & Richardson BS (2011). Maturational changes and effects of chronic hypoxemia on electrocortical activity in the ovine fetus. Brain Res 1402, 38–45. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  109. Kelleher MA, Palliser HK, Walker DW & Hirst JJ (2011). Sex‐dependent effect of a low neurosteroid environment and intrauterine growth restriction on foetal guinea pig brain development. J Endocrinol 208, 301–309. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  110. Kheirandish‐Gozal L, Yoder K, Kulkarni R, Gozal D & Decety J (2014). Preliminary functional MRI neural correlates of executive functioning and empathy in children with obstructive sleep apnea. Sleep 37, 587–592. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  111. Kidokoro H, Anderson PJ, Doyle LW, Woodward LJ, Neil JJ & Inder TE (2014). Brain injury and altered brain growth in preterm infants: predictors and prognosis. Pediatrics 134, e444–453. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  112. Kontos A, Willoughby S, van den Heuvel C, Kennedy D, Martin J, Hodge G, Worthley M, Chin AK, Nelson A, Teo K, Baumert M, Pamula Y & Lushington K (2017). Ascending aortic blood flow velocity is increased in children with primary snoring/mild sleep‐disordered breathing and associated with an increase in CD8+ T cells expressing TNFα and IFNγ. Heart Vessel 33, 537–548. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  113. Koolen N, Oberdorfer L, Rona Z, Giordano V, Werther T, Klebermass‐Schrehof K, Stevenson N & Vanhatalo S (2017). Automated classification of neonatal sleep states using EEG. Clin Neurophysiol 128, 1100–1108. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  114. Korzeniewski SJ, Allred EN, Joseph RM, Heeren T, Kuban KCK, O'Shea TM & Leviton A (2017). Neurodevelopment at age 10 years of children born < 28 weeks with fetal growth restriction. Pediatrics 140, e20170697. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  115. Kostovic I & Judas M (2010). The development of the subplate and thalamocortical connections in the human foetal brain. Acta Paediatr 99, 1119–1127. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  116. Krause AJ, Simon EB, Mander BA, Greer SM, Saletin JM, Goldstein‐Piekarski AN & Walker MP (2017). The sleep‐deprived human brain. Nat Rev Neurosci 18, 404–418. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  117. Kuban KC, Joseph RM, O'Shea TM, Heeren T, Fichorova RN, Douglass L, Jara H, Frazier JA, Hirtz D, Rollins JV, Paneth N & Extremely Low Gestational Age Newborns (ELGAN) Study Investigators (2017). Circulating inflammatory‐associated proteins in the first month of life and cognitive impairment at age 10 years in children born extremely preterm. J Pediatr 180, 116–123.e111. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  118. Kurth S, Dean DC 3rd, Achermann P, O'Muircheartaigh J, Huber R, Deoni SC & LeBourgeois MK (2016). Increased sleep depth in developing neural networks: new insights from sleep restriction in children. Front Hum Neurosci 10, 456. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  119. Kuula L, Pesonen AK, Merikanto I, Gradisar M, Lahti J, Heinonen K, Kajantie E & Raikkonen K (2017). Development of late circadian preference: sleep timing from childhood to late adolescence. J Pediat 194, 182–189.e1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  120. Kwon SH, Vasung L, Ment LR & Huppi PS (2014). The role of neuroimaging in predicting neurodevelopmental outcomes of preterm neonates. Clin Perinatol 41, 257–283. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  121. Labrecque N & Cermakian N (2015). Circadian clocks in the immune system. J Biol Rhythms 30, 277–290. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  122. Lee B, Hirst JJ & Walker DW (2002). Prostaglandin D synthase in the prenatal ovine brain and effects of its inhibition with selenium chloride on fetal sleep/wake activity in utero. J Neurosci 22, 5679–5686. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  123. Leitner Y, Bloch AM, Sadeh A, Neuderfer O, Tikotzky L, Fattal‐Valevski A & Harel S (2002). Sleep‐wake patterns in children with intrauterine growth retardation. J Child Neurol 17, 872–876. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  124. Lelis AL, Cardoso MV & Hall WA (2016). Sleep disorders in children with cerebral palsy: An integrative review. Sleep Med Rev 30, 63–71. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  125. Lin CY, Chang YC, Wang ST, Lee TY, Lin CF & Huang CC (2010). Altered inflammatory responses in preterm children with cerebral palsy. Ann Neurol 68, 204–212. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  126. Logitharajah P, Rutherford MA & Cowan FM (2009). Hypoxic‐ischemic encephalopathy in preterm infants: antecedent factors, brain imaging, and outcome. Pediatr Res 66, 222–229. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  127. Lowe CJ, Safati A & Hall PA (2017). The neurocognitive consequences of sleep restriction: A meta‐analytic review. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 80, 586–604. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  128. McCann M, Bayliss DM, Anderson M, Campbell C, French N, McMichael J, Reid C & Bucks RS (2018). The relationship between sleep problems and working memory in children born very preterm. Child Neuropsychol 24, 124–144. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  129. McClendon E, Shaver DC, Degener‐O'Brien K, Gong X, Nguyen T, Hoerder‐Suabedissen A, Molnar Z, Mohr C, Richardson BD, Rossi DJ & Back SA (2017). Transient hypoxemia chronically disrupts maturation of preterm fetal ovine subplate neuron arborization and activity. J Neurosci 37, 11912–11929. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  130. McIntosh GH, Baghurst KI, Potter BJ & Hetzel BS (1979). Foetal brain development in the sheep. Neuropathol Appl Neurobiol 5, 103–114. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  131. McMillen IC, Nowak R, Walker DW & Young IR (1990). Maternal pinealectomy alters the daily pattern of fetal breathing in sheep. Am J Physiol Regul Integr Comp Physiol 258, R284–R287. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  132. McNamara F, Lijowska AS & Thach BT (2002). Spontaneous arousal activity in infants during NREM and REM sleep. J Physiol 538, 263–269. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  133. Marks GA, Shaffery JP, Oksenberg A, Speciale SG & Roffwarg HP (1995). A functional role for REM sleep in brain maturation. Behav Brain Res 69, 1–11. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  134. Mellor DJ, Diesch T, Gunn AJ & Bennet L (2005). The importance of ‘awareness’ for understanding fetal pain. Brain Res Brain Res Rev 49, 455–471. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  135. Meng C, Bauml JG, Daamen M, Jaekel J, Neitzel J, Scheef L, Busch B, Baumann N, Boecker H, Zimmer C, Bartmann P, Wolke D, Wohlschlager AM & Sorg C (2016). Extensive and interrelated subcortical white and gray matter alterations in preterm‐born adults. Brain Struct Funct 221, 2109–2121. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  136. Mirmiran M, Maas YG & Ariagno RL (2003). Development of fetal and neonatal sleep and circadian rhythms. Sleep Med Rev 7, 321–334. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  137. Mong JA & Cusmano DM (2016). Sex differences in sleep: impact of biological sex and sex steroids. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 371, 20150110. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  138. Mulder EJ, de Heus R & Visser GH (2009). Antenatal corticosteroid therapy: short‐term effects on fetal behaviour and haemodynamics. Semin Fetal Neonatal Med 14, 151–156. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  139. Mulder EJ, Morssink LP, van der Schee T & Visser GH (1998). Acute maternal alcohol consumption disrupts behavioral state organization in the near‐term fetus. Pediatr Res 44, 774–779. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  140. Mulder EJ, Visser GH, Bekedam DJ & Prechtl HF (1987). Emergence of behavioural states in fetuses of type‐1‐diabetic women. Early Hum Dev 15, 231–251. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  141. Mullington JM, Simpson NS, Meier‐Ewert HK & Haack M (2010). Sleep loss and inflammation. Best Pract Res Clin Endocrinol Metab 24, 775–784. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  142. Munoz‐Moreno E, Fischi‐Gomez E, Batalle D, Borradori‐Tolsa C, Eixarch E, Thiran JP, Gratacos E & Huppi PS (2016). Structural brain network reorganization and social cognition related to adverse perinatal condition from infancy to early adolescence. Front Neurosci 10, 560. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  143. Murray E, Fernandes M, Fazel M, Kennedy SH, Villar J & Stein A (2015). Differential effect of intrauterine growth restriction on childhood neurodevelopment: a systematic review. BJOG 122, 1062–1072. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  144. Nguyen P, Yan EB, Castillo‐Melendez M, Walker DW & Hirst JJ (2004). Increased allopregnanolone levels in the fetal sheep brain following umbilical cord occlusion. J Physiol 560, 593–602. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  145. Nicol MB, Hirst JJ, Walker D & Thorburn GD (1997). Effect of alteration of maternal plasma progesterone concentrations on fetal behavioural state during late gestation. J Endocrinol 152, 379–386. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  146. Nijhuis IJ, ten Hof J, Mulder EJ, Nijhuis JG, Narayan H, Taylor DJ & Visser GH (2000). Fetal heart rate in relation to its variation in normal and growth retarded fetuses. Eur J Obstet Gynecol Reprod Biol 89, 27–33. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  147. Nijhuis IJ, ten Hof J, Nijhuis JG, Mulder EJ, Narayan H, Taylor DJ & Visser GH (1999). Temporal organization of fetal behavior from 24‐weeks gestation onwards in normal and complicated pregnancies. Dev Psychobiol 34, 257–268. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  148. Nijhuis JG, Prechtl HF, Martin CB Jr & Bots RS (1982). Are there behavioural states in the human fetus? Early Hum Dev 6, 177–195. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  149. Nordheim T, Rustoen T, Iversen PO & Nakstad B (2016). Quality of life in parents of preterm infants in a randomized nutritional intervention trial. Food Nutr Res 60, 32162. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  150. Omidvarnia A, Fransson P, Metsaranta M & Vanhatalo S (2013). Functional bimodality in the brain networks of preterm and term human newborns. Cereb Cortex 24, 2657–2768. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  151. Ortinau C & Neil J (2015). The neuroanatomy of prematurity: normal brain development and the impact of preterm birth. Clin Anat 28, 168–183. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  152. Ouyang M, Kang H, Detre JA, Roberts TPL & Huang H (2017). Short‐range connections in the developmental connectome during typical and atypical brain maturation. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 83, 109–122. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  153. Owens JA, Dearth‐Wesley T, Lewin D, Gioia G & Whitaker RC (2016). Self‐regulation and sleep duration, sleepiness, and chronotype in adolescents. Pediatrics 138, e20161406. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  154. Park B, Palomares JA, Woo MA, Kang DW, Macey PM, Yan‐Go FL, Harper RM & Kumar R (2016). Disrupted functional brain network organization in patients with obstructive sleep apnea. Brain Behav 6, e00441. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  155. Patrick J, Campbell K, Carmichael L, Natale R & Richardson B (1982). Patterns of gross fetal body movements over 24‐hour observation intervals during the last 10 weeks of pregnancy. Am J Obstet Gynecol 142, 363–371. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  156. Patural H, Pichot V, Jaziri F, Teyssier G, Gaspoz JM, Roche F & Barthelemy JC (2008). Autonomic cardiac control of very preterm newborns: a prolonged dysfunction. Early Hum Dev 84, 681–687. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  157. Peirano P, Algarin C & Uauy R (2003). Sleep‐wake states and their regulatory mechanisms throughout early human development. J Pediatr 143, S70–79. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  158. Perkinson‐Gloor N, Hagmann‐von Arx P, Brand S, Holsboer‐Trachsler E, Grob A, Weber P & Lemola S (2015). The role of sleep and the hypothalamic‐pituitary‐adrenal axis for behavioral and emotional problems in very preterm children during middle childhood. J Psychiatr Res 60, 141–147. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  159. Philby MF, Macey PM, Ma RA, Kumar R, Gozal D & Kheirandish‐Gozal L (2017). Reduced regional grey matter volumes in pediatric obstructive sleep apnea. Sci Rep 7, 44566. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  160. Pien GW, Pack AI, Jackson N, Maislin G, Macones GA & Schwab RJ (2014). Risk factors for sleep‐disordered breathing in pregnancy. Thorax 69, 371–377. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  161. Pillai M & James D (1990). Development of human fetal behavior: a review. Fetal Diagn Ther 5, 15–32. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  162. Ramos AR, Dong C, Rundek T, Elkind MS, Boden‐Albala B, Sacco RL & Wright CB (2014). Sleep duration is associated with white matter hyperintensity volume in older adults: the Northern Manhattan Study. J Sleep Res 23, 524–530. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  163. Rao N, Keen A, Czikk M, Frasch M & Richardson BS (2009). Behavioural state linkage in the ovine fetus near term. Brain Res 1250, 149–156. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  164. Rathbone R, Counsell SJ, Kapellou O, Dyet L, Kennea N, Hajnal J, Allsop JM, Cowan F & Edwards AD (2011). Perinatal cortical growth and childhood neurocognitive abilities. Neurology 77, 1510–1517. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  165. Ravishankar S, Bourjeily G, Lambert‐Messerlian G, He M, De Paepe ME & Gundogan F (2015). Evidence of placental hypoxia in maternal sleep disordered breathing. Pediatr Dev Pathol 18, 380–386. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  166. Reulecke S, Schulz S & Voss A (2012). Autonomic regulation during quiet and active sleep states in very preterm neonates. Front Physiol 3, 61. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  167. Richardson BS & Bocking AD (1998). Metabolic and circulatory adaptations to chronic hypoxia in the fetus. Comp Biochem Physiol A Mol IntegrPhysiol 119, 717–723. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  168. Richardson BS, Caetano H, Homan J & Carmichael L (1994). Regional brain blood flow in the ovine fetus during transition to the low‐voltage electrocortical state. Brain Res Dev Brain Res 81, 10–16. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  169. Richardson HL & Horne RS (2013). Arousal from sleep pathways are affected by the prone sleeping position and preterm birth: preterm birth, prone sleeping and arousal from sleep. Early Hum Dev 89, 705–711. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  170. Richardson HL, Walker AM & Horne RS (2008). Sleep position alters arousal processes maximally at the high‐risk age for sudden infant death syndrome. J Sleep Res 17, 450–457. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  171. Rigatto H, Moore M & Cates D (1986). Fetal breathing and behavior measured through a double‐wall Plexiglas window in sheep. J Appl Physiol 61, 160–164. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  172. Rivkin MJ, Flax J, Mozell R, Osathanondh R, Volpe JJ & Villa‐Komaroff L (1995). Oligodendroglial development in human fetal cerebrum. Ann Neurol 38, 92–101. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  173. Roffwarg HP, Muzio JN & Dement WC (1966). Ontogenetic development of the human sleep‐dream cycle. Science 152, 604–619. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  174. Rosen CL, Larkin EK, Kirchner HL, Emancipator JL, Bivins SF, Surovec SA, Martin RJ & Redline S (2003). Prevalence and risk factors for sleep‐disordered breathing in 8‐ to 11‐year‐old children: association with race and prematurity. J Pediatr 142, 383–389. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  175. Ruckebusch Y, Gaujoux M & Eghbali B (1977). Sleep cycles and kinesis in the foetal lamb. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol 42, 226–237. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  176. Rurak D ( 2017). Fetal sleep and spontaneous behaviour in utero: animal and clinical studies In Prenatal and Postnatal Determinants of Development, ed. Walker DW, pp. 89–146. Humana Press, New York. [Google Scholar]
  177. Rurak DW & Gruber NC (1983). Increased oxygen consumption associated with breathing activity in fetal lambs. J Appl Physiol 54, 701–707. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  178. Salhab WA & Perlman JM (2005). Severe fetal acidemia and subsequent neonatal encephalopathy in the larger premature infant. Pediatr Neurol 32, 25–29. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  179. Scheinost D, Kwon SH, Shen X, Lacadie C, Schneider KC, Dai F, Ment LR & Constable RT (2016). Preterm birth alters neonatal, functional rich club organization. Brain Struct Funct 221, 3211–3222. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  180. Scher MS, Johnson MW & Holditch‐Davis D (2005). Cyclicity of neonatal sleep behaviors at 25 to 30 weeks' postconceptional age. Pediatr Res 57, 879–882. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  181. Scher MS, Johnson MW, Ludington SM & Loparo K (2011). Physiologic brain dysmaturity in late preterm infants. Pediatr Res 70, 524–528. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  182. Scher MS & Loparo KA (2009). Neonatal EEG/sleep state analyses: a complex phenotype of developmental neural plasticity. Dev Neurosci 31, 259–275. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  183. Scher MS, Steppe DA, Beggarly ME, Salerno DG & Banks DL (2002). Neonatal EEG‐sleep disruption mimicking hypoxic‐ischemic encephalopathy after intrapartum asphyxia. Sleep Med 3, 411–415. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  184. Scher MS, Steppe DA, Dahl RE, Asthana S & Guthrie RD (1992). Comparison of EEG sleep measures in healthy full‐term and preterm infants at matched conceptional ages. Sleep 15, 442–448. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  185. Schmidt B, Roberts RS, Anderson PJ, Asztalos EV, Costantini L, Davis PG, Dewey D, D'Ilario J, Doyle LW, Grunau RE, Moddemann D, Nelson H, Ohlsson A, Solimano A & Tin W (2017). Academic performance, motor function, and behavior 11 years after neonatal caffeine citrate therapy for apnea of prematurity: an 11‐year follow‐up of the CAP randomized clinical trial. JAMA Pediatr 171, 564–572. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  186. Schwartz MD & Kilduff TS (2015). The neurobiology of sleep and wakefulness. Psychiatr Clin North Am 38, 615–644. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  187. Schwichtenberg AJ, Christ S, Abel E & Poehlmann‐Tynan JA (2016). Circadian sleep patterns in toddlers born preterm: longitudinal associations with developmental and health concerns. J Dev Behav Pediatr 37, 358–369. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  188. Schwichtenberg AJ & Goodlin‐Jones B (2010). Causes and correlates of frequent night awakenings in early childhood. Int Rev Neurobiol 93, 177–191. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  189. Sen A & Sellix MT (2016). The circadian timing system and environmental circadian disruption: from follicles to fertility. Endocrinology 157, 3366–3373. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  190. Seron‐Ferre M, Mendez N, Abarzua‐Catalan L, Vilches N, Valenzuela FJ, Reynolds HE, Llanos AJ, Rojas A, Valenzuela GJ & Torres‐Farfan C (2012). Circadian rhythms in the fetus. Mol Cell Endocrinol 349, 68–75. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  191. Shaw JC, Palliser HK, Walker DW & Hirst JJ (2015). Preterm birth affects GABAA receptor subunit mRNA levels during the foetal‐to‐neonatal transition in guinea pigs. J Dev Orig Health Dis 6, 250–260. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  192. Shellhaas RA, Burns JW, Hassan F, Carlson MD, Barks JDE & Chervin RD (2017). Neonatal sleep‐wake analyses predict 18‐month neurodevelopmental outcomes. Sleep 40, zsx144. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  193. Sizonenko SV, Sirimanne E, Mayall Y, Gluckman PD, Inder T & Williams C (2003). Selective cortical alteration after hypoxic‐ischemic injury in the very immature rat brain. Pediatr Res 54, 263–269. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  194. Smyser CD, Inder TE, Shimony JS, Hill JE, Degnan AJ, Snyder AZ & Neil JJ (2010). Longitudinal analysis of neural network development in preterm infants. Cereb Cortex 20, 2852–2862. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  195. Stangenes KM, Fevang SK, Grundt J, Donkor HM, Markestad T, Hysing M, Elgen IB & Bjorvatn B (2017). Children born extremely preterm had different sleeping habits at 11 years of age and more childhood sleep problems than term‐born children. Acta Paediatr 106, 1966–1972. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  196. Stiles J & Jernigan TL (2010). The basics of brain development. Neuropsychol Rev 20, 327–348. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  197. Stone PR, Burgess W, McIntyre J, Gunn AJ, Lear CA, Bennet L, Mitchell EA & Thompson JMD (2017). An investigation of fetal behavioural states during maternal sleep in healthy late gestation pregnancy: an observational study. J Physiol 595, 7441–7450. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  198. Szeto HH & Hinman DJ (1985). Prenatal development of sleep‐wake patterns in sheep. Sleep 8, 347–355. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  199. Tan S (2014). Fault and blame, insults to the perinatal brain may be remote from time of birth. Clin Perinatol 41, 105–117. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  200. Ten Hof J, Nijhuis IJ, Mulder EJ, Nijhuis JG, Narayan H, Taylor DJ, Westers P & Visser GH (2002). Longitudinal study of fetal body movements: nomograms, intrafetal consistency, and relationship with episodes of heart rate patterns a and B. Pediatr Res 52, 568–575. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  201. Thiriez G, Mougey C, Vermeylen D, Wermenbol V, Lanquart JP, Lin JS & Franco P (2015). Altered autonomic control in preterm newborns with impaired neurological outcomes. Clin Auton Res 25, 233–242. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  202. Thomason ME, Grove LE, Lozon TA Jr, Vila AM, Ye Y, Nye MJ, Manning JH, Pappas A, Hernandez‐Andrade E, Yeo L, Mody S, Berman S, Hassan SS & Romero R (2015). Age‐related increases in long‐range connectivity in fetal functional neural connectivity networks in utero. Dev Cogn Neurosci 11, 96–104. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  203. Thomason ME, Scheinost D, Manning JH, Grove LE, Hect J, Marshall N, Hernandez‐Andrade E, Berman S, Pappas A, Yeo L, Hassan SS, Constable RT, Ment LR & Romero R (2017). Weak functional connectivity in the human fetal brain prior to preterm birth. Sci Rep 7, 39286. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  204. Thompson DK, Chen J, Beare R, Adamson CL, Ellis R, Ahmadzai ZM, Kelly CE, Lee KJ, Zalesky A, Yang JYM, Hunt RW, Cheong JLY, Inder TE, Doyle LW, Seal ML & Anderson PJ (2016). Structural connectivity relates to perinatal factors and functional impairment at 7years in children born very preterm. Neuroimage 134, 328–337. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  205. Tolcos M, Petratos S, Hirst JJ, Wong F, Spencer SJ, Azhan A, Emery B & Walker DW (2017). Blocked, delayed, or obstructed: What causes poor white matter development in intrauterine growth restricted infants? Prog Neurobiol 154, 62–77. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  206. Tolonen M, Palva JM, Andersson S & Vanhatalo S (2007). Development of the spontaneous activity transients and ongoing cortical activity in human preterm babies. Neuroscience 145, 997–1006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  207. Tymofiyeva O, Hess CP, Ziv E, Tian N, Bonifacio SL, McQuillen PS, Ferriero DM, Barkovich AJ & Xu D (2012). Towards the “baby connectome”: mapping the structural connectivity of the newborn brain. PloS One 7, e31029. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  208. Van den Bergh BR & Mulder EJ (2012). Fetal sleep organization: a biological precursor of self‐regulation in childhood and adolescence? Biol Psychol 89, 584–590. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  209. van den Heuij LG, Wassink G, Gunn AJ & Bennet L (2016). Using pregnant sheep to model developmental brain damage. Neuromethods 109, 327–341. [Google Scholar]
  210. van den Hoogen A, Teunis CJ, Shellhaas RA, Pillen S, Benders M & Dudink J (2017). How to improve sleep in a neonatal intensive care unit: A systematic review. Early Hum Dev 113, 78–86. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  211. van Tilborg E, de Theije CGM, van Hal M, Wagenaar N, de Vries LS, Benders MJ, Rowitch DH & Nijboer CH (2017). Origin and dynamics of oligodendrocytes in the developing brain: Implications for perinatal white matter injury. Glia 66, 221–238. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  212. Vanhatalo S & Kaila K (2006). Development of neonatal EEG activity: from phenomenology to physiology. Semin Fetal Neonatal Med 11, 471–478. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  213. Vanhatalo S, Palva JM, Andersson S, Rivera C, Voipio J & Kaila K (2005). Slow endogenous activity transients and developmental expression of K+‐Cl cotransporter 2 in the immature human cortex. Eur J Neurosci 22, 2799–2804. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  214. Vecchierini MF, d'Allest AM & Verpillat P (2003). EEG patterns in 10 extreme premature neonates with normal neurological outcome: qualitative and quantitative data. Brain Dev 25, 330–337. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  215. Vetter C, Fischer D, Matera JL & Roenneberg T (2015). Aligning work and circadian time in shift workers improves sleep and reduces circadian disruption. Curr Biol 25, 907–911. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  216. Visser GH, Goodman JD, Levine DH & Dawes GS (1982). Diurnal and other cyclic variations in human fetal heart rate near term. Am J Obstet Gynecol 142, 535–544. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  217. Visser GH, Mulder EJ & Prechtl HF (1992). Studies on developmental neurology in the human fetus. Dev Pharmacol Ther 18, 175–183. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  218. Visser GH, Poelmann‐Weesjes G, Cohen TM & Bekedam DJ (1987). Fetal behavior at 30 to 32 weeks of gestation. Pediatr Res 22, 655–658. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  219. Walker AM, de Preu ND, Horne RS & Berger PJ (1990). Autonomic control of heart rate differs with electrocortical activity and chronic hypoxaemia in fetal lambs. J Dev Physiol 14, 43–48. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  220. Walker D, Grimwade J & Wood C (1971). The acoustic component of the foetal environment. J Reprod Fertil 24, 125–126. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  221. Walker DW, Lee B & Nitsos I (2000). Effect of hypoxia on respiratory activity in the foetus. Clin Exp Pharmacol Physiol 27, 110–113. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  222. Weisman O, Magori‐Cohen R, Louzoun Y, Eidelman AI & Feldman R (2011). Sleep‐wake transitions in premature neonates predict early development. Pediatrics 128, 706–714. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  223. Witcombe NB, Yiallourou SR, Sands SA, Walker AM & Horne RS (2012). Preterm birth alters the maturation of baroreflex sensitivity in sleeping infants. Pediatrics 129, e89–96. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  224. Wolke D, Sohne B, Riegel K, Ohrt B & Osterlund K (1998). An epidemiologic longitudinal study of sleeping problems and feeding experience of preterm and term children in southern Finland: comparison with a southern German population sample. J Pediatr 133, 224–231. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  225. Woodward LJ, Anderson PJ, Austin NC, Howard K & Inder TE (2006). Neonatal MRI to predict neurodevelopmental outcomes in preterm infants. N Engl J Med 355, 685–694. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  226. Yang JS, Nicholas CL, Nixon GM, Davey MJ, Anderson V, Walker AM, Trinder JA & Horne RS (2010). Determining sleep quality in children with sleep disordered breathing: EEG spectral analysis compared with conventional polysomnography. Sleep 33, 1165–1172. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  227. Yanni D, Korzeniewski S, Allred EN, Fichorova RN, O'Shea TM, Kuban K, Dammann O & Leviton A (2017). Both antenatal and postnatal inflammation contribute information about the risk of brain damage in extremely preterm newborns. Pediatr Res 82, 691–696. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  228. Yawno T, Hirst JJ, Castillo‐Melendez M & Walker DW (2009). Role of neurosteroids in regulating cell death and proliferation in the late gestation fetal brain. Neuroscience 163, 838–847. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  229. Yawno T, Miller SL, Bennet L, Wong F, Hirst JJ, Fahey M & Walker DW (2017). Ganaxolone: a new treatment for neonatal seizures. Front Cell Neurosci 11, 246. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  230. Yawno T, Yan EB, Hirst JJ & Walker DW (2011). Neuroactive steroids induce changes in fetal sheep behavior during normoxic and asphyxic states. Stress 14, 13–22. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  231. Yiallourou SR, Wallace EM, Whatley C, Odoi A, Hollis S, Weichard AJ, Muthusamy JS, Varma S, Cameron J, Narayan O & Horne RSC (2017). Sleep: a window into autonomic control in children born preterm and growth restricted. Sleep 40, zsx048. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  232. Yiallourou SR, Witcombe NB, Sands SA, Walker AM & Horne RS (2013). The development of autonomic cardiovascular control is altered by preterm birth. Early Hum Dev 89, 145–152. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  233. Zemdegs IZ, McMillen IC, Walker DW, Thorburn GD & Nowak R (1988). Diurnal rhythms in plasma melatonin concentrations in the fetal sheep and pregnant ewe during late gestation. Endocrinology 123, 284–289. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Articles from The Journal of Physiology are provided here courtesy of The Physiological Society

RESOURCES