Skip to main content
Health Science Reports logoLink to Health Science Reports
. 2018 Feb 21;1(3):e28. doi: 10.1002/hsr2.28

The factor structure of the Children's Eating Behaviour Questionnaire: A comparison of four models using confirmatory factor analysis

Urdur Njardvik 1,, Elin Kristin Klar 1, Fanney Thorsdottir 1
PMCID: PMC6266355  PMID: 30623064

Abstract

Aims

The Child Eating Behavior Questionnaire (CEBQ) consists of 8 subscales measuring different aspects of eating behavior and is a widely used instrument in pediatric settings, both in relation to eating disorders and overweight/obesity. However, despite its widespread usage, research results have, to this date, been inconsistent in regard to the factor structure of the CEBQ, with several factorial models suggested. The purpose of this study was to systematically compare the 4 factor structures commonly reported in the literature on the 35‐item CEBQ, using confirmatory factor analysis in the same sample.

Methods and results

In total, parents of 560 children aged 5 to 12 years old completed the CEBQ; 70 questionnaires were incomplete, resulting in a final sample of 490. Confirmatory factor analyses tested the 4 competing models: a 6‐factor model, 2 seven‐factor models, and an 8‐factor model. The 8‐factor model provided an acceptable fit to the data and turned out to be the best fitting model. Correlation coefficients between the 8 factors never exceeded r = .77, supporting the construct uniqueness of the 8 subscales. Results also indicated that the CEBQ subscales have good factorial validity and internal reliability (α ≥ .75).

Conclusion

In summary, this study of Icelandic children supports the appropriateness of using the CEBQ as a measure of 8 distinct dimensions of eating behavior style in school‐aged children.

Keywords: CEBQ, children, confirmatory factor analysis, eating behavior, factor structure, validation

Short abstract

Aims: The Child Eating Behavior Questionnaire (CEBQ) consists of 8 subscales measuring different aspects of eating behavior and is a widely used instrument in pediatric settings, both in relation to eating disorders and overweight/obesity. However, despite its widespread usage, research results have, to this date, been inconsistent in regard to the factor structure of the CEBQ, with several factorial models suggested. The purpose of this study was to systematically compare the 4 factor structures commonly reported in the literature on the 35‐item CEBQ, using confirmatory factor analysis in the same sample.

Methods and results: In total, parents of 560 children aged 5 to 12 years old completed the CEBQ; 70 questionnaires were incomplete, resulting in a final sample of 490. Confirmatory factor analyses tested the 4 competing models: a 6‐factor model, 2 seven‐factor models, and an 8‐factor model. The 8‐factor model provided an acceptable fit to the data and turned out to be the best fitting model. Correlation coefficients between the 8 factors never exceeded r = .77, supporting the construct uniqueness of the 8 subscales. Results also indicated that the CEBQ subscales have good factorial validity and internal reliability (α ≥ .75).

Conclusion: In summary, this study of Icelandic children supports the appropriateness of using the CEBQ as a measure of 8 distinct dimensions of eating behavior style in school‐aged children.

1. INTRODUCTION

Eating behavior problems are highly prevalent in young children and have been linked to underweight and poor growth, as well as to overweight and obesity.1, 2, 3, 4, 5 Although a wide range of prevalence rates have been reported because of variability in definition and heterogeneity in assessment methods, it is estimated that around 25% of normally developing children, and up to 80% of children with developmental delays, display some type of feeding problem.4, 6

The construct of eating behavior has been divided into 6 areas: satiety responsiveness, responsiveness to food cues, emotional eating, general interest in eating, speed of eating, and food fussiness.7 Eating behavior problems have been conceptualized as a spectrum, as they can range from the child being under‐ or overresponsive to cues, or eating too fast or too slow. Some of these behaviors are believed to be influenced by the caregiver, while others, such as suspiciousness of new foods, are conceptualized as having an evolutionary purpose.3, 4, 7, 8, 9 The 6 areas are not mutually exclusive, as behavior in one area may affect behavior in another. For example, a child displaying picky eating might complete a meal at a slower pace than a child with diminished response to satiety, leading to differences in speed of eating based on problems in 2 different areas.

One of the most widely used assessment instruments for eating behaviors in children is the Child Eating Behavior Questionnaire (CEBQ), a 35‐item parent‐report questionnaire.7 The CEBQ was based on the 6 eating behavior areas mentioned above, as well as on additional constructs derived from interviews with parents of young children: responsiveness to social factors, distractibility, and desire for drinks.7 Despite its widespread use and translation into several languages, the factor structure of the CEBQ has been a matter of debate. Initially, Wardle et al7 created a 57‐item scale, which was subsequently reduced to 35 items. The 57‐item version yielded 8 factors (food responsiveness [FR], enjoyment of food [EF], emotional overeating [EO], desire to drink [DD], satiety responsiveness [SR], slowness in eating [SE], emotional undereating [EU], and fussiness [FU]), while the 35‐item yielded 7 factors, maintaining the original structure, except that items on the SR and SE loaded on a single factor.7 However, SR and SE were retained as separate scales, as the correlation between the 2 was expected, as satiety response can be reflected in slower pace of eating over the course of a meal.7 Subsequent studies of the CEBQ's factor structure have provided mixed results, ranging from 3 to 8 factor solutions (see Table 1).

Table 1.

Factor analyses of the CEBQ (summary of previous studies)

Study Country Method n Age Factors Factors combined?
Wardle et al7 United Kingdom PCA 308 1‐9 7 SR and SE
Sleddens et al12 Netherlands PCA 135 6‐7 7 EO and FR
Viana et al14 Portugal PCA 240 3‐13 6 EF and FR/SR and SE
Svensson et al13 Sweden PCA 174 1‐6 7 EO and FR
Santos et al11 (2011) Chile PCA 294 6‐12 7 SR and SE
Cao et al10 China PCA 219 1‐1,5 7 FR split; SR/EF not found
Sparks and Radnitz5 United States PCA and CFA 229 2‐5 3a Multiple
Mallan et al16 Australia CFA 663 1‐5 8 N/A
Domoff et al17 United States CFA 1002 3‐4 8 N/A
Sirirassamee et al18 Thailand PCA 680 6‐11 8 N/A
a

The 3‐factor solution was based on a 15‐item version of the CEBQ.

Abbreviations: CEBQ, Child Eating Behavior Questionnaire; CFA, confirmatory factor analysis; EO, emotional overeating; EF, enjoyment of food; FR, food responsiveness; PCA, principal component analysis; SE, slowness in eating; SR, satiety responsiveness.

The 7‐factor solution has been supported most frequently, but only in studies using exploratory factor analysis (principal component analysis, PCA).10, 11, 12, 13 In these 7‐factor solutions, the EO and FR subscales have been combined into one, in both the Dutch version12 and the Swedish version,13 while the study on the Chilean version11 reported a combined SR and SE factor, in congruence with the original study by Wardle et al.7 Cao et al10 on the other hand reported a 7‐factor solution of the Chinese version, in which FR was split into 2 factors, and the SR and EF were not found. The Chinese version, therefore, appears to be substantially different from the other versions, additionally because their sample consisted of 12‐ to 18‐month‐old children, while the other studies had not only a wider age range in their sample (see Table 1) but also cultural differences in feeding practices, as explained by Cao et al.10 In the Portuguese version, and also using exploratory factor analysis, a 6‐factor solution was reported, with both EO/FR and SR/SE combined.14 Thus, except for the Chinese version, it appears that exploratory factor analyses have supported a 7‐factor solution, with either SR and SE or EO and FR combined, or a 6‐factor solution, with EO/FR and SR/SE combined.

More recent studies have reported somewhat different results using confirmatory factor analysis (CFA), which provides more accurate examination of the structure of the data and variable correlations than PCA.15 Sparks and Radnitz5 tested the 7‐factor model on 229 children in the United States and reported a poor fit. They subsequently performed an exploratory factor analysis, resulting in a proposal of a new 15‐item version with a 3‐factor solution. Mallan et al16 tested the 8‐factor model on 663 children in Australia and reported a good fit in an ethnically diverse sample. SR and SE were reported to be strongly correlated, which is in concordance with previous literature. Finally, in a study of 1002 preschool‐aged children in the United States, Domoff et al17 reported a reasonable fit for the 8‐factor model, with moderately strong correlations between SR and SE, as well as between EO and FR. The originally proposed 8‐factor structure7 has, thus, been supported in 2 different studies using confirmatory factor analyses, on children aged 5 years old or younger. Whether these results also apply to older children, however, is not clear. Although an 8‐factor solution was reported in a recent study of children aged 6 to 11 in Thailand,18 the results were based on exploratory factor analyses, as were the other previous studies including children in this age group.11, 14

The factor structure of the CEBQ, therefore, remains an unresolved issue. Ascertaining the number of discrete constructs a scale measures is of fundamental importance in both applied and scientific work. Combining possibly discrete subscales into one decreases the sensitivity of the CEBQ and, consequently, its usefulness in detecting problematic eating behaviors and measuring treatment gains. Moreover, this issue can impede interpretation of research results, as evidenced in a recent study by Mallan et al19 where the alleged multicollinearity issue between factors of the CEBQ rendered interpretation of results, difficult. The main purpose of this study was to test the 6‐, 7‐, and 8‐factor models of the CEBQ in a sample of 5‐ to 12‐year‐old children. Confirmatory factor analysis has not been used previously to test the CEBQ in this age group, nor has any previous study reported on comparing the fit of multiple models in the same sample. We aim to resolve the mixed previous literature on the number of factors in this study.

2. METHODS

2.1. Participants

Participants were parents of 560 children aged 5 to 12 years old, recruited from 4 elementary schools in the capital region of Iceland, both from the inner city and the suburbs. The questionnaires were sent home with 1331 children, and 560 (42%) were returned. No information was gathered on the children whose parents chose not to participate. Of the 560 children, 265 were girls (47.3%) and 260 were boys (46.4%); information on gender was missing for 35 (6.3%) children. A single parent per child completed the questionnaire. Of the 560 participating parents, 449 (80.2%) were mothers and 74 (13.2%) were fathers. Information on respondent's gender was missing for 37 parents (6.6%). Mean age of children was 8.53 years (SD 1.99) and parents was 38.33 years (SD 5.67; range 22‐59 y). As the Icelandic population is very homogeneous, with 92% of the population of Norse‐Celtic descent,20 questions about exogenous factors such as race and ethnicity were not included in the study. Information on other demographic variables was not collected.

2.2. Measures

The Children's Eating Behaviour Questionnaire (CEBQ) is a 35‐item parent‐report measure designed to assess 8 factors: food responsiveness, enjoyment of food, emotional overeating, desire to drink, satiety responsiveness, slowness in eating, emotional undereating, and fussiness.7 The CEBQ was translated into Icelandic by 2 experts in clinical psychology and 1 expert in anthropology. All 3 were native Icelandic speakers and fluent in English, had lived in the United States, and had extensive knowledge of both cultural environments. Translators were chosen based on Geisinger's21 guidelines, and the translation process was based on the guidelines set forth by the International Test Commission22 and Hambleton.23 The translators first made 3 individual translations of the CEBQ, and then met and discussed each item on the questionnaire to finally create a single version everyone agreed upon.

2.3. Procedure

First, IRB approval was obtained from the Icelandic Data Protection Authority. Participants were then recruited by sending the CEBQ home with children in grades 1 to 7 from participating schools, along with a letter explaining the study and asking parents/legal guardians to fill out the questionnaire and send it back to the school. The letter also explained that the action of returning the questionnaire was considered as consent to participate. The study was completely anonymous; apart from the CEBQ questions, participants were only asked about gender and age of both the respondent and their child.

2.4. Statistical analysis

In all, 70 respondents did not answer all of the items in the analysis and were, therefore, excluded from the analysis. Thus, the total number of answers analyzed was 490. Confirmatory factor analysis was used to evaluate the adequacy of the 4 different factor structures of the CEBQ scale that have been reported in the literature, using Mplus 7. The ordinal nature of the indicators was addressed by defining all of the indicators as categorical in Mplus. This uses the robust weighted least squares (WLSMV) estimator. The WLSMV estimator handles ordinal data well for moderately large samples.24 Three common fit indices were adopted to evaluate fit of the overall model: comparative fit index (CFI), Tucker‐Lewis index (TLI), and root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA). Although it is difficult to give precise cutoff values for these fit indices, the following general rules of thumb were used in the present study25: TLI/CFI > .95 (good fit), .90 to .95 (borderline fit), and < .90 (poor fit); RMSEA < .06 (good fit), .06 to .08 (fair fit), .08 to .10 (borderline fit), and >.10 (poor fit).

Four CFI models were tested. In the first model (model 1), the following 8 factors, reported in the original article from Wardle et al,7 were specified: food responsiveness (5 items, eg, my child's always asking for food), fussiness (6 items, eg, my child enjoys tasting new foods), enjoyment of food (4 items, eg, my child enjoys eating), desire to drink (3 items, eg, if given the chance, my child would always be having a drink), emotional undereating (4 items, eg, my child eats less when s/he is upset), emotional overeating (4 items, eg, my child eats more when anxious), satiety responsiveness (5 items, eg, my child has a big appetite), and slowness in eating (4 items, eg, my child finishes his/her meal quickly). The first model hypothesized a priori that 1) the responses to all items could be explained by the 8 factors, 2) each factor measure had a nonzero loading on the factor, and 3) the error terms of each of the measures were not correlated. The correlations between factors were freely estimated. In the second model (model 2), the 7‐factor model from Wardle et al7 and Santos et al11 was specified. The model tested assumed that the items originally measuring satiety responsiveness (SR) and slowness in eating (SE) had nonzero loadings on 1 factor (SR/SE). As before, the error terms of each of the items were not correlated and the correlations between factors were freely estimated. The third model (model 3) specified the 7 factors reported in the articles from Svensson et al13 and Sleddens et al.12 This model assumed that the items measuring the factors emotional overeating (EO) and food responsiveness (FR) loaded on 1 factor (EO/FR). Other specifications were identical to the specifications in the first model. In the fourth and final model (model 4), the 6 factors from Viana et al14 were hypothesized a priori. In this model, the items measuring the factors enjoyment of food (EF) and food responsiveness (FR) had nonzero loadings on 1 factor (EF/FR), and the items that measured the factors satiety responsiveness (SR) and slowness in eating (SE) had nonzero loadings on 1 factor (SR/SE). The error terms of each of the items were not allowed to correlate, and the correlations between factors were freely estimated.

Since the 7‐ and 6‐factor solutions are nested within the original 8‐factor solution,7 it is possible to test if the simpler solutions fit the data significantly worse. The Satorra‐Bentler chi‐square difference test was used to compare the fit of the nested models.26 However, since the chi‐square difference test is sensitive to sample size, the CFI was also used to test if the respecification of model 1 resulted in a significantly worse fit. To determine if an 8‐factor model should be rejected, a cutoff value of .01 was selected for the CFI difference test.27 Thus, a decrease in CFI higher than .01 suggests that the 8‐factor solution fits the data better than the 7‐ or the 6‐factor models. Value of .01 or lower, on the other hand, suggests that the simpler solution fits the data equally well as the 8‐factor model.

3. RESULTS

Table 2 shows the fit indices for each model and the results for the nested model comparisons. The original 8 factor model was first fitted. The CFI and TLI indices suggested that the fit was borderline acceptable and the RMSEA suggested that the fit was fair.

Table 2.

Goodness‐of‐fit indices of the confirmatory factor analysis

Model CFI TLI RMSEA CI for RMSEA ΔCFI SB χ2 df Δχ2 Δdf Model Selected
8 factorsa .92 .91 .076 (.073;.080) 2054.9 532
7 factorsb (SR and SE) .89 .88 .086 (.083‐.090) .03 2498.3 539 209.5 7 8 factors
7‐factorsc (EO and FR) .90 .90 .083 (.080‐.087) .02 2367.1 539 171.0 7 8 factors
6‐factorsd (EF and FR) (SR and SE) .79 .77 .12 (.117‐.124) .13 4423.7 545 855.5 13 8 factors

Abbreviations: Δχ2, delta chi‐square; ΔCFI, delta comparative fit index; Δdf, delta degrees of freedom; CFI, comparative fit index; CI, confidence interval; df, degrees of freedom; EO, emotional overeating; EF, enjoyment of food; FR, food responsiveness; RMSEA, root mean square error of approximation; SB χ2, Satorra‐Bentler chi‐square; SE, slowness in eating; SR, satiety responsiveness; TLI, Tucker‐Lewis Index.

a

Wardle et al7; Mallan et al16

b

Wardle et al7; Santos et al11.

c

Svensson et al13; Sleddens et al12

d

Viana et al14

Next, a 7‐factor model was fitted, where the original factors SR and the SE factors were merged into 1 factor. Both the CFI and TLI fit indices suggested a poor fit of this model and the RMSEA suggested that the fit was borderline. The Satorra‐Bentler chi‐square (SB χ2) difference test showed that the 7‐factor model fitted the data significantly worse than the 8‐factor model (P < .001). Moreover, the value of the CFI difference test for the 8‐ and the 7‐factor solutions was above the .01 cutoff value. The findings, therefore, supported the original 8‐factor solution over the 7‐factor model obtained by Wardle et al7 and Santos et al.11

When a model was fitted in which the EO and FR factors were merged into 1 factor, all the fit indices suggested that the fit of this 7‐factor model was borderline. The SB χ2 difference test showed that the fit of the 7‐factor (EO and FR) model fitted significantly worse than the original 8‐factor model, and the CFI difference value was above the cutoff value of .01. Again, the model comparison findings were in favor of the original 8‐factor model.

Finally, the 6‐factor model presented in Viana et al14 was estimated. In this model, the factors EF and FR, on the one hand, and SR and SE, on the other, were merged. All the fit indices indicated an unacceptable fit, and both the SB χ2 (P < .001) and CFI difference tests suggested that the 6‐factor model fitted the data significantly worse than the 8‐factor model. Thus, the final model selected was the original 8‐factor model.7

The factor loadings of the completely standardized solution for the selected 8‐factor model are summarized in Table 3. All the items loaded significantly (P < .001) on their respective factors and in the expected direction. The lowest correlation (r = .50) was between the item “My child eats less when s/he is tired” (item 11) and the EU factor, and the highest (.96) was between the item “If given the chance, my child would drink continuously throughout the day” (item 29) and the DD factor.

Table 3.

Factor loadings and Cronbach's alpha reliability estimates of CEBQ subscales

CEBQ Item Emotional Overeating (EO) Enjoyment of Food (EF) Satiety Responsiveness (SR) Slowness in Eating (SE) Food Responsiveness (FR) Desire to Drink (DD) Food Fussiness (FF) Emotional Undereating (EU)
2 .83
13 .82
15 .84
27 .84
1 .86
5 .75
20 .77
22 .88
3 .89
17 −.79
21 −.58
26 −.55
30 −.51
4 .83
8 −.85
18 −.67
35 −.60
12 .70
14 .84
19 .88
28 .60
34 .87
6 .63
29 .96
31 .95
7 .79
10 −.87
16 −.78
24 .77
32 −.91
33 .83
9 .84
11 .50
23 .70
25 .88
Cronbach's alpha .80 .84 .75 .77 .82 .84 .90 .79

Abbreviation: CEBQ, Child Eating Behavior Questionnaire.

The reliability of measurements obtained with different subscales was evaluated using Cronbach's alpha coefficient (Table 3). Cronbach's alpha ranged from .75 for the SR scale to .9 for the FF scale, suggesting acceptable reliability in all cases.

The correlation between the 8 latent factors and composite reliability for each scale is presented in Table 4. As expected, the relationships between SR and EF (r = .77), on the one hand, and FR and EO (r = .74), on the other, were quite strong, but not strong enough to suggest that construct redundancy is a problem for CEBQ. This conclusion is further supported by the fact that these factors are differently related with other factors measured with CEBQ. For instance, the correlation between SR and SE is r = .59, whereas the correlation between EF and SE is .32. Similarly, the correlation between FR and SR is r = .44, but the correlation between EO and SR is r = .19. Taken together, these findings support the conceptual uniqueness of the 8 factors measured with CEBQ.

Table 4.

Correlations between CEBQ latent factors

CEBQ Scale EO EF SR SE FR DD FF EU
Emotional overeating (EO)
Enjoyment of food (EF) 0.25***
Satiety responsiveness (SR) 0.190*** 0.767***
Slowness in eating (SE) 0.111* 0.321*** 0.589***
Food responsiveness (FR) 0.740*** 0.465*** 0.439*** 0.244***
Desire to drink (DD) 0.424*** 0.028 −0.020 −0.052 0.543***
Food fussiness (FF) 0.111 −0.615*** −0.476*** −0.159** 0.056 0.110*
Emotional undereating (EU) 0.614*** −0.053 −0.267*** −0.177*** 0.294*** 0.292*** 0.220***

Abbreviation: CEBQ, Child Eating Behavior Questionnaire.

*

P < .05.

**

P < .01.

***

P < .001.

4. DISCUSSION

Targeting eating behaviors in children is important to prevent health‐related problems, such as poor growth and obesity,1, 2, 4 and a valid instrument to measure eating behaviors that are potentially health related is a prerequisite for detection and monitoring of children at risk. The 35‐item Children's Eating Behavior Questionnaire (CEBQ), one of the most widely used measures of eating behaviors in children, is founded on an 8‐factor conceptualization of eating styles.7 However, factor‐analytic studies of the 35‐item CEBQ have caused controversy over the underlying factor structure and have raised concerns regarding the validity of the scale.

In the present study, 4 different CFA models of the CEBQ were examined based on previous research: 1) the original 8‐factor model,7 2) a 7‐factor model where the items originally measuring satiety responsiveness and slowness in eating were presumed to measure one factor,7, 11 3) a 7‐factor model where the items measuring emotional overeating and food responsiveness were hypothesized to measure 1 factor,12, 13 and finally 4) a 6‐factor model where the factors enjoyment of food and food responsiveness, on the one hand, and satiety responsiveness and slowness in eating, on the other, were merged into 2 factors.14 The models were tested on a sample of primary caregivers who rated the eating behaviors of their children aged 5 to 12.

The 8‐factor model fitted the data reasonably well, and so did the 7‐factor model where satiety responsiveness and slowness in eating were merged into 1 factor. The 8‐factor model, however, fitted the data better. These findings are not congruent with the findings of Santos et al11 who examined the factor structure of CEBQ in a Chilean sample of the same age group. The fit of the other 7 factor model, where emotional overeating (EO) and food responsiveness (FR) were merged into 1 factor, was also acceptable, but again, worse than the 8 factor model. These results differ from those of Sleddens et al12 and Svensson et al,13 who factor analyzed the CEBQ items in samples of children from the Netherlands and Sweden. In both of those studies, the findings suggested that the items measuring EO and FR should load on one and the same factor.

When the 6‐factor model was fitted to the data, the results showed a poor fit and a considerably worse fit than the 8‐factor model, contrary to the findings of Viana et al.14 They inspected the factor structure of the CEBQ in a sample of Portuguese children and their findings suggested that the factors enjoyment of food and food responsiveness should be merged into 1 factor, and the same applied to the factors satiety responsiveness and slowness in eating. There are probably many reasons for why the findings of the present study diverge from the above‐mentioned ones. With the exception of Santos et al,11 these studies focused on age groups that differed from the one in this study. Moreover, all of these studies used exploratory factor analysis (PCA), whereas CFA was used in the present study. Finally, the sample size in most of these studies was quite small for the analysis conducted, increasing the risk of obtaining unstable factor structures. This reason for why our findings diverge from those of these prior studies is supported by the fact that our findings concur with previous studies that have used larger samples using either CFA or PCA, and samples of different age groups.16, 17, 18 Taken together, these results support Wardle et al's7 original theorization of eating styles and the conceptual uniqueness of the 8 factors measured with CEBQ.

The validity of the 8 factor structure was further corroborated with the fact that the correlation coefficients between the 8 factors never exceeded .77 (for enjoyment of food and satiety responsiveness). These findings do not support the claim that construct redundancy is a problem for the CEBQ but rather support a theoretical link between constructs. The link between finding food reinforcing and consuming larger portions is well documented, both in children and adults.28, 29 Reinforcing value of food has been defined as behavior indicating preference for food over nonfood alternatives,28 and items on the enjoyment of food subscale (eg, looks forward to mealtime, loves food, and enjoys eating) seem to reflect such preferences. Items on the satiety responsiveness, on the other hand, mostly reflect the amount of food consumed (eg, leaving food on plate, becoming full easily), and as there is a correlation between finding food reinforcing and the amount of food consumed, a correlation between enjoyment of food and satiety responsiveness would be expected. The conclusion that these concepts are unique is further supported by the fact that enjoyment of food and satiety responsiveness are differently related to other factors measured by the CEBQ. For instance, the correlation between satiety responsiveness and slowness in eating is r = .59, whereas the correlation between enjoyment of food and slowness in eating is .32. Both satiety response and slowness in eating can be categorized as traits related to appetite control and, as discussed earlier, are expected to correlate, as satiety response can be reflected in a slower pace of eating over the course of a meal. Enjoyment of food, on the other hand, captures the subjective experience from eating and does not necessarily affect the pace.7, 30 Thus, different correlations between these variables are to be expected.

To further explore the psychometric properties of the Icelandic version of the CEBQ, reliability estimates and factorial validity were examined. The reliability coefficients for all 8 subscales were high and comparable with previous studies,7, 12, 13, 16, 17, 18 and all of the items loaded highly on their respective factor in the 8‐factor model, demonstrating good factorial validity.

Two main limitations of this study should be noted. First, the lack of random sampling is a limitation, as participants were recruited using a convenience sampling technique. Furthermore, even though the sample was moderately large, research on the relationship between sample size and number of parameter estimates, number of indicator variables per factor, and communalities suggest that the sample in the current study may have potentially been too small.31, 32 Secondly, external variables measuring concepts theoretically related to eating response styles were not included in the study. Previous research has, for instance, examined the relationship between various factors of the CEBQ and child weight status,3, 5, 10, 13, 14, 17 but only Domoff et al17 and McCarthy et al33 have used all 8 subscales. It is, therefore, of interest to examine the relationship between all 8 dimensions of eating response styles and weight‐related variables in future research, to further explore the validity of CEBQ. It is also important to note that even though the 8‐factor model fitted the data better than the other 3 models, the difference in fit measures for the 8‐factor model and the two 7‐factor models was small. Nevertheless, the 8‐factor model did fit the data significantly better than the other two, and the correlational analysis also supported the conceptual uniqueness of all 8 factors. This does not exclude the possibility of another true population model, which would likely allow cross‐loadings. However, according to the principle of parsimony the simplest possible model should be chosen, which, based on the current data, is the 8‐factor model.

Despite these limitations, the present study is the first to compare and test systematically different factor structures of CEBQ that have been obtained in previous studies and to provide rigorous evidence supporting the validity of the original 8‐factor structure of the CEBQ.

5. CONCLUSION

This study supports the original 8‐factor structure of the CEBQ in a sample of school‐aged children. Based on these results, we advise against combining any of the 8 subscales, as our findings suggest that all the subscales measure discrete constructs. It also suggests that the instrument functions adequately in this population and seems to have similar psychometric properties as the original UK version. The results, therefore, support the use of the instrument as a measure of 8 distinct eating style dimensions in the population of children 5 to 12 years old.

CONFLICT OF INTEREST

None declared.

AUTHOR CONTRIBUTIONS

Conceptualization: Urdur Njardvik and Fanney Thorsdottir

Investigation: Elin Klar and Urdur Njardvik

Formal Analysis: Fanney Thorsdottir

Writing ‐ original draft preparation: Urdur Njardvik and Fanney Thorsdottir

Writing ‐ review and editing: Urdur Njardvik and Fanney Thorsdottir

ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS

The authors would like to thank the parents who participated in the study and the staff of the participating schools for their help during data collection.

APPENDIX A.

A.1.

Number Item content (my child …)
1 Loves food
2 Eats more when worried
3 Has a big appetite
4 Finishes his/her meal quickly
5 Is interested in food
6 Is always asking for a drink
7 Refuses new foods at first
8 Eats slowly
9 Eats less when angry
10 Enjoys tasting new foods
11 Eats less when she/he is tired
12 Is always asking for food
13 Eats more when annoyed
14 If allowed to, would eat too much
15 Eats more when anxious
16 Enjoys a wide variety of foods
17 Leaves food on his/her plate at the end of a meal
18 Takes more than 30 min to finish a meal
19 Given the choice, would eat most of the time
20 Looks forward to mealtimes
21 Gets full before his/her meal is finished
22 Enjoys eating
23 Eats more when she/he is happy
24 Is difficult to please with meals
25 Eats less when upset
26 Gets full easily
27 Eats more when she/he has nothing else to do
28 Even if full she/he finds room to eat his/her favorite food
29 If given the chance, would drink continuously throughout the day
30 Cannot eat a meal if she/he has had a snack just before
31 If given the chance, would always be having a drink
32 Is interested in tasting food she/he hasn't tasted before
33 Decides that she/he doesn't like a food, even without tasting it
34 If given the chance, would always have food in his/her mouth
35 Eats more and more slowly during the course of a meal

Njardvik U, Klar EK, Thorsdottir F. The factor structure of the Children's Eating Behaviour Questionnaire: A comparison of four models using confirmatory factor analysis. Health Sci Rep. 2018;1:e28 10.1002/hsr2.28

REFERENCES

  • 1. Dubois L, Farmer A, Girard M, Peterson K. Preschool children's eating behaviours are related to dietary adequacy and body weight. Eur J Clin Nutr. 2007;61(7):846. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2. Finistrella V, Manco M, Ferrara A, Rustico C, Presaghi F, Morino G. Cross‐sectional exploration of maternal reports of food neophobia and pickiness in preschooler‐mother dyads. J Am Coll Nutr. 2012;31(3):152‐159. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3. Jansen PW, Roza SJ, Jaddoe VW, et al. Children's eating behavior, feeding practices of parents and weight problems in early childhood: results from the population‐based Generation R Study. Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act. 2012;9(1):130. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4. Taylor CM, Wernimont SM, Northstone K, Emmett PM. Picky/fussy eating in children: review of definitions, assessment, prevalence and dietary intakes. Appetite. 2015;95:349‐359. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5. Sparks MA, Radnitz CL. Confirmatory factor analysis of the Children's Eating Behaviour Questionnaire in a low‐income sample. Eat Behav. 2012;13(3):267‐270. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6. Bryant‐Waugh R, Markham L, Kreipe RE, Walsh BT. Feeding and eating disorders in childhood. Int J Eating Disorders. 2010;43(2):98‐111. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7. Wardle J, Guthrie CA, Sanderson S, Rapoport L. Development of the children's eating behaviour questionnaire. J Child Psychol Psychiat Allied Disciplines. 2001;42(7):963‐970. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8. Faith MS, Scanlon KS, Birch LL, Francis LA, Sherry B. Parent‐child feeding strategies and their relationships to child eating and weight status. Obesity. 2004;12(11):1711‐1722. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9. Wardle J, Carnell S. Parental feeding practices and children's weight. Acta Paediatr. 2007;96:5‐11. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10. Cao Y‐T, Svensson V, Marcus C, Zhang J, Zhang J‐D, Sobko T. Eating behaviour patterns in Chinese children aged 12‐18 months and association with relative weight‐factorial validation of the Children's Eating Behaviour Questionnaire. Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act. 2012;9(1):5. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11. Santos JL, Ho‐Urriola JA, González A, et al. Association between eating behavior scores and obesity in Chilean children. Nutr J. 2011;10(1):108. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12. Sleddens EF, Kremers SP, Thijs C. The Children's Eating Behaviour Questionnaire: factorial validity and association with body mass index in Dutch children aged 6–7. Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act. 2008;5(1):49. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13. Svensson V, Lundborg L, Cao Y, Nowicka P, Marcus C, Sobko T. Obesity related eating behaviour patterns in Swedish preschool children and association with age, gender, relative weight and parental weight‐factorial validation of the Children's Eating Behaviour Questionnaire. Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act. 2011;8(1):134. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14. Viana V, Sinde S, Saxton J. Children's Eating Behaviour Questionnaire: associations with BMI in Portuguese children. Br J Nutr. 2008;100(2):445‐450. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15. Kim H‐J. Common factor analysis versus principal component analysis: choice for symptom cluster research. Asian Nurs Res. 2008;2(1):17‐24. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16. Mallan KM, Liu W‐H, Mehta RJ, Daniels LA, Magarey A, Battistutta D. Maternal report of young children's eating styles. Validation of the Children's Eating Behaviour Questionnaire in three ethnically diverse Australian samples. Appetite. 2013;64:48‐55. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17. Domoff SE, Miller AL, Kaciroti N, Lumeng JC. Validation of the Children's Eating Behaviour Questionnaire in a low‐income preschool‐aged sample in the United States. Appetite. 2015;95:415‐420. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18. Sirirassamee T, Hunchangsith P. Children's eating behavior questionnaire: factorial validation and differences in sex and educational level in thai school‐age children. Southeast Asian J Trop Med Public Health. 2016;47(6):1325‐1334. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19. Mallan KM, Nambiar S, Magarey AM, Daniels LA. Satiety responsiveness in toddlerhood predicts energy intake and weight status at four years of age. Appetite. 2014;74:79‐85. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20. Statistics‐Iceland . Iceland in Figures. Reykjavik: Statistics Iceland; 2017. [Google Scholar]
  • 21. Geisinger KF. Cross‐cultural normative assessment: translation and adaptation issues influencing the normative interpretation of assessment instruments. Psychol Assess. 1994;6(4):304. [Google Scholar]
  • 22. International‐Test‐Commission . ITC guidelines for translating and adapting tests. (Document Reference: ITC‐G‐TA‐20140617) 2005.
  • 23. Hambleton RK. Issues, designs, and technical guidelines for adapting tests into multiple languages and cultures. Adapting educational and psychological tests for cross‐cultural assessment. 2005;1:3‐38. [Google Scholar]
  • 24. Flora DB, Curran PJ. An empirical evaluation of alternative methods of estimation for confirmatory factor analysis with ordinal data. Psychol Methods. 2004;9(4):466. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25. Hu L, Bentler PM. Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis: conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Struct Equ Model Multidiscip J. 1999;6(1):1‐55. [Google Scholar]
  • 26. Muthen L, Muthen B. Chi‐square difference testing using the SB scaled chi‐square. Note on Mplus website; 2005.
  • 27. Cheung GW, Rensvold RB. Evaluating goodness‐of‐fit indexes for testing measurement invariance. Structural equation modeling. 2002;9(2):233‐255. [Google Scholar]
  • 28. Temple JL, Legierski CM, Giacomelli AM, Salvy S‐J, Epstein LH. Overweight children find food more reinforcing and consume more energy than do nonoverweight children. Am J Clin Nutr. 2008;87(5):1121‐1127. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29. Drewnowski A. Taste preferences and food intake. Annu Rev Nutr. 1997;17(1):237‐253. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30. Llewellyn CH, van Jaarsveld CH, Johnson L, Carnell S, Wardle J. Development and factor structure of the Baby Eating Behaviour Questionnaire in the Gemini birth cohort. Appetite. 2011;57(2):388‐396. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31. MacCallum RC, Widaman KF, Preacher KJ, Hong S. Sample size in factor analysis: the role of model error. Multivar Behav Res. 2001;36(4):611‐637. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32. MacCallum RC, Widaman KF, Zhang S, Hong S. Sample size in factor analysis. Psychol Methods. 1999;4(1):84. [Google Scholar]
  • 33. McCarthy EK, Cn C, Murray DM, Hourihane JOB, Kenny LC, Kiely M. Eating behaviour and weight status at 2 years of age: data from the Cork BASELINE Birth Cohort Study. European Journal Of Clinical Nutrition. 2015;69:1356. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Articles from Health Science Reports are provided here courtesy of Wiley

RESOURCES