Skip to main content
Nutrients logoLink to Nutrients
. 2018 Nov 2;10(11):1624. doi: 10.3390/nu10111624

The Impact of Front-of-Package Label Design on Consumer Understanding of Nutrient Amounts

Samantha Goodman 1, Lana Vanderlee 1, Rachel Acton 1, Syed Mahamad 1, David Hammond 1,*
PMCID: PMC6266389  PMID: 30400146

Abstract

A between-groups experiment examined the salience of front-of-package (FOP) symbols. Adults from Canada, the US, Australia, and the UK completed an online survey (n = 11,617). Respondents were randomized to view cereal boxes displaying one of 11 FOP label conditions for ‘high’ levels of sugar and saturated fat: control (no FOP symbol), red circle, red ‘stop sign’, magnifying glass, magnifying glass + exclamation mark, and ‘caution’ triangle + exclamation mark, plus each of these five conditions accompanied by a ‘high in’ text descriptor. Participants identified the amount of saturated fat and sugar in the product (‘low’/’moderate’/’high’). Participants were more likely to correctly identify the product as ‘high’ in saturated fat or sugar when shown the stop sign, triangle + exclamation mark, red circle, or magnifying glass + exclamation mark symbols incorporating ‘high in’ text (p < 0.01). The magnifying glass was the least effective symbol. The stop sign (37.7%) and triangle + exclamation mark (22.0%) were most frequently selected as the best symbol for indicating high nutrient amounts. Overall, FOP labels with ‘high in’ descriptions, red color and intuitive ‘warning’ symbols (e.g., stop signs, exclamation marks, ‘caution’ triangles) were more effective at communicating high levels of nutrients of public health concern in a time-limited environment.

Keywords: diet, adult, front-of-package, nutrition label, experiment

1. Introduction

The global health burden from poor diets is increasing [1]. Frequent consumption of foods high in saturated fat, sugars, or sodium can lead to overweight or obesity, hypertension, and cardiovascular disease [2]. This is a concern in many countries, including in Canada, where approximately two thirds of the adult population have overweight or obesity [3,4].

Nutrition labels are a prominent population-level intervention for communicating nutrition information. Food labels are notable for their reach—more Canadians report using nutrition information from food labels on prepackaged foods than from any other source—as well as their timing of exposure at the point-of-sale and consumption [5,6,7,8]. To date, prepackaged foods in most countries display ingredient lists and quantitative information on nutrient amounts. In Canada, the Nutrition Facts table (NFt) on prepackaged foods represents a prominent, credible source of nutrition information [9,10,11,12,13,14,15,16,17]. Nevertheless, research demonstrates that consumers have difficulty understanding and applying the information provided in the NFt [18,19,20], including identifying whether nutrient amounts are ‘high’ or ‘low’ compared to daily guidelines [21,22].

Front-of-package (FOP) nutrition labels seek to provide simple nutrition information in a more accessible location than NFts, which are typically displayed on the back or side of packages. A wide variety of FOP systems have been developed, including ‘high in’ labels, which seek to identify foods high in nutrients of public health concern [23,24]. Chile was the first country to implement mandatory FOP labels that signaled ‘high’ levels of calories, sugar, saturated fat, and sodium. The Government of Canada has committed to implementing a similar ‘high in’ FOP system [25,26] to inform consumers and encourage the reformulation of prepackaged foods [25]. In their consultation to identify an appropriate FOP label design, Health Canada included four alternative approaches using different symbols to highlight ‘high’ levels of saturated fat, sodium, and sugar [27], including the use of triangles, circles, and a magnifying glass.

Research has identified several characteristics that increase the effectiveness of FOP systems, including the use of recognizable symbols that are easy to understand [28]. The use of color has also been shown to increase attention to FOP labels [29,30] and may help consumers to make healthier choices [31,32,33,34]. Simple directional text, such as ‘high in (nutrient)’ is supported by consumers and has been shown to increase understanding of the healthfulness of products [35,36]. FOP labels that combine color with simple text descriptors may also enhance understanding of nutrient amounts [37]. Collectively, these design features influence the salience of FOP labels—the extent to which they are noticed—which is an important component of effective product labels [38,39]. Several methods have been used to test the salience of FOP designs, including eye-tracking studies that examine how long consumers spend looking at various types of labels [40,41,42,43]. Other studies have used quantitative or qualitative survey approaches to explore consumers’ perceptions of the ‘noticeability’ of different FOP labels [27,44]. Experimental tasks that involve time constraints are a particularly effective test of FOP salience, given that the average consumer typically spends fewer than 10 s viewing and assessing a food product label on any given shopping trip [6,45]. The current study employed a between-groups experimental task to test the salience of different FOP labeling designs, including different symbols, colors, and text descriptors, and whether this translates to immediate understanding of nutrient levels. It was hypothesized that more salient (i.e., noticeable) labels would be associated with a greater understanding of nutrient levels.

2. Materials and Methods

The current study was conducted as part of the International Food Policy Study in Australia, Canada, the UK, and the USA. Data were collected via self-completed web-based surveys conducted in December 2017 with adults aged 18–64 years. Respondents were recruited through the Nielsen Consumer Insights Global Panel [46] and partner panels. Email invitations with unique survey access links were sent to a random sample of panelists within the specified age and country criteria; panelists known to be ineligible were not invited. Surveys were conducted in English, French, or Spanish (based on the panelist’s known language preference); the experimental task described below was administered in English surveys only. Mean survey time across countries was 33 min.

Respondents provided consent prior to completing the survey, and received remuneration in accordance with their panel’s usual incentive structure (e.g., points-based or monetary rewards, chances to win prizes). The study was reviewed by and received ethics clearance through a University of Waterloo Research Ethics Committee (ORE# 21460). A full description of the study methods and measures can be found in the study’s technical report [47].

The current study presents findings from a between-groups experimental task that tested functional understanding of various FOP labels. Participants viewed an image of a cereal box displayed on the screen for 4 s. Cereal boxes displayed FOP labels for sugar and saturated fat according to the experimental condition to which the participant was randomized: (0) control (no FOP label); (1) red circle; (2) red stop sign; (3) magnifying glass; (4) magnifying glass + exclamation mark; and (5) ‘caution’ triangle + exclamation mark. Each of the five FOP symbols was also displayed with added ‘high in’ text, for a total of 11 experimental conditions (Figure 1). All label designs were modelled after early iterations of FOP symbols proposed by Health Canada [25,48,49].

Figure 1.

Figure 1

Images with front-of-package (FOP) labels displayed on screen for 4 s during experimental task. Note that brand names and logos have been blinded for publication but were shown to participants.

After the image disappeared from the screen, participants were asked the following question for questions on saturated fat and sugar: “Is this amount of (saturated fat/sugar) in the product…? (‘Low’, ‘Moderate’, ‘High’, ‘Don’t know’, ‘Refuse’)”, with the correct response being ‘High’. For the purpose of this study, the product was considered to be ‘high’ in saturated fat and sugar.

Following the experimental task, respondents viewed all five FOP designs and were asked, “Which is the best symbol for informing consumers that a product is ‘high in’ saturated fat and sugar?” Participants selected one of the five symbols, displayed in a random position on the screen (Figure 2).

Figure 2.

Figure 2

Front-of-package symbols * displayed on screen for the question, “Which is the best symbol for informing consumers that a product is ‘high in’ saturated fat and sugar?”. * Symbols presented in this question are identical to the five FOP symbol designs (i.e., without ‘high in’ text) tested in the experimental task (see Figure 1). Note that the government attribution (‘Health Canada’) was present for all participants, regardless of country of origin.

3. Data Analysis

Analysis was conducted on a sample of 11,317 respondents who provided complete data on all measures. Chi-square tests were conducted to test for differences in sociodemographic factors (age, sex, education, and country) between experimental conditions. Separate logistic regression models were used to test for differences between conditions in the odds of responding correctly to the experimental questions on saturated fat and sugar; those who refused to answer were excluded. Models were adjusted for age group, sex, country, and highest education level (recoded to ‘Low’, ‘Middle’, or ‘High’ due to cross-country differences). Analyses were conducted using SPSS for Windows version 25.0 (IBM Corp., Armonk, NY, USA); p < 0.05 was considered significant.

4. Results

Sample characteristics are reported in Table 1. Chi-square tests revealed no significant differences in sociodemographic covariates between experimental conditions. For demographic data by country, see Table S1 (Supplementary Material).

Table 1.

Sample characteristics (n = 11,617).

Variable % (n)
Sex
Male 47.1% (5470)
Female 52.9% (6147)
Age (years)
18–24 11.3% (1314)
25–30 33.1% (3849)
31–39 11.8% (1365)
40–49 12.5% (1455)
50–59 17.9% (2083)
60–64 13.4% (1551)
Education level
Low 22.8% (2634)
Middle 27.7% (3202)
High 49.5% (5712)
Country
USA 33.2% (3855)
Canada 7.0% (815)
Australia 28.4% (3302)
UK 31.4% (3645)

4.1. Functional Understanding of FOP Labels (Experimental Task)

Table 2 shows the percentage of correct responses to the ‘high in’ saturated fat and sugar questions by experimental condition. In all cases, respondents shown the no-FOP control condition performed more poorly than those shown FOP symbols. Across conditions, responses to the question were as follow: Saturated fat: ‘Low’ (24.5%), ‘Moderate’ (26.1%), ‘High’ (7.9%), Don’t know (40.9%), Refuse (0.5%); sugar: ‘Low’ (15.7%), ‘Moderate’ (29.5%), ‘High’ (15.1%), Don’t know (39.2%), Refuse (0.5%).

Table 2.

Percentage of correct responses for each nutrient of concern by experimental condition * (n = 11,617).

FOP Experimental Condition Saturated Fat Sugar
Control (no FOP Label) 4.8% 12.3%
Magnifying glass
No ‘high in’ text 5.3% 13.5%
‘High in’ text 6.5% 14.5%
Magnifying glass + exclamation mark
No ‘high in’ text 5.6% 12.4%
‘High In’ text 8.9% 17.0%
Red circle
No ‘high in’ text 5.9% 12.8%
‘High in’ text 10.3% 17.4%
Triangle + exclamation mark
No ‘high in’ text 7.8% 15.0%
‘High in’ text 11.1% 18.5%
Red stop sign
No ‘high in’ text 8.8% 15.8%
‘High in’ text 12.7% 18.0%

FOP, front-of-package. * Responses to question, “Is this amount of (saturated fat/sugar) in the product…?” (Low, Moderate, High, Don’t know, Refuse to answer). Correct response: ‘High’; ‘Don’t know’ coded as incorrect; ‘Refuse to answer’ excluded from analyses.

Table 3 displays the results of the logistic regression (main effects model) which examined the effect of the various FOP label designs compared to the no-FOP control. For additional pairwise contrasts between FOP labels, see Table S2 (Supplementary Material). Participants who viewed the red stop sign, caution triangle + exclamation mark, red circle, or magnifying glass + exclamation mark FOP symbols incorporating ‘high in’ text were more likely to correctly identify the cereal as high in saturated fat and sugar compared to those who saw the no-FOP control. Notably, the red stop sign was effective even without ‘high in’ text. In contrast, those who saw the magnifying glass FOP labels had the lowest odds of responding correctly. Across all FOP designs, respondents who viewed labels with ‘high in’ text had greater odds of responding correctly: when ‘high in’ text was absent, the red circle and magnifying glass + exclamation mark symbols no longer significantly outperformed the control.

Table 3.

Odds (OR, 95%CI) of a correct response * for each nutrient of concern (n = 11,617) **.

Variable Saturated Fat Sugar
FOP label design Χ2 (10) = 98.50 c Χ2 (10) = 44.33 c
Control (ref) - -
Magnifying glass 1.05 (0.71, 1.57) 1.07 (0.82, 1.39)
Magnifying glass + ‘High in’ 1.41 (0.97, 2.05) 1.21 (0.94, 1.56)
Magnifying glass + Exclamation mark 1.17 (0.79, 1.72) 0.99 (0.76, 1.28)
Magnifying glass + Exclamation mark + ‘High in’ 1.94 (1.36, 2.76) c 1.43 (1.12, 1.82) b
Red circle 1.22 (0.83, 1.79) 1.03 (0.79, 1.33)
Red circle + ‘High in’ 2.29 (1.61, 3.23) c 1.48 (1.16, 1.89) b
Caution triangle + Exclamation mark 1.68 (1.17, 2.41) b 1.21 (0.94, 1.56)
Caution triangle + Exclamation mark + ‘High in’ 2.51 (1.77, 3.55) c 1.60 (1.25, 2.05) c
Red stop sign 1.92 (1.34, 2.75) c 1.34 (1.04, 1.72) a
Red stop sign + ‘High in’ 2.95 (2.10, 4.15) c 1.59 (1.25, 2.03) c
Sex Χ2 (1) = 0.23 Χ2 (1) = 1.29
Male (ref) - -
Female 1.04 (0.90, 1.19) 1.06 (0.96, 1.18)
Age (years) Χ2 (5) = 41.62 c Χ2 (5) = 18.10 b
18–24 (ref) - -
25–30 1.07 (0.85, 1.35) 0.79 (0.67, 0.95) a
31–39 0.99 (0.75, 1.30) 0.87 (0.71, 1.07)
40–49 0.78 (0.59, 1.03) 0.79 (0.64, 0.97) a
50–59 0.63 (0.48, 0.83) b 0.69 (0.57, 0.84) c
60–64 0.56 (0.41, 0.76) c 0.69 (0.56, 0.86) b
Education Χ2 (2) = 6.33 a Χ2 (2) = 22.43 c
Low (ref) - -
Middle 1.03 (0.85, 1.24) 1.00 (0.86, 1.16)
High 1.26 (1.06, 1.49) a 1.30 (1.13, 1.48) c
Country Χ2 (3) = 64.98 c Χ2 (3) = 190.20 c
USA (ref) - -
Canada 2.74 (2.11, 3.55) c 3.28 (2.70, 3.98) c
Australia 1.03 (0.85, 1.24) 0.99 (0.85, 1.14)
UK 1.26 (1.06, 1.50) b 1.55 (1.36, 1.77) c

95% CI, 95% confidence intervals; Χ2, chi-square; FOP, front-of-package; OR, odds ratio; ref, reference group. * Responses to question, “Is this amount of (saturated fat/sugar) in the product…? (Low, Moderate, High, Don’t know, Refuse to answer)”. Correct response: ‘High’; ‘Don’t know’ coded as incorrect; ‘Refuse to answer’ excluded from analyses ** Χ2 (df) reported for main effects; OR (95% CI) reported for pairwise contrasts. Significant effects are indicated in bold; reference categories are denoted with “-“; superscript letters a, b, and c indicate significance at p < 0.05, p < 0.01, and p < 0.001, respectively.

Younger (18–24 year-old) respondents had a higher likelihood of responding correctly compared to the oldest age groups. Those with a high level of education were significantly more likely to respond correctly compared to those with a low level. Finally, compared to those from the USA, respondents from Canada and the UK had a higher likelihood of responding correctly.

4.2. Perceived Effectiveness of FOP Designs

Figure 3 shows the percentage of respondents who selected each FOP symbol as being the best for informing consumers that a product is ‘high in’ saturated fat or sugar. The red stop sign (37.7%) and the triangle + exclamation mark (22.0%) were the most popular symbols; the magnifying glass (4.2%) was the least frequently selected.

Figure 3.

Figure 3

Responses to perceptions task: most informative front-of-package symbol (n = 11,617).

5. Discussion

The design of FOP labels can have a measurable impact on consumer understanding even under highly restricted time limits. For all symbols tested except the magnifying glass, the presence of ‘high in’ text was associated with an increased likelihood of correctly identifying the cereal as being high in saturated fat and sugar. The inclusion of these descriptors may have been particularly important, given that all of the FOP symbols tested in the study would have been novel to participants.

The specific type of FOP symbol also influenced the likelihood that participants could identify a food as high in saturated fat or sugar. Compared to the control condition with no FOP label, participants who viewed the red stop sign with the ‘high in’ descriptor were almost three times as likely to report that the cereal was high in saturated fat and 1.6 times more likely to report that it was high in sugar. The red circle symbol and an exclamation mark placed within either a ‘caution’ triangle or a magnifying glass were also associated with more correct responses. Stop signs, ‘caution’ triangles, and exclamation marks are all familiar symbols that communicate warning. The intuitive meaning of these symbols is illustrated by the fact that for the question on saturated fat, the stop sign and caution triangle + exclamation mark outperformed the no-FOP control even in the absence of ‘high in’ text. In other words, the symbols themselves communicated the fundamental concept of warning. In contrast, the simple magnifying glass—which was recommended to Health Canada by the food industry [48]—was the least effective of all FOP label designs, likely because magnifying glasses have no intuitive association with high levels or the concept of ‘warning’. These findings are generally consistent with previous findings using behavioral purchase tasks, in which consumers who saw an exclamation mark FOP symbol made healthier purchases than those who saw other FOP labeling designs [50], as well as research indicating that Canadians prefer ‘high in’ FOP symbols featuring exclamation mark or triangle symbols [27]. In addition, the FOP symbols that performed best in the experimental task (red stop sign and triangle + exclamation mark) were also the most popular in the perceptions task. Although the effect of color was not systematically varied in the current study, the use of red color in the circle symbol may account for its superiority relative to the control condition, despite the fact that it did not incorporate a warning symbol [29,30].

Overall, the proportion of correct responses was fairly low (<20%); however, in this study design, the FOP labels were tested in a time-constrained setting to examine differences in the salience of warnings. Indeed, an important component of FOP systems is their use of symbols or images to engage consumers and communicate nutrient information ‘at a glance’ [28]. Therefore, the absolute level of correct responses is of secondary importance to the differences observed between conditions. If participants were provided additional time to view the warnings, the proportion of correct responses would likely have increased across the entire sample.

In examining the effects of sociodemographic covariates, we found that younger people were generally more likely to correctly understand FOP labels compared to their older counterparts, consistent with previous studies [51,52,53]. Overall, higher levels of education were also associated with a higher likelihood of responding correctly, consistent with previous research suggesting greater understanding of nutrition labels among more educated individuals [51,52,53]. Finally, the reason for the higher likelihood of responding correctly among respondents from Canada and the UK compared to the US is unknown, but may be a result of greater familiarity with FOP labeling systems and/or proposals in these countries. Given that the experiment tested FOP design elements being considered by Health Canada at the time, this finding also provides some reassurance of the utility of these labels in the Canadian market.

6. Strengths and Limitations

This study is subject to several limitations. First, the experiment was conducted online, rather than using actual products in a retail setting. However, the images used in the online survey were based on an actual product package and provided a reliable method for controlling viewing time. The length of viewing time was brief (4 s), which may have resulted in lower proportions of ‘correct’ responses than would have been the case if the image had remained on the screen for longer and/or while participants responded to questions. In addition to reflecting understanding, response rates may have therefore reflected participants’ recall ability; however, the randomized design would account for any such differences across experimental conditions. Within the experimental context, the absolute values of ‘correct’ responses are less meaningful than the relative differences between conditions; indeed, even within this highly constrained viewing time, the FOP designs led to substantial increases in correctly identifying foods high in nutrients of public health concern. The manipulation of FOP designs is also a potential limitation. The FOP designs were not systematically manipulated to examine the individual effect of color and symbol, or other differences between FOP labels, such as whether separate graphic symbols were used for each nutrient of concern (as in the red circle and stop sign symbols) or represented as a single symbol (as in the magnifying glass and triangle symbols). Future studies could test a full factorial design to isolate the effects of each design feature; however, the focus of the current study was to test specific proposals from Health Canada to inform a particular regulatory decision. As noted above, all the FOP symbols tested in the study were novel to participants; familiarity and understanding of FOP systems would likely be substantially greater if these systems were widely implemented on food products. This study only tested one product (cereal); although nutrient amounts do differ across products, we do not expect that the observed impact of specific label design elements would depend on product type. Finally, the use of a between-groups experimental design, diverse sample across four countries, and ‘objective’ measures of comprehension are all strengths of the current study.

7. Conclusions

Recognizable symbols (e.g., stop signs, ‘caution’ symbols, exclamation marks), (red) color, and simple text-based messages (e.g., ‘High in saturated fat’) are salient cues that help consumers quickly recognize high levels of nutrients of concern. This is important in the context of real shopping experiences, where purchasing decisions are made quickly.

Supplementary Materials

The following are available online at http://www.mdpi.com/2072-6643/10/11/1624/s1. Table S1: Socio-demographic characteristics (%, n) by country (n = 11,617), Table S2.: Odds (OR, 95%CI) of a correct response: Additional contrasts between FOP symbol designs (n = 11,617).

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, L.V., R.A., and D.H.; Formal analysis, S.G.; Funding acquisition, D.H.; Investigation, S.G., R.A., L.V., S.M., and D.H.; Methodology, D.H.; Project administration, D.H.; Supervision, D.H.; Writing—original draft, S.G.; Writing—review & editing, S.G., R.A., L.V., S.M., and D.H.

Funding

Funding for this project was provided by a Population Health Intervention Research operating grant from the Canadian Institutes of Health Research (CIHR). Additional funding for this project has been provided by a PHAC—CIHR Chair in Applied Public Health (D.H.).

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  • 1.Forouzanfar M.H., Alexander L., Anderson H.R., Bachman V.F., Biryukov S., Brauer M., Burnett R., Casey D., Coates M.M., Cohen A., et al. Global, regional, and national comparative risk assessment of 79 behavioural, environmental and occupational, and metabolic risks or clusters of risks in 188 countries, 1990–2013: A systematic analysis for the Global Burden of Disease Study 2013. Lancet. 2015;386:2287–2323. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(15)00128-2. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Healthy Diet: Key Facts. [(accessed on 24 September 2018)]; Available online: http://www.who.int/news-room/fact-sheets/detail/healthy-diet.
  • 3.Statistics Canada Adult Body Mass Index: Health Canada Classification. Table: 13-10-0323-01. [(accessed on 24 September 2018)]; Available online: https://www150.statcan.gc.ca/t1/tbl1/en/tv.action?pid=1310032301.
  • 4.Obesity and Overweight. [(accessed on 24 September 2018)]; Available online: http://www.who.int/news-room/fact-sheets/detail/obesity-and-overweight.
  • 5.Moubarac J.-C., Batal M., Martins A.P.B., Claro R., Levy R.B., Cannon G., Monteiro C. Processed and ultra-processed food products: Consumption trends in Canada from 1938 to 2011. Can. J. Diet. Pract. Res. 2014;75:15–21. doi: 10.3148/75.1.2014.15. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Campos S., Doxey J., Hammond D. Nutrition labels on pre-packaged foods: A systematic review. Public Health Nutr. 2011;14:1496–1506. doi: 10.1017/S1368980010003290. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Goodman S., Hammond D., Pillo-Blocka F., Glanville T., Jenkins R. Use of Nutritional Information in Canada: National trends between 2004 and 2008. J. Nutr. Educ. Behav. 2011;43:356–365. doi: 10.1016/j.jneb.2011.02.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Canadian Foundation for Dietetic Research (CFDR) Presents Tracking Nutrition Trends 2013. [(accessed on 24 September 2018)]; Available online: https://www.cfdr.ca/Downloads/CCFN-docs/TNT-2013-Summary-Report.aspx.
  • 9.Misra R. Knowledge, attitudes, and label use among college students. J. Am. Diet. Assoc. 2007;107:2130–2134. doi: 10.1016/j.jada.2007.09.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Smith S.C., Taylor J.G., Stephen A.M. Use of Food Labels and Beliefs about Diet-Disease Relationships among University Students. Public Health Nutr. 2007;3:175–182. doi: 10.1017/S1368980000000203. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Sullivan A.D. Determining how low-income food shoppers perceive, understand, and use food labels. Can. J. Diet. Pract. Res. 2003;64:25–27. doi: 10.3148/64.1.2003.25. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Keller S.B., Landry M., Olson J., Velliquette A.M., Burton S., Andrews J.C. The Effects of Nutrition Package Claims, Nutrition Facts Panels, and Motivation to Process Nutrition Information on Consumer Product Evaluations. J. Public Policy Mark. 1997;16:256–269. [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Garretson J.A., Burton S. Effects of Nutrition Facts Panel Values, Nutrition Claims, and Health Claims on Consumer Attitudes, Perceptions of Disease-Related Risks, and Trust. J. Public Policy Mark. 2000;19:213–227. doi: 10.1509/jppm.19.2.213.17133. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Balasubramanian S.K., Cole C. Consumers’ search and use of nutrition information: The challenge and promise of the nutrition labelling and education act. J. Mark. 2002;66:112–127. doi: 10.1509/jmkg.66.3.112.18502. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Lindhorst K., Corby L., Roberts S., Zeiler S. Rural consumers’ attitudes: Towards nutrition labelling. Can. J. Diet. Pract. Res. 2007;68:146–149. doi: 10.3148/68.3.2007.146. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Silayoi P., Speece M. Packaging and purchase decisions: An exploratory study on the impact of involvement level and time pressure. Br. Food J. 2004;106:607–628. doi: 10.1108/00070700410553602. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Reid D.J., Hendricks S. Consumer Awareness of Nutrition Information on Food Package Labels. J. Can. Diet. Assoc. 1993;54:127–131. [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Levy A.S., Fein S.B. Consumers’ ability to perform tasks using nutrition labels. J. Nutr. Educ. 1998;30:210–217. doi: 10.1016/S0022-3182(98)70321-8. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Rothman R.L., Housam R., Weiss H., David D., Gregory R., Gebretsadik T., Shintani A., Elasy T.A. Patient understanding of food labels: The role of literacy and numeracy. Am. J. Prev. Med. 2006;31:391–398. doi: 10.1016/j.amepre.2006.07.025. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Strategic Counsel . Final Report—Canadians’ Understanding and Use of the Nutrition Facts Table: Survey Results. Health Canada; Ottawa, ON, Canada: 2012. [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Vanderlee L., White C.M., Bordes I., Hobin E.P., Hammond D. The efficacy of sugar labeling formats: Implications for labeling policy. Obesity. 2015;23:2406–2413. doi: 10.1002/oby.21316. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Nayga R.M., Lipinski D., Savur N. Consumers’ use of nutritional labels while food shopping and at home. J. Consum. Aff. 1998;32:106–120. doi: 10.1111/j.1745-6606.1998.tb00402.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Joint FAO/WHO Food Standards Programme Codex Committee on Food Labelling: Discussion Paper on Consideration of Issues Regarding Front-of-Pack Nutrition Labelling. [(accessed on 24 September 2018)]; Available online: http://www.fao.org/fao-who-codexalimentarius/sh-proxy/es/?lnk=1&url=https%253A%252F%252Fworkspace.fao.org%252Fsites%252Fcodex%252FMeetings%252FCX-714-44%252FWD%252Ffl44_07e.pdf.
  • 24.Kanter R., Vanderlee L., Vandevijvere S. Front-of-package nutrition labelling policy: Global progress and future directions. Public Health Nutr. 2018;21:1399–1408. doi: 10.1017/S1368980018000010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Food Front-of-Package Nutrition Symbol Consumer Consultation. [(accessed on 24 September 2018)]; Available online: https://www.healthyeatingconsultations.ca/front-of-package.
  • 26.Consultation on Proposed Front-Of-Package Labelling. [(accessed on 24 September 2018)]; Available online: https://www.canada.ca/en/health-canada/programs/consultation-front-of-package-nutrition-labelling-cgi.html.
  • 27.SAGE Research Corporation Consumer Opinions of “High in” Front of Pack Labelling Strategies: Final Report. [(accessed on 24 September 2018)]; Prepared for Health Canada. Available online: http://epe.lac-bac.gc.ca/100/200/301/pwgsc-tpsgc/por-ef/health/2017/060-16-e/report.pdf.
  • 28.Institute of Medicine . Front-of-Package Nutrition Rating Systems and Symbols: Promoting Healthier Choices. The National Academies Press; Washington, DC, USA: 2012. [(accessed on 24 September 2018)]. pp. 1–180. Available online: https://www.nap.edu/catalog/13221/front-of-package-nutrition-rating-systems-and-symbols-promoting-healthier. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Becker M.W., Bello N.M., Sundar R.P., Peltier C., Bix L. Front of pack labels enhance attention to nutrition information in novel and commercial brands. Food Policy. 2015;56:76–86. doi: 10.1016/j.foodpol.2015.08.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Becker M.W., Sundar R.P., Bello N., Alzahabi R., Weatherspoon L., Bix L. Assessing attentional prioritization of front-of-pack nutrition labels using change detection. Appl. Ergon. 2016;54:90–99. doi: 10.1016/j.apergo.2015.11.014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Borgmeier I., Westenhoefer J. Impact of different food label formats on healthiness evaluation and food choice of consumers: A randomized-controlled study. BMC Public Health. 2009;9:184. doi: 10.1186/1471-2458-9-184. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Kelly B., Hughes C., Chapman K., Louie J.C., Dixon H., Crawford J., King L., Daube M., Slevin T. Consumer testing of the acceptability and effectiveness of front-of-pack food labelling systems for the Australian grocery market. Health Promot. Int. 2009;24:120–129. doi: 10.1093/heapro/dap012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Babio N., López L., Salas-Salvadó J. Capacity analysis of health food choice by reference to consumers in two models of nutritional labeling; crossover study. Nutr. Hosp. 2013;28:173–181. doi: 10.3305/nh.2013.28.1.6254. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Babio N., Vicent P., López L., Benito A., Basulto J., Salas-Salvadó J. Adolescents’ ability to select healthy food using two different front-of-pack food labels: A cross-over study. Public Health Nutr. 2014;17:1403–1409. doi: 10.1017/S1368980013001274. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Arrúa A., Machín L., Curutchet M.R., Marítnez J., Antúnez L., Alcaire F., Giménez A., Ares G. Warnings as a directive front-of-pack nutrition labelling scheme: Comparison with the Guideline Daily Amount and traffic-light systems. Public Health Nutr. 2017;20:2308–2317. doi: 10.1017/S1368980017000866. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Hammond D., Vanderlee L., Hobin E.P., White C.M., Bordes I. Nutrition Facts Table: Consumer Perceptions among Young Adults. Health Canada; Ottawa, ON, Canada: 2014. [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Antúnez L., Giménez A., Maiche A., Ares G. Influence of interpretation aids on attentional capture, visual processing, and understanding of front-of-package nutrition labels. J. Nutr. Educ. Behav. 2015;47:292–299. doi: 10.1016/j.jneb.2015.02.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Wogalter M.S., Conzola V.C., Smith-Jackson T.L. Research-Based Guidelines for Warning Design and Evaluation. Appl. Ergon. 2002;33:219–230. doi: 10.1016/S0003-6870(02)00009-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Hammond D. Health warning messages on tobacco products: A review. Tob. Control. 2011;20:327–337. doi: 10.1136/tc.2010.037630. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Ares G., Giménez A., Bruzzone F., Antúnez L., Sapolinski A., Vidal L., Maiche A. Attentional capture and understanding of nutrition labelling: A study based on response times. Int. J. Food Sci. Nutr. 2012;63:679–688. doi: 10.3109/09637486.2011.652598. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Antúnez L., Vidal L., Sapolinski A., Giménez A., Maiche A., Ares G. How do design features influence consumer attention when looking for nutritional information on food labels? Results from an eye-tracking study on pan bread labels. Int. J. Food Sci. Nutr. 2013;64:515–527. doi: 10.3109/09637486.2012.759187. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Siegrist M., Leins-Hess R., Keller C. Which front-of-pack nutrition label is the most efficient one? The results of an eye-tracker study. Food Qual. Prefer. 2015;39:183–190. doi: 10.1016/j.foodqual.2014.07.010. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Bix L., Sundar R.P., Bello N.M., Peltier C., Weatherspoon L.J., Becker M.W. To see or not to see: Do front of pack nutrition labels affect attention to overall nutrition information? PLoS ONE. 2015;10:e0139732. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0139732. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Acton R.B., Vanderlee L., Roberto C.A., Hammond D. Consumer perceptions of specific design characteristics for front-of-package nutrition labels. Health Educ. Res. 2018;33:167–174. doi: 10.1093/her/cyy006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Cowburn G., Stockley L. Consumer understanding and use of nutrition labelling: A systematic review. Public Health Nutr. 2005;8:21–28. doi: 10.1079/PHN2005666. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Consumer Panels. [(accessed on 24 September 2018)]; Available online: http://www.nielsen.com/eu/en/solutions/capabilities/consumer-panels.html.
  • 47.International Food Policy Study: Technical Report—Wave 1 (2017) [(accessed on 24 September 2018)]; Available online: www.foodpolicystudy.com/methods.
  • 48.Front-of-Package Nutrition Labelling: 18 September 2017 Stakeholder Engagement Meeting. [(accessed on 24 September 2018)]; Available online: https://www.canada.ca/en/services/health/publications/food-nutrition/labelling-stakeholder-engagement-meeting-september-2017.html.
  • 49.Summary of Proposed Amendments Published in Canada Gazette, Part I: Nutrition Symbols, Other Labelling Provisions, Partially Hydrogenated Oils and Vitamin D. [(accessed on 24 September 2018)]; Available online: https://www.canada.ca/en/health-canada/programs/consultation-front-of-package-nutrition-labelling-cgi/summary-of-proposed-amendments.html.
  • 50.Acton R.B., Kirkpatrick S.I., Roberto C.A., Jones A.C., Hammond D. Taxes and front-of-package labels improve the healthiness of beverage and snack purchases: A randomized experimental marketplace. 2018. Unpublished work. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • 51.Sinclair S., Goodman S., Hammond D. Socio-demographic differences in the comprehension of nutritional labels on food products. J. Nutr. Educ. Behav. 2013;45:767–772. doi: 10.1016/j.jneb.2013.04.262. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Ducrot P., Méjean C.J., Kesse-Guyot E., Touvier M., Fezeu L., Hercberg S., Péneau S. Effectiveness of front-of-pack nutrition labels in French adults: Results from the NutriNet-Santé cohort study. PLoS ONE. 2015;10:e0140898. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0140898. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Soederberg Miller L.M., Applegate E., Beckett L.A., Wilson M.D., Gibson T.N. Age differences in the use of serving size information on food labels: Numeracy or attention? Public Health Nutr. 2017;20:786–796. doi: 10.1017/S1368980016003219. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials


Articles from Nutrients are provided here courtesy of Multidisciplinary Digital Publishing Institute (MDPI)

RESOURCES