Skip to main content
Health Science Reports logoLink to Health Science Reports
. 2018 Aug 28;1(10):e82. doi: 10.1002/hsr2.82

Risk factors for delayed social‐emotional development and behavior problems at age two: Results from the All Our Babies/Families (AOB/F) cohort

Sheila W McDonald 1,, Heather L Kehler 2, Suzanne C Tough 1
PMCID: PMC6266514  PMID: 30623038

Abstract

Background and Aims

Social‐emotional delays and behavioral problems at preschool age are associated with negative outcomes at school age, including ongoing behavior problems, poorer social functioning, and academic difficulties. Understanding modifiable risk factors for suboptimal development requires consideration of contemporary family circumstances to determine areas for effective early intervention to optimize development. This study aimed to identify risk factors for delayed social‐emotional development and behavior problems at age two among participants of the All Our Babies/Families cohort study.

Methods

Mothers (N = 1596) completed five comprehensive questionnaires spanning midpregnancy to 2 years postpartum. At child age two, behavior and competence outcomes were measured using the Brief Infant‐Toddler Social and Emotional Assessment. Chi square analysis and multivariable logistic regression modeling was used to identify key risk factors for suboptimal child outcomes. Predicted probabilities for adverse outcomes in the presence of risk were calculated.

Results

Risk factors for possible delayed social‐emotional development in children included maternal depression at 2 years postpartum (OR 2.46, 95% CI 1.63, 3.72), lower parenting self‐efficacy at 2 years postpartum (OR 2.76, 95% CI 1.51, 5.06), non‐daily play‐based interaction when child was 1 and 2 years old (OR 1.43, 95% CI 1.02, 1.99), child delayed sleep initiation at 2 years of age (OR 1.58, 95% CI 1.05, 2.37), and playgroup non‐attendance between 1 and 2 years postpartum (OR 1.43, 95% CI 1.03, 1.99). Risk factors for possible behavior problems included lower maternal optimism during pregnancy (OR 2.02, 95% CI 1.36, 2.99), maternal depression at 2 years postpartum (OR 2.19, 95% CI 1.46, 3.27), difficulty balancing responsibilities at 2 years postpartum (OR 2.32 95% CI 1.55, 3.47), child second language exposure at 2 years of age (OR 1.88, 95% CI 1.37, 2.58), child delayed sleep initiation at 2 years of age (OR 1.55 95% CI 1.06, 2.26), child frequent night wakings at 2 years of age (OR 2.95 95% CI 2.13, 4.10), and more screentime exposure at 2 years of age (OR 1.85 95% CI 1.34, 2.54).

Conclusions

This study suggests that addressing maternal mental health and promoting parenting strategies that encourage play‐based interaction, limiting screen time, preventing sleep problems, and engagement in informal playgroups would reduce the risk of behavior and social‐emotional problems.

Keywords: behavior problems, child development, cohort study, risk factors, social‐emotional development

1. INTRODUCTION

Social‐emotional and behavioral problems affect an estimated 5% to 20% of preschool‐aged children (<5 years) across a number of diverse populations, contexts, and assessment methods.1, 2, 3 In Canada, population profiles using the Early Development Index, an aggregate based indicator, found that over 40% of children in kindergarten (age 5 years) were vulnerable in social‐emotional health.4 Social‐emotional competencies in young children include helping, sharing, comforting, and exhibiting empathy and compliance, while behavioral problems include disruptive behavior, inhibition of impulse control, aggression, defiance, over‐activity, negative emotionality, anxiety, and withdrawal.5 Social‐emotional delays and behavioral problems at preschool age are associated with increased risk for negative outcomes at school age, including ongoing behavior problems, psychiatric disorders, and poorer academic achievement.6, 7 The development of social‐emotional competencies has important consequences for young children, including the ability to make and maintain quality friendships, and peer acceptance.8 Young children with social‐emotional competencies are more likely to experience positive early school adjustment and academic success.9

Early identification of children with delayed social‐emotional competencies and behavioral problems provides an opportunity for parents and care providers to implement strategies to remediate problems before school entry.10 Understanding the factors that contribute to the development of poor social‐emotional competencies and behavioral problems in the first few years of life will help mitigate long‐term adverse consequences and trajectories associated with suboptimal behavior. From a policy, program, and return on investment perspective, early interventions are cost‐effective and have greater positive impact than later remediation.11, 12

Despite evidence about the importance of early identification of children with delayed social‐emotional development and behavior problems,13, 14 there is limited Canadian research about family and community factors that may prevent poor outcomes within the context of pregnancy, and the early postpartum period. Assessing a breadth of risk factors helps inform independent effects and potential targets for early intervention. In addition, contemporary information is necessary to determine the magnitude of influence these factors have, within the current social context of dual parent working families, child care, screen time, and time stresses.

The objective of this study was to identify key risk factors for delayed social‐emotional development and behavioral problems among 2‐year‐old children in a community sample of mothers and their children. Data were analyzed from the All Our Babies/Families (AOB/F) cohort study,15 which contains prospectively collected information on maternal, family, child care, parenting, and community factors related to development. This population‐based cohort of women, recruited in Calgary, Canada, is demographically similar to the pregnant and parenting populations of westernized urban centers.15 These findings will inform and focus policy and programming to improve social‐emotional and behavior development in the preschool years.

2. METHODS

2.1. Study design

The AOB/F study is an ongoing prospective population‐based pregnancy cohort study in Calgary, Canada, that began in 2008.15 The AOB/F study was developed to investigate the relationships between the prenatal and early life period, and outcomes for infants, children, and mothers. Detailed descriptions of the AOB/F study design and methods have previously been described.15 Briefly, the cohort was established using a community‐based recruitment strategy, involving primary health care offices and a citywide laboratory service (Calgary Laboratory Service). Community posters were implemented to obtain a socioeconomically and ethnically diverse sample of women representative of the parenting population in an urban Canadian center. Recruitment began in May 2008 and was completed in December 2010. Women were eligible to participate if they were less than 25‐week gestation, 18 years of age or older, resided in the greater Calgary area, and were able to complete the written questionnaires in English. Prior to inclusion in the study, all participants provided informed consent. Participants were asked to complete three written questionnaires: (1) at study intake (second trimester), (2) during their third trimester, and (3) at 4 months postpartum. Information was collected on pregnancy history, demographics, lifestyle, health care utilization, physical and mental health, birth outcomes, child health, and parenting. Participants were also asked to consent to the research team accessing their obstetrical and birth records. In total, 3317 women consented to participate and completed at least one questionnaire. Women who consented to be contacted for future research were asked to participate in subsequent follow‐up questionnaires when their child was 1 and 2 years old. The design of the 2‐year follow‐up questionnaire was delayed due to logistical constraints including receipt of funding and ethical approval; as such, not all original study participants were eligible at 2 years, since some children were out of the relevant age range. Among those eligible (n = 2114), 1596 completed and returned the 2‐year questionnaire, for a participation rate of 75.5%. From the original total sample of participants (n = 3317), this corresponded to 48.1% participating in the 2‐year questionnaire.

At 2 years, the questionnaire collected information about their child's health, child care environment, child development, health service utilization, community resource use, demographics, maternal lifestyle, maternal mental health, work status, and parenting experience. All questionnaires were developed with input from health care providers, community program experts, and researchers with expertise in child development and family well‐being. Standardized measures were included as part of the questionnaires when available. Ethical approval for the study was provided by the Conjoint Health Research Ethics Board of the Faculty of Medicine at the University of Calgary.

2.2. Main outcomes

The Brief Infant‐Toddler Social and Emotional Assessment (BITSEA) is a concise screening instrument to identify possible behavior problems and delayed social‐emotional competencies in children aged 12 to 36 months.16 It was designed to be completed by parents or caregivers and consists of 42 items. The BITSEA measures two domains, behavior problems (eg, aggression, defiance, over‐activity, negative emotionality, anxiety, and withdrawal) and delayed social‐emotional competencies (eg, empathy, pro‐social behaviors, and compliance).16 A total behavioral problem score and a total social‐emotional competencies score are derived by summing relevant items for each domain subscale. Using the BITSEA standardized scoring cut‐offs from the published normative data, children were categorized with possible behavioral problems if they scored at or above the 75th percentile of normative data, and children were categorized with possible delayed social‐emotional competencies if they scored at or below the 15th percentile of normative data.16 The BITSEA has shown excellent test‐retest reliability (behavior problems scale: r = 0.87; social‐emotional competence scale: r = 0.85) and good interrater agreement between mothers and fathers (behavior problems scale: r = 0.68; social‐emotional competence scale: r = 0.61).17 Internal consistency measured by Cronbach's alpha was 0.79 for the behavior problems scale and 0.65 for the social‐emotional competence scale.17 Similarly, in the current sample, internal consistency was 0.75 for the behavior problem scale and 0.63 for the social‐emotional competence scale. Given the large size of our sample, we used a parent report screening measure to identify children with symptoms of behavioral problems and social emotional difficulties. Screening tools are often used in population‐based studies due to their feasibility and ease of administration compared with more time‐consuming and costly diagnostic interviews. Of note, the use of delayed social‐emotional development and behavior problems throughout the manuscript refer to “possible” delays and problems.

2.3. Independent variables

Information on a broad range of potential risk factors collected during pregnancy and the first 2 years postpartum were examined in the current study and were grouped into four broad categories: (1) socio‐demographic and adverse life event factors, (2) pregnancy and birth outcome factors, (3) 1‐year postpartum factors, and (4) 2‐year postpartum factors. The socio‐demographic factors examined were maternal age, marital status, maternal education, and household income. Adverse life event factors included a history of a mental health disorder (s) and a history of experiencing abuse (physical, emotional, sexual, financial, or neglect). Factors measured during pregnancy included maternal depressive symptoms measured by the Edinburgh Postnatal Depression Scale (EPDS),18 maternal anxiety symptoms measured by the Spielberger State Anxiety Inventory (SSAI),19 maternal stress measured by the Perceived Stress Scale (PSS),20 maternal social support measured by the MOS Social Support Scale (MOS‐SSS),21 and maternal optimism measured by the Life‐Orientation Test‐Revised (LOT‐R).22 Birth outcome factors included gestational age at birth, birth weight, and child sex. One‐year postpartum factors included maternal depressive symptoms measured by the EPDS,18 maternal anxiety symptoms measured by the SSAI,19 maternal stress measured by the PSS,20 maternal social support measured by the National Longitudinal Survey of Children and Youth (NLSCY) Social Support Scale,23 maternal relationship happiness, and co‐parenting agreement. Two‐year postpartum maternal factors included depressive symptoms measured by the Centre for Epidemiologic Studies Depression Scale (CES‐D),24 anxiety symptoms measured by the SSAI,19 social support measured by the NLSCY Social Support Scale,23 parenting self‐efficacy measured by the Parenting Sense of Competence Scale (PSOC),25 work‐life balance, parent‐child interaction (reading and playing), and community resource use (formal parenting program/group, informal playgroup). Two‐year‐old child factors included child care arrangement, second language exposure, sleep habits (sleep initiation, napping, night waking), and screen time. For the purposes of modeling, all factors were dichotomized into a possible risk and reference category. Validated cut‐offs were used for the EPDS, SSAI, and MOS‐SSS. For those scales without a validated cut‐off, including the PSS, NLSCY Social Support Scale, PSOC, and LOT‐R, one standard deviation above or below the mean, depending on scoring, was used to define “at‐risk” status. For all standardized scales, the specific cut‐off values used have been noted in Tables 2 and 4.

2.4. Data analysis

Descriptive statistics were used to describe participants socio‐demographics, maternal mental health at 2 years postpartum, and child social‐emotional and behavioral functioning at 2 years. Among those eligible for the 2‐year questionnaire (n = 2114), socio‐demographic characteristics, birth outcomes, and maternal mental health symptoms during pregnancy and at 1 year postpartum were compared between participants and non‐participants of the 2‐year questionnaire using Pearson's Chi‐square Test. An initial bivariate analysis using Pearson's Chi‐square test was conducted to identify risk factors for delayed social‐emotional development and child behavioral problems at age two. Risk factors identified at the bivariate level based on statistical significance at the P < 0.10 level were considered for inclusion in the subsequent multivariable logistic regression models. A manual stepwise approach was used to build the multivariable models, with blocks of variables being added in sequential steps in the following order: (1) socio‐demographic and adverse life event variables, (2) pregnancy and birth outcome variables, (3) 1‐year postpartum variables, and (4) 2‐year postpartum variables. To best address the research objectives, these blocks were ordered conceptually in temporal order and from least to most modifiable, as per our model building strategy used in previous research in the AOF.26, 27, 28 Risk factors within each block were identified for inclusion based on the literature. Statistical significance was set at P < 0.05 for inclusion in the next step of the logistic regression modeling, and parsimonious models were developed at each block. As a final step, we entered each factor that was dropped at previous steps to ensure model robustness. Multivariable logistic regression results are presented as odds ratios (ORs) and 95% confidence intervals (CIs). Predicted probabilities for delayed social‐emotional development and behavior problems were calculated for the combination of risk factors identified from the final models. Stata Version 12.1 was used for the statistical analysis.

3. RESULTS

3.1. Maternal participant characteristics

The characteristics of mothers who participated in the 2‐year questionnaire (n = 1596) are described in Table 1. Compared with women who were eligible for the 2‐year questionnaire but did not participate, women who participated in the 2‐year questionnaire were more likely to have post‐secondary education and household incomes greater than $80 000 CAD and be married or in a common law relationship and Caucasian (all P < 0.05). Two‐year questionnaire participants were less likely to both experience symptoms of anxiety during pregnancy and have a preterm birth, than non‐participants (all P < 0.05). There were no differences in maternal age, maternal country of birth, pre‐pregnancy mental health history, depressive symptoms during pregnancy, child birthweight, and mental health symptoms at 1‐year postpartum between participants and non‐participants.

Table 1.

Participant characteristics

Characteristic N = 1596
N (%)
Maternal age at delivery
24 or younger 78 (4.89)
25‐34 years of age 1085 (67.98)
35 or older 360 (22.56)
Missing 73 (4.57)
Marital status at 2 years postpartum
Married/common law 1536 (96.24)
Single/separated/divorced/widowed 60 (3.76)
Missing 0 (0.00)
Maternal education at study intake
Has not completed post‐secondary 347 (21.74)
Graduated post‐secondary 1237 (77.51)
Missing 12 (0.75)
Household income before taxes and deductions at study intake
Less than $60 000 207 (12.97)
$60 000‐$79 999 211 (13.22)
$80 000 or more 1107 (69.36)
Missing 71 (4.45)
Mother was born in Canada
Yes 1276 (79.95)
No 309 (19.36)
Missing 11 (0.69)
Maternal race
White/Caucasian 1301 (81.52)
Other 283 (17.73)
Missing 12 (0.75)
Maternal depressive symptoms at 2 years postpartum (CES‐D)
No (CES‐D < 16) 1396 (87.47)
Yes (CES‐D ≥ 16) 200 (12.53)
Missing 0 (0.00)
Maternal anxiety symptoms at 2 years postpartum (SSAI)
No (SSAI <40) 1334 (83.58)
Yes (SSAI ≥40) 242 (15.16)
Missing 20 (1.25)
Child social‐emotional competencies at age 2 years (BITSEA)
No delay 1369 (85.78)
Possible delay 210 (13.16)
Missing 17 (1.07)
Child behavior problems at age 2 years (BITSEA)
No behavior problems 1344 (84.21)
Possible behavior problems 236 (14.79)
Missing 16 (1.00)

Abbreviations: BITSEA, Brief Infant‐Toddler Social and Emotional Assessment; CES‐D, Centre for Epidemiologic Studies Depression Scale; SSAI, Spielberger State Anxiety Inventory.

3.2. Child development outcomes at 2 years

At questionnaire completion, children were an average age of 2.03 years (sd = 0.10, range = 1.88‐2.84 years). Based on maternal report on the BITSEA, 13% (n = 210/1579) of children were identified with possible delayed social‐emotional development and 15% (n = 236/1580) of children with possible behavior problems at age two (Table 1).

3.3. Risk factors for delayed social‐emotional development at age two

Bivariate associations between all potential risk factors examined and delayed social‐emotional development at age two are summarized in Table 2. Multivariable modelling identified the combination of risk factors most predictive of delayed social‐emotional development at age two, which included maternal depression at 2 years postpartum (OR 2.46, 95% CI 1.63, 3.72), lower parenting self‐efficacy at 2 years postpartum (OR 2.76, 95% CI 1.51, 5.06), lack of parent‐child engagement in daily imitation play at 1 and 2 years (OR 1.43, 95% CI 1.02, 1.99), more than 30 minutes to initiate child night sleep at 2 years of age (OR 1.58, 95% CI 1.05, 2.37), and not attending an informal play group between 1 and 2 years postpartum (OR 1.43, 95% CI 1.03, 1.99) (Table 3). The predicted probability of delayed social‐emotional development at age 2 was 65% in the presence of all five risk factors.

Table 2.

Risk factors for delayed social‐emotional development at age 2

Category Risk Factor No Delay (BITSEA) N = 1369 N (%) Possible Delay (BITSEA) N = 210 N (%) Pearson Chi‐Square Test X2, P‐value
Socio‐demographic and adverse life event factors Maternal age at delivery
34 years old or less 1018 (77.18) 142 (70.65) 4.12, 0.042
35 years old or more 301 (22.82) 59 (29.35)
Marital status at 2 years postpartum 0.11, 0.741
Married or common law 1317 (96.20) 203 (96.67)
Single, separated, divorced, widowed 52 (3.80) 7 (3.33)
Maternal education at study intake 0.63, 0.426
Has not completed post‐secondary 294 (21.59) 50 (24.04)
Graduated post‐secondary 1068 (78.41) 158 (75.96)
Household income at 1 year postpartum 4.42, 0.035
Less than $80 000 339 (30.19) 66 (38.15)
$80 000 or more 784 (69.81) 107 (61.85)
History of any mental health disorder 1.62, 0.204
Yes 441 (32.40) 77 (36.84)
No 920 (67.60) 132 (63.16)
History of ever experiencing abuse (physical, emotional, sexual, financial, neglect) 0.80, 0.370
Yes 325 (24.38) 45 (21.53)
No 1008 (75.62) 164 (78.47)
Pregnancy and birth outcome factors Depressive symptoms during pregnancy (EPDS) 5.21, 0.022
Yes (EPDS ≥13) 85 (6.25) 22 (10.53)
No (EPDS <13) 1275 (93.75) 187 (89.47)
Anxiety symptoms during pregnancy (SSAI) 4.66, 0.031
Yes (SSAI ≥40) 174 (13.03) 38 (18.63)
No (SSAI <40) 1161 (86.97) 166 (81.37)
Stress during pregnancy (PSS)
Higher (PSS ≥20)
8.15, 0.004
Lower (PSS <20) 184 (13.57) 44 (21.05)
1172 (86.43) 165 (78.95)
Social support during pregnancy (MOS) 20.96, <0.001
Higher (MOS >69) 1214 (89.79) 164 (78.85)
Lower (MOS ≤69) 138 (10.21) 44 (21.15)
Optimism during pregnancy (LOT‐R) 6.10, 0.013
Higher (LOT‐R > 13) 1171 (87.85) 169 (81.64)
Lower (LOT‐R ≤ 13) 162 (12.15) 38 (18.36)
Gestational age at delivery 2.70, 0.100
Preterm (<37 weeks) 85 (6.37) 19 (9.50)
Term (≥37 weeks) 1250 (93.63) 181 (90.50)
Birth weight 3.61, 0.057
Low birth weight (<2500 grams) 69 (5.43)
1202 (94.57)
17 (8.90)
174 (91.10)
Not low birth weight (≥2500 grams)
Child sex 3.67, 0.055
Male 685 (50.97) 117 (58.21)
Female 659 (49.03) 84 (41.79)
1 year postpartum factors Depressive symptoms (EPDS) 11.07, 0.001
Yes (EPDS ≥13)
No (EPDS <13) 57 (5.05)
1071 (94.95)
20 (11.43)
155 (88.57)
Anxiety symptoms (SSAI) 9.73, 0.002
Yes (SSAI ≥40) 166 (15.09) 41 (24.70)
No (SSAI <40) 934 (84.91) 125 (75.30)
Stress symptoms (PSS) 9.14, 0.002
Higher (PSS ≥19) 210 (18.95) 49 (28.99)
Lower (PSS <19) 898 (81.05) 120 (71.01)
Social support (NLSCY) 2.83, 0.093
Higher (NLSCY >17) 921 (81.50) 134 (76.14)
Lower (NLSCY ≤17) 209 (18.50) 42 (23.86)
Relationship happiness 10.30, 0.001
Happy 1032 (92.39) 148 (85.06)
Unhappy 85 (7.61) 26 (14.94)
Co‐parenting agreement 6.25, 0.012
Agree 961 (84.74) 136 (77.27)
Not sure or disagree 173 (15.26) 40 (22.73)
2‐year postpartum factors Depressive symptoms (CES‐D) 30.47, <0.001
Yes (CES‐D ≥ 16) 147 (10.74) 51 (24.29)
No (CES‐D < 16) 1222 (89.26) 159 (75.71)
Anxiety symptoms (SSAI) 22.10, <0.001
Yes (SSAI ≥40) 185 (13.70) 55 (26.32)
No (SSAI <40) 1165 (86.30) 154 (73.68)
Social support (NLSCY) 11.25, 0.001
Higher (NLSCY >17) 1185 (86.81) 164 (78.10)
Lower (NLSCY ≤17) 180 (13.19) 46 (21.90)
Parenting self‐efficacy (PSOC) 32.78, <0.001
Higher (PSOC >17)
Lower (PSOC ≤17)
1322 (96.78) 185 (88.10)
44 (3.22) 25 (11.90)
Child was consistently read to daily at 1 and 2 years 9.59, 0.002
Yes 832 (74.09) 110 (62.86)
No 291 (25.91) 65 (37.14)
An adult in the household consistently played imitation games with the child daily at 1 and 2 years 9.18, 0.002
Yes 743 (65.75) 95 (53.98)
No 387 (34.25) 81 (46.02)
Mother's perceived ability to fulfill family, work, or other responsibilities 2.67, 0.102
It is never or sometimes difficult 1214 (88.68)
155 (11.32)
178 (84.76)
32 (15.24)
It is difficult most of the time/it is always difficult
Mother attended a parenting group, program, or center in the past year 0.20, 0.659
Yes 384 (28.05) 62 (29.52)
No 985 (71.95) 148 (70.48)
Mother attended an informal play group in the past year 9.29, 0.002
Yes 830 (60.63) 104 (49.52)
No 539 (39.37) 106 (50.48)
Childcare arrangement at 2 years 0.92, 0.337
Parent/relative/nanny 856 (62.76) 139 (66.19)
Childcare center/day home 508 (37.24) 71 (33.81)
Child regular second language exposure at 2 years 5.08, 0.024
Yes 379 (27.68) 74 (35.24)
No 990 (72.32) 136 (64.76)
Child night sleep initiation time at 2 years (in general) 4.87, 0.027
30 min or less 1175 (85.83) 168 (80.00)
More than 30 min 194 (14.17) 42 (20.00)
Total time child spends napping per day at 2 years (average) 0.36, 0.548
Less than 1 h 71 (5.19) 13 (6.19)
At least 1 h 1297 (94.81) 197 (93.81)
How often child wakes at night at 2 years (in general) 0.04, 0.841
2 or fewer times per week 1071 (78.23) 163 (77.62)
3 or more times per week 298 (21.77) 47 (22.38)
Child total screen time per day at 2 years (television, movies, computer/tablet) 2.46, 0.117
Less than 1 h 686 (51.73) 94 (45.85)
1 h or more 640 (48.27) 111 (54.15)

Abbreviations: BITSEA, Brief Infant Toddler Social Emotional Assessment; CES‐D, Centre for Epidemiologic Studies Depression Scale; EPDS, Edinburgh Postnatal Depression Scale; LOT‐R, Life‐Orientation Test‐Revised; MOS, MOS Social Support Scale; NLSCY, National Longitudinal Survey of Children and Youth Social Support Scale; PSOC, Parenting Sense of Competence Scale; PSS, Perceived Stress Scale; SSAI, Spielberger State Anxiety Inventory.

Table 3.

Multivariable model of risk factors for delayed social‐emotional development at age 2

Risk Factor Adjusted Odds Ratio 95% Confidence Interval
Maternal depressive symptoms at 2 years postpartum
(CES‐D)
Yes (CES‐D ≥ 16)
No (CES‐D < 16)
2.46
Reference
[1.63, 3.72]*
Parenting self‐efficacy at 2 years postpartum (PSOC)
Lower (PSOC ≤17.98)
Higher (PSOC >17.98)
2.76
Reference
[1.51, 5.06]*
Consistent daily play with an adult at 1 and 2 years
No
Yes
1.43
Reference
[1.02, 1.99]*
Child night sleep initiation time at 2 years (in general)
More than 30 min
30 min or less
1.58
Reference
[1.05, 2.37]*
Mother attended an informal play group when child was between the ages of 1 and 2
No
Yes
1.43
Reference
[1.03, 1.99]*

Abbreviations: CES‐D, Centre for Epidemiologic Studies Depression Scale; PSOC, Parenting Sense of Competence Scale.

*

P < 0.05.

3.4. Risk factors for behavior problems at age two

Bivariate associations between all potential risk factors examined and child behavior problems at age two are summarized in Table 4. Multivariable modeling revealed the combination of risk factors most predictive of behavior problems at age two to be having a mother who reported lower optimism during pregnancy (OR 2.02, 95% CI 1.36, 2.99), having a mother experiencing depression at 2 years postpartum (OR 2.19, 95% CI 1.46, 3.27), having a mother who reported more difficulty balancing family, work, and other responsibilities at 2 years postpartum (OR 2.32 95% CI 1.55, 3.47), child exposure to a second language on a regular basis at 2 years of age (OR 1.88, 95% CI 1.37, 2.58), more than 30 minutes to initiate child night sleep at 2 years of age (OR 1.55 95% CI 1.06, 2.26), child night wakings at least three times a week at 2 years of age (OR 2.95 95% CI 2.13, 4.10), and child exposure to at least 1 hour of daily screen time on any type of media (television, movies, computer, tablet, etc.) at 2 years of age (OR 1.85 95% CI 1.34, 2.54) (Table 5). The predicted probability of behavior problems at age 2 was 88% in the presence of all seven risk factors.

Table 4.

Risk factors for behavioral problems at age 2

Category Risk Factor No Problems (BITSEA) N = 1344N (%) Possible Behavior Problems (BITSEA)N = 236N (%) Pearson Chi‐Square TestX2, P‐value
Socio‐
demographic and adverse life event factors
Maternal age at delivery
34 years old or less
35 years old or more
982 (75.65)
316 (24.35)
179 (80.27)
44 (19.73)
2.24, 0.134
Marital status at 2 years postpartum
Married or common law
Single, separated, divorced, widowed
1299 (96.65)
45 (3.35)
222 (94.07)
14 (5.93)
3.73, 0.053
Maternal education at study intake
Has not completed post‐secondary
Graduated post‐secondary
278 (20.78)
 
1060 (79.22)
67 (28.76)
 
166 (71.24)
7.37, 0.007
Household income at 1 year postpartum
Less than $80 000
$80 000 or more
324 (29.01)
793 (70.99)
81 (45.25)
98 (54.75)
18.95, <0.001
History of any mental health disorder
Yes
No
410 (30.67)
927 (69.33)
108 (46.15)
126 (53.85)
21.62, <0.001
History of ever experiencing abuse (physical, emotional, sexual, financial, neglect)
Yes
No
300 (22.83)
1014 (77.17)
71 (31.00)
158 (69.00)
7.13, 0.008
Pregnancy and birth outcome factors Depressive symptoms during pregnancy (EPDS)
Yes (EPDS ≥13)
No (EPDS <13)
74 (5.54)
1262 (94.46)
33 (14.10)
201 (85.90)
22.99, <0.001
Anxiety symptoms during pregnancy (SSAI)
Yes (SSAI ≥40)
No (SSAI <40)
160 (12.21)
1150 (87.79)
52 (22.61)
178 (77.39)
17.80, <0.001
Stress during pregnancy (PSS)
Higher (PSS ≥20)
Lower (PSS <20)
166 (12.46)
1166 (87.54)
62 (26.50)
172 (73.50)
31.51, <0.001
Social support during pregnancy (MOS)
Higher (MOS >69)
Lower (MOS ≤69)
1196 (89.86)
135 (10.14)
183 (79.57)
47 (20.43)
20.17, <0.001
Optimism during pregnancy (LOT‐R)
Higher (LOT‐R > 13)
Lower (LOT‐R ≤ 13)
1173 (89.20)
142 (10.80)
167 (73.89)
59 (26.11)
39.84, <0.001
Gestational age at delivery
Preterm (<37 weeks)
Term (≥37 weeks)
84 (6.40)
1229 (93.60)
20 (8.97)
203 (91.03)
2.00, 0.158
Birth weight
Low birth weight (<2500 grams)
Not low birth weight (≥2500 grams)
68 (5.47)
1176 (94.53)
18 (8.22)
201 (91.78)
2.55, 0.110
Child sex
Male
Female
691 (52.47)
626 (47.53)
112 (48.91)
117 (51.09)
0.99, 0.320
1 year postpartum factors Depressive symptoms (EPDS)
Yes (EPDS ≥13)
No (EPDS <13)
55 (4.91)
1066 (95.09)
22 (12.02)
161 (87.98)
14.34, <0.001
Anxiety symptoms (SSAI)
Yes (SSAI ≥40)
No (SSAI <40)
156 (14.21)
942 (85.79)
51 (30.18)
118 (69.82)
27.33, <0.001
Stress symptoms (PSS)
Higher (PSS ≥19)
Lower (PSS <19)
197 (17.89)
904 (82.11)
62 (35.03)
115 (64.97)
27.71, <0.001
Social support (NLSCY)
Higher (NLSCY >17)
Lower (NLSCY ≤17)
929 (82.65)
195 (17.35)
127 (69.40)
56 (30.60)
17.81, <0.001
Relationship happiness
Happy
Unhappy
1029 (92.29)
86 (7.71)
152 (85.88)
25 (14.12)
8.00, 0.005
Co‐parenting agreement
Agree
Not sure or disagree
957 (84.84)
171 (15.16)
141 (77.05)
42 (22.95)
7.02, 0.008
2‐year postpartum factors Depressive symptoms (CES‐D)
Yes (CES‐D ≥ 16)
No (CES‐D < 16)
134 (9.97)
1210 (90.03)
64 (27.12)
172 (72.88)
53.86, <0.001
Anxiety symptoms (SSAI)
Yes (SSAI ≥40)
No (SSAI <40)
179 (13.47)
1150 (86.53)
61 (26.41)
170 (73.59)
25.31, <0.001
Social support (NLSCY)
Higher (NLSCY >17)
Lower (NLSCY ≤17)
1175 (87.69)
165 (12.31)
175 (74.15)
61 (25.85)
29.92, <0.001
Parenting self‐efficacy (PSOC)
Higher (PSOC >17)
Lower (PSOC ≤17)
1291 (96.20)
51 (3.80)
217 (92.34)
18 (7.66)
7.12, 0.008
Child was consistently read to daily at 1 and 2 years
Yes
No
822 (73.52)
296 (26.48)
120 (66.30)
61 (33.70)
4.08, 0.043
An adult in the household consistently played imitation games with the child daily at 1 and 2 years
Yes
No
731 (64.98)
394 (35.02)
107 (58.79)
75 (41.21)
2.61, 0.106
Mother's perceived ability to fulfill family, work, or other responsibilities
It is never or sometimes difficult
It is difficult most of the time/it is always difficult
1212 (90.18)
132 (9.82)
181 (76.69)
55 (23.31)
34.98, <0.001
Mother attended a parenting group, program, or center in the past year
Yes
No
370 (27.53)
974 (72.47)
76 (32.20)
160 (67.80)
2.16, 0.141
Mother attended an informal play group in the past year
Yes
No
814 (60.57)
530 (39.43)
120 (50.85)
116 (49.15)
7.84, 0.005
Childcare arrangement at 2 years
Parent/relative/nanny
Childcare center/day home
855 (63.81)
485 (36.19)
140 (59.57)
95 (40.43)
1.54, 0.215
Child regular second language exposure at 2 years
Yes
No
350 (26.04)
994 (73.96)
104 (44.07)
132 (55.93)
31.86, <0.001
Child night sleep initiation time at 2 years (in general)
30 min or less
More than 30 min
1169 (86.98)
175 (13.02)
175 (74.15)
61 (25.85)
25.99, <0.001
Total time child spends napping per day at 2 years (average)
Less than 1 h
At least 1 h
66 (4.91)
1278 (95.09)
18 (7.66)
217 (92.34)
3.00, 0.083
How often child wakes at night at 2 years (in general)
2 or fewer times per week
3 or more times per week
1094 (81.40)
250 (18.60)
140 (59.32)
96 (40.68)
57.21, <0.001
Child total screen time per day at 2 years (television, movies, computer/tablet)
Less than 1 h
1 h or more
700 (53.68)
604 (46.32)
80 (35.24)
147 (64.76)
26.30, <0.001

Abbreviations: CES‐D, Centre for Epidemiologic Studies Depression Scale; EPDS, Edinburgh Postnatal Depression Scale; LOT‐R, Life‐Orientation Test‐Revised; MOS, MOS Social Support Scale; NLSCY, National Longitudinal Survey of Children and Youth Social Support Scale; PSOC, Parenting Sense of Competence Scale; PSS, Perceived Stress Scale; SSAI, Spielberger State Anxiety Inventory.

Table 5.

Multivariable model of risk factors for behavioral problems at age 2

Risk Factor Adjusted Odds Ratio 95% Confidence Interval
Higher optimism during late pregnancy (LOT‐R)
Lower optimism (LOT‐R < 15) 2.02 [1.36, 2.99]*
Higher optimism (LOT‐R ≥ 15) Reference
Maternal depressive symptoms at 2 years postpartum (CES‐D)
Yes (CES‐D ≥ 16) [1.46, 3.27]*
No (CES‐D < 16) 2.19
Reference
Perceived difficulty fulfilling family, work, or other responsibilities when child was 2 years old [1.55, 3.47]*
More difficulty 2.32
Less difficulty Reference
Child is regularly exposed to a language other than English at age 2 [1.37, 2.58]*
Yes 1.88
No Reference
Child night sleep initiation time at 2 years (in general) [1.06, 2.26]*
More than 30 min 1.55
30 min or less Reference
How often child wakes at night at age 2
3 or more times per week
2 or fewer times per week
2.95
Reference
[2.13, 4.10]*
Child screen time per day on any type of media (television, movies, computer/tablet) at age 2 [1.34, 2.54]*
1 h or more 1.85
Less than 1 h Reference

Abbreviations: CES‐D, Centre for Epidemiologic Studies Depression Scale; LOT‐R, Life‐Orientation Test‐Revised.

*

P < 0.05.

4. DISCUSSION

Early behavior and social‐emotional development is important for children's ongoing behavior, social competence, mental health, and academic success. Utilizing data from the AOB/F cohort, we identified factors that were associated with delayed social‐emotional development and behavior problems at age two, providing insight into beneficial targets for interventions.

Women who were experiencing depression when their child was 2 years old were more likely to have a child with delayed social‐emotional competencies and behavior problems than women who were not experiencing depression at 2 years postpartum, controlling for other factors. Our findings align with a recent meta‐analysis of 193 studies examining the association between maternal depression and child behavioral and emotional functioning, which concluded that maternal depression is significantly related to lower levels of positive affect and behavior in young children.29 While depression at all time points measured (during pregnancy, four months postpartum, 1 year postpartum, and 2 years postpartum) were each significant in the bivariate analysis for both child development outcomes in the present study, only depression at 2 years postpartum remained in the final multivariable models, which suggests that current maternal mental health status is closely linked to children's current social‐emotional and behavioral development. Consequently, identifying and intervening to support mothers with symptoms of depression could positively impact child social‐emotional and behavioral development.

Having a mother who reported lower parenting self‐efficacy when their child was 2 years old increased the likelihood that their child would have delayed social‐emotional competencies at age two. Perceived parental efficacy describes “beliefs or judgments a parent holds of their capabilities to organize and execute a set of tasks related to parenting a child.”30 Our findings align with previous research reporting a positive association between parenting self‐efficacy and children's social competence.5 Identifying parents of young children with low parenting self‐efficacy and providing appropriate support and services to enhance parenting self‐efficacy may positively influence children's social‐emotional development.

Children who were not engaged in daily play‐based interactions with their parents at 1 and 2 years were significantly more likely to experience delays in their social‐emotional development at 2 years of age compared with children who were engaged in daily play‐based interactions with their parents. Play contributes to the cognitive, physical, social, and emotional well‐being of children.31 In our cohort, over a third of children were not being engaged in consistent daily play‐based interactions with their parents. Encouraging parents or other caregivers to protect time for daily play and encouraging new parents to engage in imitation play would support social emotional development.

Children with sleep onset delays of 30 minutes or greater were more likely to have delayed social‐emotional abilities and behavior problems compared with children with shorter sleep onset. Similarly, behavior problems were more likely for children who experienced frequent night wakings compared with children with less frequent night wakings. Child sleep problems have previously been linked to child behavior problem, poorer learning capacity, and academic performance.32 This research builds on these findings, adding that poor sleep habits and difficulty initiating sleep, may also have negative consequences on social‐emotional development in young children. Consequently, educating and providing parents with strategies on how to establish healthy sleep habits in infancy and early childhood may protect against adverse social‐emotional and behavioral development outcomes.

Children who did not attend an informal playgroup were more likely to show delays in social‐emotional competencies than children who attended informal playgroups. We posit that the impact of attending informal playgroups on child social‐emotional development may be twofold: (1) they provide an opportunity for mothers to receive social support, previously identified as a protective factor for child development outcomes33 and (2) they provide an opportunity for child socialization, which is associated with the development of social competence.34 Parenting programs and public health professionals could inform parents about the potential benefits of informal playgroups, including opportunities for social support and child socialization, and how these can positively impact child development. Decision makers could encourage communities to provide spaces for parents to congregate to build social support networks, which would enhance well‐being for children and families.

Mothers who reported lower levels of optimism during pregnancy were more likely to have a child with behavioral problems at 2 years of age than mothers with higher levels of optimism during pregnancy. Optimistic individuals adapt to stressors more effectively,35 and mothers who are optimistic may more easily adapt to their new role as a parent and the associated challenges of raising a young child. Optimism has been linked to positive parenting practices, including competence‐promoting parenting practices and effective child management.36 These findings linking maternal optimism to improved coping with stress and positive parenting practices may help to explain how maternal optimism may play a role in children's behavioral development. Although dispositional optimism is a relatively stable trait, research also indicates that, to some extent, optimism can be taught37 and, therefore, parenting intervention and prevention programs for behavior development in young children could include content aimed at cultivating maternal optimism.36

Mothers were asked to describe their ability to fulfill family, work, or other responsibilities (eg, volunteer work, household duties, and other children) since giving birth to their child. Mothers who reported “it is difficult most or all of the time” were more likely to have a child with behavior problems at 2 years of age compared with mothers who reported “it is never or sometimes difficult.” This concept of challenges and conflict among women's life roles has been well documented in the literature38; however, we did not find any previous studies that directly examined the relationship between balancing family, work, and life roles, and child development. Mothers who experience more challenges balancing the conflicting demands from their family, work, and life are likely experiencing challenges with coping. Poor maternal coping has been found to be associated with poor child functioning, including behavior problems, poorer child self‐regulation, and emotional difficulties.39 Challenges with balancing roles is also associated with a number of indicators of maternal wellbeing, including increased rates of depression, stress, anxiety, and lower life satisfaction.40 Supporting new parents with strategies to prevent parents from feeling overwhelmed with balancing their roles and responsibilities provides an opportunity to improve maternal wellbeing, and our results extend the current findings suggesting an opportunity to protect against early child behavioral problems as well.

Having at least 1 hour of daily screen time on any type of media (television, DVDs, movies, computer, tablet) was associated with an increased risk of behavioral problems at age two. A longitudinal study has previously reported an association between early childhood television exposure and behavior problems,41 although this study only examined television viewing time, not total screen time in preschool aged children. Our analysis adds to the current literature by providing support of an association between all forms of screen time and child behavior problems. As society (including preschool aged children) spends more time on tablets, smartphones, and computers, it will be important to consider screen time from all types of media, not just television viewing. To protect against behavior problems at age two, limiting all forms of screen time to less than 1 hour per day may be an important and fairly simple strategy to share with parents of young children, and aligns with recommendations from the Canadian Pediatric Society.42

Children who were exposed to a second language on a regular basis were more likely to have behavior problems at age two than children in single language home settings. The association between exposure to a second language and child behavior has not previously been reported. From the current study, this was a single question that did not ask about the number of hours children were exposed to a second language; therefore, more refined analyses are warranted to investigate this relationship. It is also possible that this variable is a proxy for something else, like cultural influences, which may influence parenting style, discipline strategies, and child behavior.

Although our analyses identified independent risk factors, it is likely that these factors cluster together, and if most or all present, compound the risk of suboptimal social‐emotional competence and behavior; this is suggested by the high predicted probabilities for the outcomes when all risk factors are considered present. Further research could examine a risk profile for suboptimal child outcomes in early childhood, and intervention strategies that target a range of risk factors would be beneficial. For example, many of the factors associated with adverse outcomes in preschool children could be addressed through strategies that normalize help‐seeking behavior for parents of newborns and encourage use of low‐cost community resources. Many parenting programs address content related to efficacy, sleep, mental health, community resources, and child development. Health and child care providers could encourage parents to engage with existing supports and services, which may have the compound benefit of providing parents with skills and strategies for parenting, while creating supportive relationships. Our results also suggest that universal intervention strategies could target maternal depression in early childhood and child sleep hygiene, given that these risk factors were significant for both outcomes. Interaction factors and community engagement were specific to child social‐emotional competencies, while maternal coping, and exposure to screens and a second language were specific to child behavior. Further research is warranted to confirm these factors, which would inform targeted interventions.

4.1. Study limitations

Our outcome measure, the BITSEA, which provided measures of children's social‐emotional competencies and behavior problems, was based on maternal report; it is a screening instrument, and, therefore, follow‐up and more comprehensive evaluation is needed to rule in a diagnosis of a developmental delay or behavioral problem. Multi‐informant information would be valuable for identification of at‐risk children, such as teacher reports in addition to parental reports. Similarly, all independent predictor data was obtained through participants completing questionnaires, and, therefore, outcomes such as maternal depression and anxiety were not based on clinical diagnosis. However, the measures selected for use in the AOB/F study are standardized measures that are widely used by researchers with established satisfactory psychometric properties. The BITSEA and other standardized measures were not validated using a Canadian population; however, previous work shows that maternal mental health measures are valid in the AOF cohort,43 and we have no reason to expect that the BITSEA would perform differently in this sample as compared with the validation sample.

Our analyses are restricted to identifying risk factors for the occurrence of social‐emotional delays and behavior problems at 2 years of age. We are, therefore, unable to examine the extent to which these factors increase the risk for poor outcomes across time. In addition, given that some single items in the BITSEA replicate risk factor constructs (eg, sleep problems) there may be a slight overestimation of the association between factors present at both the exposure and outcome level, and we acknowledge the potential bi‐directional relationships between factors measured at the same time point (eg, child behavior and each of parental self‐efficacy and work‐life balance). Although a similar bi‐directionality might exist between maternal depression and child outcomes, we also are ensuring that reporting bias is accounted for, given that mothers with depressive symptoms might overestimate problematic behavior in their children. There is also potential for reporting bias among mothers with low optimism who may overestimate behavioral challenges in their children. Finally, there is the potential for selection bias and limits to generalizability, given that participants who completed the 2‐year follow‐up differed from those eligible, in that they were more likely to have higher education and income, be married, and Caucasian. As such, caution is warranted in the interpretation of the results in the context of families that are more vulnerable.

5. CONCLUSIONS

Our study extends prior research by analyzing data from a large‐scale, community‐based pregnancy cohort, the AOB/F study, which is representative of the parenting and child population in an urban center in Canada. The AOB/F study also provided the unique opportunity to comprehensively examine an extensive number of contemporary factors, as well as socio‐demographic, gestational, and birth variables. This analysis allowed for the evaluation of numerous factors simultaneously, and the predicted probability of poor outcomes for these exposures. A venue for further research in this cohort would be to examine longitudinal trajectories of children identified at risk, as well as mechanisms underlying associations across time and subgroups of families that would benefit from targeted intervention approaches.

Despite this comprehensive approach to considering a large number of risk factors from the prenatal, birth, early infancy, and early childhood periods, our results showed that in general, the factors most predictive of poor child behavior and social‐emotional development co‐occurred with developmental outcomes at age two. Risk factors co‐occur in families and communities, and the high predicted probability of adverse outcomes associated with exposure to multiple risk factors highlights the need to identify families early and invest appropriately for optimal outcomes.44 The importance of a diverse array of environmental factors can help guide strategies that normalize help seeking and uptake of parenting programs and engagement in community social support opportunities.

Identifying and providing support and intervention to mothers of young children experiencing poor mental health is critical to improving women's mental health and children's psychosocial development. Population‐based strategies that support parents of young families in establishing healthy sleep habits, engaging in daily play, attending informal playgroups, and limiting screen time would enhance children's behavior and social‐emotional development at age two.

CONFLICT OF INTEREST

The authors declare that they have no conflicts of interest.

AUTHOR CONTRIBUTIONS

Conceptualization: Sheila McDonald, Heather Kehler, Suzanne Tough

Formal Analysis: Heather Kehler

Methodology: Sheila McDonald, Heather Kehler, Suzanne Tough

Writing‐Original Draft Preparation: Sheila McDonald, Heather Kehler

Writing‐Review and Editing: Sheila McDonald, Heather Kehler, Suzanne Tough

ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS

We are extremely grateful to all the families who took part in this study and the All Our Babies/Families research team. We are extremely grateful to the investigators, coordinators, research assistants, graduate and undergraduate students, volunteers, clerical staff, and managers. Alberta Innovates Health Solutions provided funding for this cohort (AHFMR Interdisciplinary Team Grants Program #200700595). Additional funding support for cohort maintenance and follow‐up was provided by the Alberta Children's Hospital Foundation and the Max Bell Foundation.

McDonald SW, Kehler HL, Tough SC. Risk factors for delayed social‐emotional development and behavior problems at age two: Results from the All Our Babies/Families (AOB/F) cohort. Health Sci Rep. 2018;1:e82 10.1002/hsr2.82

REFERENCES

  • 1. Briggs‐Gowan MJ, Carter AS, Skuban EM, Horwitz SM. Prevalence of social‐emotional and behavioral problems in a community sample of 1‐and 2‐year‐old children. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2001;40:811‐819. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2. Velderman MK, Crone MR, Wiefferink CH, Reijneveld SA. Identification and management of psychosocial problems among toddlers by preventive child health care professionals. Eur J Public Health. 2010;20:332‐338. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3. Weitzman C, Edmonds D, Davagnino J, Briggs‐Gowan MJ. Young child socioemotional/behavioral problems and cumulative psychosocial risk. Infant Ment Health J. 2014;35:1‐9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4. Thomson KC, Guhn M, Richardson CG, Ark TK, Shoveller J. Profiles of children's social‐emotional health at school entry and associated income, gender and language inequalities: a cross‐sectional population‐based study in British Columbia, Canada. BMJ Open. 2017;7. e015353 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5. Junttila N, Vauras M, Laakkonen E. The role of parenting self‐efficacy in childrenś social and academic behavior. Euro J Psycho Ed. 2007;22:41‐61. [Google Scholar]
  • 6. Campbell SB, Spieker S, Burchinal M, Poe MD. Trajectories of aggression from toddlerhood to age 9 predict academic and social functioning through age 12. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2006;47(8):791‐800. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7. Briggs‐Gowan MJ, Carter AS. Social‐emotional screening status in early childhood predicts elementary school outcomes. Pediatrics. 2008;121:957‐962. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8. Dunn J, Cutting AL. Understanding others, and individual differences in friendship interactions in young children. Soc Dev. 1999. [Google Scholar]
  • 9. Denham SA, Bassett HH, Way E, Mincic M, Zinsser K, Graling K. Preschoolers' emotion knowledge: self‐regulatory foundations, and predictions of early school success. Cognit Emot. 2012;26:667‐679. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10. Elliot J, Prior M, Merrigan C, Ballinger K. Evaluation of a community intervention programme for preschool behaviour problems. J Paediatr Child Health. 2002;38:41‐50. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11. Doyle O, Harmon CP, Heckman JJ, Tremblay RE. Investing in early human development: timing and economic efficiency. Econ Hum Biol. 2009;7:1‐6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12. Heckman JJ. The developmental origins of health. Health Econ. 2012;21:24‐29. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13. US Department of Health, Human Services . Report of the Surgeon General's Conference on Children's Mental Health: a national action agenda. 2000. [PubMed]
  • 14. Committee on Children with Disabilities. Developmental surveillance and screening of infants and young children. Pediatrics. 2001;108:192‐195. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15. Tough SC, McDonald SW, Collisson BA, et al. Cohort profile: the All Our Babies pregnancy cohort (AOB). Int J Epidemiol. 2017;46(5):1389‐1390k. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16. Briggs‐Gowan MJ, Carter AS. BITSEA: Brief Infant‐Toddler Social and Emotional Assessment. Examiner's Manual. Harcourt Assessment; 2006.
  • 17. Briggs‐Gowan MJ, Carter AS, Irwin JR, Wachtel K, Cicchetti DV. The Brief Infant‐Toddler Social and Emotional Assessment: screening for social‐emotional problems and delays in competence. J Pediatr Psychol. 2004;29:143‐155. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18. Cox JL, Holden JM, Sagovsky R. Detection of postnatal depression. Development of the 10‐item Edinburgh Postnatal Depression Scale. Br J Psychiatry. 1987;150:782‐786. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19. Spielberger CD, Gorsuch RL, Lushene R, Vagg PR, Jacobs GA. Manual for the State‐Trait Anxiety Inventory. Consulting Psychologists Press; 1983. [Google Scholar]
  • 20. Cohen S, Kamarck T, Mermelstein R. A global measure of perceived stress. J Health Soc Behav. 1983;24:385‐396. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21. Sherbourne CD, Stewart AL. The MOS social support survey. Soc Sci Med. 1991;32:705‐714. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22. Scheier MF, Carver CS, Bridges MW. Distinguishing optimism from neuroticism (and trait anxiety, self‐mastery, and self‐esteem): a reevaluation of the Life Orientation Test. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1994;67:1063‐1078. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23. Statistics Canada . Microdata User Guide, National Longitudental Survey of Children and Youth, Cycle 7, September 2006 to July 2007.
  • 24. Radloff LS. The CES‐D scale a self‐report depression scale for research in the general population. Appl Psychol Measur. 1977;1:385‐401. [Google Scholar]
  • 25. Johnston C, Mash EJ. A measure of parenting satisfaction and efficacy. J Clin Child Psychol. 1989;18:167‐175. [Google Scholar]
  • 26. McDonald S, Kehler H, Bayrampour H, Fraser‐Lee N, Tough S. Risk and protective factors in early child development: results from the All Our Babies (AOB) pregnancy cohort. Res Dev Disabil. 2016;58:20‐30. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27. McDonald SW, Hicks M, Rasmussen C, Nagulesapillai T, Cook J, Tough SC. Characteristics of women who consume alcohol before and after pregnancy recognition in a Canadian sample: a prospective cohort study. Alcohol Clin Exp Res. 2014;38:3008‐3016. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28. Bayrampour H, McDonald S, Tough S. Risk factors of transient and persistent anxiety during pregnancy. Midwifery. 2015;31:582‐589. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29. Goodman SH, Rouse MH, Connell AM, Broth MR, Hall CM, Heyward D. Maternal depression and child psychopathology: a meta‐analytic review. Clin Child Fam Psychol Rev. 2011;14:1‐27. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30. Montigny F, Lacharité C. Perceived parental efficacy: concept analysis. J Adv Nurs. 2005;49:387‐396. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31. Ginsburg KR. The importance of play in promoting healthy child development and maintaining strong parent‐child bonds. Pediatrics. 2007;119:182‐191. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32. Dewald JF, Meijer AM, Oort FJ, Kerkhof GA, Bögels SM. The influence of sleep quality, sleep duration and sleepiness on school performance in children and adolescents: A meta‐analytic review. Sleep Med Rev. 2010;14:179‐189. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33. Huang CY, Costeines J, Ayala C, Kaufman JS. Parenting stress, social support, and depression for ethnic minority adolescent mothers: impact on child development. J Child Fam Stud. 2014;23:255‐262. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34. Early Child Care Research Network . Child care and children's peer interaction at 24 and 36 months: the NICHD study of early child care. Child Dev. 2001;72:1478‐1500. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35. Wrosch C, Scheier MF. Personality and quality of life: the importance of optimism and goal adjustment. Qual Life Res. 2003;12(Suppl 1):59‐72. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36. Jones DJ, Forehand R, Brody GH, Armistead L. Positive parenting and child psychosocial adjustment in inner‐city single‐parent African American families: the role of maternal optimism. Behav Modif. 2002;26:464‐481. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37. Seligman M. Positive psychology, positive prevention, and positive therapy In Snyder C. & Lopez S. (Eds). Handbook of positive psychology. New York: Oxford; 2002. [Google Scholar]
  • 38. Amstad FT, Meier LL, Fasel U, Elfering A, Semmer NK. A meta‐analysis of work‐family conflict and various outcomes with a special emphasis on cross‐domain versus matching‐domain relations. J Occup Health Psychol. 2011;16:151. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39. Crnic KA, Gaze C, Hoffman C. Cumulative parenting stress across the preschool period: Relations to maternal parenting and child behaviour at age 5. Infant Child Dev. 2005;14:117‐132. [Google Scholar]
  • 40. Allen TD, Herst DE, Bruck CS, Sutton M. Consequences associated with work‐to‐family conflict: a review and agenda for future research. J Occup Health Psychol. 2000;5:278‐308. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41. Cheng S, Maeda T, Yoichi S, Yamagata Z, Tomiwa K. Japan Children's Study G. Early television exposure and children's behavioral and social outcomes at age 30 months. J Epidemiol. 2010;20(Suppl 2):S482‐S489. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42. Canadian Paediatric Society . Digital Health Task Force. Screen time and young children: promoting health and development in a digital world. Paediatrics Child Health. 2017;22:461‐468. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43. Benediktsson I, McDonald S, Tough S. Examining the psychometric properties of three standardized screening tools in a pregnant and parenting population. Matern Child Health J. 2017;21:253‐259. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44. Evans GW, Li D, Whipple SS. Cumulative risk and child development. Psychol Bull. 2013;139:1342. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Articles from Health Science Reports are provided here courtesy of Wiley

RESOURCES