Skip to main content
Springer logoLink to Springer
. 2018 Apr 18;39(5):749–775. doi: 10.1007/s10764-018-0027-9

Breaking Through Disciplinary Barriers: Human–Wildlife Interactions and Multispecies Ethnography

Hannah E Parathian 1, Matthew R McLennan 2,3, Catherine M Hill 2, Amélia Frazão-Moreira 1,4, Kimberley J Hockings 1,4,5,
PMCID: PMC6267646  PMID: 30573938

Abstract

One of the main challenges when integrating biological and social perspectives in primatology is overcoming interdisciplinary barriers. Unfamiliarity with subject-specific theory and language, distinct disciplinary-bound approaches to research, and academic boundaries aimed at “preserving the integrity” of subject disciplines can hinder developments in interdisciplinary research. With growing interest in how humans and other primates share landscapes, and recognition of the importance of combining biological and social information to do this effectively, the disparate use of terminology is becoming more evident. To tackle this problem, we dissect the meaning of what the biological sciences term studies in “human–wildlife conflict” or more recently “human–wildlife interactions” and compare it to what anthropology terms “multispecies ethnography.” In the biological sciences, human–wildlife interactions are the actions resulting from people and wild animals sharing landscapes and resources, with outcomes ranging from being beneficial or harmful to one or both species. In the social sciences, human–nonhuman relationships have been explored on a philosophical, analytical, and empirical level. Building on previous work, we advocate viewing landscapes through an interdisciplinary “multispecies lens” in which humans are observed as one of multiple organisms that interact with other species to shape and create environments. To illustrate these interconnections we use the case study of coexistence between people of the Nalu ethnic group and Critically Endangered western chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes verus) at Cantanhez National Park in Guinea-Bissau, to demonstrate how biological and social research approaches can be complementary and can inform conservation initiatives at the human–primate interface. Finally, we discuss how combining perspectives from ethnoprimatology with those from multispecies ethnography can advance the study of ethnoprimatology to aid productive discourse and enhance future interdisciplinary research.

Keywords: Conservation conflict, Ethnoprimatology, Human–wildlife conflict, Human–wildlife interactions, Interdisciplinary research, Multispecies ethnography, Primate conservation


Humans have presumably coexisted with nonhuman primates (hereafter primates) throughout our evolution, yet there can be little doubt that today humans and primates share landscapes to an unprecedented extent (Humle and Hill 2016; McKinney 2015; Paterson and Wallis 2005). Identifying strategies to overcome constraints to sustainable coexistence must become a priority for conservation if primates are to survive the Anthropocene (the current geological epoch of human dominance of geological, biological, and chemical processes on Earth, usually dating from 1945 in ecology and conservation; Corlett 2015) (Estrada et al. 2017; Fuentes and Wolfe 2002; Hockings et al. 2015; McLennan et al. 2017). To develop effective, locally appropriate strategies to conserve primates and other wildlife, it is essential to understand human social and cultural variables alongside wildlife behavioral and population patterns. This requires a combination of social science and biological science methods of inquiry (Bennett et al. 2017a, b; Dore et al. 2017; Jost Robinson and Remis 2014; Mascia et al. 2003; Redford 2011; Setchell et al. 2017; Wolverton et al. 2014). Conservation biology increasingly engages with social science, including anthropology, sociology, political ecology, and psychology (Daily and Ehrlich 1999; Mascia et al. 2003; Newing 2010; Teel et al. 2018), yet interdisciplinary barriers to communication can hinder development of productive discourse (Bennett et al. 2017b; Decker et al. 1987; Fox et al. 2006; Fuentes 2006). Potential collaborations are restricted through disparate academic terminologies and use of vocabulary often understood only by those with subject-specific knowledge (Moon and Blackman 2014). In the biological sciences humans are considered part of nature in an evolutionary sense, but are traditionally viewed as separate from nature in an ecological sense (Sponsel 1997). In accordance with this perspective, until recently primatologists and other biological scientists interested in the adaptive significance of behaviors sought to study animals in so-called “natural” environments, supposedly free of human influence. Consequently, there was less interest in the bidirectional interactions between people and wild animals, despite the fact that humans have long been a part of most ecosystems where primates and other wildlife are studied (Hockings et al. 2015; Riley 2006; Tutin and Oslisly 1995). In contrast, social scientists among other scholars in the humanities (e.g., Gillespie and Collard 2015; Keil 2016; Rose et al. 2012; Wilkie 2015) have considered the multiple possible realities perceived by diverse human communities and individuals that are shaped by religious and cultural beliefs, historical and social backgrounds, and ontological reasoning. Among human societies, people’s associations with wildlife range from ambiguous species boundaries and holistic concepts of nature that unite people, plants, animals, and supernatural beings to much more dualistic understandings whereby humans and animals, including primates, are considered as very separate entities, occupying distinct spaces (Aisher 2007; Aisher and Damodaran 2016).

Ethnoprimatology has taken steps toward combining social and biological science approaches to develop a more holistic understanding of primate ecology and conservation (e.g., Fuentes 2010a, 2012; Hardin and Remis 2006; Jost Robinson and Remis 2014; Malone et al. 2014; Remis and Hardin 2009; Remis and Jost Robinson 2017; Riley 2006, 2013; Sponsel 1997), but disciplinary barriers persist. To tackle this problem, we examine differences in the meanings of some commonly used terminology in the biological and social sciences. Specifically, we dissect the meaning of what the biological sciences (including primatology) term studies in “human–wildlife conflict” or more recently “human–wildlife interactions” (e.g., Hockings 2016; Humle and Hill 2016; Woodroffe et al. 2005) and compare it to what the social sciences term “multispecies ethnography” (e.g., Haraway 2008; Kirksey and Helmreich 2010). Using examples from ethnoprimatology we highlight the “distinct” approaches of biological and social science and discuss how combining them can enhance our understanding of shared landscapes and advance research at the human–primate interface. As we demonstrate using research examples in the text that follows, the distinction between these two approaches is increasingly blurred as biologically trained scientists seek qualitative nuance, and as socially trained scientists seek quantitative data on the nonhuman agents living among and influencing the behavior and lives of their human neighbors (Rust et al. 2017). We then critically interrogate a number of key concepts and advocate for an integration of multispecies approaches with ethnoprimatology (Fuentes 2010a; Malone et al. 2014; Palmer and Malone 2018; Remis and Jost Robinson 2017). Finally, we illustrate these links using a case study of coexistence between people of the Nalu ethnic group and Critically Endangered western chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes verus) at Cantanhez National Park in Guinea-Bissau, West Africa, to demonstrate how disciplinary theories, descending from biological and social science, can be combined and applied practically through interdisciplinary research approaches.

Biological Sciences: Recognizing the Value of Social Science to Conservation

Biological approaches to understanding human–wildlife interactions and ecological relationships are grounded in the disciplines of behavioral ecology and conservation biology, originally the domain of ecologists and zoologists. Behavioral ecology is concerned mainly with the causes, evolution, and adaptive variation in behavior of individuals, whereas conservation biology has an interest in populations, especially their response to disturbance or environmental changes caused by humans (Caro and Eadie 2005; Sih et al. 2011). Using principles from ecology, population genetics, and systematics, conservation biology seeks to describe biological diversity and identify ways to conserve species and ecosystems (Mascia et al. 2003; Simberloff 1988). In the biological and conservation sciences, there has been a predominant focus on the ways in which wildlife “conflict” with the interests of humans (Angelici 2016; Messmer 2009; Paterson and Wallis 2005; Treves and Karanth 2003), often with a goal to identify general “large-scale” trends in the nature of interactions (e.g., Inskip and Zimmerman 2009; Seoraj-Pillai and Pillay 2017). For the last 20 years or so some primatologists (especially those who received their training within Anthropology departments), and some biologists, have approached human–wildlife interactions as dynamic and bidirectional (e.g., Fuentes and Wolfe 2002; Humle and Hill 2016; Hurn 2017; McLennan et al. 2017; Redpath et al. 2013; Wheatley 1999), though this remains a minority approach. While identifying large-scale trends is important, “small-scale” site-specific data are also needed to fully understand the diverse ways in which humans and wildlife interact in shared heterogeneous landscapes (Hockings 2016). Today, conservationists increasingly recognize that the success of conservation policies and practice inherently depends on understanding and addressing human social phenomena (Bennett et al. 2017b; Berkes 2004; Redford 2011; Redpath et al. 2013), and where conservation interventions pay inadequate attention to social factors they fail to conserve target species and ecosystems (e.g., Agrawal and Redford 2006; McLennan and Hill 2013; Rönnbäck et al. 2003; Rust et al. 2016).

Social science disciplines include subjects such as anthropology, psychology, sociology, politics, and international studies, and therefore have analytical tools that explain and predict patterns of human behavior and attempt to find meaning behind cultural or subjective phenomena. These offer unique and important insights into a given society’s understanding of their associations with wildlife, which has strong relevance for conservation practice and outcomes (Mascia et al. 2003). For example, social and cultural anthropology methods of inquiry can document the spiritual value of biodiversity to people. In primatology, this can be applied to identify conservation-relevant cultural beliefs and values that serve as foundations for formal regulations that protect primate species and habitats, or help guide locally appropriate conservation initiatives (Baker et al. 2014; Etiendem et al. 2011; Remis and Hardin 2009; Jones et al. 2008; Jost Robinson and Remis 2014; Köhler 2005; Wheatley 1999; Yamakoshi and Leblan 2013). While government and nongovernmental organizations (NGOs) increasingly take steps to integrate social science information into conservation decision making and long-term environmental management, success is still hampered by economic and institutional challenges. These include conflicts between stakeholders, inadequate financial support for local monitoring and governance (Sandker et al. 2009), and legal frameworks (specifically tenure and economic laws) that can present significant constraints to the longevity of such interventions (Pasquini et al. 2011). Barriers to effective collaboration and understanding between social and biological scientists and conservation practitioners further impede these developments (Fox et al. 2006).

Social Sciences: Embracing the Complexity of Human–Animal Relationships

Anthropologists have repeatedly challenged environmental discourse that oversimplifies the complex relationships between humans and nonhuman species (e.g., Atran 1999; Descola 1994; Ingold 2000; Kohn 2007, 2013). Some of the earliest scientific studies addressing the intersection between biology, culture, and sociality originate in the discipline of ethnobiology. Ethnobiology encompasses botany, zoology, and ecology, and is broadly defined as the study of how living things are treated or used by different human cultures (Ellen 2006). While ethnobiology once focused largely on studies of folk classification (Bulmer 1967; Conklin 1954; Ellen 2006; Hunn 1977), today it is recognized essentially as the study of how people from different cultures conceptualize, represent, use, and manage their knowledge of environments and living organisms. As Ellen (2006, p. 3) suggests, “ethnobiology – like anthropology more broadly – seeks to go beyond the local, to compare such knowledge and its consequences between different human populations, and to establish generalizations that are valid at the regional, global, and species level.”

There has been a proliferation of interdisciplinary terms and fields of study by anthropologists, sociologists, and human geographers particularly, as they explore ways of incorporating nonhuman species into social science research. From Lestel’s “ecoanthropology and ethnobiology” (Lestel et al. 2006; Lestel and Taylor 2013) to Haraway’s (2010) “companion species,” researchers have endeavored to develop innovative frameworks for conceptualizing relationships between human and nonhuman species. For example, anthrozoology draws from various disciplines including anthropology, psychology, and zoology to examine human–animal relationships in relation to animal representations, symbols, and stories, and their physical presence in human societies (York and Mancus 2013). Meanwhile, zooanthropology explores relationship dynamics between humans and animals with a focus on animal sentience and well-being (Aerts et al. 2016; Marchesini 2016). As the name suggests, ethnoethology explores the methodological overlap of ethnology and ethology, examining the characteristics of different peoples and their relationships with animals and ecosystems (see Glossary for further examples). Early examples of interdisciplinary research in primatology include the work of Barbara Smuts and Shirley Strum, whose accounts of baboon groups in Tanzania and Kenya transgress the positivist norms of ethology as an observational science (Despret 2013; Smuts 2009; Strum 1987). Although these fields of study adopt differing perspectives, they offer useful methods for overcoming nature–culture duality and have been used to examine human–primate interactions and social representations of primates (for examples with African great apes: Giles-Vernick and Rupp 2006; Köhler 2005; Lingomo and Kimura 2009; Oishi 2013; Richards 1995; see also Jost Robinson and Remis 2014). Such studies provide insights into local understandings of nature that are highly relevant to establishing locally appropriate conservation practices. For example, interdisciplinary studies have revealed how Western-dominated ideals vs local perceptions of wildlife influence support, or lack thereof, for conservation (Jalais 2008), and how the choice of conservation flagship species needs to be appropriate to the target audience, taking into account local attitudes toward, beliefs about, and experience of local species (Sousa et al. 2018).

Ethnoprimatology as an Interdisciplinary Study

Traditional Western primatology (compared to Japanese primatology; Asquith 1986 and de Waal 2001 provide comparisons of the two) has strived to adopt an objective view of the biological and psychological similarities between humans and primates. In contrast to the approach of traditional field primatology, ethnoprimatology aims to acquire an anthropological understanding of primates through examining their associations with human cultures and societies (Fuentes and Wolfe 2002; Papworth et al. 2013; Paterson and Wallis 2005; Sponsel 1997). Ethnoprimatological research employs mixed methods and embraces a multidisciplinary theoretical perspective to examine the multifarious interactions and interfaces at integrated and shared ecological and social spaces (Fuentes 2012; Hockings et al. 2015; Sponsel 1997). The goal of many ethnoprimatology studies is to engage with the needs of local human populations to enhance primate conservation and ensure the longevity of conservation projects by understanding the biological and social dynamics between humans and primates (Cormier 2010; Fuentes 2012; Jost Robinson and Remis 2014; Lee 2010; Malone et al. 2014; Papworth et al. 2013; Riley 2013; Wheatley 1999). The ethnoprimatological approach is described by Fuentes et al. (2017, p. 297) as “a mosaic of approaches that is developing, and reshaping, the ways in which humans position themselves relative to nonhuman primates (NHPs), and the ways in which NHPs are seen as agential in human-dominated landscapes, ecologies, and lifeways.” Social anthropologists have sought similar understandings of human–primate relationships. For example, ethnographic studies of traditional people’s understandings of African great apes incorporate local knowledge systems into conservation narratives (Etiendem et al. 2011; Giles-Vernick and Rupp 2006; Köhler 2005; Lingomo and Kimura 2009; Oishi 2013; Richards 1995). As with other interdisciplinary approaches discussed above, ethnoprimatology demonstrates an epistemological affinity between biological and sociocultural anthropology by acknowledging humans as active and integral members of biological communities (Leblan 2013; Riley 2006, 2010, 2013).

Growing enthusiasm for the ethnoprimatology approach, and recognition among conservation funding agencies that (for ethical and practical reasons) conservation in most instances is unsuccessful without integrating the needs of local people, has encouraged recent developments in primatology. The predominant emphasis on conflict and competition in studies of human–primate interactions (McLennan et al. 2017; Paterson and Wallis 2005) is gradually giving way to a greater appreciation of the complexities of these relationships, including “positive” interactions (Frank 2016). For example, research at Bossou in the Republic of Guinea showed how consumption of cultivated cocoa by western chimpanzees, and subsequent dispersal of seeds, led to the widespread distribution of cocoa plants in the habitat, benefitting both local farmers and chimpanzees (Hockings et al. 2017). In parallel, there have been calls for a linguistic shift in how human–primate interactions are framed and described (e.g., from “crop raiding” with its aggressive connotations to a more neutral “crop feeding” or “crop foraging”; Hill 2005, 2015, 2017; Hill et al. 2017). It is now broadly accepted that humans are key components of ecosystems where primates live (Fuentes and Wolfe 2002; Hockings et al. 2015; McKinney 2015; McLennan et al. 2017). Rather than viewing human communities and practices as uniformly damaging to natural habitats, the traditional methods that local people have used and adapted over millennia to manage and monitor landscapes are increasingly acknowledged as potentially useful foundations for developing practical conservation strategies (Berkes et al. 2000; Thompson et al. 2008; Yamakoshi and Leblan 2013).

Studies examining the social constituents of primate conservation have revealed that people’s views of primates are influenced by political, social, and economic factors, which are not fixed but change over time (e.g., Hill and Webber 2010 in Uganda; Parathian and Maldonado 2010 in the Colombian Amazon). Other studies demonstrate how unique belief systems and human–primate associations can support protection of primate species, for example, long-tailed macaques (Macaca fascicularis) in Bali: Fuentes et al. (2005) and Wheatley (1999); and Tonkean and booted macaques (Macaca tonkeana and Macaca ochreata) in Sulawesi: Riley (2007, 2008, 2010); Riley and Fuentes (2011); and Riley and Priston (2010). Similarly, a study by Etiendem and colleagues discusses how traditional totemic beliefs about Cross River gorillas (Gorilla gorilla diehli) in southwest Cameroon can be revived and promoted to foster positive attitudes to gorilla conservation (Etiendem et al. 2011). While significant progress has been made in the field of ethnoprimatology, further developments are essential in terms of primatologists adopting mixed epistemologies and methodologies. Moreover, until recently there have been few sources of funding available to provide graduate training to link disparate fields or offer financial support to projects that study complex interactions through interdisciplinary concepts and practice (Fuentes et al. 2017; Palsson et al. 2013).

Barriers to Interdisciplinary Communication

Recognizing that conservation is as much about people as about other species and habitats requires significant modifications to how science is used and applied in conservation. Bennett et al. (2017b) outline major barriers to the meaningful integration of social science into conservation science, stemming from unfamiliarity with subject-specific principles, limited collaboration, and academic boundaries aimed at “preserving the integrity” of subject disciplines. Academic researchers are usually trained in traditional disciplines and may lack the tools or willingness to make bridges between fields. They may have differing “theories of knowledge,” including their philosophies, worldviews, and epistemologies, which can lead to incompatible ways of perceiving human–wildlife interactions or approaching research into these phenomena (Moon and Blackman 2014; Rust et al. 2017). For example, in a study exploring the environmental impacts of deforestation the social scientist may begin by talking to people in a local village to understand the effects on human behavior, while the natural scientist may begin by exploring ecological indicators (Bennett et al. 2017b). Furthermore, discipline-specific language and the different theories applied to understand particular topics can be inaccessible to nonspecialists or specialists in other subjects. Issues of familiarity with the diverse literature and associated nuances in language can present additional obstacles (Lemke 2001), while subject-specific discourse used by social scientists and biologists presents boundaries to cross-disciplinary collaboration. The language used by social scientists can be intentionally ambiguous, to reflect alternative worldviews of cultures that oppose Western dichotomized notions of nature (Descola 2014; Kohn 2007), and/or to challenge preconceived ideas and assumptions about the world that characterize a Western scientific approach. For these reasons, biological anthropologists trained to be objective, realist, and positivist in their research approach can find these concepts difficult to grasp. They may view social studies as too time-consuming (when conservation decisions often need to be made rapidly), or vague and “esoteric.” Conversely, social anthropologists tend to consider biological methods as overly pragmatic and rigid in their application (especially as real-world problems are complex). This can lead to important but not immediately visible information being overlooked. The core beliefs and ideas of these disciplines can appear so different that biological and social scientists have been said to come from different “academic cultures” (Morris 1969; Sutherland 1998; see Glossary). This may indeed be true, but as Kohn reminds us: “The goal [in multispecies ethnography] should not just be to give voice, agency or subjectivity to the nonhuman—to recognize them as others, visible in their difference—but to force us to radically rethink these categories of our analysis as they pertain to all beings [March 29, 2010]” (in Kirksey and Helmreich 2010, p. 563).

In biological anthropology sympatric species are viewed as individuals engaged in bidirectional dyadic relationships, which meet temporally or have some impact on each other’s lives, for example, by affecting the availability of certain resources or shaping forest habitats in certain ways. These “human–wildlife interactions” result in either positive or negative outcomes for one or both species. By comparison, in social anthropology humans and wildlife (including nonhuman organisms broadly) are considered as close companions (Haraway 2010, 2016), innately and immutably linked through complex ecological, historical, social, cultural, and political networks; for examples, see Locke’s (2013, 2017) exploration of human–elephant relations in Asia, and Jost Robinson and Remis’s (2014) analysis of the mutual ecologies of “the hunter and hunted” in Central Africa. These ideas describe the long-term mutual exchange and emergence of human and nonhuman companions including other primates.

In the social sciences the term “multispecies ethnography” refers to a methodological approach and theoretical perspective proposed to enable the understanding of habitats as “multispecies landscapes” (see Glossary). Multispecies ethnography introduces a posthumanist perspective that deconstructs the “humanism” of landscapes. It recognizes that “other-than-humans” exist, and explores human social and cultural phenomena with respect to people’s relationships with other species through a network of interspecies encounters. Kirksey and Helmreich’s (2010) proposal for a “multispecies ethnography” has gained considerable support, as it allows broader manifestations of nonhuman organisms to appear alongside humans as animated beings (Baynes-Rock 2013; Lestel and Taylor 2013). Multispecies studies perceive nonhumans acting with “agency and intent” (see Glossary), while some definitions draw on understandings from Actor–Network Theory that considers agency as an effect rather than the product of subjective intentionality (Ogden et al. 2013 provide a detailed explanation) (Locke 2017). In this perspective, “creatures previously appearing on the margins of anthropology – as part of the landscape, as food for humans, as symbols – [are] pressed into the foreground of recent ethnographies” (Kirksey and Helmreich 2010, p. 545). Similar ideas have been described as “a more-than-human approach to ethnographic research” (Locke and Münster 2015, p. 1) and “an anthropology beyond the human” (Kohn 2013).

Viewing humans and nonhuman species as interacting organisms that shape and create ecosystems reflects the worldviews of many animist communities (Descola 1994; Ingold 2000, 2011; Kohn 2013). For some human groups, such as the Nyishi people of upland Arunachal Pradesh in northeast India, “animated beings” extend to include natural entities and supernatural beings as well as living organisms (Aisher 2007; Aisher and Damodaran 2016). Therefore, approaching primate conservation through a multispecies lens and understanding habitats as multispecies landscapes not only supports the conservation of wildlife for its intrinsic value, regardless of function or value to humans (Pearson 2016); it also promotes the cultural diversity of local communities. It acknowledges alternative realities that guide a conceptual shift toward environments being viewed and managed with respect to the ontologies of local people, which could improve the long-term outcomes of conservation initiatives (Keil 2016).

The idea that humans and nonhuman species shape environments through their interactions with each other is also explored in the biological sciences through niche construction (Barker and Odling-Smee 2014; Day et al. 2003; Odling-Smee et al. 2013) and through natureculture “contact zones”—terms adopted from the social sciences (Fuentes 2010a; Riley and Fuentes 2011) (see Glossary). In ethnoprimatology, Fuentes (2010a) employs the biological “niche construction model” and theory of “mutual ecologies” (Barker and Odling-Smee 2014; Odling-Smee et al. 2003) alongside Haraway’s (2008) “contact zones” (see Glossary) to describe the interface between tourists and long-tailed macaques (Macaca fascicularis) at temples in Bali. His description of “natureculture contact zones” (Fuentes 2012) recognizes that broad species characteristics as well as individual idiosyncracies are both the cause and outcome of the ways in which individuals act and interact. Sympatric primate species maintain their individuality, yet their destinies are united through historical events and embedded in shared environments (Haraway 1997, 2010). Fuentes argues that in doing so, the boundaries separating humans and macaques are broken down as the overlapping ecologies of these coexisting species generate coproduced niches. Ecological interactions are incorporated alongside social, historical, political, and economic drivers demonstrating that the inclusion of anthropological elements is core to primatological inquiry.

Ethnoprimatology deepens our understanding of human–primate coexistence by exploring overlapping ecologies at the human–primate interface, and integrating multispecies approaches with ethnoprimatology takes this concept a step further (Fuentes 2010a; Malone et al. 2014; Palmer and Malone 2018; Remis and Jost Robinson 2017). Combining ideas from multispecies ethnography (such as viewing environments as multispecies landscapes) with terminologies already applied in ethnoprimatology (such as the coexistence of sympatric species in shared ecological and social spaces) encourages researchers to revise the way they think and talk about environments and nonhuman species. This perspective helps deconstruct deep-seated preconceptions about the “humanism” of places and habitats and allows focus on the connections between multiple species (including people and primates) (Locke and Münster 2015). The case study that follows describes research carried out by three of our authors (K. J. Hockings, A. Frazão-Moreira, and H. E. Parathian) between January 2012 and November 2013 to explore coexistence between humans and other primates in Guinea-Bissau, West Africa. It illustrates how combined methods and theories from ethnoprimatology and multispecies ethnography can be applied through interdisciplinary research approaches to explore the connections between humans and primates sharing ecological and social spaces, and how this information can be used to inform conservation guidelines.

A Cross-Disciplinary Understanding of Human–Chimpanzee Coexistence at Cantanhez National Park, Guinea-Bissau

Cantanhez National Park (CNP) is located in the southern Tombali administrative region of Cubucaré in Guinea-Bissau (Fig. 1). Covering an area of 1067 km2, the park is a mosaic of settlements, agricultural fields, subhumid and secondary forest, mangrove, and savanna. Six ethnic groups live within CNP with a total human population of ca. 22,500 individuals (Temudo 2009). Historically, all ethnic groups apart from the Balanta (who adopted Christianity alongside animism) were Islamized during the late nineteenth and early twentieth century. This led to the regional assimilation of Islamic and animist beliefs and practices (Frazão-Moreira 2009, 2010; Sousa et al. 2017). The Nalu ethnic group was among those people who were Islamized. The Nalu practice swidden agriculture and harvest wild resources for a range of uses, and their traditional practices link spirits (irã) and ancestors to local territory and Nalu homeland (see Frazão-Moreira 2009, 2016b). The forests of CNP are also inhabited by western chimpanzees whose range covers part of the protected area legally recognized as Nalu homeland (including the population of chimpanzees that were the focus of our study; Bessa et al. 2015; Hockings and Sousa 2012, 2013). As occurs elsewhere in tropical Africa (Hockings and McLennan 2016; McLennan and Hockings 2016), people and chimpanzees at CNP encounter each other frequently on roads, paths, in agricultural fields, and in the forest, and overlap in their use of wild and cultivated resources (Fig. 2a–c) (Hockings and Sousa 2012, 2013; Sousa 2009; Sousa and Frazão-Moreira 2010). Following the formation of CNP in 2008 the Nalu maintained ownership over part of the forest and have continued to play a role in its management, including the distribution of land to incoming settlers (Frazão-Moreira 2009, 2010). Therefore, our research focused on interactions between chimpanzees and Nalu people in particular. The known complexity of factors influencing the availability and management of resources in CNP, as well as limited data on overlapping habitat and resource use by people and chimpanzees, impelled us to design and implement a mixed-methods approach. We explored these dynamics from a multispecies perspective, combining ethnoprimatology with multispecies ethnography, which further integrates anthropological and biological approaches.

Fig. 1.

Fig. 1

Map showing Cantanhez National Park in Guinea-Bissau, West Africa.

Fig. 2.

Fig. 2

a Local people and chimpanzees encountering each other on a road in Cantanhez National Park (photo by K. Hockings). b A cyclist passing a chimpanzee that is crossing the road in Cantanhez National Park (photo by K. Hockings). c An adult male chimpanzee transporting cultivated oranges in an agricultural field next to the village (photo by J. Bessa).

Previous Studies in CNP

Previous ethnographic and botanical accounts among Nalu people have resulted in in-depth and insightful publications on indigenous plant use (Catarino et al. 2008; Frazão-Moreira 2009, 2010, 2016a, b), while ethnoprimatological studies have explored Nalu relationships with chimpanzees in CNP (Costa et al. 2017; Hockings and Sousa 2013; Sousa and Frazão-Moreira 2010; Sousa et al. 2014, 2017, 2018). These studies show that Nalu people have a syncretic Islamic-animist view toward animals, which combines the idea that “dari i pekador” (“the chimpanzee is human”) and the general belief that all nonhuman species have reputed access to resources in ancestral lands, with Muslim aram, which prevents the killing and eating of any animal with canine teeth, including primates. The Nalu recognize the similarities chimpanzees share with humans both physically and behaviorally (e.g., “Dari are like humans because they walk without putting their hands on the ground” and “They are like us. They use the same plants that we use”) (Sousa and Frazão-Moreira 2010). Other ethnographic accounts suggest Nalu people’s attitudes toward chimpanzees in CNP stem from an animist ontology which guides local beliefs that nonhuman species exist either as “true animals,” or some other animal form transformed by irãs (Sousa et al. 2017, 2018). This idea that humans and great apes shape-shift into each other’s physical forms is shared by people elsewhere in West and Central Africa (Giles-Vernick and Rupp 2006; Hockings et al. 2010; Köhler 2005; Leblan and Bricka 2013; Oishi 2013; Richards 1995). The underlying components of a pre-Islamic ontology combined with Muslim beliefs is key to understanding human–chimpanzee coexistence in CNP (Costa et al. 2017; Sousa and Frazão-Moreira 2010).

As well as processes of religious and cultural syncretism, local perceptions have evolved in CNP with conservation and ecotourism development supporting the protection of chimpanzees (Costa et al. 2017; Sousa et al. 2014, 2017, 2018). Despite strict beliefs that prevent chimpanzee hunting and consumption of their meat, according to some Nalu people conflicts between people and chimpanzees occurred in the past over highly valued agricultural resources (e.g., cultivated fruits and cash crops such as oranges and papaya). This reportedly led to potentially accidental killings of chimpanzees, where chimpanzees were shot at by local people to keep them away from crops (specifically during harvest and fruiting seasons) (Sousa and Frazão-Moreira 2010). However, since the active promotion of chimpanzee conservation and “ecotourism” by outside agencies and local NGOs, villagers claimed they no longer shot at chimpanzees for fear of retribution from the authorities (Sousa and Frazão-Moreira 2010). Other social science research suggests tensions exist between local people and NGOs, and that the sense of an urgent need to conserve wildlife in CNP—conveyed by National Park authorities and conservationists working in the region—is not always shared by local people (Sousa et al. 2018; Temudo 2012). For example, Temudo (2012) argues that outside agencies have constructed a need for conservation intervention in CNP based on inaccurate predictions (of the rate of deforestation, and the growth of human population densities) and the oversight of Nalu natural resource management institutions and practices, resulting in negative consequences for local people.

Our Research Team and Field Study Approach

Our field research team comprised primatologists with broad experience in human–chimpanzee coexistence in Africa (K. J. Hockings, C. Sousa) as well as social scientists with long-term fieldwork experience among rural communities in Guinea-Bissau (A. Frazão-Moreira, H. E. Parathian), and three of our researchers had designed and implemented mixed-methods studies in previous research projects (K. J. Hockings, A. Frazão-Moreira, H. E. Parathian). Combining multispecies ethnography with ethnoprimatology, we began by carrying out an in-depth ethnography of Nalu beliefs and practices associated with the forest and primates to examine the connections between Nalu people and chimpanzees in CNP. Qualitative social data added context to quantitative findings and provided information about current local attitudes toward chimpanzees. This provided a strong starting point from where we were able to explore the influence of individual species behaviors, and the impact of interspecies interactions, on the local landscape from a multispecies perspective. To further explore human–chimpanzee coexistence and resource-sharing at a social, historical, and ecological level, and the influence of local Nalu cultural and religious beliefs on these dynamics, we employed tools from ethnoprimatology and ethnobotany. Over eleven months the social science researchers (H. E. Parathian and A. Frazão-Moreira) and biological science researchers (K. J. Hockings, C. Sousa, and J. Bessa) conducted complementary research on the use of wild and cultivated resources by sympatric humans and chimpanzees using comparative methods. We collected quantitative data (through direct observation, feeding traces, and fecal analysis) to determine which plants and plant parts were consumed by chimpanzees. We compared these data with quantitative data on human plant use (collected through participant observation, semistructured interviews, and all-occurrence sampling). Finally, we carried out spatial mapping to identify overlapping areas where humans and chimpanzees used plants, providing a visual representation of the CNP forest as a multispecies landscape shared and shaped by sympatric species.

Openness, trust, and good communication among our field team were key to the smooth running of our study. Project planning took place with input from our biological and social science researchers to limit misunderstandings and prevent disciplinary disputes between researchers from different academic fields. We held regular meetings to share data and discuss the progress of each component of the research. All members of the team were motivated to work together despite differences in disciplinary training, because of a common concern for conservation, alongside enthusiasm for the research proposal, and mutual respect for the value of each other’s work. While the primatologists were concerned mainly with understanding the behavior and ecology of chimpanzees, and how these are influenced by people (data important for chimpanzee conservation in CNP), they recognized the value of local concepts of forest management, and the importance of understanding plant use overlap between villagers and chimpanzees to predict the sustainability of their interactions in this shared environment. For the social scientists, their motivation was guided by an interest in supporting indigenous advocacy and establishing the rights of local people to access natural resources in CNP. An integral part of supporting people’s access to resources involved exploring local environmental perceptions, including understanding people’s representations of wildlife. For the Nalu, ideas about chimpanzees and plant use form a central part of explaining their perspectives of and attitudes toward wildlife, and our research team appreciated the interdisciplinary focus of the study was a vital component to interpreting this accurately.

Summary of Findings

Our study showed that Nalu people and chimpanzees “meet” frequently in CNP and overlap extensively in their use of wild resources, including important chimpanzee foods such as oil palm (Elaeis guineensis), velvet tamarind (Dialium guineensis), and saba (Saba senegalensis) (Hockings et al. unpubl. data). The regular overlap of land and resource use between these sympatric species has led to a degree of mutual tolerance. The chimpanzees have adapted their foraging behavior in response to changes in human foraging and cultivating patterns; for example, the chimpanzees frequently consume cultivated foods (Bessa et al. 2015). For their part, Nalu people have moved away from using rifles and some have adopted alternative strategies and precautions to prevent crop damage and reduce negative interactions with chimpanzees, with some people reporting that they intentionally do not cut important chimpanzee wild food species. When people encounter chimpanzees on roads, in their gardens, or near their homes, they generally respond calmly to their presence. Only on occasions when chimpanzees are in close proximity to children or women are people likely to shout and throw objects such as sticks in an effort to deter chimpanzees from approaching. Such behaviors reportedly can incite retaliatory aggression from chimpanzees elsewhere (McLennan and Hockings 2016); however, harmful behavior by chimpanzees toward people has rarely been reported at CNP (Hockings and Sousa 2013; Sousa et al. 2017). Moreover, Nalu people coexist with chimpanzees with relatively low levels of hostility as compared to interactions in some other regions (e.g., parts of western Uganda where chimpanzee habitat has been converted to agricultural land and spatial overlap with villagers is exceptionally high: Hockings and McLennan 2016; McLennan 2008). This relative tolerance of Nalu people toward chimpanzees arises from complex cultural, economic, and ecological factors that may be resource specific. For example, our findings show that chimpanzees are not considered to cause significant damage to the main cash crop, cashew (Anacardium occidentale), as chimpanzees feed only on the cashew pseudofruit, leaving the economically valuable cashew nut undamaged. According to Nalu people, chimpanzees leave the nuts in piles, thus helping them with the cashew nut harvest; the cashew fruit consumed by the chimpanzees in the process is regarded as fair payoff in exchange (Bessa et al. 2015; Hockings and Sousa 2012).

While local people are tolerant of chimpanzees, these interactions may not be quite as straightforward as they first appear because of associations with sorcery, where chimpanzees are incorporated into local cosmologies via their association with witchcraft. Other studies show that the complexity of local people’s relationships with chimpanzees (and some other wild animal species) has consequential, sometimes unforeseen outcomes for conservation. For example, Sousa et al. (2017, 2018) reported that the stories and descriptions about chimpanzees shared by local people with outsiders do not always represent their true sentiments about these great apes or certain local conservation initiatives. Despite no attacks being reported during our research period, local descriptions of chimpanzee attacks on people recorded by Sousa and colleagues distinguish between attacks by “clean” animals and attacks by “unclean” or “shape-shifted” individuals. Attacks by “unclean” chimpanzees, i.e., sorcerers who practice shape-shifting and have taken on the appearance of chimpanzees to further their own interests, are associated with situations of perceived abuse of power and expressions of greed. Attacks on persons by “clean” animals, i.e., chimpanzees responding to an antagonistic situation/stimulus, are interpreted as animals defending themselves or their group members against a tangible threat, and therefore are regarded as “natural” and a reasonable response on the part of the animal. Therefore, under certain circumstances local people perceive chimpanzees as akin to humans who commit socially or culturally harmful behaviors to others. In recent years this analogy has been extended to include the abuse of power that sometimes exists between NGOs and local people, suggesting a degree of unease among the local population, directed at conservation more generally rather than toward the chimpanzees themselves (Sousa et al. 2017, 2018).

Furthermore, while some studies at CNP indicate a degree of resistance among local people toward conservation initiatives (Sousa et al. 2017, 2018; Temudo 2012), our findings suggest that cultural and religious beliefs alongside economic and ecological factors result in conservation outcomes that protect chimpanzees in CNP to some extent. We held a participatory workshop in December 2016 to share research findings and consult with local people on chimpanzee conservation. Participants, including young people, women, men, male and female leaders, and guides working for the National Park, not only indicated tolerance toward chimpanzees feeding on plant species that are highly valued by people, but also suggested a general acceptance and acknowledgment over conservation concerns among researchers and NGOs developing chimpanzee conservation in the region. These findings, along with our data on human and chimpanzee plant use in CNP, are currently being used to inform decisions going forward for chimpanzee conservation at a local and national level in Guinea-Bissau.

Summary

Merging various methodologies enabled us to advance beyond more typical ethnoprimatology techniques (discussed previously) and adopt a multispecies approach, viewing CNP from a Nalu perspective and acknowledging chimpanzees as compatriots living alongside them with ancestral and historical links to Nalu territory (cf. Jost Robinson and Remis 2014; Remis and Jost Robinson 2017). This approach allowed us to begin to explore the local landscape and the sustainability of human–chimpanzee coexistence in CNP, giving equal weight to both species, within changing environmental, social, and economic conditions. We have shown how humans and chimpanzees are constituted in and by their relations to each other where they meet and “mingle” (Haraway 2008, 2010), sharing habitat and resources. As human populations expand, in part due to migration from nearby countries, pressure on key resources such as land and certain wild plants will increase in CNP, which may again cause changes to human–chimpanzee relationships in response to new conditions, as seen elsewhere (e.g., in Uganda: McLennan and Hill 2012, and in Central African Republic: Jost Robinson and Remis 2014). Understanding human–primate coexistence alongside different interest groups’ agendas and priorities becomes critical if environmental and conservation policies are to be effective and keep pace with these changes. Studies that explore advanced approaches in ethnoprimatology and encourage mixed-methods research, such as ours, provide new possibilities for locally appropriate conservation in shared landscapes.

Conclusions: Mainstreaming the Multispecies Approach in Primate Conservation

As major niche constructors, humans have had a consequential impact on the lives of other primates (Fuentes 2010a), just as living with primates has likely characterized much of our own evolutionary history (Riley 2006; Tutin and Oslisly 1995) and continues to do so. Integrating social science with conservation science approaches is crucial to understanding when and under what conditions human–primate sympatry is sustainable (McLennan et al. 2017). We have described how the multispecies approach is part of a broader aim by social scientists to overcome anthropocentrism in the study of human–nature interactions by theoretically integrating relational perspectives into Western science (Locke and Münster 2015). Continuing to apply a multispecies lens to ethnoprimatological research and maintaining the shift in focus from a conflict to coexistence narrative has the potential to produce more positive long-term outcomes for people and wildlife (Fuentes and Hockings 2010; Fuentes et al. 2016; Hardin and Remis 2006; Hill and Wallace 2012; McLennan et al. 2017). This entails bridging theory between the biological and social sciences and integrating our efforts to ensure productive conservation discourse for the benefit of both people and wildlife. We have shown that a more cohesive study of human–primate worlds can inform our understanding about interspecies interactions and multispecies landscapes. Our case study presents one example of how promoting engagement between the social sciences and disciplines traditionally grounded in the biological sciences can further develop the ethnoprimatology approach to deepen our understanding of environments from a multispecies perspective. Supporting a perceptual shift toward interdisciplinary research that combines multispecies ethnography with ethnoprimatology will further advance the development of these ideas, helping establish a more integrated and holistic biological and cultural conservation.

Working to improve interdisciplinary collaboration presents a challenge for academics and practitioners alike, but may be crucial to avert the extirpation of primates among other wildlife across the globe (Estrada et al. 2017). The true mainstreaming of social science in conservation needs visionary leadership and a dramatic change in organizational behavior (Bennett et al. 2017a, b; Mascia et al. 2003), potentially including the reorganizing of academic communities, funding, and institutions as a way of increasing avenues for collaboration between the different sciences (Palsson et al. 2013; Teel et al. 2018). This requires building social science capacity into conservation agencies, promoting engagement between the social sciences and disciplines traditionally grounded in the biological sciences including primatology, overcoming the associated political challenges that cross-disciplinary engagement often incurs, and willingness among social scientists to engage with biological scientists and share knowledge, insights and recommendations in an open and constructive way (Palsson et al. 2013; Redford 2011). Methodological expertise and skilled practice are not easily acquired, providing a further incentive for cross-disciplinary collaboration. If done well, this could produce positive results in the field of primate conservation. Primate researchers must rise to the challenge and become skilled at bridging disciplinary boundaries to provide a better understanding of the complexity in which conservation occurs (Fuentes and Hockings 2010; Fuentes et al. 2016; Riley and Fuentes 2011; Setchell et al. 2017). As Castree et al. (2014, p. 763) write, “interdisciplinary dialogue [we suggest] should engender plural representations of Earth’s present and future that are reflective of divergent human values and aspirations.”

Data Availability

The datasets analyzed during the current study are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.

Acknowledgements

We thank Erin Riley and Sindhu Radhakrishna for inviting us to contribute to this Special Issue, and the four anonymous reviewers and guest editor for their helpful suggestions and comments. We would also like to thank Cláudia Sousa and Joana Bessa for their invaluable contributions to this research project. The authors were supported by a fellowship from the Centre for Research in Anthropology (CRIA-FCSH/NOVA) to H. E. Parathian; an early career fellowship from the Leverhulme Trust to M. R. McLennan; and a fellowship and research grant (IF/01128/2014) from Fundação para a Ciência e a Tecnologia (FCT), Portugal, and funding from the Arcus Foundation to K. J. Hockings.

Glossary

Academic culture

The totality of socially transmitted behaviors, beliefs, institutions, and other products of human work and thought, with respect to a particular field, subject, or mode of expression (Morris 1969; Sutherland 1998).

Commonly Used Terminology in Biological Anthropology

Biological anthropology

“Biological” (or “physical”) anthropology is concerned with the biological and behavioral aspects of humans, nonhuman primates, and their extinct hominin ancestors. It provides a biological perspective to the systematic study of primates. As a subdiscipline of anthropology, biological anthropology is divided into several branches united in their common application of evolutionary theory to understanding human morphology and behavior, such as paleoanthropology and primatology (Fuentes 2010b).

Coexisting/sympatric species

Species that occur at the same time period and in the same place and can potentially interact (Cormier 2010; Fuentes and Wolfe 2002; Wheatley 1999).

Ethnoprimatology

Interdisciplinary study developed by primatologists, combining primatological and ethnographic practice to examine the multifarious interactions and interfaces between humans and nonhuman primates living in integrated and shared ecological and social spaces (Fuentes 2012; Hockings et al. 2015; Sponsel 1997). The goal of many ethnoprimatology studies is to understand the perceptions of local people and engage with their needs to enhance primate conservation and ensure the longevity of conservation projects (Lee 2010; Wheatley 1999). It adopts a mosaic of approaches that develops and reshapes the ways in which humans position themselves relative to human-dominated landscapes and ecologies (Dore et al. 2017).

Human–primate interface

Description of overlapping ecologies of human–nonhuman primate communities, viewing humans as literal and figurative kin to other primates. This term plays a core linking role in ethnoprimatology, between anthropology and primatology studies (Fuentes 2012; Leblan 2013; Wheatley 1999).

Human–wildlife conflict

Negative interactions between humans and wildlife where one or both species suffers as a consequence (Woodroffe et al. 2005). Researchers are increasingly moving away from this term when referring to scenarios in which wildlife impact on people’s livelihoods, security, or personal safety. Its use obscures the fact that these “conflicts” often stem from differential values, needs, priorities, and power relations between the human groups concerned (Hill 2015, 2017; Hill et al. 2017; Redpath et al. 2013).

Human–wildlife interactions

Traditionally understood in biology as people and wildlife sharing landscapes and resources, ranging from being beneficial or harmful to one species or the other. In ethnoprimatology human–wildlife interactions are increasingly understood as being dynamic and bidirectional (Humle and Hill 2016; Lee 2010; Wheatley 1999).

Niche construction

The creation and destruction of environments by organisms, and their interactions with other individuals (comprising synergistic interactions between organisms and environments). Through these processes the selective pressures that impact organisms are shaped (Barker and Odling-Smee 2014; Day et al. 2003; Odling-Smee et al. 2003, 2013). Specifically, in terms of anthropology, this perspective suggests ways in which behavioral and symbolic systems construct and interact with social and ecological niches and how, in turn, these systems interact with genetic systems (Fuentes 2010a).

Commonly Used Terminology in Social/Cultural Anthropology

Agency/intent

Having an independent capability or ability to act on one’s will. The capacity of individuals to make their own free choices and their reasons for acting are affected by cognitive belief structures that form through experiences, and societal/individual perceptions. This contrasts with structure, which describes factors of influence (such as social class, religion, gender, ethnicity, ability, customs, etc.) that determine or limit an agent and his or her decisions (Barker 2005).

Companion species

A term used to describe the historical emergence of wild and domestic animals in human lives. Nonhuman species are recognized as individuals who are part of historical relationships with individual people and human communities (Baynes-Rock 2013; Haraway 2010, 2016).

Contact zone

Places where humans and nonhumans share physiological, ecological, and social spaces across scales of ecological intersection. It signifies how subjects are constituted in and by their relations to each other (Haraway 2008).

Interspecies mingling

A term used to describe the mixing or bringing together of different human–nonhuman species without the individual’s fundamental loss of identity (Haraway 2010).

More-than-human /Anthropology beyond the human

These terms introduce perspectives that extend anthropology to a posthumanist inquiry through the application of multispecies ethnographies (Kirksey and Helmreich 2010; Kohn 2013).

Multispecies ethnography

A methodological approach and theoretical perspective rooted in anthropology that deconstructs the “humanism” of landscapes (Kirksey and Helmreich 2010) and enables the understanding of habitats as “multispecies landscapes.” Multispecies ethnography is concerned with the connections between humans and other life forms (which also have agency and intent or whose actions are the result of agency as an effect, rather than as the product of subjective intentionality; see earlier). It acknowledges that the human condition cannot be understood in isolation from nonhuman species (Locke 2017; Ogden et al. 2013).

Multispecies lens

Examining human–nonhuman interactions where humans are viewed as one of several organisms that shape, create, and form an integral part of their environment, because of engagements and interactions with nonhumans. In this context environments are viewed as “multispecies landscapes” through a “multispecies lens” (Aisher and Damodaran 2016).

Social/Cultural Anthropology

The fields of “social” and “cultural” anthropology overlap to a considerable extent. Broadly, the term “cultural anthropology” relates to an approach prominent in the French tradition and the United States. It stresses the coherence of human cultures, including their rules of behavior, language, material creations, and ideas about the world. “Social anthropology,” developed in the United Kingdom during the early years of the twentieth century, is a scientific discipline with an emphasis on human social institutions, their interrelationships, and the organizing principles of social and cultural life (Erickson 2011).

Compliance with Ethical Standards

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that they have no conflict of interest.

Contributor Information

Hannah E. Parathian, Email: hparathian@fcsh.unl.pt

Kimberley J. Hockings, Email: K.Hockings@exeter.ac.uk

References

  1. Aerts S, Bovenkerk B, Brando S. A philosophical and technical critique of zooanthropology as a moral and practical paradigm. In: Olsson AS, Araújo SM, Fátima Vieira M, editors. Food futures: Ethics, science and culture. Wageningen: Wageningen Academic; 2016. pp. 65–81. [Google Scholar]
  2. Agrawal A, Redford K. Poverty, development, and biodiversity conservation: Shooting in the dark? Wildlife Conservation Society Working Paper Series, 26. Bronx: Wildlife Conservation Society; 2006. [Google Scholar]
  3. Aisher A. Voices of uncertainty: Spirits, humans and forests in upland Arunachal Pradesh, India. South Asia: Journal of South Asian Studies. 2007;30(3):479–498. [Google Scholar]
  4. Aisher A, Damodaran V. Introduction: Human–nature interactions through a multispecies lens. Conservation and Society. 2016;14:293–304. [Google Scholar]
  5. Angelici FM, editor. Problematic wildlife: A cross-disciplinary approach. Cham: Springer; 2016. [Google Scholar]
  6. Asquith PJ. Anthropomorphism and the Japanese and Western traditions in primatology. In: Else JG, Lee PC, editors. Primate ontogeny, cognition, and social behavior. New York: Cambridge University Press; 1986. pp. 61–71. [Google Scholar]
  7. Atran S. Itzaj Maya folkbiological taxonomy: Cognitive universals and cultural particulars. In: Medin DL, Atran S, editors. Folkbiology. Cambridge: MIT Press; 1999. pp. 119–213. [Google Scholar]
  8. Baker LR, Olubode OS, Tanimola AA, Garshelis DL. Role of local culture, religion, and human attitudes in the conservation of sacred populations of a threatened ‘pest’ species. Biodiversity and Conservation. 2014;23:1895–1909. [Google Scholar]
  9. Barker C. Cultural studies: Theory and practice. London: SAGE; 2005. [Google Scholar]
  10. Barker G, Odling-Smee J. Integrating ecology and evolution: Niche construction and ecological engineering. In: Barker G, Desjardins GE, Pearce T, editors. Entangled life: Organism and environment in the biological and social sciences. Dordrecht: Springer; 2014. pp. 187–211. [Google Scholar]
  11. Baynes-Rock M. Life and death in the multispecies commons. Social Science Information. 2013;52(2):210–227. [Google Scholar]
  12. Bennett NJ, Roth R, Klain SC, Chan KMA, Christie P, et al. Conservation social science: Understanding and integrating human dimensions to improve conservation. Biological Conservation. 2017;205:93–108. [Google Scholar]
  13. Bennett NJ, Roth R, Klain SC, Chan KMA, Clark DA, Cullman G, Epstein G, Nelson MP, Stedman R, Teel TL, Thomas REW, Wyborn C, Curran D, Greenberg A, Sandlos J, Veríssimo D. Mainstreaming the social sciences in conservation. Conservation Biology. 2017;31:56–66. doi: 10.1111/cobi.12788. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Berkes F. Rethinking community-based conservation. Conservation Biology. 2004;18:621–630. [Google Scholar]
  15. Berkes F, Colding J, Folke C. Rediscovery of traditional ecological knowledge as adaptive management. Ecological Applications. 2000;10:1251–1262. [Google Scholar]
  16. Bessa J, Sousa C, Hockings KJ. Feeding ecology of chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes verus) inhabiting a forest-mangrove-savanna-agricultural matrix at Caiquene-Cadique, Cantanhez National Park, Guinea-Bissau. American Journal of Primatology. 2015;77(6):651–665. doi: 10.1002/ajp.22388. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Bulmer R. Why is the cassowary not a bird? A problem of zoological taxonomy among the Karam of the New Guinea Highlands. Man. 1967;2:5–25. [Google Scholar]
  18. Caro T, Eadie J. Animal behavior and conservation biology. In: Bolhuis JJ, Giraldeau LA, editors. The behavior of animals: Mechanisms, function and evolution. Oxford: Blackwell Science; 2005. pp. 367–392. [Google Scholar]
  19. Castree N, Adams WM, Barry J, Brockington D, Büscher B, Corbera E, Demeritt D, Duffy R, Felt U, Neves K, Newell P, Pellizzoni L, Rigby K, Robbins P, Robin L, Rose DB, Ross A, Schlosberg D, Sörlin S, West P, Whitehead M, Wynne B. Changing the intellectual climate. Nature Climate Change. 2014;4:763–768. [Google Scholar]
  20. Catarino L, Martins ES, Basto MF, Diniz MA. An annotated checklist of the vascular flora of Guinea-Bissau (West Africa). Blumea: Biodiversity, evolution and biogeography of plants, 53, 1–222. Chicago: Naturalis Biodiversity Center; 2008. [Google Scholar]
  21. Conklin, H. C. (1954). The relation of Hanunóo culture to the plant world. Ph.D. dissertation, Yale University.
  22. Corlett RT. The Anthropocene concept in ecology and conservation. Trends in Ecology & Evolution. 2015;30:36–41. doi: 10.1016/j.tree.2014.10.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Cormier LA. Kinship with monkeys: The Guajá foragers of eastern Amazonia. New York: Columbia University Press; 2010. [Google Scholar]
  24. Costa S, Casanova C, Lee P. What does conservation mean for women? The case of the Cantanhez Forest National Park. Conservation and Society. 2017;15:168–178. [Google Scholar]
  25. Daily GC, Ehrlich PR. Managing Earth’s ecosystems: An interdisciplinary challenge. Ecosystems. 1999;2:277–280. [Google Scholar]
  26. Day RL, Laland KN, Odling-Smee FJ. Rethinking adaptation: The niche-construction perspective. Perspectives in Biology and Medicine. 2003;46(1):80–95. doi: 10.1353/pbm.2003.0003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. De Waal F. The ape and the sushi master: Cultural reflections of a primatologist. New York: Basic Books; 2001. [Google Scholar]
  28. Decker DJ, Brown TL, Mattfeld GF. Integrating social science into wildlife management: Barriers and limitations. In: Miller MI, Gale RP, Brown PJ, editors. Social science in natural resource management systems. Boulder: Westview Press; 1987. pp. 83–92. [Google Scholar]
  29. Descola P. In the society of nature: A native ecology in Amazonia. New York: Cambridge University Press; 1994. [Google Scholar]
  30. Descola P. Modes of being and forms of predication. Journal of Ethnographic Theory. 2014;4:271–280. [Google Scholar]
  31. Despret V. Responding bodies and partial affinities in human–animal worlds. Theory, Culture & Society. 2013;30:51–76. [Google Scholar]
  32. Dore KM, Riley EP, Fuentes A, editors. Ethnoprimatology: A practical guide to research at the human–primate interface. New York: Cambridge University Press; 2017. [Google Scholar]
  33. Ellen RF. Ethnobiology and the science of humankind. Oxford: Blackwell Science; 2006. [Google Scholar]
  34. Erickson F. A history of qualitative inquiry in social and educational research. In: Denzin NK, Lincoln YS, editors. The Sage handbook of qualitative research 4. California: SAGE; 2011. pp. 43–59. [Google Scholar]
  35. Estrada A, Garber PA, Rylands AB, Roos C, Fernandez-Duque E, et al. Impending extinction crisis of the world’s primates: Why primates matter. Science Advances. 2017;3:e1600946. doi: 10.1126/sciadv.1600946. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Etiendem DN, Hens L, Pereboom Z. Traditional knowledge systems and the conservation of Cross River gorillas: A case study of Bechati, Fossimondi, Besali, Cameroon. Ecology and Society. 2011;16:22. [Google Scholar]
  37. Fox HE, Christian C, Nordby JC, Pergams OR, Peterson GD, et al. Perceived barriers to integrating social science and conservation. Conservation Biology. 2006;20:1817–1820. doi: 10.1111/j.1523-1739.2006.00598.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Frank B. Human–wildlife conflicts and the need to include tolerance and coexistence: An introductory comment. Society & Natural Resources. 2016;29:738–743. [Google Scholar]
  39. Frazão-Moreira A. Plantas e “Pecadores.” Percepções da Natureza em África. Lisboa: Livros Horizonte; 2009. [Google Scholar]
  40. Frazão-Moreira A. A natureza em perspectiva: Reflexões sobre saberes ecológicos locais e conhecimentos científicos. In: Alves A, Souto F, Peroni N, editors. Etnoecologia em perspectiva: Natureza, cultura e conservação. Recife: NUPEEA; 2010. pp. 73–88. [Google Scholar]
  41. Frazão-Moreira AF. Challenging approaches and crossovers in anthropology and conservation in Guinea-Bissau. Etnografica. 2016;20:663–667. [Google Scholar]
  42. Frazão-Moreira AF. The symbolic efficacy of medicinal plants: Practices, knowledge, and religious beliefs amongst the Nalu healers of Guinea-Bissau. Journal of Ethnobiology and Ethnomedicine. 2016;12:24. doi: 10.1186/s13002-016-0095-x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Fuentes A. Human–nonhuman primate interconnections and their relevance to anthropology. Ecological and Environmental Anthropology. 2006;2:1–11. [Google Scholar]
  44. Fuentes A. Naturalcultural encounters in Bali: Monkeys, temples, tourists, and ethnoprimatology. Cultural Anthropology. 2010;25:600–624. [Google Scholar]
  45. Fuentes A. The new biological anthropology: Bringing Washburn’s new physical anthropology into 2010 and beyond: The 2008 AAPA luncheon lecture. American Journal of Physical Anthropology. 2010;143(S51):2–12. doi: 10.1002/ajpa.21438. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Fuentes A. Ethnoprimatology and the anthropology of the human–primate interface. Annual Review of Anthropology. 2012;41:101–117. [Google Scholar]
  47. Fuentes A, Hockings KJ. The ethnoprimatological approach in primatology. American Journal of Primatology. 2010;72:841–847. doi: 10.1002/ajp.20844. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Fuentes A, Wolfe LD, editors. Primates face to face: The conservation implications of human–nonhuman primate interconnections. New York: Cambridge University Press; 2002. [Google Scholar]
  49. Fuentes A, Southern M, Suaryana KG. Monkey forests and human landscapes: Is extensive sympatry sustainable for Homo sapiens and Macaca fascicularis on Bali? In: Paterson JD, Wallis J, editors. Commensalism and conflict: The human–primate interface. Norman: American Society of Primatologists; 2005. pp. 168–195. [Google Scholar]
  50. Fuentes A, Cortez AD, Peterson JV. Ethnoprimatology and conservation: Applying insights and developing practice. In: Waller M, editor. Ethnoprimatology: Primate conservation in the 21st century. Cham: Springer; 2016. pp. 1–19. [Google Scholar]
  51. Fuentes A, Riley EP, Dore KM. Ethnoprimatology matters: Integration, innovation, and intellectual generosity. In: Dore KM, Riley EP, Fuentes A, editors. Ethnoprimatology: A practical guide to research at the human–primate interface. New York: Cambridge University Press; 2017. pp. 297–302. [Google Scholar]
  52. Giles-Vernick T, Rupp SK. Visions of apes, reflections on change: Telling tales of great apes in equatorial Africa. African Studies Review. 2006;49:51–73. [Google Scholar]
  53. Gillespie K, Collard RC, editors. Critical animal geographies: Politics, intersections and hierarchies in a multispecies world. London and New York: Routledge; 2015. [Google Scholar]
  54. Haraway DJ. Modest_Witness@Second_Millennium. FemaleMan©_Meets_OncoMouse™: Feminism and Technoscience. London and New York: Routledge; 1997. [Google Scholar]
  55. Haraway DJ. When species meet. Minneapolis: University of Minnesota Press; 2008. [Google Scholar]
  56. Haraway DJ. When species meet: Staying with the trouble. Environment and Planning D: Society and Space. 2010;28:53–55. [Google Scholar]
  57. Haraway DJ. Staying with the trouble: Making kin in the Chthulucene. Durham: Duke University Press; 2016. [Google Scholar]
  58. Hardin R, Remis MJ. Biological and cultural anthropology of a changing tropical forest: A fruitful collaboration across subfields. American Anthropologist. 2006;108:273–285. [Google Scholar]
  59. Hill CM. People, crops and primates: A conflict of interests. In: Paterson JD, Wallis J, editors. Commensalism and conflict: The human–primate interface. Norman: American Society of Primatologists; 2005. pp. 40–59. [Google Scholar]
  60. Hill CM. Perspectives of ‘conflict’ at the wildlife-agriculture boundary: 10 years on. Human Dimensions of Wildlife. 2015;20(4):296–301. [Google Scholar]
  61. Hill CM. Primate crop feeding behavior, crop protection, and conservation. International Journal of Primatology. 2017;38:385–400. [Google Scholar]
  62. Hill CM, Wallace GE. Crop protection and conflict mitigation: Reducing the costs of living alongside non-human primates. Biodiversity and Conservation. 2012;21:2569–2587. [Google Scholar]
  63. Hill CM, Webber AD. Perceptions of nonhuman primates in human–wildlife conflict scenarios. American Journal of Primatology. 2010;72:919–924. doi: 10.1002/ajp.20845. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  64. Hill CM, Webber AD, Priston NE, editors. Understanding conflicts about wildlife: A biosocial approach. New York and Oxford: Berghahn Books; 2017. [Google Scholar]
  65. Hockings KJ. Mitigating human–nonhuman primate conflict. In: Fuentes A, editor. The international encyclopedia of primatology, 1–2. Chichester: Wiley; 2016. pp. 1–8. [Google Scholar]
  66. Hockings KJ, McLennan MR. Problematic primate behaviour in agricultural landscapes: Chimpanzees as ‘pests’ and ‘predators’. In: Waller M, editor. Ethnoprimatology: Primate conservation in the 21st century. Cham: Springer; 2016. pp. 137–156. [Google Scholar]
  67. Hockings KJ, Sousa C. Differential exploitation of cashew – a low conflict crop – by sympatric humans and chimpanzees. Oryx. 2012;46:375–381. [Google Scholar]
  68. Hockings KJ, Sousa C. Human–chimpanzee sympatry and interactions in Cantanhez National Park, Guinea-Bissau: Current research and future directions. Primate Conservation. 2013;26:57–65. [Google Scholar]
  69. Hockings KJ, Yamakoshi G, Kabasawa A, Matsuzawa T. Attacks on local persons by chimpanzees in Bossou, Republic of Guinea: Long-term perspectives. American Journal of Primatology. 2010;72:887–896. doi: 10.1002/ajp.20784. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  70. Hockings KJ, McLennan MR, Carvalho S, Ancrenaz M, Bobe R, Byrne RW, Dunbar RIM, Matsuzawa T, McGrew WC, Williamson EA, Wilson ML, Wood B, Wrangham RW, Hill CM. Apes in the Anthropocene: Flexibility and survival. Trends in Ecology & Evolution. 2015;30:215–222. doi: 10.1016/j.tree.2015.02.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  71. Hockings KJ, Yamakoshi G, Matsuzawa T. Dispersal of a human-cultivated crop by wild chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes verus) in a forest–farm matrix. International Journal of Primatology. 2017;38:172–193. [Google Scholar]
  72. Humle T, Hill C. People–primate interactions: Implications for primate conservation. In: Wich SA, Marshall AJ, editors. An introduction to primate conservation. Don Mills: Oxford University Press; 2016. pp. 219–240. [Google Scholar]
  73. Hunn ES. Tzeltal folk-zoology: The classification of discontinuities in nature. San Diego: Elsevier; 1977. [Google Scholar]
  74. Hurn S. Human–animal relations (HAR) In: Fuentes A, editor. The international encyclopedia of primatology, 2, 511. Chichester: Wiley; 2017. [Google Scholar]
  75. Ingold T. The perception of the environment: Essays in livelihood, dwelling and skill. London and New York: Routledge; 2000. [Google Scholar]
  76. Ingold T. Being alive: Essays on movement, knowledge and description. London and New York: Routledge; 2011. [Google Scholar]
  77. Inskip Chloe, Zimmermann Alexandra. Human-felid conflict: a review of patterns and priorities worldwide. Oryx. 2009;43(01):18. [Google Scholar]
  78. Jalais A. Unmasking the cosmopolitan tiger. Nature and Culture. 2008;3:25–40. [Google Scholar]
  79. Jones JP, Andriamarovololona MM, Hockley N. The importance of taboos and social norms to conservation in Madagascar. Conservation Biology. 2008;22:976–986. doi: 10.1111/j.1523-1739.2008.00970.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  80. Jost Robinson CAJ, Remis MJ. Entangled realms: Hunters and hunted in the Dzanga-Sangha Dense Forest Reserve (APDS), Central African Republic. Anthropological Quarterly. 2014;87(3):613–633. [Google Scholar]
  81. Keil, P. G. (2016). Colonising in the footsteps of elephants: Interspecies pathways through North-East India and beyond. Paper presented at SOAS Elephant Conference, Centre for Ecological Sciences [CES], Indian Institute of Science [IISc], Bangalore, 4–6 April 2016.
  82. Kirksey SE, Helmreich S. The emergence of multispecies ethnography. Cultural Anthropology. 2010;25:545–576. [Google Scholar]
  83. Köhler A. Of apes and men: Baka and Bantu attitudes to wildlife and the making of eco-goodies and baddies. Conservation and Society. 2005;3:407–435. [Google Scholar]
  84. Kohn E. How dogs dream: Amazonian natures and the politics of transspecies engagement. American Ethnologist. 2007;34:3–24. [Google Scholar]
  85. Kohn E. How forests think: Toward an anthropology beyond the human. Oakland: University of California Press; 2013. [Google Scholar]
  86. Leblan, V. (2013). Introduction: Emerging approaches in the anthropology/primatology borderland. Revue de Primatologie, (5), 62.
  87. Leblan V, Bricka B. Genies or the opacity of human–animal relationships in Kakande, Guinea. African Study Monographs. 2013;34:85–108. [Google Scholar]
  88. Lee PC. Sharing space: Can ethnoprimatology contribute to the survival of nonhuman primates in human-dominated globalized landscapes? American Journal of Primatology. 2010;72:925–931. doi: 10.1002/ajp.20789. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  89. Lemke JL. Articulating communities: Sociocultural perspectives on science education. Journal of Research in Science Teaching. 2001;38(3):296–316. [Google Scholar]
  90. Lestel D, Taylor H. Shared life: An introduction. Social Science Information. 2013;52:183–186. [Google Scholar]
  91. Lestel D, Brunois F, Gaunet F. Etho-ethnology and ethno-ethology. Social Science Information. 2006;45:155–177. [Google Scholar]
  92. Lingomo B, Kimura D. Taboo of eating bonobo among the Bongando people in the Wamba region, Democratic Republic of Congo. African Study Monographs. 2009;30:209–225. [Google Scholar]
  93. Locke P. Explorations in ethnoelephantology: Social, historical, and ecological intersections between Asian elephants and humans. Environment and Society: Advances in Research. 2013;4(1):79–97. [Google Scholar]
  94. Locke P. Elephants as persons, affective apprenticeship, and fieldwork with nonhuman informants in Nepal. HAU: Journal of Ethnographic Theory. 2017;7:353–376. [Google Scholar]
  95. Locke P, Münster U. Multispecies anthropology. Oxford: Oxford University Press; 2015. [Google Scholar]
  96. Malone N, Wade AS, Fuentes A, Riley EP, Remis M, et al. Ethnoprimatology: Critical interdisciplinarity and multispecies approaches in anthropology. Critique of Anthropology. 2014;34:8–29. [Google Scholar]
  97. Marchesini R. Zoomimesis: Animal inspiration. Angelaki. 2016;21:175–197. [Google Scholar]
  98. Mascia MB, Brosius JP, Dobson TA, Forbes BC, Horowitz L, McKean MA, Turner NJ. Conservation and the social sciences. Conservation Biology. 2003;17:649–650. [Google Scholar]
  99. McKinney T. A classification system for describing anthropogenic influence on nonhuman primate populations. American Journal of Primatology. 2015;77:715–726. doi: 10.1002/ajp.22395. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  100. McLennan MR. Beleaguered chimpanzees in the agricultural district of Hoima, western Uganda. Primate Conservation. 2008;23:45–54. [Google Scholar]
  101. McLennan MR, Hill CM. Troublesome neighbours: Changing attitudes towards chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) in a human-dominated landscape in Uganda. Journal for Nature Conservation. 2012;20:219–227. [Google Scholar]
  102. McLennan MR, Hill CM. Ethical issues in the study and conservation of an African great ape in an unprotected, human-dominated landscape in western Uganda. In: MacClancy J, Fuentes A, editors. Ethics in the field: Contemporary challenges. Oxford: Berghahn Books; 2013. pp. 42–66. [Google Scholar]
  103. McLennan MR, Hockings KJ. The aggressive apes? Causes and contexts of great ape attacks on local persons. In: Angelici FM, editor. Problematic wildlife: A cross-disciplinary approach. Cham: Springer; 2016. pp. 373–394. [Google Scholar]
  104. McLennan MR, Spagnoletti N, Hockings KJ. The implications of primate behavioral flexibility for sustainable human–primate coexistence in anthropogenic habitats. International Journal of Primatology. 2017;38:105–121. [Google Scholar]
  105. Messmer TA. Human–wildlife conflicts: Emerging challenges and opportunities. Human–Wildlife Conflicts. 2009;3:10–17. [Google Scholar]
  106. Moon K, Blackman D. A guide to understanding social science research for natural scientists. Conservation Biology. 2014;28:1167–1177. doi: 10.1111/cobi.12326. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  107. Morris W, editor. American heritage dictionary of the English language. Rockville: American Heritage; 1969. [Google Scholar]
  108. Newing H. Conducting research in conservation: A social science perspective. London: Routledge; 2010. [Google Scholar]
  109. Odling-Smee FJ, Laland KN, Feldman MW. Niche construction: The neglected process in evolution. Princeton: Princeton University Press; 2003. [Google Scholar]
  110. Odling-Smee FJ, Erwin DH, Palkovacs EP, Feldman MW, Laland KN. Niche construction theory: A practical guide for ecologists. The Quarterly Review of Biology. 2013;88:3–28. doi: 10.1086/669266. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  111. Ogden LA, Hall B, Tanita K. Animals, plants, people, and things: A review of multispecies ethnography. Environment and Society. 2013;4:5–24. [Google Scholar]
  112. Oishi, T. (2013). Human-gorilla and gorilla-human: Dynamics of human–animal boundaries and interethnic relationships in the central African rainforest. Revue de Primatologie, (5), 63.
  113. Palmer, A., & Malone, N. (2018). Extending ethnoprimatology: Human–alloprimate relationships in managed settings. International Journal of Primatology. 10.1007/s10764-017-0006-6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  114. Palsson G, Szerszynski B, Sörlin S, Marks J, Avril B, Crumley C, Hackmann H, Holm P, Ingram J, Kirman A, Buendía MP, Weehuizen R. Reconceptualizing the ‘Anthropos’ in the Anthropocene: Integrating the social sciences and humanities in global environmental change research. Environmental Science & Policy. 2013;28:3–13. [Google Scholar]
  115. Papworth S, Milner-Gulland EJ, Slocombe K. The natural place to begin: The ethnoprimatology of the Waorani. American Journal of Primatology. 2013;75:1117–1128. doi: 10.1002/ajp.22173. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  116. Parathian HE, Maldonado AM. Human–nonhuman primate interactions amongst Tikuna people: Perceptions and local initiatives for resource management in Amacayacu in the Colombian Amazon. American Journal of Primatology. 2010;72:855–865. doi: 10.1002/ajp.20816. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  117. Pasquini L, Fitzsimons J, Cowell S, Brandon K, Wescott G. The establishment of large private nature reserves by conservation NGOs: Key factors for successful implementation. Oryx. 2011;45:373–380. [Google Scholar]
  118. Paterson JD, Wallis J, editors. Commensalism and conflict: The human–primate interface. Norman: American Society of Primatologists; 2005. [Google Scholar]
  119. Pearson RG. Reasons to conserve nature. Trends in Ecology & Evolution. 2016;31(5):366–371. doi: 10.1016/j.tree.2016.02.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  120. Redford K. Misreading the conservation landscape. Oryx. 2011;45:324–330. [Google Scholar]
  121. Redpath SM, Young J, Evely A, Adams WM, Sutherland WJ, Whitehouse A, Amar A, Lambert RA, Linnell JDC, Watt A, Gutiérrez RJ. Understanding and managing conservation conflicts. Trends in Ecology & Evolution. 2013;28:100–109. doi: 10.1016/j.tree.2012.08.021. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  122. Remis MJ, Hardin R. Transvalued species in an African forest. Conservation Biology. 2009;23(6):1588–1596. doi: 10.1111/j.1523-1739.2009.01290.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  123. Remis MJ, Jost Robinson CA. Nonhuman primates and “others” in the Dzanga Sangha reserve: The role of anthropology and multispecies approaches in ethnoprimatology. In: Dore KM, Riley EP, Fuentes A, editors. Ethnoprimatology: A practical guide to research at the human–primate interface. New York: Cambridge University Press; 2017. pp. 190–203. [Google Scholar]
  124. Richards P. Local understanding of primates and evolution: Some Mende beliefs concerning chimpanzees. In: Corbey R, Theunissen B, editors. Ape, man, apeman: Changing views since 1600. The Netherlands: Leiden University; 1995. pp. 262–273. [Google Scholar]
  125. Riley EP. Ethnoprimatology: Toward reconciliation between biological and cultural anthropology. Ecological and Environmental Anthropology. 2006;2:75–86. [Google Scholar]
  126. Riley EP. The human–macaque interface: Conservation implications of current and future overlap and conflict in Lore Lindu National Park, Sulawesi, Indonesia. American Anthropologist. 2007;109:473–484. [Google Scholar]
  127. Riley EP. Ranging patterns and habitat use of Sulawesi Tonkean macaques (Macaca tonkeana) in a human-modified habitat. American Journal of Primatology. 2008;70:670–679. doi: 10.1002/ajp.20543. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  128. Riley EP. The importance of human–macaque folklore for conservation in Lore Lindu National Park, Sulawesi, Indonesia. Oryx. 2010;44:235–240. [Google Scholar]
  129. Riley EP. Contemporary primatology in anthropology: Beyond the epistemological abyss. American Anthropologist. 2013;115:411–422. [Google Scholar]
  130. Riley EP, Fuentes A. Conserving social–ecological systems in Indonesia: Human–nonhuman primate interconnections in Bali and Sulawesi. American Journal of Primatology. 2011;73:62–74. doi: 10.1002/ajp.20834. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  131. Riley EP, Priston NEC. Macaques in farms and folklore: Exploring the human–nonhuman primate interface in Sulawesi, Indonesia. American Journal of Primatology. 2010;72:848–854. doi: 10.1002/ajp.20798. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  132. Rönnbäck P, Troell M, Zetterström T, Babu D. Mangrove dependence and socio-economic concerns in shrimp hatcheries of Andhra Pradesh, India. Environmental Conservation. 2003;30:344–352. [Google Scholar]
  133. Rose DB, van Dooren T, Chrulew M, Cooke S, Kearnes M, et al. Thinking through the environment, unsettling the humanities. Environmental Humanities. 2012;1:1–5. [Google Scholar]
  134. Rust NA, Tzanopoulos J, Humle T, MacMillan DC. Why has human–carnivore conflict not been resolved in Namibia? Society & Natural Resources. 2016;29:1079–1094. [Google Scholar]
  135. Rust NA, Abrams A, Challender DW, Chapron G, Ghoddousi A, et al. Quantity does not always mean quality: The importance of qualitative social science in conservation research. Society & Natural Resources. 2017;30:1304–1310. [Google Scholar]
  136. Sandker M, Campbell BM, Nzooh Z, Sunderland T, Amougou V, Defo L, Sayer J. Exploring the effectiveness of integrated conservation and development interventions in a central African forest landscape. Biodiversity and Conservation. 2009;18:2875–2892. [Google Scholar]
  137. Seoraj-Pillai N, Pillay N. A meta-analysis of human–wildlife conflict: South African and global perspectives. Sustainability. 2017;9:34. [Google Scholar]
  138. Setchell JM, Fairet E, Shutt K, Waters S, Bell S. Biosocial conservation: Integrating biological and ethnographic methods to study human–primate interactions. International Journal of Primatology. 2017;38:401–426. doi: 10.1007/s10764-016-9938-5. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  139. Sih A, Ferrari MC, Harris DJ. Evolution and behavioural responses to human-induced rapid environmental change. Evolutionary Applications. 2011;4:367–387. doi: 10.1111/j.1752-4571.2010.00166.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  140. Simberloff D. The contribution of population and community biology to conservation science. Annual Review of Ecology and Systematics. 1988;19:473–511. [Google Scholar]
  141. Smuts, B. B. (2009. First printing 1985). Sex and friendship in baboons. New York: Aldine.
  142. Sousa, F. M. (2009). Densidade de Pan troglodytes verus e Uuso de recursos naturais pela população local (Gadamael, República da Guiné-Bissau). Tese de Mestrado, Faculty of Sciences of Lisbon University.
  143. Sousa C, Frazão-Moreira A. Etnoprimatologia ao serviço da conservação na Guiné-Bissau: o chimpanzé como exemplo. In: Alves A, Souto F, Peroni N, editors. Etnoecologia em Perspectiva: Natureza, Cultura e Conservação. Recife: NUPEEA; 2010. pp. 187–200. [Google Scholar]
  144. Sousa J, Vicente L, Gippoliti S, Casanova C, Sousa C. Local knowledge and perceptions of chimpanzees in Cantanhez National Park, Guinea-Bissau. American Journal of Primatology. 2014;76:122–134. doi: 10.1002/ajp.22215. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  145. Sousa J, Hill CM, Ainslie A. Chimpanzees, sorcery and contestation in a protected area in Guinea-Bissau. Social Anthropology. 2017;25:364–379. [Google Scholar]
  146. Sousa J, Ainslie A, Hill CM. Sorcery and nature conservation. Environmental Conservation. 2018;45:90–95. doi: 10.1017/S0376892917000327. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  147. Sponsel LE. The human niche in Amazonia: Explorations in ethnoprimatology. In: Kinzey WG, editor. New world primates: Ecology, evolution, and behavior. New York: Aldine Gruyter; 1997. pp. 143–165. [Google Scholar]
  148. Strum SC. Almost human: A journey into the world of baboons. Chicago: University of Chicago Press; 1987. [Google Scholar]
  149. Sutherland WJ. The importance of behavioural studies in conservation biology. Animal Behaviour. 1998;56:801–809. doi: 10.1006/anbe.1998.0896. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  150. Teel TL, Anderson CB, Burgman MA, Cinner J, Clark D, Estévez RA, Jones JPG, McClanahan TR, Reed MS, Sandbrook C, St. John FAV. Publishing social science research in Conservation Biology to move beyond biology. Conservation Biology. 2018;32:6–8. doi: 10.1111/cobi.13059. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  151. Temudo MP. From the margins of the state to the presidential palace: The Balanta case in Guinea-Bissau. African Studies Review. 2009;52:47–67. [Google Scholar]
  152. Temudo M. “The white men bought the forests”: Conservation and contestation in Guinea-Bissau, western Africa. Conservation and Society. 2012;10:354–366. [Google Scholar]
  153. Thompson JM, Nestor LM, Kabanda RB. Traditional land-use practices for bonobo conservation. In: Furuichi T, Thompson J, editors. The bonobos: Behavior, ecology, and conservation. New York: Springer Science+Business Media; 2008. pp. 227–244. [Google Scholar]
  154. Treves A, Karanth KU. Human–carnivore conflict and perspectives on carnivore management worldwide. Conservation Biology. 2003;17:1491–1499. [Google Scholar]
  155. Tutin CEG, Oslisly R. Homo, Pan and Gorilla: Co-existence over 60,000 years at Lopé in Central Gabon. Journal of Human Evolution. 1995;28:597–602. [Google Scholar]
  156. Wheatley BP. Sacred monkeys of Bali. Long Grove: Waveland Press; 1999. [Google Scholar]
  157. Wilkie R. Multispecies scholarship and encounters: Changing assumptions at the human–animal nexus. Sociology. 2015;49:323–339. [Google Scholar]
  158. Wolverton S, Nolan JM, Ahmed W. Ethnobiology, political ecology, and conservation. Journal of Ethnobiology. 2014;34:125–152. [Google Scholar]
  159. Woodroffe R, Thirgood S, Rabinowitz A, editors. People and wildlife: Conflict or co-existence? New York: Cambridge University Press; 2005. [Google Scholar]
  160. Yamakoshi G, Leblan V. Conflicts between indigenous and scientific concepts of landscape management for wildlife conservation: Human–chimpanzee politics of coexistence at Bossou, Guinea. Revue de Primatologie. 2013;5:64. [Google Scholar]
  161. York R, Mancus P. The invisible animal. Sociological Theory. 2013;31:75–91. [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Data Availability Statement

The datasets analyzed during the current study are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.


Articles from International Journal of Primatology are provided here courtesy of Springer

RESOURCES