ABSTRACT
Very few animal species habitually make and use foraging tools. We recently discovered that the Hawaiian crow is a highly skilled, natural tool user. Most captive adults in our experiment spontaneously used sticks to access out-of-reach food from a range of extraction tasks, exhibiting a surprising degree of dexterity. Moreover, many birds modified tools before or during deployment, and some even manufactured tools from raw materials. In this invited addendum article, we describe and discuss these observations in more detail. Our preliminary data, and comparisons with the better-studied New Caledonian crow, suggest that the Hawaiian crow has extensive tool-modification and manufacture abilities. To chart the full extent of the species’ natural tool-making repertoire, we have started conducting dedicated experiments where subjects are given access to suitable raw materials for tool manufacture, but not ready-to-use tools.
KEYWORDS: Alala, construction behaviour, corvid, extractive foraging, Hawaiian crow, material selectivity, New Caledonian crow, tool manufacture, tool use
Only a handful of bird species habitually make and use foraging tools [1], amongst them the well-known New Caledonian crow Corvus moneduloides [2,3]. Recently, we discovered that a second tropical corvid – the Hawaiian crow or ‘Alalā C. hawaiiensis – is a highly skilled, natural tool user [4]. The species became extinct in the wild in 2002, but is being propagated in two captive breeding facilities on the islands of Hawai‘i and Maui, to support an ongoing reintroduction programme [5,6]. To test the tool-using abilities of ‘Alalā, we provided subjects (104 of 109 captive birds in 2013) with: a wooden log containing several meat-baited extraction tasks (crevices and drilled holes); vegetation as material for tool manufacture (dead forked branches, and on Hawaii Island, additionally live forked branches and fern sections of native species); and assorted sticks as potential tools (for details, see [4]). Our experiment revealed that tool use is a species-wide capacity (93% of all adult birds spontaneously used tools to extract bait), and that at least some birds are capable of modifying (abbreviated henceforth as MO) and manufacturing (MA) tools [4]. Here, we provide a more detailed description of these preliminary observations.
Shortening sticks (MO-1) was the most frequent tool modification, with 67% of the 64 individually-tested tool users exhibiting this behaviour either before or during tool use; some representative examples are shown in Supplementary Video 2 of our earlier paper [4]. Additionally, 8% of birds performed other modes of modification: nibbling the end of a tool (MO-2; 4 cases by 3 birds); removing parts of a leaf from a stem (MO-3; 1 case); reducing the size of pieces of wood through forceful pecking (MO-4; 3 cases by one bird) or of a piece of bark by subtracting material (MO-5; 1 case; see also bark-tool manufacture discussed below); and stripping bark from a stick (MO-6; 1 case).
Especially interesting is behaviour MO-6, where a bird removed bark from a stick it had previously inserted into a vertical hole in the experimental log (Figure 1a) – a behaviour that resembled the bark stripping expressed by some New Caledonian crows during hooked stick tool manufacture from forked branches [7,8]. Removing the outer layer of bark may enhance tool functionality by reducing friction (a smooth tool may be easier to insert) and/or increasing visibility (a light-coloured tool may be easier to see when deployed in dark holes or crevices) [7]. While our subject used the tool again following bark removal, it appeared to insert the non-processed end (the tool was hidden behind the bird’s body, so tool placement had to be inferred).
Figure 1.
Examples of spontaneous tool-processing behaviours observed during a controlled experiment with captive ‘Alalā ([4]; still images taken from video footage, cropped and resized). (a) Adult female (studbook number #77) removing bark from a stick it had previously inserted into a hole in the experimental log (Koa Acacia koa) (1 image plus zoomed-in inset; modification behaviour MO-6). (b) Adult female (#94) snipping-off a twig from a supplied forked branch (2 images; manufacture behaviour MA-1). (c) Adult female (#148) peeling-off bark from the experimental log (3 images; manufacture behaviour MA-6). In all three cases, birds used the modified or manufactured tool for probing into holes or crevices in the experimental log.
Tool manufacture was much rarer than tool modification, with only 14% of all individually-tested tool users making and subsequently using tools from dead and/or live plant materials. This is unsurprising, since suitable stick tools were always readily available as part of the experimental set-up [4]. We observed birds: snipping-off twigs from dead forked branches (MA-1; 3 cases by 3 birds; see Figure 1b); reducing the size of a piece of wood by chiselling, so it could be used as a tool (MA-2; 1 case); and removing: the end of a leaf petiole from a leaf (MA-3; 1 case), most of the leafed section of a live plant stem (MA-4; 1 case), a young plant growing naturally in the aviary, with subsequent processing of the material (MA-5; 1 case), or bark from either the experimental log (MA-6; 10 cases by 2 birds; see Figure 1c) or from larger pieces of bark (MA-7; 2 cases by 2 birds). Examples of behaviours MA-1, MA-2, MA-5 and MA-6 can be found in Supplementary Video 2 of our earlier paper [4].
The removal of bark to be used as tools (MA-6, MA-7; see Figure 1c) is noteworthy since wild ‘Alalā in the late 1970s and early 1980s spent a large proportion of their foraging time flaking bark to access hidden prey ([9,10]; although no tool use was reported). New Caledonian crows also frequently engage in bark flaking (43% of recorded bill interactions in a study using bird-borne miniature video cameras [11]), and exhibit a wide range of tool-processing behaviours [2,3,8,11–18], but to our knowledge, there have been no reports of bark tool use in the wild. Interestingly, the only observation to date of a wild American crow C. brachyrhynchos using a tool for (presumed) extractive foraging involved the use of a wooden splinter, which the bird had released from a fence post through pecking and pulling [19]. A useful non-corvid comparison is provided by nuthatches Sitta spp. that are known to use bark tools, illustrating how this tool type can aid foraging for arthropod prey (for a review and photos illustrating the behaviour, see [20]).
The study of ‘Alalā tool behaviour is still in its infancy, and many exciting research opportunities lie ahead [4,21]. Our preliminary observations have confirmed that ‘Alalā are capable of modifying and manufacturing tools in a variety of ways, paving the way for dedicated experiments that systematically chart the species’ natural tool-making repertoire, as well as its ontogenetic development [4,22–24]. Importantly, subjects need to be tested under conditions where they have access to a wide variety of naturalistic foraging tasks (including those routinely encountered by wild New Caledonian crows [2,3,11,12,16]), and different plant materials for tool manufacture, but not to sticks or other objects that could be used as tools without further processing (as was the case in our first study [4]). This work programme, which we have recently launched, borrows experimental designs we developed for assaying the tool-manufacture behaviour of wild-caught New Caledonian crows [8,25–27]. With a cohort of ‘Alalā recently released into the wild, our controlled studies in captivity can be complemented with detailed field observations, to determine the extent to which birds use tools, and other extractive foraging techniques, under contemporary ecological conditions [4].
Funding Statement
This project was funded through a BBSRC (Biotechnology and Biological Sciences Research Council) David Phillips Fellowship (BB/G023913/2 to C.R.), and a PhD studentship supported by the BBSRC and the University of St Andrews (to B.K.). Funding for the ‘Alalā conservation breeding programme was provided by the U.S. Fish and Wildlife Service, State of Hawaii Division of Forestry and Wildlife, Moore Family Foundation, several anonymous donors, and San Diego Zoo Global.
Acknowledgments
We thank Rich Switzer, Rosanna Leighton, Lisa Komarczyk and Josh Kramer for facilitating project logistics, and San Diego Zoo Global’s staff for assistance with experiments.
Disclosure statement
No potential conflict of interest was reported by the authors.
References
- [1].Shumaker RW, Walkup KR, Beck BB.. Animal tool behavior: the use and manufacture of tools by animals. Baltimore: Johns Hopkins University Press; 2011. [Google Scholar]
- [2].Hunt GR. Manufacture and use of hook-tools by New Caledonian crows. Nature. 1996;379:249–251. [Google Scholar]
- [3].Rutz C, St Clair JJH. The evolutionary origins and ecological context of tool use in New Caledonian crows. Behav Processes. 2012;89:153–165. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [4].Rutz C, Klump BC, Komarczyk L, et al. Discovery of species-wide tool use in the Hawaiian crow. Nature. 2016;537:403–407. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [5].Banko PC. ‘Alala In: Pratt TK, Atkinson CT, Banko PC, et al, editors. Conservation biology of Hawaiian forest birds: implications for island avifauna. Yale: Yale University Press; 2009. [Google Scholar]
- [6].U.S. Fish and Wildlife Service (USFWS) Revised recovery plan for the ‘Alalā (Corvus hawaiiensis). Portland, Oregon: USFWS; 2009. [Google Scholar]
- [7].St Clair JJH, Rutz C. New Caledonian crows attend to multiple functional properties of complex tools. Phil Trans R Soc B. 2013;368:20120415. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [8].Klump BC, Sugasawa S, St Clair JJH, et al. Hook tool manufacture in New Caledonian crows: behavioural variation and the influence of raw materials. BMC Biol. 2015;13:97. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [9].Giffin JG. ‘Alala investigation final report. Honolulu, Hawaii: Dept. Land and Nat. Res., Div. Forestry and Wildlife; 1983. [Google Scholar]
- [10].Sakai HF, Ralph CJ, Jenkins CD. Foraging ecology of the Hawaiian crow, an endangered generalist. Condor. 1986;88:211–219. [Google Scholar]
- [11].Troscianko J, Rutz C. Activity profiles and hook-tool use of New Caledonian crows recorded by bird-borne video cameras. Biol Lett. 2015;11:20150777. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [12].Hunt GR. Tool use by the New Caledonian crow Corvus moneduloides to obtain Cerambycidae from dead wood. Emu. 2000;100:109–114. [Google Scholar]
- [13].Hunt GR, Gray RD. The crafting of hook tools by wild New Caledonian crows. Proc R Soc B. 2004;271:S88–S90. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [14].Hunt GR, Gray RD. Direct observations of pandanus-tool manufacture and use by a New Caledonian crow (Corvus moneduloides). Anim Cogn. 2004;7:114–120. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [15].Hunt GR, Abdelkrim J, Anderson MG, et al. Innovative pandanus-tool folding by New Caledonian crows. Austr J Zool. 2007;5:291–298. [Google Scholar]
- [16].Troscianko J, Bluff LA, Rutz C. Grass-stem tool use in New Caledonian crows Corvus moneduloides. Ardea. 2008;96:283–285. [Google Scholar]
- [17].Holzhaider JC, Hunt GR, Gray RD. The development of pandanus tool manufacture in wild New Caledonian crows. Behaviour. 2010;147:553–586. [Google Scholar]
- [18].Rutz C, Sugasawa S, van der Wal JEM, et al. Tool bending in New Caledonian crows. R Soc Open Sci. 2016;3:160439. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [19].Caffrey C. Tool modification and use by an American crow. Wilson Bull. 2000;112:283–284. [Google Scholar]
- [20].Rutz C, Deans S. Nuthatch uses tool in London park. Ethology. 2018;124:135–138. [Google Scholar]
- [21].Uomini N, Hunt G. A new tool-using bird to crow about. Learn Behav. 2017;45:205–206. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [22].Kenward B, Weir AAS, Rutz C, et al. Tool manufacture by naive juvenile crows. Nature. 2005;433:121. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [23].Kenward B, Rutz C, Weir AAS, et al. Development of tool use in New Caledonian crows: inherited action patterns and social influences. Anim Behav. 2006;72:1329–1343. [Google Scholar]
- [24].Hunt GR, Lambert C, Gray RD. Cognitive requirements for tool use by New Caledonian crows (Corvus moneduloides). NZ J Zool. 2007;34:1–7. [Google Scholar]
- [25].St Clair JJH, Klump BC, van der Wal JEM, et al. Strong between-site variation in New Caledonian crows’ use of hook-tool-making materials. Biol J Linn Soc. 2016;118:226–232. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [26].Sugasawa S, Klump BC, St Clair JJH, et al. Causes and consequences of tool shape variation in New Caledonian crows. Curr Biol. 2017;27:3885–3890. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [27].St Clair JJH, Klump BC, Sugasawa S, et al. Hook innovation boosts foraging efficiency in tool-using crows. Nat Ecol Evol. 2018;2:441–444. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]