Skip to main content
Journal of Rehabilitation and Assistive Technologies Engineering logoLink to Journal of Rehabilitation and Assistive Technologies Engineering
. 2017 Oct 5;4:2055668317733257. doi: 10.1177/2055668317733257

An exploratory study of mobile messaging preferences by age: Middle-aged and older adults compared to younger adults

Alexis Kuerbis 1,, Katherine van Stolk-Cooke 2, Frederick Muench 3
PMCID: PMC6284808  NIHMSID: NIHMS985444  PMID: 30533217

Abstract

Introduction

Mobile technologies, such as short message service or text messaging, can be an important way to reach individuals with medical and behavioral health problems who are homebound or geographically isolated. Optimally tailoring messages in short message service interventions according to preferences can enhance engagement and positive health outcomes; however, little is known about the messaging preferences of middle-aged and older adults.

Methods

Utilizing secondary data, global messaging preferences were examined to inform the development of short message service interventions for adults of all ages. Two hundred and seventy-seven adults were recruited through an online labor market. They completed an online survey by evaluating message dyads in 22 content groupings. Dyads were identical in subject matter but structurally or linguistically varied. Participants selected the message in each dyad they would prefer to receive when attempting to meet a self-selected personal goal. Preferences were tested for two age groups ≤50 and 51 and older.

Results

Findings reveal adults 51 and older have clear messaging preferences that differ significantly from the younger group for only two content groupings; specifically, they prefer no emoticon to a smiley face emoticon and “you” statements rather than “we” statements.

Conclusion

Recommendations for optimizing messaging for older adults are reviewed.

Keywords: Older adults, mHealth, short message service intervention, text message, health

Introduction

Harnessing contemporary mobile technologies, such as cell phones, smartphones, and tablets, for the purposes of developing effective frontline interventions for medical and behavioral health problems has been the focus of much research over the last decade.1,2 Mobile health interventions (mHealth) provide an affordable, flexible, convenient, and accessible format for assessment and intervention, particularly among individuals who may be isolated due to being homebound or in rural communities. In addition, mobile interventions have demonstrated efficacy and effectiveness across numerous health domains.2,3

While much of research and development of mHealth focuses on smartphone applications, smartphone ownership, though growing, remains limited in the US.4 In 2015 in the US, only 54% of adults 50–64 years old and 27% of adults 65 years old and older reported owning a smartphone. Internationally, particularly among countries with emerging economies, smartphone ownership is still relatively rare among those 35 and older.5 Smartphone applications are not yet reaching middle-aged and older adults at the same rates as younger adults (79+%),4 nor are they reaching those in the lowest socioeconomic classes in the US or abroad.4,5

A primary medium for implementing mHealth interventions to middle-aged and older adults, with a range of economic resources, is short message service (SMS), otherwise referred to as text messaging. Although smartphone ownership remains limited, data reveal high levels of mobile phone access and text messaging usage by this group. In 2014, 74% of individuals 65 and older and 88% of those aged 50–64 in the US owned a cell phone.6 Among cell and smartphone owners over the age of 50 in 2015, 92% used the text messaging feature on their phones,6 often daily. In addition to widespread access, a majority of older adults (including those 75 and older) report positive attitudes toward mobile technology, including text messaging and smartphones.713 Thus, SMS-based interventions could be a highly accessible, feasible, and desirable approach to helping middle-aged and older adults with medical and behavioral health problems, especially those who may be relatively isolated.

Existing SMS interventions demonstrate efficacy in improving both physical and mental health outcomes across a variety of demographics and conditions2,1416; however, older adults have generally been excluded from studies on SMS interventions. A review of the literature reveals that very few studies examining text messaging interventions focus on adults over 50 as a target population, report findings by age group, or include this population in study samples at all.17 Among the few studies that have assessed age differences related to willingness to use text messaging interventions, results show no differences in feasibility or willingness of adults aged 50 and older to receive text message appointment or medication adherence reminders compared to younger adults.18

Very few studies have tested text messaging interventions specifically designed for adults ranging in age from 55 to 83 for conditions such as diabetes and coronary heart disease or to increase mobility,12,13,19,20 and only two of those studies had sample sizes of at least 30 participants. Findings demonstrate these interventions are well received and are initially efficacious in improving overall medication adherence or increased mobility compared to controls who did not receive text messages. Despite the fact that each of these studies focused on SMS as a behavioral intervention, none of these studies reported on factors related to message tone and content that might be critical for impacting behavior. One of the studies did not disclose the message content at all,13 and another reported that messages only contained questions about the participants' daily behaviors (e.g. assessment focused).19 The other two studies looked at the impact of either motivating messages to enhance mobility20 or personalized messages and education to improve medication adherence for heart disease.12 In neither of these studies was there a comparison of the tone and structure of the messages themselves to determine how that might facilitate or inhibit engagement with the intervention and ultimately impact behavior. Indeed, across populations, one systematic review of Hall et al.16 found that very few studies included in reviews could delineate characteristics of text messaging interventions that might help or hinder intervention engagement or efficacy. Personalization and tailoring to needs have been associated with larger effects among some studies (e.g. Head et al.21 and Finitsis et al.22), but it is unknown how adults over 50 might differentially engage with different types of messaging.

Much remains unknown about how best to foster older adult engagement with SMS interventions. To take an initial step in facilitating the development of SMS interventions that target middle-aged and older adults and optimize their engagement, more research is needed about how messaging content may be differentially received and perceived by adults in these age groups. For example, what specific features of a text message make it more or less acceptable to a middle-aged or older adult?

Across age groups, message content features, such as tone and structure, can impact user receptivity and engagement.1 Message framing and its impact on users' overall experiences and outcomes is a growing research area with important implications for intervention tailoring.2328 While middle-aged and older adults appear to be open to the idea of text message-based health resources, age may have an important impact on messaging preferences. For example, starting at age 50, age-related changes in cognitive processing cause individuals to process visual information, including fonts and punctuation, differently,29 and in some cases, formatting of visual information can inhibit effective information retrieval. It is therefore important to better understand how messaging preferences may differ across age groups in order to promote user engagement. Importantly, these authors found no research on the SMS preferences of adults 50 and older. In a previous study, we looked at age as a moderator of message preferences, but did not explore the specific preferences of those over the age of 50.1 Given the gaps in the literature, the aim of the current study was to explore age-specific preferences for short messages to promote goal-directed behaviors among adults 51 and older.

Method

This study implemented a secondary data analysis on data collected in 2012 for a study on messaging preferences among the general population.1

Recruitment

Participants were recruited online through Mechanical Turk (MTurk), Amazon's online labor market. MTurk is a platform through which “workers” are contracted to complete tasks, called “human intelligence tasks” (HITs), such as beta testing software or providing consumer opinions. For completing a HIT, workers are compensated by the “requester” who published the task and approves their work. Compensation is generally low—often times below $1—and commensurate with the task intensity. Increasingly, MTurk is being used for social sciences research with comparable results to more traditional sampling methods when validity checks are included in the design.1,30 For the purpose of the parent study, investigators set worker qualifications to the study to be a HIT approval rate of 95% or greater out of at least 500 completed. This ensured response quality, as these qualifications denoted consistent and reliable performance on previous HITs, as well as a certain level of computer and internet literacy. Workers were also limited to those in the US.

Participants

Individuals (N = 277) met basic worker criteria and passed consistency and validity checks described in the parent study.1 Respondents ranged in age from 18 to 70. The typical participant was between the ages of 18 and 30, more likely to be female (56.5%) than male, Caucasian (81.1%), and had a college degree or higher (59.8%). There were a total of 37 individuals aged 51 or older.

Procedures

Qualified workers could view the study HIT, titled Answer a survey about your text message preferences. Within the HIT, there was a link to an external, web-based survey hosted by Survey Monkey.com. Prior to completing the survey, participants completed a brief consent form for anonymous survey-based research, which also provided investigator and IRB contact information. In the consent form, participants were informed that the study's aim was to understand the types of text messages individuals would prefer to receive when trying to achieve a personal goal. Once participants completed the survey, they were provided with a survey code to enter into their MTurk account to await requester review and compensation.

The survey was published via MTurk four times, and workers were barred from participating in the survey more than once. Each survey included a total of about 90 items required about 10 min to complete. After the first two published surveys were completed, preliminary data analysis was performed. In cases of clear consensus about messaging, a number of message dyads and groupings (described further below) were removed from subsequent iterations of the survey, and a number of new dyads and groupings were added. These revisions account for differences in sample sizes across groupings.

Measures

Demographics

Participants were asked questions about demographics, cell phone usage, and text messaging plans.

Personal goal

Participants were asked to supply a personal goal they would like to achieve. Goals did not have to be health related and could vary from flossing to getting promoted to being more assertive.

Messages preferences

Participants were asked to rate their message preferences based on the personal goal they selected. The survey contained approximately 70 message dyads in 22 groupings, with three dyads per grouping. Message structure and content were developed based on principles of motivational and behavior change literature (e.g. Gollwitzer31 and Bandura32), as well as public health campaigns. Mirrored messages were created to delineate preference, such that messages had the same semantic content but differed in structure or wording. For example, in testing manipulation of voice, if one message included the word you, we created an identical or mirrored message with the word we in place of “you,” with other appropriate changes to grammar included. Message type, grouping, description, and examples are presented in detail elsewhere.1 Message descriptions and examples are reexamined here in Table 1.

Table 1.

Message grouping with examples.

Dyad grouping Dyad description Dyad example
Gain framing versus loss framing
Smiley versus sad emoticon
Smiley emoticon messages contain a smiley face to make the content gain framed. Don't give up:-)
Sad emoticon messages contain a sad face to make the content loss framed. Don't give up:-(
Benefit- versus consequence-oriented
Benefit-oriented messages consist of language that is gain framed. Close your eyes—imagine the benefits of changing.
Consequence-oriented messages consist of language that is loss framed. Close your eyes—imagine the consequences if you don't change.
Personal/emotional emphasis
Coaching versus uncoached direction
Coaching messages contain a direction or recommendation with positively framed emotional emphasis. You've been doing great, don't quit now.
Uncoached direction messages contain a direction or recommendation with no additional emphasis. The most important thing you can do to reach your goal is not give up.
Goal setting and task performance
Implementation intention versus general goal
Implementation intention messages consist of an if-then plan to trigger a specific action If I start to get down on myself, I will think of all my previous successes.
General goal messages consist of an open-ended, nonspecific if-then plan. If I start to get down on myself, I will do something to make me feel better.
Locus of control theory
Intrinsic versus extrinsic locus of control
Intrinsic locus of control messages emphasize an internal locus of control over goal attainment. You are responsible when you don't meet your goal.
Extrinsic locus of control messages emphasize the degree to which external factors influence goal attainment Many different aspects of your environment play a role when you don't meet your goal.
Spelling and grammatical manipulations
Correct grammar versus grammatical errors
Correct grammar messages contain no grammatical errors. If you accept where you are now, you're way ahead of the pack.
Grammatical error messages contain grammatical errors. If you accept where you are now you're way ahead of the pack.
Textese versus nontextese
Textese messages utilize the spelling abbreviations common to text messaging. u have changed b4, u can meet ur goals today. b who u r.
Nontextese messages You have changed before, you can meet your goals today. Be who you are.
Manipulations of visible emphasis
Single punctuation versus multiple punctuation
Single punctuation messages utilize only a single punctuation mark between phrases or clauses. Reinvent yourself!
Multiple punctuation messages utilize multiple punctuation marks between phrases or clauses for emphasis. Reinvent yourself!!!
Smiley emoticon versus no emoticon
Smiley emoticon messages contain a smiley face to enhance a friendly or positive tone. You are on the right track:-) just keep going!
No emoticon messages contain the same language as their smiley emoticon counterparts but do not include an emoticon. You are on the right track – just keep going!
CAPS (capitalization) emphasis versus no visible emphasis
CAPS emphasis messages contain at least one world that is spelled in all capital letters for emphasis. When it comes to the negative consequences of a bad habit, you are NOT the exception.
No visible emphasis messages do not include any all-caps words When it comes to the negative consequences of a bad habit, you are not the exception.
Manipulations of voice, person, or origin
“I” statement versus “We” statement
“I” statement messages employ a singular first person point of view Changing can be hard: I promise it will get better.
“We” statement messages employ a plural first person (or collectivist) point of view Changing can be hard: we promise it will get better.
“You” statement versus “We” statement
“You” statement messages employ a singular second person point of view. Your past should motivate you to change—not paralyze you!
“We” statement messages employ a plural first person (or collectivist) point of view. Our pasts should motivate us to change—not paralyze us!
Cited versus uncited
Cited messages refer to a source/sources of the information presented. Studies show that simply visualizing your future actions makes them more likely to come true!
Uncited messages provide no point of reference for the information presented. Simply visualizing your future actions makes them more likely to come true!
Manipulations of tone
Direction versus passive
Direction messages express a command. Think about what you will lose if you give up on your goals.
Passive messages express a suggestion in a passive or nonurgent tone. It could be helpful to think about what you will lose if you give up on your goals.
Statement versus question
Statement messages utilize declarative language. Committing to your goals today will help you in the long-run.
Question messages utilize interrogative language. How will committing to your goals today help you in the long-run?
Aggression versus nonaggression
Aggression messages utilize a confrontational or shaming tone. Do you seriously think that blaming others will help you change for the better?
Nonaggression messages utilize a nonconfrontational tone. Blaming others probably won't help you change for the better.
Polite versus nonpolite
Polite messages include words such as please and thank you. Please text us to let us know if you received this message.
Nonpolite messages do not include words such as please and thank you. Text us to let us know if you received this message.
Directive versus nondirective statement
Directive messages contain an imperative statement within the context of a time frame Call a friend to help you feel better as soon as you have a free moment.
Nondirective statement messages offer suggestions with no direction or time-sensitive context. Going out with friends is a good idea to help you feel better.
Humor versus gravity
Humor messages include a joke or playful tone to suggest levity. Why did the chicken cross the road? Because it knew that action creates change.
Gravity messages are serious in tone and do not contain playful or jocular language. Action creates change.
Symbolic language
Metaphor versus literal
Metaphor messages contain symbolic imagery. When you reach the end of your rope, tie a knot and hang on.
Literal messages present content in plain terms When you feel like giving up, keep going until it passes.
Brevity versus added meaning
Short versus long
Short messages contain as little content as possible to convey meaning. Your actions define you.
Long messages are designed to convey additional meaning. Your actions define you: the world looks at you differently when you act differently.

Note: This table was adapted from Muench et al.1 and reproduced here for reference.

Analytic plan

Analyses occurred in three basic steps. First, descriptive statistics were used to characterize two age groups (<51; ≥51). Chi square and t-tests were implemented to test whether group differences were statistically significant where appropriate. Next, frequencies of message preferences among both age groups were calculated. Binomial tests were implemented to see if message preferences among either age group were significantly different from chance at the p < .05 level. Message preferences among the younger cohort were identical to findings from our previous analysis1 and are therefore excluded from our “Results” section. Only binomial tests related to the older group are reported here. Finally, Chi square was used to test for significant differences in the proportions of preferences by age group.

Results

Sample description

Table 2 shows the basic demographics and cell phone characteristics of this sample. Adults 51 and older were significantly more likely to be female and more educated than adults in the younger cohort. Both groups were predominantly non-Hispanic, Caucasian. As expected, a significantly larger proportion of the younger cohort was employed full or part time than the older group. While the older group contained 22% retirees, there were no retirees in the younger group. Almost all participants (99.6%) owned a cell phone. Participants in the younger group were significantly more likely than those in the older group to have phone plans that included unlimited texting. Conversely, the older group was more likely to have a limited or pay per message text messaging plan. There were significant differences by age in how many text messages were sent and received in the past week (t(231.7) = 5.4, p < .001), such that the younger cohort reported a mean of 220 text messages sent or received (SD = 506), while the older group reported a mean of 27 (SD = 53).

Table 2.

Characteristics of study sample.

Age group
<51
≥51
Variable (N = 240) % (N = 37) %
Demographics
Age
 18–30 years old 47.1
 31–40 years old 37.5
 41–50 years old 15.4
 51–60 years old 81.1
 61–70 years old 18.9
Femalea 53.1 78.4
Race/ethnicity
 Hispanic/Latino, any race 8.8 2.7
 White/Caucasian, non-Hispanic 74.6 94.6
 Other 16.6 2.7
Educationb
 High school or equivalent 13.1 5.7
 Some college 36.1 28.5
 Bachelor's degree or higher 37.4 42.9
 Graduate education 13.5 22.9
Employmentc
 Employed full time 50.0 27.8
 Employed part time 16.3 30.6
 Retired 0.0 22.2
 Unemployed 14.6 13.9
 Other (not disabled) 19.2 8.1
Cell phone characteristics
Own a cell phone in past year 99.6 100
Type of text messaging planb
 Unlimited 74.6 48.6
 Limit of 200 per month or  similar 15.4 27.0
 Pay per message 8.8 21.6
 Can't receive messages even  if wanted to 1.3 2.7
a

p < .01.

b

p < .05.

c

p < .001.

Across age cohorts, almost half of the sample (49.5%) reported personal goals relating to their physical health and well-being, such as diet and exercise. There were no significant differences in the types of personal goals generated by older or younger adults.

Message preferences among the older group only

Table 3 shows message preferences among the older age group for each content grouping. For this study, we define “clear preference” as a preference among 75% or more of the sample and with a significant binomial test. Similar to analyses that included all age groups, the older adult group demonstrated a clear preference for messages that were grammatically correct, did not use textese (e.g. u r gr8), referenced an internal locus of control, were benefit oriented, were directive, included positive as opposed to negative images (i.e. a smiley versus frown-faced emoticon), did not attempt humor, employed statements rather than questions, were nonaggressive and polite. In addition, the older adult group also clearly preferred messages with single punctuation and “you” statements (as opposed to “we” statements).

Table 3.

Message preferences among adults 51 and older (N = 37).

Message type greater preference % Message type lesser preference % na
Correct grammar 100 Grammatical errors 0.0 28b
Nontextese 100 Textese 0.0 30b
Locus of control: intrinsic 100 Locus of control: extrinsic 0.0 6b
Benefit oriented 100 Consequence oriented 0.0 17b
Direction 96.7 Passive 3.3 30b
Smiley emoticon 93.1 Sad emoticon 6.9 29b
No humor 86.2 Humor 13.8 29b
Statement 84.8 Question 15.2 33b
Single punctuationc 83.3 Multiple punctuation 16.7 24b
Polite 83.3 Impolite 16.7 30b
“You” statementc 81.1 “We” statement 18.9 37b
Nonaggression 80.6 Aggression 19.4 36b
Implementation intentiond 80.0 General goal 20.0 5
Nondirective 74.3 Command 25.7 35b
Coaching 71.4 Uncoached direction 28.6 7
“I” statement 63.9 “We” statement 36.1 36
No emoticond 61.1 Smiley emoticon 38.9 36
Literal 58.3 Metaphorical 41.7 36
Uncited 51.4 Cited 48.6 37
Longd 51.4 Short 48.6 37
CAPS (capitalization) emphasis 50 No visible emphasis 50 36
a

The n applies to both message types.

b

Not the result of chance using a nonparametric binomial test to ensure that the difference between groups was greater than a 50% chance (p < .05).

c

Proportion of preference is significantly greater than the younger cohort.

d

Preference direction differs from younger cohort.

Differences between age groups

For three message dyads, older adults preferred the opposite form of the message from the form preferred by the younger cohort. While not significantly greater than chance, based solely on proportions, older adults appeared to prefer implementation intentions to general statements, no emoticon to smiley emoticons, and messages with added meaning (long) to short messages. Younger adults had greater proportions who preferred general statements, smiley emoticons, and short messages.

When testing each message preference dyad separately for difference by age, only two message categories emerged as significant. A significantly (x2(1) = 4.14, p < .05) greater proportion of older adults (83.3%; n = 24) preferred single punctuation over multiple punctuation compared to younger adults (62.0%, n = 150). In addition, consistent with the findings of those 40 and older in our previous analysis, a significantly (x2(1) = 6.09, p < .05) greater proportion of older adults (81.1%; n = 37) preferred “you” statements to “we” statements compared to younger adults (60.0%, n = 235). Finally, age groups differed at the trend level for smiley emoticon versus no emoticon (x2(1) = 3.63, p = .057). A greater proportion of older adults preferred no emoticon (61.1%, n = 36) over a smiley emoticon compared to younger adults (44.1%, n = 238).

Discussion

To our knowledge, this is the first study to explicitly examine message preferences of adults over 50 compared to a younger cohort. Findings contribute to the limited knowledge about text messages received by middle-aged and older adults to promote personal goal attainment and demonstrate that middle-aged and older adults have clear preferences related to message structure, voice, and tone. Interestingly, almost all of the preferences for messaging were in the same direction as younger adults, suggesting that general guidelines for messaging may be used for most age groups. When constructing messages for adults 50 and older, interventionists should consider the preferences they share with younger adults, such as avoidance of textese and attempts at humor in messages. Nonaggressive, polite messages with good grammar emphasizing the benefits of change are likely to be the most well-received messages across age groups.

Older adults in this sample differed from the younger cohort in some expected ways. They were more likely to be retired than the younger cohort and less likely to be employed full or part time. Interestingly, there were no differences between age groups on cell phone ownership. This difference contrasts with earlier studies; however, this sample is biased toward individuals with a working knowledge of computers. Adults over 50 were more likely to have limited text messaging plans, which is an important limitation for providers to consider when developing mobile interventions for older adults. Individuals in this population may be unable or unwilling to participate in a text messaging intervention due to cost and plan limitations. This is consistent with previous literature on barriers to engagement with mobile technology among older adults,33 in which cost is cited as the largest barrier.

Because age also moderated preferences in several categories, additional consideration must be given to preferences of adults 50 and older. For adults over 50, phrasing messages using “you” (as opposed to “we”) and avoiding multiple punctuation (e.g.! instead of !!!) may optimize message tailoring to this age group. In addition, while older adults prefer a happy emoticon to a sad emoticon, messages tailored to adults over 50 probably should not have an emoticon at all to increase engagement as a general rule. By making such accommodations in tailoring messaging to older adults, health promotion interventions that utilize SMS will have a more optimal opportunity to engage the adult in the intervention.

Study limitations

As mentioned above, caution should be used in generalizing findings to a broad range of adults over 50 who may vary in their working knowledge of digital technology. Due to the fact that the sample was recruited from an online labor market, it is safe to assume that individuals less familiar with computers are less likely to have been recruited for the present study. While this prevents generalizability to all middle-aged and older adults with a spectrum of knowledge of digital technology, there is evidence that large group middle-aged and older adults are already pursuing online34 and text messaging health interventions.35 Understanding how best to intervene with this group of relatively technology savvy individuals is only an initial step in serving individuals in middle and older age.

Sample size for some of the message dyads is a clear limitation, given that in some cases the dyads were evaluated by fewer than 10 people. Another key limitation is that these message preferences were assessed in the context of personal goals defined by the respondents. It is not known how these preferences might change within person when considering different personal goals. Messaging preferences also may be context specific, such that preferences delineated during an online survey may or may not reflect preferences if an individual were actually receiving messages on his or her phone. Messaging preferences have yet to be linked to outcomes, and it remains unknown whether preferred messages are the most effective in promoting behavior change among this age group. Finally, age may be just one of many factors that determine messaging preferences.

Conclusions

Despite these limitations, this study provides important preliminary information about message preferences among adults over 50. Mobile messaging intervention development can be informed by these message preferences in order to increase user engagement in SMS and mobile app push alert interventions to improve health outcomes. While older adults had similar messaging preferences to younger adults in most categories, differences in specific categories—specifically, preferring no emoticon and “you” based messages—suggest the need to further test preferences with a larger sample and in the context of an actual intervention in order to link messaging preferences to outcomes among middle-aged and older adults. In addition, research on text messaging interventions with this age group must report on the structure, tone, and voice used in the messages so that more information can be gained about how the format of messaging encourages or discourages engagement. These will be important initial steps in expanding and honing mHealth for older adults, both within and outside the home.

Declaration of conflicting interests

The author(s) declared no potential conflicts of interest with respect to the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article.

Funding

The author(s) disclosed receipt of the following financial support for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article: This study was supported with funding from the National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism (Grant R34 AA 021502A; PI: Muench).

Guarantor

AK.

Contributorship

FM designed and implemented the parent study with KVS as the research assistant and AK as a co-collaborator. AK performed this secondary data analysis and wrote the manuscript with edits from KVS and FM.

References

  • 1.Muench F, van Stolk-Cooke K, Morgenstern J, et al. Understanding messaging preferences to inform development of mobile goal-directed behavioral interventions. J Med Internet Res 2014; 16: e14. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Free C, Phillips G, Galli L, et al. The effectiveness of mobile-health technology-based health behaviour change or disease management interventions for health care consumers: a systematic review. PLoS Med 2013; 10: e1001362. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Hall AK, Cole-Lewis H, Bernhardt JM. Mobile text messaging for health: a systematic review of reviews. Annu Rev Public Health 2015; 36: 393–415. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Pew Research Center. The smartphone difference. Washington, DC: Pew Research Center. http://www.pewinternet.org/2015/04/01/us-smartphone-use-in-2015/ (2015, accessed 20 August 2017).
  • 5.Pew Research Center. Smartphone ownership and internet usage continues to climb in emerging economies. Washington, DC: Pew Research Center. http://www.pewglobal.org/files/2016/02/pew_research_center_global_technology_report_final_february_22__2016.pdf (2016, accessed August 2016).
  • 6.Pew Research Center. Mobile technology fact sheet. http://www.pewinternet.org/fact-sheets/mobile-technology-fact-sheet/ (accessed August 2016).
  • 7.Mitzner TL, Boron JB, Fausset CB, et al. Older adults talk technology: technology usage and attitudes. Comput Human Behav 2010; 26: 1710–1721. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Nahm E-, Bausell B, Resnick B, et al. Online research in older adults: lessons learned from conducting an online randomized controlled trial. Appl Nurs Res 2011; 24: 269–275. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Melenhorst AS, Rogers WA, Bouwhuis DG. Older adults' motivated choice for technological innovation: evidence for benefit driven selectivity. Psychol Aging 2006; 21: 190–195. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Lin CA, Neafsey PJ, Strickler Z. Usability testing by older adults of a computer-mediated health communication program. J Health Commun 2009; 14: 102–118. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Zaphiris P, Kurniawan S, Ghiawadwala M. A systematic approach to the development of research-based web design guidelines for older people. Univ Access Inf Soc 2007; 6: 59–75. [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Park LG, Howie-Esquivel J, Chung ML, et al. A text messaging intervention to promote medication adherence for patients with coronary heart disease: a randomized controlled trial. Patient Educ Counsel 2014; 94: 261–268. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Foreman KF, Stockl KM, Le LB, et al. Impact of a text messaging pilot program on patient medication adherence. Clin Therap 2012; 34: 1084–1091. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Cole-Lewis H, Kershaw T. Text messaging as a tool for behavior change in disease prevention and management. Epidemiol Rev 2010; 32: 56–69. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Heron KE, Smyth JM. Ecological momentary interventions: incorporating mobile technology into psychosocial and health behaviour treatments. Br J Health Psychol 2010; 15: 1–39. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Hall AK, Cole-Lewis H, Bernhardt JM. Mobile text messaging for health: a systematic review of reviews. Annu Rev Public Health 2015; 36: 393–415. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Kuerbis A, Mulliken A, Muench F, et al. Older adults and mobile technology: Toward a better understanding of utilization for applications to behavioral health. Mental Health and Addiction Research 2017; 2(2): 1–11. DOI: 10.15761/MHAR.1000136.
  • 18.Hughes LD, Done J, Young A. Not 2 old 2 TXT: there is potential to use email and SMS text message healthcare reminders for rheumatology patients up to 65 years old. Health Inform J 2011; 17: 266–276. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Dick JJ, Nundy S, Solomon MC, et al. Feasibility and usability of a text message-based program for diabetes self-management in an urban African-American population. J Diabetes Sci Technol 2011; 5: 1246–1254. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Kim BH, Glanz K. Text messaging to motivate walking in older African Americans. Am J Prevent Med 2013; 44: 71–75. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Head KJ, Noar SM, Iannarino NT, et al. Efficacy of text messaging-based interventions for health promotion: a meta-analysis. Soc Sci Med 2013; 97: 41–48. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Finitsis DJ, Pellowski JA, Johnson BT. Text message intervention designs to promote adherence to antiretroviral therapy (ART): a meta-analysis of randomized controlled trials. PLoS One 2014; 9: e88166. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Noar SM, Harrington NG, Aldrich RS. The role of message tailoring in the development of persuasive health communication messages. In: Beck CS. (ed). Communication Yearbook 33, New York: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc., 2009, pp. 73–133. [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Dijkstra A. The psychology of tailoring-ingredients in computer-tailored persuasion. Soc Pers Psychol Compass 2008; 2: 765–784. [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Kreuter MW, Strecher VJ, Glassman B. One size does not fit all: the case for tailoring print materials. Ann Behav Med 1999; 21: 276–283. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Williams-Piehota P, Schneider TR, Pizarro J, et al. Matching health messages to locus of control beliefs for promoting mammography utilization. Psychol Health 2004; 19: 407–423. [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Williams-Piehota P, Schneider TR, Pizarro J, et al. Matching health messages to information-processing styles: need for cognition and mammography utilization. Health Commun 2003; 15: 375–392. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Nyatsanza F, McSorley J, Murphy S, et al. ‘It's all in the message’: the utility of personalised short message service (SMS) texts to remind patients at higher risk of STIs and HIV to reattend for testing—a repeat before and after study. Sex Transm Infect 2016; 92: 393. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Hanson VL. Technology skill and age: what will be the same 20 years from now? Univ Access Inform Soc 2011; 10: 443–452. [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Mason W, Suri S. Conducting behavioral research on Amazon's Mechanical Turk. Behav Res Methods 2012; 44: 1–23. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Gollwitzer PM. Implementation intentions. Am Psychol 1999; 54: 493–503. [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Bandura A. Social cognitive theory: an agentic perspective. Annu Rev Psychol 2001; 52: 1–26. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Morrow-Howell N, Proctor E, Choi S, et al. Depression in public community long-term care: implications for intervention development. J Behav Health Serv Res 2008; 35: 37–51. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Han B, Masukawa K, Rosenbloom D, et al. Web-based screening and brief intervention for unhealthy alcohol use among older adults. Under review. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • 35.Muench F, van Stolk-Cooke K, Kuerbis A, et al. A randomized controlled pilot trial of different mobile messaging interventions for problem drinking compared to weekly drink tracking. PLoS One 2017; 12: e0167900. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Articles from Journal of Rehabilitation and Assistive Technologies Engineering are provided here courtesy of SAGE Publications

RESOURCES