Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2019 Oct 1.
Published in final edited form as: J Youth Adolesc. 2018 Jun 7;47(10):2181–2205. doi: 10.1007/s10964-018-0862-1

Explicating Acculturation Strategies among Asian American Youth: Subtypes and Correlates across Filipino and Korean Americans

Yoonsun Choi 1, Michael Park 1, Jeanette Park Lee 1, Miwa Yasui 1, Tae Yeun Kim 2
PMCID: PMC6286232  NIHMSID: NIHMS973731  PMID: 29881910

Abstract

Acculturation strategy, a varying combination of heritage and mainstream cultural orientations and one of the significant determinants of youth development, has been understudied with Asian American youth and particularly at a subgroup-specific level. This study used person-oriented latent profile analysis (LPA) to identify acculturation strategy subtypes among Filipino American and Korean American adolescents living in the Midwest. Associations between the subtypes and numerous correlates including demographics, family process and youth outcomes were also examined. Using large scale survey data (N=1,580; 379 Filipino American youth and 377 parents, and 410 Korean American youth and 414 parents; MAGE of youth=15.01), the study found three acculturation subtypes for Filipino American youth: High Assimilation with Ethnic Identity, Integrated Bicultural with Strongest Ethnic Identity, and Modest Bicultural with Strong Ethnic Identity; and three acculturation subtypes for Korean American youth: Separation, Integrated Bicultural, and Modest Bicultural with Strong Ethnic Identity. Both Filipino American and Korean American youth exhibited immersion in the host culture while retaining a strong heritage identity. Although bicultural strategies appear most favorable, the results varied by gender and ethnicity, e.g., integrated bicultural Filipino Americans, comprised of more girls, might do well at school but were at risk of poor mental health. Korean American separation, comprised of more boys, demonstrated a small but significant risk in family process and substance use behaviors that merits in-depth examination. The findings deepen the understanding of heterogeneous acculturation strategies among Asian American youth and provide implications for future research.

Keywords: Acculturation strategy, Filipino American, Korean American, Asian American, family process, race/ethnicity, youth development

Introduction

Adolescence is a life stage marked by the challenge of identity development (Erikson, 1968). Identity development unfolds as the adolescent differentiates the self from traditional constraints associated with family and institutional expectations (Bukobza, 2009). Biologically, adolescents are primed to engage in risk-taking as they test the boundaries of self (Steinberg, 2010). Decisions made in the impulsivity of adolescence can have long-reaching physical, social, and legal ramifications, and understanding the risks and protective factors of adolescent outcomes are a strong public health concern (Patton et al., 2016). Adolescence may be especially fraught for young Asian Americans, the overwhelming majority of whom have foreign-born parents (Pew Research Center, 2017). Asian American adolescents form identities and make decisions under the dual pressures of acculturation (the adoption of the mainstream culture) and enculturation (the retention of the heritage culture) (Choi, Tan, Yasui & Hahm, 2016; Castillo et al., 2012; Garcia Coll et al., 1996). Acculturation and enculturation are further complicated by family process, defined by parenting styles, values, behaviors and parent-child relationships. Family process is influential on youth acculturation and enculturation (Choi et al., 2016; Kim & Hou, 2016; Umana-Taylor et al., 2013; Knight et al., 2011). Youth acculturation strategy, in turn, may determine how youth perceive and thereby interact with family process (Choi, Kim, Pekelnicky, Kim & Kim, 2017; Choi, He, & Harachi, 2008). There is evidence that this interplay of acculturation and family process is among the most influential factors in developmental outcomes among cultural minority youth (Lui, 2015; Lim et al., 2009; Kim & Cain, 2008; Garcia Coll & Pachter, 2002).

Despite the consequential impact of acculturation and family process on Asian American adolescent development, few studies have examined how acculturation strategies correlate with family process and youth outcomes among Asian Americans. In particular, there is a scarcity of scholarship at the subgroup level. Though Asian Americans broadly may share aspects of family process, specific variables such as the gendered treatment of children and boundaries of family obligations are likely to differ across subgroups (Choi, Kim, Noh, Lee, & Takeuchi, 2017; Paik et al., 2017; Choi et al., 2008). Contemporary scholarship on Asian Americans, spurred by unprecedented levels of Asian immigration to the U.S., has only recently begun to differentiate among the over twenty Asian American subgroups, each with their distinct culture and sociopolitical context (Trinh-Shevrin, Islam, & Rey, 2009). To address these gaps in the literature, the present study uses latent profile analysis (LPA) to uncover latent subtypes of acculturation strategy among Filipino American and Korean American adolescents. These two groups share the pan-ethnic label of “Asian American,” but are notably dissimilar in acculturation patterns, family characteristics and youth outcomes (Choi, 2008; Choi et al., 2017). Identifying associations between the subtypes and a wide range of correlates including family process variables and youth outcomes will illuminate constructive acculturation strategies among Asian American subgroups, as well as bolster effective intervention among the rapidly growing population of Asian American adolescents.

Berry’s Model of Acculturation and Asian American Adolescents

In describing acculturation, we follow Kim and Abreu’s (2001) convention using linearity to refer to the number of cultures the model references, and dimensionality to the multiple domains on which acculturation can take place. The concomitant processes of acculturation and enculturation have been examined under Berry’s (1997) four-cell rubric of assimilation, integration, separation and marginalization. Berry’s bilinear model contravenes earlier unilinear models of acculturation to propose that acculturation represents coetaneous orientations to both the heritage and mainstream culture, albeit at variable levels (Berry, 1980). Assimilation denotes a maximum level of engagement with mainstream culture; integration, frequently referred to as biculturalism (Schwartz & Zamboanga, 2008), simultaneously engages both mainstream and heritage culture; separation is an exclusion of the mainstream culture and complete allegiance to the heritage culture; and marginalization indicates alienation from both mainstream and heritage culture.

Associations between these acculturation strategies and health and psychosocial outcomes are mixed and highly dependent on the characteristics of the sample population, as well as statistical method employed and specific operationalization of dimensions measured (Schwartz et al., 2010). Research concurs that acculturation happens at variable rates on multiple dimensions, such as mainstream language acquisition, ethnic pride, and cultural knowledge (Schwartz et al., 2010; Miller, 2007; Szapocznik et al., 1978). Nevertheless, a number of important studies have found that integration leads to the most adaptive functioning and those who are marginalized as experiencing the worst outcomes (Berry, 2005). Assimilation and separation, both of which entail individuals inhabiting either host or heritage culture to the relative exclusion of the other, are often thought to be maladaptive, but both strategies have differential impact among different immigrant groups. For example, assimilation has been associated with feelings of betrayal and distress as Asian American young adults experience racial/ethnic discrimination and structural inequality despite their assimilation into the host culture (Park, Schwartz, Lee, Kim, & Rodriguez, 2013). At the same time, assimilation has been found to be beneficial in certain domains, such as help-seeking behaviors (Miller et al., 2013). Similarly, separation strategy is largely found to be less adaptive than integration (Berry, 2005), as it isolates individuals from the majority culture. Yet, it has been found to be beneficial in certain circumstances; e.g., Korean immigrants have employed a separation strategy to build successful economic and social communities counter to the social exclusion and segregation practiced by their host culture (Min, 2006). Finally, while marginalization is uniformly found to be the most maladaptive (Fox, Merz, Solorzano, & Roesch, 2013), its prevalence in community samples has been challenged (del Pilar & Udasco, 2004; Schwartz & Zamboanga, 2008).

Among Asian American adolescents, acculturative variables have been associated with a variety of physical, psychological, and social outcomes including school performance (Fuligni, Witkow, & Garcia Coll, 2005), suicide attempts (Wong & Maffini, 2011), psychological functioning (Kim & Omizo, 2010), intergenerational conflict and depression (Ying & Han, 2007), smoking attitudes and behaviors (Weiss & Garbanati, 2004), binge drinking and alcohol use (Hahm, Lahiff, & Guterman, 2004), sexual behavior (Tong, 2013), and weight change (Diep, Baranowski, & Kimbro, 2017). With notable exceptions, the literature suggests that heritage retention, such as ethnic identity and heritage language competence, is associated with positive development among Asian American adolescents. A handful of studies have found that Asian American youth who retain, at least partially, heritage language competency or ethnic identity had positive associations with their family processes, demonstrating that family process may mediate the impact of acculturation strategies on youth outcomes (Choi, Kim, Pekelnicky, et al., 2017; Dinh & Nguyen, 2006; Tseng & Fuligni, 2000). Other studies uphold the immigrant paradox, whereby positive adolescent outcomes diminish as assimilation into the host country increases (Takeuchi, Hong, Gile, & Alegria, 2007). Though rapid assimilation of immigrants may be seen as desirable by the mainstream culture (Vigor, 2008), the actual impact of assimilation on family process should not be presumed, particularly as studies show that correlates in adolescent outcomes may differ across disparate domains of acculturation and enculturation (Choi, Kim, Pekelnicky, et al., 2017; Choi et al., 2016). This study, although not testing the interplay of acculturation, family process and youth outcomes in mediational models, aims to enhance a general understanding by investigating bivariate associations among the variables, which can lay the foundation for future and more complex analyses.

Identifying Acculturation Strategies

Investigating acculturation, family process, and youth outcomes among Asian American youth at a subgroup level faces several methodological challenges. First, notwithstanding scholarship cited above, acculturation at the adolescent level of development remains relatively sparse. A 22-year review found that more than half of the studies on acculturation were conducted with college students, who are by definition marked by a specific academic and social environment as well as unique transition into adulthood (Yang, Langehr, & Ong, 2011). Further, measures of family process validated across Asian American subgroups are rare (for an exception, see Choi, Kim, Noh, et al., 2017), as are models of acculturation that account for subgroup differences. Berry’s model, while still influential, has been criticized for insensitivity to group saliency (Schwartz & Zamboanga, 2008). A more nuanced approach to acculturation that takes into account specific characteristics of individuals, such as country of origin, socioeconomic status (SES), and heritage language, may have greater explanatory power and applicability to Asian American subjects (Chung, Kim, & Abreu, 2004; Schwartz, Unger, Zamboanga, & Szapocznik, 2010).

The mixed findings on associations between acculturation strategies and developmental outcomes may be attributed in part to a priori classification rules, such as those used in the mean-split method, that assume all groups will fit into one of Berry’s four typology (Schwartz et al., 2010). In fact, when more statistically robust approaches, such as LPA, are used, multiple variants of Berry’s typology are uncovered. One such example is Choi et al. (2016)’s study finding that Korean American middle schoolers mostly utilized an integrated bicultural strategy with a strong sense of ethnic identity, some adopted separation or “modestly bicultural” strategy but none used assimilation or marginalization, with no participants altogether disavowing heritage culture and ethnicity. Another example is Chia and Costigan (2006)’s finding that Chinese Canadian college students are identified with, in addition to Berry’s four typology, two additional subtypes of acculturation strategy the authors labeled as “integrated group with Chinese practices” and “marginalized group with Chinese practices.” These variations are not unexpected, given the subgroup variability such as in sociopolitical and historical context of immigration, and traditional values and behaviors of heritage culture espoused by different Asian American subgroups.

In the present study, LPA allows us to derive sample-specific and person-centered subgroups that reflect the heterogeneity of acculturation strategies among Asian American subgroups (e.g., Fox et al., 2013; Chia and Costigan, 2006). The present study utilizes measures that are bilinear (i.e., acculturation and enculturation) and multidimensional (i.e., language, identity and cultural behavioral participation) to generate subtypes of acculturation strategy, which together allow for differential rates of acculturation across dimensions. For example, minority groups have been found to be more likely to adopt the behavioral norms of the dominant culture more readily than the values of the dominant culture (Rosenthal & Feldman, 1992). This finding was later affirmed with Asian American populations (Choi et al., 2018). In the present study, three distinct dimensions of cultural orientation are used in generating acculturation strategies: language competency, strength of identity, and behavioral cultural participation within host culture versus heritage culture (Choi et al., 2016; Ward, 2001). These dimensions are especially salient to Asian Americans, who often exhibit limited heritage language proficiency while varying in their espousal of strong ethnic identity or behavioral cultural participation (Tsai, Chentsova-Dutton, & Wong, 2002).

This study further examines how these measures are associated with various contextual variables, such as demographics, parental acculturation, family process and core cultural values. The examination of these associations examines and facilitates a comprehensive understanding of how Filipino American and Korean American adolescents vary across dimensions of acculturation strategies; how demographics and parental characteristics may contribute to the formation of those strategies; and the ways in which different acculturation strategies may shape youth perception of family process and race/ethnicity and, most importantly, may predict youth outcomes.

Filipino and Korean Americans

Filipino Americans and Korean Americans are the third and fifth most populous groups of Asian Americans, respectively, and as subjects of this study offer important points of overlap and divergence. These two Asian American subgroups share global indicators of SES, but differ in family process and acculturation (Russell, Crockett, & Chao, 2010) as well as academic and psychosocial outcomes among their progeny. U.S.-born Filipino Americans lag behind Korean Americans in college graduation, and in general Filipino Americans trail most of other Asian Americans in developmental outcomes (Choi, 2008; Ong & Viernes, 2013). Filipino Americans share a unique affinity with Latino culture (Ocampo, 2014) and are least likely, among all Asian American subgroups, to have limited English proficiency (Ramakrishnan & Ahmad, 2014). Filipino Americans, along with Japanese Americans, are least likely among all Asian American subgroups to live in a homogenous ethnic enclave (Ling & Austin, 2015).

Family processes among Filipino Americans are also distinct. Family dynamics are more egalitarian in Filipino culture as compared to other Asian subgroups (Paik, Witenstein, & Choe, 2016), but are more hierarchical and gendered than in White families, with Filipina youth experiencing particular expectations of filial loyalty (Espiritu, 2003). Immigrants from the Philippines are often thought to be the most assimilated of all Asian American subgroups (Vigor, 2008). In comparison, Korean Americans are a culturally and ethnically separated subgroup, with relatively low rates of English proficiency among Korean immigrant adults and higher tendency to self-segregate in ethnic enclaves (Min, 2006). Where Filipino culture emphasizes the girl’s responsibility to uphold filial loyalty, in traditional Korean culture, it is the eldest son that is expected to care for aging parents (Duk, 2013). These significant differences can be instructive in our understanding of acculturative strategies and their associated developmental outcomes among second-generation Filipino American and Korean American adolescents.

Correlates of Acculturation Strategy

There is a plethora of variables that are intertwined with acculturation/enculturation process and the resulting acculturation strategy. Some variables may be determinants of acculturation strategy (e.g., demographics). Other variables may be influenced by the specified acculturation strategy (e.g., family process), or the relationships may be reciprocal (e.g. Umana-Taylor et al., 2013). This study focuses on correlates from demographics, family process, race-ethnicity related and youth outcomes that are particularly pertinent to Asian American youth development. The list of correlates is intentionally extensive to provide as comprehensive a picture of relations as possible to serve as a springboard for future research. There are few, if any, studies that look at associations among these correlates and acculturation strategies across Asian American subgroups. Below is an explication of the correlates that are used in this study.

Demographics

Gender and age are differentially associated with the rates and patterns of acculturation and enculturation. For example, female gender predicts higher endorsement of host culture and its values, perhaps because of mainstream Western culture’s relatively egalitarian attitude toward women (Berry, 1997). Younger age at time of immigration predicts rapid acculturation and, in particular, language proficiency which is itself an oft-used proxy for acculturation (Portes & Rumbaut, 2001). In contrast, older age at immigration often predicts acculturation struggles and resistance to host culture (Cheung, Chudek, & Heine, 2010). Similarly, nativity and length of years in the U.S. predict higher host culture orientation. Parent’s demographics (e.g., age, years living in U.S., and immigrant and citizenship status) can also be significant predictors of youth acculturation strategies, in that longer years of residence in the U.S. and citizenship among parents predict more integrated acculturation strategies among youth (Choi et al., 2016).

Family Process

Family process includes parenting values, parenting behaviors and parent-child relationships. Asian American family process uniquely blends traditional Asian culture, the experiences of immigration and racial/ethnic minority status, and universal family process that is common across race/ethnicity (Choi, Kim, Noh, et al., 2017). In short, culture, race/ethnicity and immigration fundamentally shape Asian American family process, which may shape (or be shaped by) acculturation strategy. This paper organizes and reviews Asian American family process in the domains of (1) traditional core familism values, (2) indigenous family process that reflects heritage culture, (3) immigrant family process, (4) conventional family process, and (5) racial/ethnic socialization in the family.

Traditional core familism values

Among Filipino American and Korean American families, traditional core familism values include reinforcing respect for adults, family hierarchy and age veneration in family and social relations, making sacrifices to uphold harmony within the family, and remaining loyal to family obligations (Choi, Kim, Noh, et al., 2017). Family obligation may have a far greater ramification on Filipino American individuals than on Korean Americans, as Filipino Americans include extended family and close non-family members within their boundary of family (Espiritu, 2003).

Indigenous family process

Core familism values among Asian Americans are transmitted, inter alia, via parenting behaviors. Filipino American and Korean American parents, for example, reinforce heritage values by enforcing traditional manners and etiquettes that symbolize respect for adults and family hierarchy (Choi, Kim, Noh, et al., 2017). Asian parent behaviors are often viewed as more controlling and gendered than their Western counterparts (Garcia Coll & Pachter, 2002). However, Asian parental control (coined as order-keeping control) is different from Western parental control (i.e., coercive control) (Kagitçibasi, 2007) and likely expressed in terms of close monitoring (Chao, 1994), academically orientated control, and family obligation (Fuligni, 2007). Meanwhile, parental warmth and affection are often expressed indirectly in Asian American families. Wu and Chao (2011) operationalized the concept of qin as Asian American parental warmth characterized by implicit parental affection via instrumental support, devotion, and support for education rather than physical, verbal, and emotional expressions. Asian American adolescents’ acculturation strategies influence how they perceive their family process (Choi, Kim, Pekelnicky, et al., 2017). In addition, indigenous parenting may facilitate youth enculturation. Youth growing up in families practicing more traditional parenting may understand the intentions of parental behaviors and possibly internalize them. Conversely, culturally assimilated Asian American youth may adopt the mainstream stereotypes of their parents and view them as gendered, controlling, or emotionally distant (Choi, Kim, Pekelnicky, et al., 2017).

Conventional family process

Certain aspects of Asian American family process are culturally invariant and can be assessed by conventional, predominantly Western measures, for example, parenting style (authoritative and authoritarian), parent-child bonding and conflict, parental acceptance and rejection, parent-child communication, autonomy, and parental rules and restrictions (e.g. Sorkhabi, 2005). When bilinear and multidimensional cultural orientations were examined in relation to youth perception of family process, identity and behavioral enculturation positively predicted parent-child bonding, which was a notable protective factor for youth (Choi, Kim, Pekelnicky, et al., 2017). It may be that youth’s enculturation diminishes the acculturation gap and helps parent-child relations. It is also plausible that parent’s positive behaviors toward their child and expressive affection may help youth better understand parental culture, thus increase enculturation.

Immigrant family process

Culturally unique aspects of Asian American parenting may be further solidified by an immigrant ethos, i.e., shared traits among immigrants, including vigorous emphasis on education and a drive to succeed that is often accompanied by parental sacrifice to promote those values (Lee & Bean, 2010). Assimilation to the host culture often is associated with a weakened immigrant ethos (Portes & Rumbaut, 2001). Asian American adolescents can feel embarrassed by their parents, often because of their parents’ culturally inept behaviors or poor English (Choi & Kim, 2010), which may lead to conflict; indeed, intergenerational cultural conflict between parent and child is normative among immigrant families (Choi, He, & Harachi, 2008).

Racial/ethnic socialization

Racial/ethnic socialization by parents (i.e., cultural socialization, promotion of mistrust and preparation for bias) is a crucial aspect of both family process and acculturation strategies among racial/ethnic minority families (Hughes et al., 2008). Parental messages about race and ethnicity are generally meant to instill pride, prepare their children for bias, and/or promote mistrust of the mainstream. Active racial/ethnic socialization in the family, in particular that which promotes pride, is often identified as a protective factor for minority youth. However, the extent to which it interacts with acculturation strategy remains unclear.

Race/Ethnicity Related

Race/ethnicity related variables, such as an experience of racial discrimination, have notable associations with acculturation strategies. There is some evidence that immigrant children may idealize White families (Chung & Shibusawa, 2013); their perception of mainstream culture, especially if idealized, may encourage youth to prefer assimilation to host culture. Notably, a strong racial/ethnic identity, generally regarded as a protective factor, is correlated with greater reports of discrimination, while experiences of racial discrimination have a detrimental effect (Huynh & Fuligni, 2010; Operario & Fiske, 2001). How race/ethnicity variables interact with acculturation strategies remains to be examined.

Youth outcomes

Several studies establish that youth outcomes can vary dramatically among Asian American subgroups (Choi, 2008). As described above, varying acculturation strategies may confer differential risks on adolescent outcomes, and it is crucial to understand the interactional contexts in which acculturation occurs to accurately understand these risks (Schwartz et al., 2010). However, few studies look at this relationship across Asian American subgroups. Additionally, while Asian Americans in the aggregate may exhibit low rates of externalized problems, they endorse higher rates of internalized problems, including depression and suicidal ideation (Duldulao, Takeuchi, & Hong, 2009). In this study, the youth outcome correlates include both internal and externalized indicators, such as depressive symptoms, school grades, antisocial behaviors and substance use. A granular understanding of how internal and external problems are related to acculturation can help inform culturally competent interventions at the subgroup level.

Present Study

Using LPA, the present study develops the most parsimonious number of meaningful latent subtypes that best describes acculturation strategies in Korean American and Filipino American adolescents, without forcing fit onto any subtype. This study further expands prior studies by examining how different dimensions of acculturation collectively form variant strategies and how each strategy is associated with a detailed list of correlates, which are designed to accurately capture the nuanced experiences of growing up Asian Americans. Family process variables are specified to reflect cultural distinctions of Filipino American and Korean American families.

Explicating Acculturation Strategies

A total of 6 indicators used to generate acculturation strategies reflect bilinearity (acculturation and enculturation) and multidimensionality. Specifically, the dimensions of heritage language, ethnic identity, heritage cultural participation, English, American identity and host cultural participation were deliberately chosen to reflect the most relevant domains of acculturation among Asian American youth (Choi, Kim, Pekelnicky, et al., 2017; Choi et al., 2016).

Filipino and Korean American parents significantly differ in acculturation. Filipino American parents showed greater assimilation while Korean American parents endorsed more separation, placing the two subgroups of parents almost at opposing ends of the acculturation spectrum (Choi, Kim, Noh, et al., 2017). In contrast, based on prior limited studies, as well as the demographic data of our present sample, we expect Korean American and Filipino American adolescents to share a similarly high degree of linguistic assimilation, simply by the function of their nativity. The majority of Asian American youth are U.S.-born children of immigrants or children who immigrated at a young age. The rates of heritage language retention and linguistic enculturation are quite low, ranging from 1% to 10%, among U.S.-born Asian Americans (Portes & Rumbaut, 2001). Filipino American and Korean American adolescents may vary in other dimensions, such as identity and cultural practices. Similar to linguistic acculturation, we would expect a high degree of identity and behavioral acculturation across groups in our sample (Portes & Rumbaut, 2001). Enculturation is not straightforward. A handful of studies has found that children can concurrently exhibit a high level of linguistic and behavioral acculturation while also endorsing a strong racial/ethnic identity and frequently practicing certain aspects of their heritage culture (Atzaba-Poria & Pike, 2007; Costigan & Dokis, 2006; Telzer, 2010). Such a pattern has been found among the children of African or Caribbean immigrants (Waters, Ueba, & Marrow, 2007) and speaks to the differential experiences of immigrant parents and their progeny. We hypothesize that we will find a similar pattern across both our samples. We do not expect to find marginalization in this group of community samples.

We further hypothesize that, relative to Korean American youth, Filipino American youth would be acculturation leaning: self-reporting high fluency in English but limited heritage language and endorsing more host cultural behaviors than heritage ones. The notion of colonial mentality complicates our hypothesis vis-à-vis ethnic versus American identity. Some scholars argue that colonial mentality among Filipino American parents has been passed on to their children (David & Okazaki, 2006), while others point to lack of empirical data to support the claim. For example, in an in-depth study of over 100 Filipino American second-generation young adults, the participants demonstrated a solid and positive sense of ethnic identity, despite their perception of colonial mentality prominent among their parents (Ferrera, 2011). The sparse and conflicting research leaves us without a firm hypothesis regarding Filipino American youths’ sense of identities.

Conversely, in line with recent findings on middle school youth (Choi et al., 2016), Korean American youth are expected to show a largely integrated strategy with a strong sense of ethnic identity, with a small group of separation and modest bicultural youth but no assimilation group. Korean immigrant parents are known to adhere to their heritage culture and strong identity, residing mostly within ethnic enclaves and remaining monolingual (Min, 2006). In contrast, Korean American youth are predominantly English speaking with a low to modest heritage language retention but a strong sense of ethnic identity and varying degrees of heritage cultural participation (Choi et al., 2016). Relative to Filipino American youth, we expect Korean American youth to exhibit greater competency in their heritage language, mainly because of Korean parents’ lack of English proficiency (Choi et al., 2013).

Comparisons of Correlates

This study uses data from the Midwest Longitudinal Study of Asian American Families (ML-SAAF), a longitudinal study of Filipino and Korean American youth and their parents. ML-SAAF presents a rare opportunity to examine and compare two large Asian American subgroups of adolescents across a comprehensive list of correlates and their associations with acculturation strategies. Accurately measuring Asian American family process is challenging. The majority of existing measures were developed using White families (Chao, 1994) and they alone cannot provide a complete picture. ML-SAAF has significantly expanded the conventional repertoire of family process variables to include subgroup specific measures of core traditional values, and indigenous family process (for details, see Choi, Kim, Noh, et al., 2017; Choi, Park, et al., 2017). The latent subgroups were compared across the following sets of important correlates: demographics, family process including core cultural values, indigenous, conventional and immigrant family process, and racial/ethnic socialization, race/ethnicity related, and youth outcomes.

First, demographic correlates include age, gender, nativity, and the years of living in U.S. for youth and age, years living in the U.S., citizenship and permanent residence status for parents. We hypothesize that female gender, younger age, nativity, longer years of residence among youth, and parental acculturation indicators would be positively related with more acculturation than enculturation leaning strategies.

For family process, we examined core familism values including respect for adults, harmony and sacrifice, family obligation and boundary of the family. Indigenous Asian American family process correlates include parental emphasis on traditional manners and etiquettes that symbolize the core familism values, age veneration, gendered expectation, and academically oriented control. Addressing the nature of implicit parental affection among Asian parents, the parental affection construct includes both direct and indirect parental expression as well as the concept of qin which assesses instrumental support as a surrogate for expressions of affection. Additionally, several correlates include aspects of the immigrant ethos, such as parental emphasis on education, pressure to succeed, intergenerational cultural conflict between parent and child, and feeling embarrassed about immigrant parents. We also examined conventional family process correlates, including parenting styles (authoritarian and authoritative), parent-child bonding and conflict, parental acceptance and rejection, parent-child communication, autonomy, parental rules and regulations.

While the general pattern found in existing literature is that those youth who maintain aspects of their heritage culture are more likely to appreciate parental cultural behaviors and values, we lack a density of studies to guide us in forming specific hypotheses for each correlate. Therefore, we make general and directional hypotheses in regard to the associations between acculturation strategies and family process correlates. We expect that acculturation leaning strategies such as assimilation would correlate with lower core family values, indigenous family process, and immigrant family process, while enculturation leaning strategies such as separation would show reversed relationships. In addition, acculturation leaning strategies would be related to higher authoritarian style, parent-child conflict, parental rejection, and more rules and regulations, but lower authoritative style, parent-child bonding, parental acceptance, parent-child communication, and granting autonomy. Enculturation leaning strategies would show reversed relationships. Bicultural integration strategy, high both on acculturation and enculturation, is expected to show a pattern similar to that of enculturation leaning strategies in family process.

We examined three aspects of racial/ethnic socialization: cultural socialization, promotion of mistrust and preparation for bias. In addition, we included two race/ethnicity related constructs – experience of racial discrimination and youth idealized perception of White family. Although findings are mixed in the literature, enculturation is in general enhanced by active racial/ethnic socialization in the family, and increases an awareness of racial discrimination and protects youth from envying mainstream culture and mainstream families. We expect that separation and integration, which are high on enculturation, would report higher racial/ethnic socialization, higher racial discrimination but lower idealized perception of White family than assimilation. We expect assimilation to show an opposite pattern.

Finally, we included youth developmental outcomes as correlates, including depressive symptoms, school grade, antisocial behaviors and substance use. Biculturalism, also referred to as integration, has been hailed as the most protective for youth development (Berry, 1997; Nguyen & Benet-Martinez, 2013) and we hypothesize it will show most favorable adolescent outcomes in both external and internal behaviors. Assimilation and separation require alienation from either host or heritage culture, and are expected to show less favorable outcomes than integration.

The present study will shed light on how an integrated strategy of biculturalism may be related to multifaceted family process among Asian Americans. The numerous correlates presented in this study build a solid empirical foundation upon which future studies may examine multivariate relationships between acculturation strategies and family process, cultural value retention, racial/ethnic socialization, and youth outcomes.

Methods

Overview of the Project

Wave 1 of ML-SAAF surveyed a total of 1,580 individuals, comprised of 379 Filipino American youth and 377 parents (365 families were parent-child dyads) and 410 Korean American youth and 414 parents (407 families were parent-child dyads). All participants resided in Chicago and surrounding Midwest areas and were recruited from multiple sources, including phonebooks, public and private schools, ethnic churches/temples, ethnic grocery stores and ethnic community organizations. A proactive campaign and outreach about the survey continued to respective communities and organizations until the project reached its target numbers (at least 350 families for each subgroup).

The sampling unit was the family. Only families with an adolescent child ages between 12 and 17 (or in middle or high school) whose mother is of Filipino or Korean heritage were eligible. A single youth and his/her primary caretaker in each family were asked to participate. Although the majority of participating families were parent-child dyads, a small number of families included only the youth or the parent. Parents were approached first and, with their consent, youth were asked to participate. The study and its goal of better understanding Asian American family process and its impact on youth development were explained at initial contact as well as during the process of informed consent and assent. Participants received incentives ($40 for adults and $20 for youth). Parent and youth were separately surveyed to protect privacy. On average, the parent survey took about 1 hour and the youth survey 40 minutes. The study procedures were approved by the University’s IRB.

The participants were asked to be in-person interviewed but some preferred self-administration. Eighty-four percent of the survey was conducted by trained bilingual interviewers and the rest was self-administered. A slightly modified version of the interviewer-assisted survey was used for the self-administrated survey (e.g., changing “you” to “I”). The only significant demographic difference between self-administered and interviewer-assisted survey youth participants was their age. Self-administered participants were slightly older (15.33 vs. 14.95, p<.01). The ML-SAAF questionnaires, available both in paper-pencil and web-survey formats, were distributed to eligible participants and collected in person, by mail, or via web. The majority of self-administered surveys were done online and survey formats did not produce any significant differences in the key demographic variables. The questionnaires were available in English, Korean and Tagalog. The English version of survey was translated respectively into Korean and Tagalog using a committee translation process in which multiple translators made independent translations of the same questionnaire and, at a consensus meeting, the committee reconciled discrepancies and agreed on a final version. About 8% of youth participants used a heritage language version of the survey (61 Korean youth and 3 Filipino youth). The rest used an English version. The initial version of the survey was pilot-tested with 341 dyads (186 Korean American child-parent dyads, 155 Filipino American child-parent dyads) for psychometric properties and further revised for clarity before administered to participants in the main study.

Sample Characteristics

The average age was 15.28 (SD=1.89) for Filipino American youth and 14.76 (SD=1.91) for Korean American youth, with a larger proportion of high school students (78.69% Filipino American and 75.25% Korean American) than middle school students. Gender distribution among youth was about equal (56.20% Filipino American and 47.56% Korean American were girls). About 71% Filipino American and 58.29% Korean American youth were U.S.-born, and the average years of living in the U.S. among foreign-born were 8.47 years (SD= 4.24) for Filipino Americans and 8.13 years (SD= 4.28) for Korean Americans. The average age of parents was 46.21 years (SD=5.79) for Filipino Americans and 45.32 years (SD=3.76) for Korean Americans.

The participating parents were predominantly biological mothers (95.65% of Korean Americans and 92.02% of Filipino Americans), foreign-born (98.55% Korean Americans and 90.43% Filipino Americans) with an average of 16.04 years (SD=8.53) of living in the U.S. for Korean Americans and 21.38 years (SD=11.01) for Filipino Americans, highly educated (83.09% of Korean American mothers and 88.56% of Filipino American mothers having at least some college education either in Korea, Philippines or in the U.S.), currently married (92.03% Korean Americans and 88.56% Filipino Americans), and employed (64.69% of Korean American mothers and 87.23% of Filipino American mothers). Less than a quarter of Korean American and about 20% Filipino American families have received free/reduced-price school lunch. These demographic characteristics indicate highly educated middle income families and are consistent with those of Filipino American and Korean American families in Census or national-level data such as Add Health.

Measures

Unless noted otherwise, response options for all measures were an ordinal Likert scale, ranging from 1 to 5, e.g., never or not at all (1) to always or strongly (5). A higher score indicates a higher level of the constructs.

Indicators of subtypes

Language competency

Adopted from the Language, Identity, and Behavior (LIB) (Birman & Trickett, 2002), two sets of two parallel items (four total) measured youth language competency in speaking and understanding both heritage language (Filipino or Korean) and host language (English). Examples of Filipino languages include Tagalog, Bisayan (or Visaya), Ilocano, Kapampangan, and several others. [r=.84 (Filipino American) and .89 (Korean American) for heritage language; r=.75 (Filipino American) and .84 (Korean American) for English].

Behavioral cultural participation

Also adopted from the LIB (Birman & Trickett, 2002), 10 items asked about participation in either heritage or American cultural activities such as social gatherings, media use, and peer composition [α=.79 (Filipino American) and .74 (Korean American) for heritage cultural participation; α=.75 (Filipino American) and .75(Korean American) for host cultural participation].

Identity

Ethnic and American identity were assessed respectively using 10 questions from LIB (Birman & Trickett, 2002), asking the extent to which youth identified themselves as Filipino/Korean or American [α=.76 (Filipino American) and .77 (Korean American) for ethnic identity; α=.81(Filipino American) and .77 (Korean American) for American identity].

Correlates

Demographics

We included several youth-reported and parent-reported demographic variables such as age, gender (0 girls, 1 boys), place of birth (0 foreign born, 1 U.S.-born), years of living in the United States and immigrant legal status including U.S. citizen (0 no, 1 yes) and permanent resident (0 no, 1 yes).

Family process

Traditional core cultural values

Several scales were used to examine youth’s endorsement of traditional core cultural values. They include (1) Respect for Adults: 3 items, e.g., “Even if I am very angry at an older person, I shouldn’t fight or talk back, out of respect,” “Parents and grandparents should be treated with great respect regardless of their differences in views.” [α=.74 (Filipino American) and .72 (Korean American)]; (2) Harmony and Sacrifice: 3 questions to ask about the importance of maintaining harmony and sacrificing individual needs for greater units like family and community, e.g., “It is important to ensure harmonious relations with family members, even at the expense of my own desires,” “It is important to sacrifice individual(s) for the greater good (e.g., family, friends, or community)” [α=.71 (Filipino American) and .60 (Korean American)]; (3) Family Obligations: 7 items adopted from two sources (Choi, 2007; Fuligni & Zhang, 2004), assessing the extent to which youth help out with their family, e.g., “How often do you translate or interpret for your parent?” “How often do you help take care of your family members (for example, younger brothers or sisters, grandparents, sick parents, etc.)?” [α=.63 (Filipino American) and .61 (Korean American)]; (4) Boundary of the Family asked participants who they regard as family, from 8 categories [e.g., “my parents and siblings”, extended family members (“my grandparents of father side” “cousins”) to godparents or close family friends]. A total number of chosen categories were counted as an index to measure the boundary of the family.

Indigenous family process

Five scales were used tap into youth perception of their parent’s traditional values and behaviors that conventional measures of parenting may not capture. The constructs were (1) Traditional Manners and Etiquettes: a 4-item scale, asking youth how much their parents emphasize ethnic behaviors that symbolize respect of the elders, e.g., “To greet adults/elders properly (e.g., [for Koreans] bowing to adults and saying “an-nyung-ha-se-yo” or [for Filipinos] gently placing the back of one’s hand on elders’ forehead and saying “monopo” “To use proper addressing terms (e.g., calling family members with addressing terms instead of first names [for Koreans] unni/nuna, oppa/hyung, harabugi/halmony and [for Filipinos] ate, kuya, lola/lolo, ninang/ninong)” [α=.83 (Filipino American) and .80 (Korean American)]; (2) Gendered Expectation: 7 items adopted from multiple sources (de Guzman, 2011; Espiritu, 2003; Nadal, 2011; Wolf, 1997) to measure youth perception of parental restrictive norms towards girls (e.g., “My parents think that girls should not date while in high school” “My parents think that girls should not stay out late” or “My parents think that boys should avoid anything girlish or feminine” [α=.81 (Filipino American) and .79 (Korean American)]; (3) Academically Orientated Parental Control: 8 items from Chao and Wu (2001) to assess parental control specific to academic related child’s behaviors (e.g., How often do your parents “make sure you do homework,” “limit your social activities (e.g., hanging out with your friends or partying) so that you can work (e.g., studying or practicing musical instruments” or “punish if your grades are down” [α=.67 (Filipino American) and .73 (Korean American)]; (4) Qin (Wu & Chao, 2011): 9-item scale that assesses Asian parent-child bonding through instrumental support (e.g., My parents “invest all what they have for my education” “know all my possible needs before I am aware of them” and “understand my difficulties even though they don’t say anything” [α= .85 (Filipino American) and .84 (Korean American)]; and (5) Parental Affection: 5 questions from the Korean American Families (KAF) Project (Choi, 2007) assessing the extent to which youth perceive parental affection when expressed directly or indirectly (e.g., You feel loved when your parents “tell you that they love you” “when they are strict about bad things like smoking and violence” or “even if they don’t say, when they do things for you, like making your favorite food, putting your needs before theirs, being there for you when you have hard times”) [α= .83 (Filipino American) and .78 (Korean American)].

Conventional family process

Eleven existing conventional family process measures were used to assess youth perception of parenting styles, behaviors and parent-child relations. They were (1) Authoritarian Parenting Style: 4 questions from Parenting Authority Questionnaire (PAQ) (Buri, 1991) that assess stern and strict parenting (e.g., not allowing to question any decision parent makes, getting very upset if child tries to disagree with parent). [α=.80 (Filipino American) and .78 (Korean American)]; (2) Authoritative Parenting Style: 2 questions also adopted from PAQ (Buri, 1991) (e.g., my parent directs my activities and decisions through reasoning and disciplines or consistently gives me direction and guidance in rational and objective ways) [r=.63 (Filipino American) and .60 (Korean American)]; (3) Parent-Child Bonding: 5 questions from the Add Health, asking the extent to which the child feels close to his/her mom and wants to be the kind of person she is [α=.88 (Filipino American) and .85 (Korean American)]; (4) Parent-Child Conflict : 4 questions from Prinz (1977) about getting angry at each other or the parent not listening to child’s side of the story [α=.83 (Filipino American) and .79 (Korean American)]; (5) Parental Acceptance: 9 questions from Parental Acceptance-Rejection Questionnaire (PARQ) (Rohner, 2004), e.g., “My mom makes me feel wanted and needed.”(α=.90 for both); (6) Parental Rejection: 15 PARQ items (Rohner, 2004), e.g., “My mom is too busy to answer my questions.”[α=.89 (Filipino American) and .84 (Korean American)]; (7) Parent-Child Communication: 4 questions from the LIFT project (Eddy, Reid, & Fetrow, 2000), e.g., “I talk with my parents about what is going on in my life.” [α=.93 (Filipino American) and .92 (Korean American)]; (8) Granting Autonomy: 8 items from Silk, Morris, Kanaya, and Steinberg (2003) and Grolnick, Ryan, and Deci (1991), e.g., “My mom keeps pushing me to think independently.” [α=.77 (Filipino American) and .72 (Korean American)]; (9) Parental Rules and (10) Parental Restrictions were a compilation of rules and disciplines that parents may use such as curfew, homework rules, grounding and corporal punishment and the response options were binary (Yes and No).

Immigrant family process

Four additional constructs pertinent to the immigrant family were included: (1) Emphasis on Child’s Education: 2-item measure from Wu and Chao (2011) examining parental willingness to support and sacrifice for their child’s education (e.g., “My parents work very hard to provide the best for my education” “My parents do everything for my education and make any sacrifices” [(r=.62 (Filipino American) and .69 (Korean American)]; (2) Intergenerational Cultural Conflict was from Lee and his colleagues (2000), assessing discrepancy of values across immigrant parents and their children (e.g., “Your parents tell you what to do with your life, but you want to make your own decisions.”) [α=.89 (Filipino American) and .85 (Korean American)]; (3) Pressured to Succeed: 10-item scale from multiple sources (Frost, Marten, Lahart, & Rosenblate, 1990; Silk et al., 2003; Soenens, Vansteenkiste, & Luyten, 2010), e.g., “My mother shows she loves me less if I perform poor.” “When I get a good grade, my mother says my other grades should be as good” [α=.85 (Filipino American) and .87 (Korean American)]; (4) Feeling Embarrassed by Parents: 5 questions from KAF (Choi, 2007), assessing the degrees to which the child feels embarrassed about their parent’s poor English or culturally awkward behaviors (e.g., “There are times when I feel embarrassed about my parents’ poor English” “about my parents being awkward with other Americans” [α=.75 (Filipino American) and .81 (Korean American)].

Racial/ethnic socialization

Three racial/ethnic socialization correlates were: (1) Cultural Socialization: 3 questions (Choi, 2007), asking youth how much their parents emphasize “feeling proud of being Korean/Filipino” and “maintaining Korean/Filipino traditions and values” [α=.72 (Filipino American) and .74 (Korean American)]; (2) Promotion of Mistrust: 3-item scale was from Tran and Lee (2010), asking youth how their parents told them to avoid or keep distance from other racial/ethnic groups [α=.83 (Filipino American) and .86 (Korean American)]; (3) Preparation for Bias: 5-item scale also from Tran and Lee (2010) asks youth how often their parents to prepare them against racial/ethnic stereotypes, prejudice, and/or discrimination (α=.81 for both).

Race/ethnicity related

Two correlates were included that are related to race/ethnicity. (1) Perceived Racial Discrimination: 5-item scale developed for the ML-SAAF study based on several focus groups conducted prior to the survey, assessed how often youth felt discriminated by, e.g., whites, nonwhites, racial/ethnic minorities, peers and teachers (α=.74 for both); (5) Youth Perception of White Family: 11-item scale, developed for this study and measuring youth perception of White parents being more lenient than Asian parents (e.g., “Filipino/Korean parents are stricter than White parents”; “White kids can decide things for themselves more so than Filipino/Korean kids can”). [α=.91 (Filipino American) and .87 (Korean American)].

Youth outcomes

Youth outcomes included (1) Depressive Symptoms: 14 depressive symptom items from the Children’s Depressive Inventory (Angold, Costello, Messer, & Pickles, 1995) and the Seattle Personality Questionnaire for Children (Kusche, Greenberg, & Beilke, 1988) [α=.94 (Filipino American) and .93 (Korean American)]; (2) School Grade was computed based on grades in English, math, social studies, and science; (3) Antisocial Behaviors:19-item list adopted from DSM-IV conduct disorder criteria (Gelhorn et al., 2009) and Add Health, including bullying, physical fights, hurting others, and stealing. The variable was constructed to 0 for none and 1 for any antisocial behavior, (4) Substance Use was constructed based on five substance uses (past and current smoking, unsupervised drinking, illegal substances and unauthorized prescription drugs). Similar to antisocial behaviors, a binary variable was created to indicate 0 for no substance use and 1 for any substance use.

Analysis

Using Mplus v.7.4 (L. K. Muthén & Muthén, 2013), we conducted LPA to generate the most parsimonious number of subtypes describing youth acculturation strategy patterns. LPA is a person-oriented approach that categorizes individuals who share common characteristics that are derived from continuous observed variables (B. O. Muthén & Muthén, 2000). In the LPA framework, individuals are assigned probabilities according to their likelihood of membership in each group and then allocated into the group with the highest probability (Magnusson, 1998). Six variables were used to derive the subtypes: heritage and English language competency, heritage and American cultural participation, ethnic and American identity.

To identify the ideal number of subtypes in the samples, several fit statistics were examined, for example, the Akaike information criterion (AIC), Bayesian information criteria (BIC), sample-size adjusted BIC, entropy, Vuong-Lo-Mendell-Rubin (LMR-LRT), and the bootstrapped likelihood ratio test (L. K. Muthén & Muthén, 2013). Specifically, AIC, BIC, and the sample-size adjusted BIC serve as a measure of the goodness of fit. Smaller values indicate a better fit. The entropy concerns the accuracy of assignment of respondents to the subtypes, with a value closer to 1 suggesting a more accurate classification. The LMR-LRT and the bootstrapped likelihood ratio test are direct tests of significance between models (e.g., 1 vs. 2 subtypes; 2 vs. 3 subtypes). Once a model reaches nonsignificance (p > 0.05), the model prior to the nonsignificant model is preferred (L. K. Muthén & Muthén, 2013). In addition to the statistical fit indices, substantive theory guided the identification of the most appropriate number and pattern of subtypes. At each step of analysis, the total number and pattern of subtypes were evaluated against Berry’s (1997) model and others whenever necessary.

After finalizing the subtypes, we used the Auxiliary (e) command in Mplus to describe the subgroups by the correlates. The Auxiliary (e) command uses posterior probability-based multiple imputations to determine differences in a given variable across latent classes without using that outcome to define latent classes (L. K. Muthén & Muthén, 2013). Owing to its probabilistic nature of class classification and determination in latent profile analysis, the observed class assignment is likely to introduce error into the analysis. Thus, to maintain the inherent probabilistic uncertainties associated with latent profile analysis, significant pairwise t- tests on the equality of means across classes (df =1), using posterior probability-based multiple imputations, were used to compare differences across the subgroups. Lastly, missing data were handled using maximum likelihood in Mplus. The rates of missing data were very low. The average missing rate was less than 1% and none of the variables had more than 5% missing rates. To compare associations between acculturation strategies and correlates, we used independent t-test or χ2 tests to examine statistical mean or proportion differences across the latent subtype groups.

Results

Explicating Subtypes

The fit indices are summarized in Table 1, which provide fit statistics for 1 to 6 subtype solutions by each group. For Filipino American youth, the 4-subtype solution showed the highest entropy (.999), suggesting high classification accuracy. AIC, BIC, and the sample-size adjusted BIC suggested a solution of 5 subtypes. The bootstrapped likelihood ratio test suggested more than 6 subtypes (p <.0001). However, LMR-LRT indicated that the 3-subtype solution (p = 0.0004) is significantly better than any other models. For Korean youth, the 3-subtype solution showed the highest entropy (.965), except the 6-subtype that included a subtype with n=4. AIC, BIC, and the sample-size adjusted BIC suggested a solution of more than 6 subtypes. The bootstrapped likelihood ratio test also suggested more than 6 subtypes (p <.0001). LMR-LRT suggested that the 5-subtype solution (p = 0.0018) is significantly better than any other subtype solutions.

Table 1.

Fit Indices of Latent Profile Analysis

Filipino Americans AIC BIC Sample-size adjusted BIC Entropy Vuong-Lo-Mendell-Rubin Test Bootstrapped Likelihood Ratio Test Sample Size of Smallest Subtype
1 Subtype 4873.122 4920.372 4882.299 N/A N/A N/A N/A
2 Subtypes 4479.458 4554.271 4493.988 0.994 0.0460 0.0000 39
3 Subtypes 4287.058 4389.434 4306.942 0.816 0.0004 0.0000 39
4 Subtypes 3791.589 3921.528 3816.826 0.999 0.8624 0.0000 8
5 Subtypes 3619.683 3777.184 3650.273 0.880 0.1224 0.0000 8
6 Subtypes 4907.730 5092.794 4943.673 0.785 0.5018 0.0000 6

Korean Americans

1 Subtype 5762.442 5810.635 5772.557 N/A N/A N/A N/A
2 Subtypes 5433.325 5509.631 5449.341 0.906 0.1121 0.0000 98
3 Subtypes 5233.384 5337.804 5255.301 0.965 0.0001 0.0000 31
4 Subtypes 5172.014 5304.547 5199.832 0.790 0.0441 0.0000 31
5 Subtypes 5090.831 5251.478 5124.550 0.833 0.0018 0.0000 6
6 Subtypes 3818.752 4007.512 3858.372 1.000 0.8358 0.0000 4
*

Note: The bold indicates fit indices for the chosen subtype for each group.

In addition to these fit indices, the nature of each group (i.e., of 3 vs. 4 vs. 5 vs. 6 subtype solutions) was evaluated in regard to six indicators that were used to generate the subtypes and against the theoretical models. We also considered the number of samples in each group to see whether each subgroup had reasonable sample sizes for post hoc comparisons on various correlates. For example, from the four-subtype solution, sample size of smallest subtype became less than eight for Filipino American youth. Thus, based on various considerations, we ultimately chose the three-subtype solution for both groups, as it seemed to best fit with substantive theory as well as the empirical fit indices. The three subtypes are summarized in Table 2.

Table 2.

Characteristics of Latent Subtypes by Indicators

Filipino Americans Korean Americans

Class 1
High Assimilation w/Ethnic Identity
Class 2
Integrated Bicultural w/Strongest Ethnic Identity
Class 3
Modest Bicultural w/Strong Ethnic Identity
Significant Differences at p<0.05 Class 1
Separation
Class 2
Integrated Bicultural
Class 3
Modest Bicultural w/Strong Ethnic Identity
Significant Differences at p<0.05 Ethnic Group Diff p<0.05
Proportion (%)
Sample size (n)
100%
n=379
45.4%
n=172
44.3%
n=168
10.3%
n=39
100%
n=410
7.6%
n=31
74.4%
n=305
18.0%
n=74

Heritage Language 2.55 1.73 3.27 3.14 1<2, 1<3 3.35 4.10 3.24 3.52 1>2, 1>3, 2<3 FA<KA1
English 4.84 4.97 4.94 3.85 1>3, 2>3 4.59 3.02 4.92 3.93 1<2, 1<3, 2>3 FA>KA
Heritage Culture 2.68 2.13 3.17 3.01 1<2, 1<3 2.94 3.64 2.85 3.03 1>2, 1>3 FA<KA
Host Culture 4.27 4.46 4.16 3.89 1>2, 1>3, 2>3 3.9 2.93 4.05 3.72 1<2, 1<3, 2>3 FA>KA
Ethnic Identity 4.35 4.02 4.69 4.42 1<2, 1<3, 2>3 4.15 4.07 4.16 4.15 FA>KA
American Identity 3.86 4.15 3.71 3.23 1>2, 1>3, 2>3 3.53 2.46 3.76 3.10 1<2, 1<3, 2>3 FA>KA

Note:

1

FA means Filipino Americans, KA means Korean Americans

Characteristics of the Subtypes by Indicators

Based on their characteristics, we named the three Filipino American subtypes as (1) High Assimilation with Ethnic Identity (2) Integrated Bicultural with Strongest Ethnic Identity, and (3) Modest Bicultural with Strong Ethnic Identity. The high assimilation with ethnic identity was one of the two largest groups (45.4%, n = 172), reporting the highest host orientation and lowest heritage orientation compared to other two groups (summarized in Table 2 and Figure 1). Specifically, high assimilation with ethnic identity youth reported the highest English language competency, host cultural participation, and American identity but the lowest heritage language competency (1.73) and heritage cultural participation (2.13). Ethnic identity (4.02), although lower than among the other two groups, was still high in this group, comparable to their American identity (4.15). With this strong bicultural identity, this group did not squarely fit the traditional assimilation model, thus we added “ethnic identity” as a part of the name.

Figure 1.

Figure 1

Filipino American Youth Acculturation Strategy Profiles

The integrated bicultural with strongest ethnic identity (44.3%, n = 168) showed a high host cultural orientation that was fairly comparable to the high assimilation with ethnic identity group but a conspicuously strong heritage orientation that is higher than the high assimilation with ethnic identity but similar to the modest bicultural with strong ethnic identity (see below). Although host language and cultural participation were higher than those of heritage culture, ethnic identity among the integrated bicultural with strongest ethnic identity was noticeably higher (4.69) than American identity (3.71) and the highest among the three groups.

The third group, the modest bicultural with strong ethnic identity (10.3%, n = 39) reported modest bilingual competency and bicultural orientation with a strong ethnic identity. Specifically, this group reported lower host orientation in all three dimensions of language, cultural participation and identity, compared to the other two groups. Heritage orientation among this group was higher than in the high assimilation with ethnic identity group but lower than among the integrated bicultural with strongest ethnic identity. However, this group’s ethnic identity (4.42) was much stronger than its American identity (3.23). In all three groups, host orientation was stronger than heritage orientation with an exception of identity dimension. The prominence of strong ethnic identity among Filipino American youth led us to include ethnic identity in the name all three subtypes to highlight the vivid presence of identity enculturation even among highly assimilated youth. The mean of ethnic identity was in fact higher among Filipino American youth than Korean American youth (Table 2).

Among Korean American youth, we also identified three groups: (1) Separation (2) Integrated Bicultural and (3) Modest Bicultural with Strong Ethnic Identity, summarized in Table 2 and Figure 2. The separation group (7.6%, n = 31) was characterized by the lowest host culture orientation in all dimensions of language, cultural participation and identity, and reported the highest heritage culture orientation in language and cultural participation. Their ethnic identity (4.07), although not the highest among the groups, was quite strong, and showed the largest deviation from American identity (2.46), indicating a solid separation pattern.

Figure 2.

Figure 2

Korean American Youth Acculturation Strategy Profiles

The integrated bicultural group was the largest group in size (74.4%; n = 350), reporting the highest bilingual competence but also a stronger ethnic identity than American identity. Specifically, this group reported the highest English competency, host cultural participation, and American identity. Although heritage language competency (3.24) and heritage cultural participation (2.85) were lowest among the three groups, the means of these dimensions of heritage cultural orientation were around three out of a one to five scale. In addition, their ethnic identity was the strongest among the three groups. These characteristics when considered together seemed to fit integrated biculturalism.

Lastly, the modest bicultural with strong ethnic identity (18%; n = 74) was largely similar to the integrated bicultural in reporting stronger host cultural orientation than heritage cultural orientation, as well as a very strong Korean identity (4.15) that is much higher than American identity (3.10). While the pattern was similar (higher acculturation than enculturation) in the two groups, the overall means of acculturation among the modest bicultural with strong ethnic identity youth were lower than those of the integrated bicultural group.

Characteristics of the Subtypes by Correlates

Demographics

Summarized in Table 3, the modest bicultural with strong ethnic identity for Filipino American youth consisted of more boys (66.3%) than girls, the integrated bicultural with strongest ethnic identity consisted of more girls (67%) than boys, and the high assimilation with ethnic identity group was predominantly U.S.-born (88%) and had lived in the U.S. the longest. Parents of the high assimilation with ethnic identity had also lived in the U.S. the longest and were mostly U.S citizens (90%).

Table 3.

Comparisons of Correlates by Acculturation Strategies

Filipino Americans Korean Americans
Variable Names Full Sample Class 1
High Assimilation w/1 Ethnic Identity
Class 2
Integrated Bicultural w/ Strongest Ethnic Identity
Class 3
Modest Bicultural w/ Strong Ethnic Identity
Significant Differences at p<0.05 Full Sample Class 1
Separation
Class 2
Integrated Bicultural
Class 3
Modest Bicultural w/ Strong Ethnic Identity
Significant Differences at p<0.05 Ethnic Group Diff p<0.05
Proportion (%)
Sample Size (n)
100%
n=379
45.4%
n=172
44.3%
n=168
10.3%
n=39
100%
n=410
7.6%
n=31
74.4%
n=305
18.0%
n=74
Youth Demographics
Age 15.28 15.19 15.36 15.41 14.76 15.42 14.79 14.41 1>3 FA>KA3
% Boys 44% 49% 33% 66% 1>2, 1<3, 2<3 52% 71% 50% 55% 1>2 FA<KA
% US born 71% 88% 61% 41% 1>2, 1>3, 2>3 58% 20% 67% 40% 1<2, 1<3, 2>3 FA>KA
Years living in US2 13.29 14.57 12.69 10.09 1>2, 1>3, 2>3 11.78 5.93 13.03 9.22 1<2, 1<3, 2>3 FA>KA
Parent Demographics
Age 46.13 46.67 45.55 46.2 45.29 44.44 45.38 45.26 FA>KA
Years living in US 23.2 28.86 18.62 16.17 1>2, 1>3 16.03 7.41 17.90 11.84 1<2, 1<3, 2>3 FA>KA
% US citizen 80% 90% 74% 51% 1>3, 1>2, 2>3 49% 27% 57% 27% 1<2, 2>3 FA>KA
% Permanent resident 15% 9% 18% 34% 1<2, 1<3 34% 27% 32% 45% FA<KA
Family Process
Core cultural values
Respect for adults 4.09 3.96 4.23 4.13 1<2 3.85 3.52 3.88 3.86 1<2, 1<3 FA>KA
Harmony & sacrifice 3.81 3.70 3.95 3.74 1<2 3.81 3.54 3.84 3.76 1<2
Family obligation 3.65 3.53 3.75 3.79 1<2, 1<3 3.62 3.24 3.64 3.69 1<2, 1<3
Boundary of the family 6.32 6.18 6.64 5.58 1<2, 2>3 4.6 4.80 4.52 4.80 FA>KA
Indigenous family process
Traditional etiquettes 3.77 3.13 4.34 4.23 1<2, 1<3 4.31 4.23 4.33 4.29 FA<KA
Gendered expectation 2.85 2.53 3.11 3.16 1<2, 1<3 2.66 2.58 2.65 2.72 FA>KA
Academic parental control 3.02 2.92 3.09 3.13 2.91 2.61 2.96 2.83 1<2 FA>KA
Qin 4.13 4.05 4.24 4.00 1<2, 2>3 3.88 3.48 3.96 3.76 1<2, 2>3 FA>KA
Parental affection 4.24 4.19 4.34 4.06 2>3 3.94 3.47 4.01 3.86 1<2, 1<3 FA>KA
Conventional family process
Authoritarian style 3.36 3.19 3.50 3.47 1<2 2.95 2.59 2.99 2.93 1<2 FA>KA
Authoritative style 3.54 3.33 3.74 3.62 1<2 3.28 3.33 3.28 3.28 FA>KA
Parent-child bonding 3.97 3.93 4.04 3.84 4 3.90 4.03 3.96
Parent-child conflict 2.44 2.48 2.44 2.27 2.22 2.25 2.23 2.17 FA>KA
Parental acceptance 3.8 3.79 3.86 3.60 3.75 3.56 3.80 3.63
Parental rejection 1.61 1.58 1.61 1.70 1.58 1.77 1.55 1.63 1>2
Parent-child communication 2.97 2.95 2.99 2.96 2.94 2.82 2.95 2.94 1<2
Granting autonomy 3.55 3.55 3.57 3.47 3.67 3.52 3.72 3.52 2>3 FA<KA
Parental rules 3.77 3.56 3.89 4.23 1<3 2.81 1.87 2.94 2.68 1<2, 1<3 FA>KA
Parental restrictions 4.81 4.75 4.84 4.97 4.39 3.59 4.48 4.36 1<2 FA>KA
Immigrant family process
Emphasis on education 4.7 4.64 4.78 4.57 1<2, 2>3 4.55 4.09 4.62 4.49 1<2, 1<3 FA>KA
Cultural conflict 2.6 2.37 2.79 2.84 1<2, 1<3 2.37 2.47 2.35 2.43 FA>KA
Pressure to succeed 2.72 2.58 2.79 3.05 1<2, 1<3 2.45 2.19 2.47 2.45 1<2 FA>KA
Embarrassed by parents 1.78 1.62 1.91 1.93 1<2, 1<3 2.11 1.77 2.15 2.11 1<2, 1<3 FA<KA
Racial/ethnic socialization
Cultural socialization 3.64 3.11 4.12 4.02 1<2, 1<3 3.73 3.44 3.74 3.80 1<3
Promotion of mistrust 1.51 1.37 1.63 1.64 1<2 1.5 1.56 1.49 1.51
Preparation for bias 2.14 1.97 2.33 2.08 1<2 2.13 2.07 2.18 1.98
Race/Ethnicity Related
Racial discrimination 1.36 1.28 1.43 1.37 1<2 1.5 1.55 1.49 1.53 FA<KA
Perception of white family 3.18 2.92 3.42 3.30 1<2, 1<3 3.15 3.14 3.16 3.15
Youth Outcomes
Depressive symptoms 1.81 1.69 1.90 1.98 1<2 1.81 2.02 1.79 1.83
School grade 3.48 3.49 3.54 3.18 1>3, 2>3 3.54 3.42 3.57 3.48
Antisocial behaviors 44% 0.45 0.46 0.38 36% 0.53 0.34 0.38 FA>KA
Substance use 25% 0.26 0.25 0.18 20% 0.36 0.18 0.20 1>2

Note:

1

“W/” is an abbreviation of “with.”

2

The number of years living in the U.S. equals to one’s age among U.S.-born youth.

3

FA means Filipino Americans, KA means Korean Americans

For Korean American youth, the separation group primarily consisted of older youth (15.42 years old on average) and more boys (71.4% male), and the integrated bicultural group was predominantly U.S.-born (66.8%) and had lived in the U.S. the longest. Parents of integrated bicultural youth had also lived in the U.S. the longest. Parents of the separation group had more temporary visa holders (41.9%) than the other groups.

Family Process

Traditional core values

Among Filipino American youth, the high assimilation with ethnic identity group reported significantly lower rates in all variables of traditional core cultural values than the integrated bicultural with strongest ethnic identity group of youth (e.g., lower respect for adults, harmony and sacrifice, family obligation, and least expansive boundary of the family). Their family obligation was lower than the modest bicultural with strong ethnic identity. The integrated bicultural with strongest ethnic identity group reported the most expansive definition of family. Among Korean American youth, contrary to the expectation, the separation group reported lower rates in traditional cultural values; respect for adults, harmony and sacrifice, and family obligation were lower than the integrated bicultural group, and respect for adults and family obligation were lower than the modest bicultural with strong ethnic identity group. The boundary of the family did not differ among the Korean American subtypes.

Indigenous family process

The high assimilation with ethnic identity Filipino American youth group reported the lowest rates of traditional manners and etiquettes and gendered expectations among the three groups, and the integrated bicultural with strongest ethnic identity group reported the highest rates of qin among the three groups and stronger parental affection than the modest bicultural with strong ethnic identity group. These three Filipino American youth groups differed significantly only on the measure of academically oriented parental control. For Korean American youth, the integrated bicultural group reported the highest rate of qin, and higher academically oriented parental control than the separation group. The latter group actually reported lowest parental affection.

Conventional family process

The three Filipino American youth subtypes did not differ significantly on conventional measures of parent-child bonding and conflict, parental acceptance and rejection, parent-child communication, granting autonomy, and parental restrictions. However, the integrated bicultural with strongest ethnic identity group reported higher authoritarian and authoritative parenting styles than the high assimilation with ethnic identity. It is also noted that the modest bicultural with strong ethnic identity group reported higher numbers of parental rules than the high assimilation with ethnic identity group. The three Korean American youth groups were not statistically different in authoritative parenting style, parent-child bonding and conflict, and parental acceptance. However, the separation group reported the lowest number of parental rules and lower authoritarian style parent-child communication and parental restrictions, but higher parental rejection than the integrated bicultural group. The integrated bicultural group also had a higher rate of granting autonomy than the modest bicultural with strong ethnic identity group.

Immigrant family process

For Filipino American youth, the high assimilation with ethnic identity group showed marked differences in the immigrant family process variables. Specifically, they reported the lowest rates of cultural conflict with parents, pressure to succeed, and feeling embarrassed by parents among the three groups, while the integrated bicultural with strongest ethnic identity group scored the highest on emphasis on education. Among Korean American youth, the separation group reported the lowest rates of emphasis on education and feeling embarrassed by parents, and lower pressure to succeed than the integrated bicultural group. Cultural conflict was not significantly different across the Korean American subtypes.

Racial/ethnic socialization

The high assimilation with ethnic identity Filipino American youth reported the lowest level of cultural socialization. They also reported a lower level of promotion of mistrust and preparation for bias than the integrated bicultural with strongest ethnic identity. Conversely, the difference among the Korean American subtypes was found only in one variable, namely, the separation group reported a lower cultural socialization than the modest bicultural with strong ethnic identity

Race/Ethnicity Related

The high assimilation with ethnic identity Filipino American youth reported the lowest level of idealized perception of the White family. They also reported a lower level of perceived racial discrimination than the integrated bicultural with strongest ethnic identity. The Korean American subtypes did not differ in the levels of racial discrimination and idealized perception of the White family.

Youth Outcomes

A few significant differences in youth outcomes emerged among Filipino American youth but much less significantly among Korean American youth. For example, among Filipino American youth, the integrated bicultural with strongest ethnic identity reported higher rates of depressive symptoms than the high assimilation with ethnic identity group. However, the integrated bicultural with strongest ethnic identity also scored higher school grades than the modest bicultural with strong ethnic identity group. In fact, the modest bicultural with strong ethnic identity group reported the lowest school grades among the three Filipino American youth groups. Antisocial behaviors and substance use were not significantly different across the Filipino American subtypes. The three Korean American subtypes did not differ significantly in terms of depressive symptoms, academic performance, and antisocial behaviors and conduct disorders. The separation group did report higher substance use than the integrated bicultural group.

Alternative Models

Although we chose the three-subtype solution for this paper, the four-subtype solution could be considered as an alternative model for both ethnic groups. Among Filipino American youth one group dominated in the four-subtype solution. Eighty-two percent of Filipino youth (n=311) formed one group characterized by high host language and cultural participation but high ethnic identity. The remaining three groups varied mainly in the levels of heritage language competence and culture but were rather small, with one group just 2.1% (n=8) of the sample. Such small subgroups inhibit significant testing across subtypes and meaningful comparisons. For Korean Americans, the four-subtype solution maintained separation and modest bicultural with strong ethnic identity groups, but divided integrated bicultural youth into two group. The two groups differed mainly in heritage cultural retention. One group (38.3%, n=157) showed lower heritage language competence and lower heritage culture participation than the other group (36.1%, n=148), although the differences were only modest. When correlates were compared across the subtypes, these two groups often did not differ. When differences emerged, they were mainly with separation and modest bicultural with strong ethnic identity groups.

Discussion

Berry’s four-fold typology has been highly influential in challenging the notion that immigrants, especially their children, shed their heritage beliefs, values, and practices as they integrate into their host country. Consistent with Berry’s work, as well as our hypotheses, we find that all participants simultaneously endorsed, to varying degrees, aspects of both their heritage and their host culture. Indeed, Filipino American and Korean American youth exhibited striking commonality in the extent to which they embraced their host culture while concurrently affirming their heritage culture. This was true even among highly assimilated participants with low heritage language competency and heritage cultural participation. This study revitalizes the meaning of “growing up American” for Asian American adolescents.

Acculturation Strategy Subtypes

While Filipino American youth reported more assimilative behaviors than their Korean American counterparts, Filipino American youth with even the most acculturated behaviors endorsed significant ethnic identity. This finding stands in contrast to literature that portrays Filipino American youth as having a weak to modest ethnic identity (see, e.g., Ocampo, 2014). Colonial mentality, even if palpable among the first immigrant generation parents, has not been passed onto Filipino American youth. Our findings underscore a conceptualization of assimilation strategy that includes continued subscription of an ethnic identity regardless of linguistic and behavioral acculturation. Identity assimilation may actually be constrained as racial/ethnic minority youth experience systematic discrimination that contributes to a keen awareness of their minority status (Lien et al., 2003).

Korean American subtypes include a separation group, which was not found among Filipino American youth. Still, the majority of Korean American youth, like Filipino American youth, were behaviorally well acculturated with a strong sense of ethnic identity. Consequently, corroborating research among other immigrant populations (see, e.g., Schwartz & Zamboanga, 2008), we find that neither Filipino American nor Korean American youth fit strictly into Berry’s four categories. Rather, Filipino American youth fit into two variants of biculturalism (Integrated Bicultural with Strongest Ethnic Identity and Modest Bicultural with Strong Ethnic Identity) and one variant of assimilation (High Assimilation with Ethnic Identity), with Berry’s separation and marginalization categories not represented among Filipino American youth. Korean American youth fit into Berry’s separation and integrated/bicultural groups as well as an additional variant of biculturalism which we labeled Modest Bicultural with Strong Ethnic Identity. We did not find any Korean American youth that fit into either an assimilation or marginalization group. This pattern parallels prior research (Choi, 2016). The absence of a marginalization group among both Filipino American and Korean American participants echoes questions regarding the validity of Berry’s marginalization strategy elsewhere (Schwartz & Zamboanga, 2008; del Pilar & Udasco, 2004). Alternatively, it is possible that marginalized youth might be less likely to participate in surveys, and that the population of marginalized youth, even if extant in the sample, might have been too small to emerge as a distinct group.

Correlates of Acculturation Strategy Subtypes

Demographics

In the present study, the largest subtype among Filipino American youth was the high assimilation with ethnic identity group, at 45.38%, closely followed by the integrated bicultural with strongest ethnic identity group, at 44.32%. The latter group had the highest percentage of girls among the three Filipino American subtypes, while the modest bicultural with strong ethnic identity had the largest percentage of boys. The gender differences among the subtypes are noteworthy, as the integrated bicultural with strongest ethnic identity also reported higher rates of depressive symptoms than did the high assimilation with ethnic identity subtype. Filipino American girls have been found to have some of the highest rates of depressive symptoms and suicidal ideations among Asian Americans, with some researchers theorizing a causal link with gendered cultural expectations in Filipino American families (Espiritu, 2003). In addition, we expected more acculturation leaning among girls, but Filipino American girls were more bicultural than any of the other subtypes. Future studies should examine how gender and ethnicity together moderate the development of acculturation strategies and related outcomes.

Further, as expected, the high assimilation with ethnic identity group, with the largest U.S.-born population, had lived the longest number of years in the U.S., while the modest bicultural with strong ethnic identity had the shortest duration in the U.S., along with the smallest percentage of youth born in the U.S. Our results seem to affirm the general practice of using nativity as a proxy for acculturation, but the fact that even the most behaviorally acculturated Filipino American youth reported a strong sense of ethnic identity bears close analysis. As this present study demonstrates, acculturation is indeed a multidimensional process among Asian Americans, and wholesale assimilation into the host culture may not be possible or desirable, regardless of years of living in the U.S. or nativity.

Family Process

Traditional core cultural values

Among Korean American youth, bicultural youth, both integrated and modest reported higher awareness of culturally distinct elements than separation Korean American youth. Contrary to expectations, separation youth showed lower endorsement of traditional cultural values such as respect for adults, harmony and sacrifice, and family obligation. The only aspect in which separation youth accorded with expectations was that they were less likely to report feeling embarrassed by their parents. In other words, integrated bicultural Korean American youth were surprisingly more enculturated in heritage values than were separated Korean American youth. We posit several explanations for why this may be the case. Demographics of the separation group show that these youth have lived in the U.S. less than 6 years on average and their parents less than 8 years. It is possible that their endorsement patterns are a reflection of Korean values predominant at the time of their more recent immigration while families of integrated bicultural youth may have preserved the cultural values of Korean society present at the time of their earlier immigration. The rapid Westernization of Korea has been well documented (see, e.g., Duk, 2013), and the Korea of a recent immigrant generation may have integrated more Western values and norms than the Korea of an earlier immigrant generation. This theory is supported by scholarship on paradoxical acculturation which avers that immigrant communities adhere to their heritage tradition more so than their nonimmigrant counterparts in the country of origin (Rosenthal & Feldman, 1992). It is also possible that parents of the separation group do not feel a pressing need to enculturate their children, as their children have already been enculturated through their recent earlier years in Korea. The parents of the integrated bicultural groups, on the other hand, may feel they must counter the prevalent pressures of the host culture by underscoring traditional values to their children.

Indigenous & immigrant family process

We found that integrated strategies in both Filipino American and Korean American groups were more likely than other subtypes to report higher perception of culturally distinct or immigrant elements in the family. Specifically, the integrated bicultural Filipino American youth group reported higher parental emphasis on traditional manners and etiquettes, gendered expectation, qin, emphasis on education, intergenerational cultural conflict, pressure to succeed, and feeling embarrassed by parents than assimilated Filipino American youth. Although modestly bicultural youth reported overall higher rates of these constructs than highly assimilated youth, the means among the integrated bicultural youth were sometimes higher than those of modestly bicultural youth. The directionality cannot be determined but it is most plausible that indigenous parenting and youth’s enculturation interact with one another: more traditional parenting enhances youth enculturation, which enables youth to recognize parental cultural behaviors. As long as youth maintain bicultural competency, youth may be able to discern and recognize indigenous parenting even as they are acculturated to the host culture.

This study found that highly assimilated Filipino American youth reported the lowest rate of intergenerational conflict. The acculturation gap-distress model postulates that when youth assimilate at a faster rate than their parents, intergenerational conflict arises (Portes and Rumbaut, 2001). Here, the highly assimilated Filipino American youth participants had parents whose length of residence in U.S. was the longest among all six groups at nearly 30 years. It may be that there simply is no acculturation gap as these youth have parents who are mostly fluent in English and perhaps more consonant with their children’s rate of assimilation than other Asian American subgroups.

We note that Korean American youth groups did not differ in the extent to which their parents emphasized traditional manners and etiquettes (all above 4.20 out of 5.0 scales). This finding bolsters evidence that traditional Korean manners and etiquettes are deeply embedded as behavioral codes and less likely to be diminished by other acculturative factors. The non-significance in gendered expectations (all below 2.72) may suggest that Korean family process, heavily gendered in the past, is now less gendered in both Korean immigrant communities and contemporary South Korea. Alternatively, it is possible that our measures did not adequately capture the ways in which contemporary Korean American family process is changing but remains gendered. For example, our survey inquired as to parents’ views on high school girls dating. There are signs that both male and female high school dating is now widely acceptable as a cultural norm among Koreans (Kim, 2016), which would result in participant responses that can belie the extent to which their values and behaviors are gendered.

Conventional family process

In conventional family process, Filipino American and Korean American youth groups differed in limited but consequential ways. For example, integrated bicultural Korean American youth perceive their parents as more authoritarian than do separated youth. Integrated bicultural Korean American youth are more likely than separated youth to share the view of their host culture, which has often portrayed Asian parenting as harsh and authoritarian. However, integrated bicultural Filipino American youth were more likely to perceive their parents as authoritative or authoritarian than were highly assimilated Filipino American youth, running counter to our expectations. Further, Korean American youth overall reported lower rates of authoritarian parenting style than authoritative parenting, while Filipino American youth reported relatively high on both styles. Parents’ self-reports of parenting styles corroborate youth reports in the data. It may be that Filipino American parents actively use both styles more than Korean American parents, and this pattern may be more pronounced among integrated bicultural youths’ parents. The authoritarian and authoritative parenting styles are often regarded as mutually exclusive concepts, although some studies have shown a coexistence of these styles among Asian American families (Chao, 1995; Choi, Kim, Pekelnicky, & Kim, 2013). The possibility that these two competing styles may coexist in Asian American families, probably in the form of bicultural parenting, warrants further future analysis.

Racial/ethnic socialization and race/ethnic related

Not surprisingly, the high assimilation with ethnic identity Filipino American youth group had lower rates of race/ethnicity related constructs than other groups, pointing to a low level of racial/ethnic socialization in the family. Specified dimensions of parental racial/ethnic socialization have been found to serve as a protective factor against the negative effects of discrimination (Neblett Jr., Rivas-Drake, & Umaña-Taylor, 2012); with it, minority youth may find remedial strength in standing in solidarity with the racial/ethnic group ascribed on them by larger society. These youth also reported lower rates of racial discrimination and idealized perception of White family. The latter, in particular, was contrary to our expectations. From the data, it is hard to discern whether Filipino American youth report less racial discrimination and less idealization of the White family because of decreased racial/ethnic socialization or because of their community’s longer history of engagement with the host culture. What is clear is that Filipino Americans continue to experience racial discrimination (Ocampo, 2014) and strong ethnic identity has been linked to well-being among Asian American adolescents and emerging adults, especially in its role in mitigating the negativity of racial discrimination (Brown & Ling, 2012; Liu & Lau, 2013). Thus, minority youth may find it beneficial to strategically maintain a strong ethnic identity even as other ties to their traditional culture are modified.

Finally, the three subtypes among Korean American youth did not differ in race/ethnicity related constructs, except that separation youth reported lower cultural socialization. Similar to earlier discussion, parents of separation youth may feel less need to enculturate their children. Regardless of the acculturation strategy of Korean American youth, they all reported similar levels of parental promotion of mistrust, parental preparation for bias, perceived racial discrimination, and youth perception of White families. There is a substantial research showing that Korean Americans have the highest level of intergroup conflict among all Asian American subgroups (e.g., Min, 2006). Hence, the nonsignificant differences across variant acculturation strategies may indicate an intentional, shared attention to race/ethnicity related matters among Korean American families.

Youth Outcomes

Among Korean Americans, the integrated bicultural group reported lower rates of substance use than did the separation group. Since the separation group is more likely to have been Korean-born, it is possible that members of this group have had longer exposure to adolescent alcohol and tobacco usage, which historically has been more widely accepted in Korea than in America. So too, Korean society is more lenient towards boys who use substances than towards girls. Interestingly, the separation group in general also had lower rates of heritage cultural elements in the family but more parental rejection and lower parent-child communication than the integrated bicultural Korean American youth. The separation group was also least likely to live with their fathers, even if their parents were not divorced, in a popular phenomenon where Korean fathers remain in Korea while the mother brings the children to America for the sake of education (Chang, 2018). Although not statistically significant, separation youth showed more problems than other groups; these differences might have been significant with a bigger sample sizes. The outlying nature of the separation group calls for a closer look at factors beyond language, cultural participation, and identity in youth formation of an acculturation strategy.

Among Filipino Americans, integrated bicultural Filipino American youth group reported higher mental health problems while having better grades than other subtypes. This mixed pattern of negative internal outcomes paired with positive external outcomes is a paradoxical and particularly Asian American pattern of youth development (Choi, 2008). Among Filipino Americans, girls comprised the majority of the integrated bicultural group and this pattern may underscore the vulnerability of Filipino American young women who obscure psychological difficulties behind high external functioning. This finding may also imply that, although often hailed as most adaptive and ideal, bicultural acculturation strategies are not uniformly beneficial across all domains of development. In this case, psychological outcomes did not fare as well as behavioral outcomes in the integrated bicultural group. Future research should further refine the variegated associations of different acculturation strategies and identify the configuration of the most beneficial bicultural strategy that maximizes youth potential without incurring a psychological toll.

Limitations

This study utilized families as a sample unit, and required parental consent as well as in-person interviews. The emphasis on family participation, as well as the potential exposure of an in-person interview, may have discouraged Korean American and Filipino American parent-youth dyads undergoing relational or development difficulties from participating. Additionally, socially and culturally marginalized families were less likely to be identified and recruited, which may explain the absence of marginalization subtype in the results.

Study data were derived mainly from the Midwest region, and the findings, although important to understanding youth acculturation strategies, should not be generalized to other regions. This regional focus was an intentional effort to broaden scholarship on Asian American families, which tends to concentrate in either coastal area of the U.S.

The age range of the study samples was from 12 to 17, which includes both middle and high school youth. This span encompasses a wide range of developmental issues and was purposefully chosen to illuminate any differences between subjects in middle and late adolescence. However, when examined separately by middle and high school samples, the subtype patterns did not significantly different from the current findings presented here. In addition, more numerous subtype solutions produced groups that were too small for meaningful group comparisons.

This study also does not claim any causality of relationships. We tested correlative relations and mean/proportion difference tests by subtypes which, while informative, do not provide conclusions about directionality. Future research using longitudinal data will provide more information.

Finally, the correlates reported in this study are youth-reported. Because parent and youth are often found to perceive and report the same phenomenon very differently, ML-SAAF collected data on both youth- and parent-reported variables. When examined, however, parent-reported measures of the same variables mostly did not yield significant differences, and were thus not included in the present study.

Conclusion

This study provides evidence that Asian American adolescents resist simplistic orientations towards either host or heritage culture, and highlights the need for more refined examination and analysis of Asian American acculturation. Filipino American and Korean American adolescents exhibit expected commonalities, yet resist pan-ethnic generalizations. Most significantly, all three subtypes across both subgroups endorsed ethnic identity, regardless of level of assimilation in other domains of acculturation. This finding underscores that Asian American youth are forging an identity that encompasses both American culture and their specific heritage. This identity looks different across groups and subtypes. For example, Filipino American adolescents show signs of greater assimilation, particularly in the dimensions of language and heritage culture activities. At the same time, and surprisingly, they also endorsed stronger ethnic identity than did Korean American adolescents. Conversely, only Korean American adolescents fit into a separation subtype. This unique group, comprised of largely recent immigrants, may also reflect the culture-specific phenomenon of transatlantic-split families in Korea.

In our study, Filipino American adolescents with low heritage language competency and heritage cultural participation actually had the lowest level of intergenerational conflict with their parents. Filipino bicultural youth, both integrated and modest, report more conflict, feeling greater pressure to succeed, and more embarrassment by their parents. Although not as extensive, Korean bicultural youth too reported more pressure to succeed and embarrassment by their parents. Our study findings, together with earlier findings (Choi, et al., 2016), bolster research that acculturation gaps do not axiomatically lead to intergenerational conflict. Rather, acculturation gaps affect families differently, depending on differential rates among domains of acculturation among both parent and child, as well as contextual and developmental characteristics of the family (Lim et al., 2009; Telzer, 2010). The data gathered here provide a foundation for future research along these lines. Moreover, when additional longitudinal data of the ML-SAAF study become available, future research can examine whether and how the acculturation strategies identified in this study change over time and relate to youth development. An acculturation strategy subtype that was beneficial during adolescence may prove to be detrimental over time. For example, high rates of enculturation may serve youth well in facilitating better parent-child communications and relationships during adolescence but may be a hindrance as youth enter professional working life where host-culture competencies become more important. Conversely, continued integrated bicultural competence may help young adults maintain close parent-child relations and function well in a globalized society.

Finally, though Berry (1997) and subsequent researchers (e.g. Nguyen & Benet-Martinez, 2013) concluded that biculturalism was the most adaptive acculturation strategy, we find that the benefits of biculturalism are more ambiguous. There seem to be harmful cultural traits, such as gendered socialization, that are not beneficial in the lives of Asian American adolescents. There are also beneficial cultural traits, such as respect for elders, that are associated with positive youth outcomes. Gender as a moderator seems to be a particularly important avenue for future research among Asian American adolescents. Additional multivariate analyses are needed to provide specific clinical guidelines, but the study findings highlight the importance of tailored approaches in promoting an ideal acculturation strategy that reduces risk and increases resilience among Asian American subgroups. The ML-SAAF data presented here will be useful to understanding how Asian American youth and their families can optimally engage with their dual identities over time.

References

  1. Angold AC, Costello EJ, Messer SC, Pickles A. Development of a short questionnaire for use in epidemiological studies of depression in children and adolescents. International Journal of Methods in Psychiatric Research. 1995;5(4):237–249. [Google Scholar]
  2. Atzaba-Poria N, Pike A. Are ethnic minority adolescents at risk for problem behavior? Acculturation and intergenerational acculturation discrepancies in early adolescence. British Journal of Developmental Psychology. 2007;25:527–541. [Google Scholar]
  3. Berry JW. Immigration, acculturation, and adaptation. Applied Psychology: International Review. 1997;46(1):5–34. [Google Scholar]
  4. Berry JW. Acculturation as varieties of adaptation. In: Padilla AM, editor. Acculturation: Theory, models and some new findings. Boulder, CO: Westview; 1980. pp. 9–25. [Google Scholar]
  5. Berry JW. Acculturation: Living successfully in two cultures. International Journal of Intercultural Relations. 2005;29:697–712. [Google Scholar]
  6. Birman D, Trickett EJ. Psychology. University of Illinois; Chicago: 2002. The “language, identity, and behavior” (LIB) acculturation measure. [Google Scholar]
  7. Brown CM, Ling W. Ethnic-racial socialization has an indirect effect on self-esteem for Asian American emerging adults. Psychology. 2012;3(1):78–81. [Google Scholar]
  8. Bukobza G. Relations between rebelliousness, risk-taking behavior, and identity status during emerging adulthood. Identity: An International Journal of Theory and Research. 2009;9(2):159–177. [Google Scholar]
  9. Buri JR. Parental Authority Questionnaire. Journal of Personality Assessment. 1991;57(1):110–119. doi: 10.1207/s15327752jpa5701_13. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Castillo LG, Zahn MP, Cano MA. Predictors of familial acculturative stress in Asian American college students. Journal of College Counseling. 2012;15(1):52–64. [Google Scholar]
  11. Chang ES. Kirogi Women’s Psychological Well-Being: The relative contributions of marital quality, mother–child relationship quality, and youth’s educational Aadjustment. Journal of Family Issues. 2018;39(1):209–229. [Google Scholar]
  12. Chao RK. Beyond parental control and authoritarian parenting style: understanding Chinese parenting through the cultural notion of training. Child Development. 1994;65:1111–1119. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.1994.tb00806.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Chao RK. Chinese and European American cultural models of the self reflected in mother’s childrearing beliefs. Ethos. 1995;23(3):328–354. [Google Scholar]
  14. Chao RK, Wu C. Psychology. University of California at Riverside; Riverside: 2001. Guan parenting behaviors. [Google Scholar]
  15. Cheung BY, Chudek M, Heine SJ. Evidence for a sensitive period for acculturation: Younger immigrants report acculturating at a faster rate. Psychological Science. 2010;22(2):147–152. doi: 10.1177/0956797610394661. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Chia AL, Costigan CL. A person-centered approach to identifying acculturation groups among Chinese Canadians. International Journal of Psychology. 2006;41(5):397–412. [Google Scholar]
  17. Choi Y. Korean American Families (KAF) Project Measures School of Social Service Administration. University of Chicago; 2007. [Google Scholar]
  18. Choi Y. Diversity within: Subgroup differences of youth behaviors among Asian Pacific Islander American adolescents. Journal of Community Psychology. 2008;36(3):352–370. doi: 10.1002/jcop.20196. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Choi Y, He M, Harachi TW. Intergenerational cultural dissonance, family conflict, parent-child bonding, and youth antisocial behaviors among Vietnamese and Cambodian immigrant families. Journal of Youth and Adolescence. 2008;37:85–96. doi: 10.1007/s10964-007-9217-z. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Choi Y, Kim TY, Noh S, Lee JP, Takeuchi D. Culture and Family Process: Measures of Familism for Filipino and Korean American Parents. Family Process. 2017 doi: 10.1111/famp.12322. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Choi Y, Kim TY, Pekelnicky DD, Kim K, Kim YS. Impact of youth cultural orientations on perception of family process and development among Korean Americans. Cultural Diversity and Ethnic Minority Psychology. 2017;23(2):244–257. doi: 10.1037/cdp0000093. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Choi Y, Park M, Lee J, Kim TY, Tan KPH. Culture and family process: Examination of culture specific family process via development of new parenting measures among Filipino and Korean American families with adolescents. In: Choi Y, Hahm HC, editors. Asian American Parenting: Family Process and Intervention ( New York, NY: Springer; 2017. pp. 37–68. [Google Scholar]
  23. Choi Y, Kim YS. Acculturation and enculturation: Core vs. peripheral changes in the family socialization among Korean Americans. Korean Journal of Studies of Koreans Abroad. 2010;21:135–190. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Choi Y, Kim YS, Lee JP, Kim H, Kim TY, Kim SY. Bilinear, multidimensional cultural orientations and indigenous family process among Korean immigrant mothers and fathers. Asian American Journal of Psychology. 2018 doi: 10.1037/aap0000097. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Choi Y, Kim YS, Pekelnicky DD, Kim H. Preservation and Modification of Culture in Family Socialization: Development of Parenting Measures for Korean Immigrant Families. Asian American Journal of Psychology. 2013;4(2):143–154. doi: 10.1037/a0028772. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Choi Y, Tan KPH, Yasui M, Hahm HC. Advancing Understanding of Acculturation for Adolescents of Immigrants: Person-Oriented Analysis of Acculturation Strategy among Korean American Youth. Journal of Youth and Adolescence. 2016;45(7):1380–1395. doi: 10.1007/s10964-016-0496-0. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Chung I, Shibusawa T. Contemporary clinical practice with Asian immigrants: A relational framework with culturally responsive approaches. Routledge; 2013. [Google Scholar]
  28. Chung RHG, Kim BSK, Abreu JM. Asian American multidimensional acculturation scale: Development, factor analysis, reliability and validity. Cultural Diversity and Ethnic Minority Psychology. 2004;10(1):66–80. doi: 10.1037/1099-9809.10.1.66. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Costigan CL, Dokis DP. Similarities and differences in acculturation among mothers, fathers, and children in immigrant Chinese families. Journal of Cross Cultural Psychology. 2006;37:723–741. [Google Scholar]
  30. David EJR, Okazaki S. The Colonial Mentality Scale (CMS) for Filipino Americans: Scale construction and psychological implications. Journal of Counseling Psychology. 2006;53(2):241–252. [Google Scholar]
  31. de Guzman J. Family resilience and Filipino immigrant families: Navigating the adolescence life-stage. University of Guelph; Guelph, Ontario, CA: 2011. (Master’s) [Google Scholar]
  32. del Pilar JA, Udasco JO. Deculturation: Its lack of validity. Cultural Diversity and Ethnic Minority Psychology. 2004;10:169–176. doi: 10.1037/1099-9809.10.2.169. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Diep CS, Baranowski T, Kimbro RT. Acculturation and weight change in Asian American children: Evidence from the ECLS-K:2011. Preventive Medicine. 2017;99:286–292. doi: 10.1016/j.ypmed.2017.03.019. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Dinh KT, Nguyen HH. The effects of acculturative variables on Asian American parent-child relationships. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships. 2006;23(3):407–426. [Google Scholar]
  35. Duk YJ. Globalization and recent changes to daily life in the Republic of Korea. In: Lewis JB, Sesey A, editors. Korea and Globalization: Politics, Economics, and Culture. New Yrok: Routledge; 2013. [Google Scholar]
  36. Duldulao AA, Takeuchi DT, Hong S. Correlates of suicidal behaviors among Asian Americans. Archives of Suicide Research. 2009;13:277–290. doi: 10.1080/13811110903044567. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Eddy JM, Reid JB, Fetrow RA. An Elementary School-Based Prevention Program Targeting Modifiable Antecedents of Youth Delinquency and Violence: Linking the Interests of Families and Teachers (LIFT) Journal of Emotional and Behavioral Disorders. 2000;8(3):165–176. [Google Scholar]
  38. Erikson EH. Identity: Youth and crisis. New York: W.W Norton; 1968. [Google Scholar]
  39. Espiritu YL. Home bound: Filipino American lives across cultures, communities, and countries. Berkeley: University of California Press; 2003. [Google Scholar]
  40. Ferrera M. Doctoral Dissertation. University of Chicago; Chicago: 2011. The intersection of colonial mentality, family socialization, and ethnic identity formation among second generation Filipino American youth. [Google Scholar]
  41. Fox RS, Merz EL, Solorzano MT, Roesch SC. Further examining Berry’s model: The applicability of latent profile analysis to acculturation. Measurement and Evaluation in Counseling and Development. 2013;46(4):270–288. doi: 10.1177/0748175613497036. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Frost RO, Marten P, Lahart C, Rosenblate R. The dimensions of perfectionism. Cognitive therapy and research. 1990;14(5):449–468. [Google Scholar]
  43. Fuligni AJ. Family obligation, college enrollment, and emerging adulthood in Asian and Latin American families. Child Development Perspectives. 2007;1(2):90–100. [Google Scholar]
  44. Fuligni AJ, Witkow MR, Garcia Coll C. Ethnic identity and the academic adjustment of adolescents from Mexican, Chinese, and European backgrounds. Developmental Psychology. 2005;41:799–811. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.41.5.799. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Fuligni AJ, Zhang W. Attitudes toward family obligation among adolescents in contemporary urban and rural China. Child Development. 2004;74(1):180–192. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2004.00662.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Garcia Coll C, Lamberty G, Jenkins R, McAdoo HP, Crnic K, Wasik BH, Vázquez García H. An integrative model for the study of developmental competencies in minority children. Child Development. 1996;(5):1891. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Garcia Coll C, Pachter LM. Ethnic and Minority Parenting. In: Bornstein MH, editor. Handbook of Parenting. Vol. 4. Mahwah, New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates; 2002. pp. 1–20. [Google Scholar]
  48. Gelhorn HL, Hartman C, Sakai J, Mikulich-Gilbertson S, Stallings M, Young S, Crowley T. An item response theory analysis of DSM-IV conduct disorder. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry. 2009;48(1):42–50. doi: 10.1097/CHI.0b013e31818b1c4e. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Gordon M. Assimilation in American life: the role of race, religion and national origins. New York: Oxford University Press; 1964. [Google Scholar]
  50. Grolnick WS, Ryan RM, Deci EL. Inner Resources for School Achievement: Motivational Mediators of Children’s Perceptions. Journal of Educational Psychology. 1991;83(4):508–517. [Google Scholar]
  51. Hahm HC, Lahiff M, Guterman NB. Asian American adolescents’ acculturation, binge drinking, and alcohol- and tobacco-using peers. Journal of Community Psychology. 2004;32(3):295–308. [Google Scholar]
  52. Hughes D, Rivas D, Foust M, Hagelskamp C, Gersick S, Way N. How to catch a moonbeam: A mixed-methods approach to understanding ethnic socialization processes in ethnically diverse families. In: Quintana SM, McKown C, editors. Handbook of race, racism, and the developing child. Hoboken, NJ: Wiley; 2008. pp. 226–277. [Google Scholar]
  53. Huynh VW, Fuligni AJ. Discrimination Hurts: The Academic, Psychological, and Physical Well-Being of Adolescents. Journal of Research on Adolescence. 2010;20(4):916–941. [Google Scholar]
  54. Juang LP, Yoo HC, Atkins A. A critical race perspective on an empirical review of Asian American parental racial-ethnic socialization. In: Choi Y, Hahm HC, editors. Asian American parenting: Family process and intervention. New York: Springer; 2017. [Google Scholar]
  55. Kagitçibasi Ç. Family, self, and human development across cultures: Theory and applications. second. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum; 2007. [Google Scholar]
  56. Kim BSK, Abreu JM. Acculturation measurement: Theory, current instruments, and future directions. In: Ponterotto JG, Casas JM, Suzuki LA, Alexander CM, editors. Handbook of multicultural counseling. 2nd. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage; 2001. pp. 394–424. [Google Scholar]
  57. Kim BSK, Omizo MM. Behavioral Enculturation and Acculturation, Psychological Functioning, and Help-Seeking Attitudes Among Asian American Adolescents. Asian American Journal of Psychology. 2010;1(3):175–185. doi: 10.1037/a0021125. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Kim E, Cain KC. Korean American adolescent depression and parenting. Journal of Child and Adolescent Psychiatric Nursing. 2008;21:105–115. doi: 10.1111/j.1744-6171.2008.00137.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Kim M. Teen dating in South Korea: Earlier, more open, and more digital. Hakyoreh. 2016 Retrieved from http://english.hani.co.kr/arti/english_edition/e_national/751031.html.
  60. Kim SY, Hou Y. Intergenerational Transmission of Tridimensional Cultural Orientations in Chinese American Families: The Role of Bicultural Socialization. Journal Of Youth & Adolescence. 2016;45(7):1452–1465. doi: 10.1007/s10964-016-0423-4. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Knight GP, Berkel C, Uman˜a-Taylor AJ, Gonzales NA, Ettekal I, Jaconis M, Boyd BM. The familial socialization of culturally related values in Mexican American families. Journal of Marriage and Family. 2011;73:913–925. doi: 10.1111/j.1741-3737.2011.00856.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Kusche CA, Greenberg MT, Beilke R. Seattle Personality Questionnaire for Young School-Aged Children Department of Psychology. University of Washington; Seattle: 1988. [Google Scholar]
  63. Lee J, Bean FD. The Diversity Paradox: Immigration and the color line in 21st century America. New York: Russell Sage Foundation; 2010. [Google Scholar]
  64. Lee RM, Choe J, Kim G, Ngo V. Construction of the Asian American Family Conflicts Scale. Journal of Counseling Psychology. 2000;47(2):211–222. [Google Scholar]
  65. Lien P, Conway MM, Wong J. The contours and sources of ethnicidentity choices among Asian Americans. Social Science Quarterly. 2003;84(2):461–481. [Google Scholar]
  66. Lim SL, Yeh M, Liang J, Lau AS, McCabe K. Acculturation gap, intergenerational conflict, parenting style, and youth distress in immigrant Chinese American families. Marriage & Family Review. 2009;45:84–106. [Google Scholar]
  67. Ling H, Austin A. Asian American History and Culture: An Encyclopedia 2015 [Google Scholar]
  68. Liu LL, Lau AS. Teaching about race/ethnicity and racism matters: An examination of how perceived ethnic racial socialization processes are associated with depression symptoms. Cultural Diversity and Ethnic Minority Psychology. 2013;19(4):383–394. doi: 10.1037/a0033447. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  69. Lui PP. Intergenerational cultural conflict, mental health, and educational outcomes among Asian and Latino/a Americans: Qualitative and meta-analytic review. Psychological Bulletin. 2015;141:404–446. doi: 10.1037/a0038449. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  70. Magnusson D. The logic and implications of a person-oriented approach. In: Cairns RB, Bergman LR, Kagan J, editors. Methods and models for studying the individual ( Thousand Oaks: Sage Publications, Inc; 1998. pp. 33–64. [Google Scholar]
  71. Miller MJ. A bilinear multidimensional measurement model of Asian American acculturation and enculturation: Implications for counseling interventions. Journal of Counseling Psychology. 2007;54:118–131. [Google Scholar]
  72. Miller MJ, Yang M, Lim RH, Hui K, Choi NY, Fan X, Blackmon Sk. A test of the domain-specific acculturation strategy hypothesis. Cultural Diversity and Ethnic Minority Psychology. 2013;19(1):1–12. doi: 10.1037/a0030499. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  73. Min PG. Korean Americans. In: Min PG, editor. Asian Americans: Contemporary trends and issues ( Thousand Oaks: Pine Forge Press; 2006. pp. 230–259. [Google Scholar]
  74. Muthén BO, Muthén LK. Integrating Person-Centered and Variable-Centered Analyses: Growth Mixture Modeling With Latent Trajectory Classes. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research. 2000;24(6):882–891. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  75. Muthén LK, Muthén BO. Mplus User’s Guide. Los Angeles, CA: Muthen & Muthen; 2013. [Google Scholar]
  76. Nadal KL. Filipino American Psychology Hoboken. New Jersey: John Wiley & Sons; 2011. [Google Scholar]
  77. Nguyen AD, Benet-Martínez V. Biculturalism and adjustment: A meta-analysis. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology. 2013;44:122–159. [Google Scholar]
  78. Nguyen DJ, Kim JJ, Weiss B, Ngo V, Lau AS. Prospective relations between parent–adolescent acculturation conflict and mental health symptoms among Vietnamese American adolescents. Cultural Diversity and Ethnic Minority Psychology. 2018;24(2):151–161. doi: 10.1037/cdp0000157. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  79. Neblett EW, Jr, Rivas-Drake D, Umaña-Taylor AJ. The Promise of Racial and Ethnic Protective Factors in Promoting Ethnic Minority Youth Development. Child Development Perspectives. 2012;6(3):295–303. [Google Scholar]
  80. Ocampo AC. Are second-generation Filipinos ‘becoming’ Asian American or Latino? Historical colonialism, culture and panethnicity. Ethnic and Racial Studies. 2014;37(3):425–445. [Google Scholar]
  81. Ong P, Viernes K. Filipino Americans and educational downward mobility. Asian American Policy Review. 2013;23:21–39. [Google Scholar]
  82. Operario D, Fiske ST. Ethnic identity moderates perceptions of prejudice: Judgments of personal versus group discrimination and subtle versus blatant bias. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin. 2001;27(5):550–561. [Google Scholar]
  83. Paik SS, Rahman Z, Kula SM, Saito LE, Witenstein MA. Diverse Asian American families and communities: Culture, structure, and education (Part 1: Why they differ) School Community Journal. 2017;27(2):35–66. [Google Scholar]
  84. Paik SJ, Witenstein MA, Choe SMM. Filipinos in the U.S.: Historical, Social, and Educational Experiences. Social and Education History. 2016;5(2):134–160. [Google Scholar]
  85. Park IJ, Schwartz SJ, Lee RM, Kim M, Rodriguez L. Perceived racial/ethnic discrimination and antisocial behaviors among Asian American college students: Testing the moderating roles of ethnic and American identity. Cultural Diversity and Ethnic Minority Psychology. 2013;19(2):166–176. doi: 10.1037/a0028640. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  86. Patton GC, Sawyer SM, Santelli JS, Ross DA, Afifi R, Allen Viner RM. Our future: A Lancet commission on adolescent health and wellbeing. Lancet. 2016;387:2423–2478. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(16)00579-1. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  87. Pew Research Center. Key Facts About Asian Americans. Washington, D.C: Sep 8, 2017. http://www.pewresearch.org/fact-tank/2017/09/08/key-facts-about-asian-americans/ [Google Scholar]
  88. Portes A, Rumbaut RG, editors. Legacies: the story of the immigrant second generation Berkeley. New York: University of California Press & Russell Sage Foundation; 2001. [Google Scholar]
  89. Prinz R. doctoral dissertation. Stony Brook: State University of New York; 1977. The Assessment of Parent-Adolescent Relations: Discriminating Distressed and Non-distressed Dyads. [Google Scholar]
  90. Ramakrishnan K, Ahmad FZ. Center for American Progress. 2014. Language diversity and English proficiency; pp. 1–9. [Google Scholar]
  91. Rohner RP. Parental Acceptance-Rejection Questionnaire (PARQ) University of Conneticut; 2004. [Google Scholar]
  92. Rosenthal DA, Feldman SS. The nature and stability of ethnic identity in Chinese youth: Effects of length of residence in two cultural contexts. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology. 1992;23(2):214–227. [Google Scholar]
  93. Russell ST, Crockett LJ, Chao RK. Asian American Parenting and Parent-Adolescent Relationships. NY: Springer; 2010. [Google Scholar]
  94. Schwartz SJ, Unger JB, Zamboanga BL, Szapocznik J. Rethinking the concept of acculturation: Implication for theory and research. American Psychologist. 2010;65(4):237–251. doi: 10.1037/a0019330. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  95. Schwartz SJ, Zamboanga BL. Testing Berry’s Model of Acculturation: A Confirmatory Latent Class Approach. Cultural Diversity and Ethnic Minority Psychology. 2008;14(4):275–285. doi: 10.1037/a0012818. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  96. Silk JS, Morris AS, Kanaya T, Steinberg L. Psychological Control and Autonomy Granting: Opposite Ends of a Continuum or Distinct Constructs? Journal of Research on Adolescence. 2003;13(1):113–128. [Google Scholar]
  97. Soenens B, Vansteenkiste M, Luyten P. Toward a Domain‐Specific Approach to the Study of Parental Psychological Control: Distinguishing Between Dependency‐Oriented and Achievement‐Oriented Psychological Control. Journal of personality. 2010;78(1):217–256. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-6494.2009.00614.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  98. Sorkhabi N. Applicability of Baumrind’s parent typology to collective cultures: Analysis of cultural explanations of parent socialization effects. International Journal Of Behavioral Development. 2005;29(6):552–563. [Google Scholar]
  99. Steinberg L. A dual systems model of adolescent risk-taking. Developmental Psychobiology. 2010;52(3):216–224. doi: 10.1002/dev.20445. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  100. Szapocznik J, Scopetta M, Aranalde MD, Kurtines WM. Cuban value structure: Treatment implications. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 1978;46:961–970. doi: 10.1037//0022-006x.46.5.961. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  101. Takeuchi DT, Hong S, Gile K, Alegria M. Developmental contexts and mental disorders among Asian Americans. Research in Human Development. 2007;4:49–69. doi: 10.1080/15427600701480998. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  102. Telzer EH. Expanding the acculturation gap-distress model: An integrative review of research. Human Development. 2010;53(6):313–340. [Google Scholar]
  103. Tong Y. Acculturation, gender disparity, and the sexual behavior of Asian American youth. Journal of Sex Research. 2013;50(6):560–573. doi: 10.1080/00224499.2012.668976. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  104. Tran AGTT, Lee RM. Perceived ethnic-racial socialization, ethnic identity, and social competence among Asian American late adolescents. Cultural Diversity and Ethnic Minority Psychology. 2010;16(2):169–178. doi: 10.1037/a0016400. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  105. Trinh-Shevrin C, Islam NS, Rey MJ, editors. Asian American communities and health: Context, research, policy, and action. Vol. 22. San Francisco, CA: John Wiley & Sons; 2009. [Google Scholar]
  106. Tsai JL, Chentsova-Dutton Y, Wong Y. Why and how researchers should study ethnic identity, acculturation, and cultural orientation. In: Nagayama Hall GC, Okazaki S, editors. Asian American psychology: The science of lives in context. Washington D.C: American Psychological Association; 2002. pp. 41–65. [Google Scholar]
  107. Tseng V, Fuligni AJ. Parent-Adolescent Language Use and Relationships Among Immigrant Families With East Asian, Filipino, and Latin American Backgrounds. Journal of Marriage and Family. 2000;62(2):465–476. [Google Scholar]
  108. Uman˜a-Taylor AJ, Zeiders KH, Updegraff KA. Family ethnic socialization and ethnic identity: A family-driven, youth-driven, or reciprocal process? Journal of Family Psychology. 2013;27:137–146. doi: 10.1037/a0031105. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  109. Vigor JL. Measuring immigrant assimilation in the United States. 2008 Retrieved from https://www.manhattan-institute.org/html/measuring-immigrant-assimilation-united-states-5835.html.
  110. Ward C. The A, B, Cs of Acculturation. In: Matsumoto D, editor. The Handbook of Culture and Psychology. New York: Oxford University Press; 2001. pp. 411–445. [Google Scholar]
  111. Waters MC, Ueba R, Marrow H, editors. The new Americans: A guide to immigration since. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press; 2007. 1965. [Google Scholar]
  112. Weiss J, Garbanati JA. Relationship of acculturation and family functioning to smoking attitudes and behaviors among Asian American adolescents. Journal of Child and Family Studies. 2004;13(2):194–204. [Google Scholar]
  113. Wolf DL. Family secrets: Transnational struggles among children of Filipino immigrants. Sociological Perspectives. 1997;40(3):457–482. [Google Scholar]
  114. Wong YJ, Maffini CS. Predictors of Asian American Adolescents’ Suicide Attempts: A Latent Class Regression Analysis. Journal of Youth and Adolescence. 2011;40:1453–1464. doi: 10.1007/s10964-011-9701-3. (11 DO - 10.1007/s10964-011-9701-3) LA - English. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  115. Wu C, Chao RK. Intergenerational cultural dissonance in parent-adolescent relationships among Chinese and European Americans. Developmental Psychology. 2011;47(2):493–508. doi: 10.1037/a0021063. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  116. Ying YW, Han M. The longitudinal effect of intergenerational gap in acculturation on conflict and mental health in Southeast Asian American adolescents. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry. 2007;77(1):61–66. doi: 10.1037/0002-9432.77.1.61. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  117. Yoon E, Langrehr K, Ong LA. Content analysis of acculturation research in counseling and counseling psychology: A 22-year review. Journal of Counseling Psychology. 2011;58:83–96. doi: 10.1037/a0021128. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES