Abstract
Experience of traumatic events in childhood is linked to an elevated risk of developing psychiatric disorders in adulthood. The neurobiological mechanisms underlying this phenomenon are not fully understood. The limbic system, particularly the hippocampus, is significantly impacted by childhood trauma. In particular, it has been hypothesised that childhood stress may impact adult hippocampal neurogenesis (AHN) and related behaviours, conferring increased risk for later mental illness. Stress in utero can lead to impaired hippocampal synaptic plasticity, and stress in the first 2–3 weeks of life reduces AHN in animal models. Less is known about the effects of stress in the post-weaning, pre-pubertal phase, a developmental time-point more akin to human childhood. Therefore, we investigated persistent effects of pre-pubertal stress (PPS) on functional and molecular aspects of the hippocampus. AHN was altered following PPS in male rats only. Specifically males showed reduced production of new neurons following PPS, but increased survival in the ventral dentate gyrus. In adult males, but not females, pattern separation and trace fear conditioning, behaviours that rely heavily on AHN, were also impaired after PPS. PPS also increased the expression of parvalbumin-positive GABAergic interneurons in the ventral dentate gyrus and increased glutamic acid decarboxylase 67 expression in the ventral hilus, in males only. Our results demonstrate the lasting effects of PPS on the hippocampus in a sex- and time-dependent manner, provide a potential mechanistic link between PPS and later behavioural impairments, and highlight sex differences in vulnerability to neuropsychiatric conditions after early-life stress.
Introduction
Childhood adversity has repeatedly been identified as a risk factor for mental and physical illness1. However, neurobiological mechanisms underpinning this relationship are not fully understood. Stressful experiences activate the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal (HPA) axis, resulting in the release of corticosteroid hormones. These hormones traverse the blood–brain barrier, providing a pathway through which stress can alter brain development and leave individuals susceptible to a range of psychiatric disorders later in life. The limbic system, especially the hippocampus, is predicted to display considerable sensitivity to stress during early life; it contains high levels of glucocorticoid receptors, and is undergoing significant structural and functional changes as it matures2,3. Accordingly, childhood maltreatment is consistently associated in humans with reduced hippocampal volume, particularly in the dentate gyrus and Cornu Ammonis 3 (CA3) subfields1,4–7. Hippocampal-dependent behaviours, such as pattern recognition, spatial memory and verbal declarative memory, are also impaired in human sufferers of childhood maltreatment8–10. Although informative, studies in humans do not allow extensive investigation into the underlying mechanisms of the impact of early-life stress on the hippocampus to be explored.
In rodents, stress early in life can produce significant alterations in the adult hippocampus. Stress in utero can lead to impaired hippocampal synaptic plasticity, and stress in the first 2–3 weeks of life reduces adult hippocampal neurogenesis (AHN) in the dentate gyrus and brain-derived neurotrophic factor expression11–13. Less is known about the effects of stress in the post-weaning, pre-pubertal phase, a developmental time-point more akin to human childhood14. Studies to date have revealed that pre-pubertal stress (PPS) impairs synaptic plasticity in the dorsal hippocampus, alters the expression of corticosteroid receptors and impairs performance in a hippocampal-dependent contextual fear task in males15–17.
AHN is believed to play a crucial role in a number of hippocampal-dependent cognitive processes, including contextual fear responses, trace fear conditioning and pattern separation18–21. Pattern separation is a computational process that keeps similar input patterns distinct, allowing similar memories to remain separate22. The more comparable the memories, the harder it is to ‘pattern separate’ them. Experimentally, pattern separation can be assessed through analysing the ability to spatially discriminate between objects placed relatively far apart (easy condition, spatial locations do not overlap greatly) or close together (difficult condition, spatial overlap and potential for interference greater)20,21,23. This function is attributed to the dentate gyrus, through the unique action of young adult-born neurons20–22,24,25. These neurons also play a specific role in fear conditioning—reductions in AHN using either antimitotic agents or transgenic models specifically impairs fear learning only when a delay is interposed between the fear-associated and aversive stimuli, known as trace conditioning26–28. AHN is exquisitely sensitive to a range of environmental factors, notably stress, physical exercise and environmental enrichment19. Therefore, the first aim of this study was to determine whether PPS would decrease AHN and impair behaviours that rely on AHN, specifically, pattern separation and trace conditioning.
A number of intracellular pathways and extrinsic factors regulate AHN29,30. GABA signalling plays a particularly important role in AHN, regulating division of neural stems cells and modulating survival of newborn neurons31,32. Therefore, the second aim of this study was to investigate the consequences of PPS on parvalbumin (PV) + GABAergic (γ-aminobutyric acid) interneurons and glutamic acid decarboxylase 67 (GAD67) expression to elucidate a mechanistic link between PPS and adult hippocampal function.
Methods and materials
Animals
Animal experiments adhered to the European regulations on animal experimentation (Directive 2010/63/EU) and the UK Home Office Animals (Scientific Procedures) Act 1986. Male and female Lister-hooded rats were bred in-house at Cardiff University from adult pairs (Charles River) for this study. Litters were weaned on postnatal day 21 (PND 21), and housed in same-sex cages (32 cm × 50 cm × 21 cm) with littermates. Males and females were housed in the same room, food and water were provided ad libitum unless stated otherwise, cages were lined with wood shavings, a cardboard tube and wooden stick were provided as enrichment and light was maintained on a 12:12-h light/dark cycle (not reverse). Between 9 and 13 animals per group and sex were used in each experiment, and separate cohorts of animals were used for each behavioural experiment and for immunohistochemistry. Sample sizes were based on power calculations from previous studies using the same animal model and similar behavioural analyses16,17.
Pre-pubertal stress
On PND 25–27, half of the litters experienced a short-term PPS protocol that has been described previously16,33,34, and was originally described by Jacobson-Pick and Richter-Levin35. This took place in a designated room, separate from the holding room. On PND 25, between 9:00 a.m. and 13.00 p.m., animals were given a 10 min swim stress in an opaque swimming tank (25 cm high, 34 cm diameter), 12 L capacity filled with 6 L of 25 ± 1 °C water. PND 26 involved restraint stress: between 9:00 a.m. and 13:00 p.m. animals were placed into plastic restraint tubes (15 cm length, 5 cm diameter) for three sessions of 30 min, separated by 30 min breaks in the home cages. The final stressor on PND 27 involved placing animals onto an elevated platform (15 × 15 cm2, 115 cm high) for three 30-min sessions, separated by 60 min in the home cage between 9:00 a.m. and 5:00 p.m. Animals were then returned to their home cages and holding rooms and left undisturbed (aside from cage cleaning) until early adulthood (PND 60).
Litters were alternately allocated to experimental groups (PPS or control) by order of birth. A maximum of three animals per litter were used in each experiment, and a minimum of five litters per group (PPS or control) were generated to minimise effects of pseudoreplication36. The litter of origin was accounted for in all statistical analyses.
BrdU administration
Forty-four rats (male: 10 control, 13 PPS; female: 10 control, 11 PPS) were injected intraperitoneally on PND 60–65 with bromodeoxyuridine (BrdU, 200 mg/kg in 0.9% sterile saline solution) and killed by transcardial perfusion with 0.01 M phosphate-buffered saline (PBS) and 4% paraformadelhyde (PFA) under anaesthesia 24 h later to assess baseline rates of cell proliferation and neurogenesis in the dentate gyrus. Brains were left in PFA overnight (4 °C), and transferred to 30% sucrose solution for cryoprotection. Coronal 30-µm sections were cut through the entire hippocampal extent on a freezing microtome (Leica RM2245) and placed into a solution of cryoprotectant for storage at −20 °C until immunohistochemical analysis.
Immunohistochemisty
Sections were stained for either: (i) BrdU and doublecortin (DCX), (ii) parvalbumin and DCX or (iii) GAD67.
BrdU and DCX
Sections were washed between each step for 3 × 5 min in 0.01 M Tris-buffered saline (TBS, pH 7.4) and all steps were carried out at room temperature, unless otherwise specified. One in every 12 sections was denatured in 45 °C, 1 M HCl for 30 min, followed by incubation in blocking solution (0.3% Triton-X in 0.01 M TBS (TTBS), 2% donkey serum) for 60 min, rat anti-BrdU (1:100 in blocking solution, OBT0030, ABD Serotec, UK) for 48 h at 4 °C and Alexa Fluor 488 (5 µg/ml in TTBS, donkey anti-rat, Life Technologies, UK) for 2 h in the dark. Sections were then incubated with goat anti-DCX (1:100 in blocking solution, SC8066, Santa Cruz Biotechnology, UK) for 24 h at 4 °C to identify newly differentiated immature neurons, Alexa Fluor 647 (10 µg/ml in TTBS, donkey anti-goat, Life Technologies, UK) for 2 h in the dark. Sections were then exposed to DAPI (4′,6-diamidino-2-phenylindole; 1:3000 in TBS, D9542, Sigma UK) for 5 min. Washed sections were then mounted onto glass microscope slides and coverslipped with fluorescence mounting medium (S3023, Dako, UK). The number of cells double labelled with BrdU and DCX were counted in the supra- and infrapyramidal blades of each dentate gyrus in the dorsal (Bregma −1.72 to −5.28 mm) and ventral (Bregma −5.28 to −6.72 mm) portions of the hippocampus according to the Atlas of Paxinos and Watson (2009). The total number of double-labelled cells was estimated by multiplying the total number counted for each area (dorsal or ventral, supra- or infrapyramidal blade) by 1237. Counts and total volume of the hippocampus was analysed using Zen Blue (Carl Zeiss Microscopy, Germany).
Parvalbumin and DCX
All steps were carried out at room temperature, unless otherwise specified. One in every 12 sections was blocked in blocking solution (1% Triton-X in 0.01 M PBS, 10% donkey serum) for 2 h, goat anti DCX (1:100, SC8066, Santa Cruz) and mouse anti-parvalbumin (1:1000, P3088, Sigma-Aldrich) in 0.1% Triton-X, and 0.2% donkey serum in 0.01 M PBS overnight at 4 °C. Sections were washed in 0.1% Triton-X, 0.2% donkey serum in 0.01 M PBS at least three times before incubation with donkey anti-goat Alexa 647 (10 µg/ml in 0.01 M PBS, Life Technologies UK) and donkey anti-mouse Alexa 555 (1:1000 in 0.01 M PBS, Life Technologies UK) for 2 h in the dark. Sections were washed with 0.01 M PBS and incubated for 10 min in the dark with DAPI stain (1:3000 in 0.01 M PBS, D9542, Sigma UK) for nuclei staining. Sections were washed twice more in 0.01 M PBS before being mounted on standard microscopy slides using Mowiol aqueous mounting medium (Sigma, UK) and standard cover slips. Sections were imaged using an epifluorescent microscope (Leica DM6000B Upright Timelapse System with Leica Application Suite Advanced Fluorescence 3.0.0 build 8134 software, Leica Microsystems). The number of cells were quantified by manual counting in the supra- and infrapyramidal blades of the dorsal (−2.28 to – 5.04 mm) and ventral (−5.16 to −6.48 mm) dentate gyrus. The area was calculated for each region and the number of immunopositive cells quantified through visual counting, giving a cell count per mm2. The analysis was performed using Image J, with the plugin ‘Cell Counter’.
GAD67
All steps were carried out at room temperature, unless otherwise specified. Sections were washed between each step for 3 × 30 min in blocking solution (0.2% Triton-X in 0.01 M PBS (TTBS) containing 5% donkey serum, 2.5% bovine serum albumin). One in every 12 sections was incubated in blocking solution for 160 min, GAD67 antibody (1:500 in blocking solution, MAB5406, Millipore, UK) for 48 h at 4 °C and Alexa 647 (1:1000 in blocking solution, donkey anti-mouse, Life Technologies, UK) for 150 min in the dark. Sections were then placed into DAPI (1:200 in 0.01 M PBS, D9542, Sigma, UK) for 10 min in the dark before being mounted onto glass slides as above. The number of cells labelled with GAD67 were counted through the entirety of the dentate gyrus (infra- and suprapyramidal blades), CA1, CA2 and CA3 and hilus in the ventral (Bregma 5.28 to −6.72 mm) and dorsal (Bregma −1.72 to −5.28 mm) portions of the hippocampus, according to the Atlas of Paxinos and Watson (2009). The number of cells per measured area was obtained using Zen Blue (Zeiss).
All slides were imaged at ×20 using Axio Scan Z1 (Zeiss). Spearman's ρ was used to investigate correlations between PV+, DCX+ and BrdU+ cells.
Behaviour
Pattern separation
Animals
Pattern separation ability was assessed using a modified spontaneous location recognition paradigm20,21. Forty two rats (male: 11 control, 10 PPS; female: 12 control, 9 PPS) were food restricted to 85–90% of their free feeding weight 1 week before testing (PND 60–100). Animals were handled daily for 5 min during this time.
Apparatus
Testing took place in a black plastic circular arena (45 cm high x 75 cm diameter) with wood shavings covering the floor. The arena was placed in the centre of a dimly lit room, and three proximal spatial cues were placed equidistant around the arena to aid spatial orientation. Objects used were tin cans (7.5 cm × 7.5 cm × 11 cm) and small beer bottles (5.5 cm × 5.5 cm × 15 cm) with the labels removed. Blu-tack was used to fix them to the floor of the arena, and they and the arena were cleaned with 70% ethanol solution between trials. A video camera was suspended directly above the arena and trials were recorded for later analysis.
Behaviour
Animals freely explored the arena for 10 min a day for 5 days. On day 6, objects were placed into the arena. Objects were tin cans for half of the animals from each group and sex, and bottles for the remaining animals. Animals were tested in two conditions—easy (large spatial separation, LS) and difficult (small spatial separation, SS). Three identical objects, A1, A2 and A3 were placed 12.5 cm away from the walls of the arena, 25 cm from the centre with either A2 and A3 at 120° (LS) or 50° (SS) apart (Fig. 1). Animals explored the arena for 10 min (sample phase). After 24 h, animals were returned for 5 min (choice phase). The arena now contained two new objects, A4 and A5, identical in appearance to those used in the sample phase. A4 was in the same (familiar) location as A1, and A5 was placed equidistant to the previous locations of A2 and A3 (novel spatial location, Fig. 1). Rodents display an innate desire to explore novel objects and objects in novel locations over familiar ones20,38, thus animals will explore A5 in preference to A1. It will be harder to recognise A5 in the novel location in the SS condition than in the LS condition because SS places greater demand on pattern separation abilities than LS20,21. The amount of time animals spent exploring objects in the sample and choice phases was assessed from video recordings by an observer blind to the group. A discrimination ratio was then calculated20,38:
Half of the animals from each group and sex were tested in the LS first then the SS, the other half experienced SS then LS. Exploration was counted as the rat having its head directed at the object (2 cm or less), or touching the object with its nose. Sitting on or climbing over the object was not counted.
Fear conditioning
Animals
Animals were trained in trace, delay or control protocols at PND 60–100. One hundred and thirty rats (male: 33 control, 36 PPS; female: 30 control, 31 PPS) were used, with sample sizes of 9–13 per protocol per sex. Three days before testing, animals were handled daily and taken to the testing room for habituation to transport and handling.
Apparatus
Testing took place in two identical standard modular test chambers (32 cm × 25.5 cm × 27 cm, Sandown Scientific, UK) with grid floors (19 stainless-steel rods, 1 cm apart) underneath which was a stainless-steel pan. The ceiling, front and back walls of the boxes were clear plexiglass, the sides walls stainless steel, and the chamber was enclosed within a sound-attenuating chamber with a ventilation fan providing a background noise of 63 dB and a video camera attached to the inside of the door. The grid floor was connected to a shock generator, and there was a speaker attached to the inside of the sound-attenuating chamber. The boxes were connected to a computer and shock, light, sound and video generation were controlled by a computer. One box (context one, C1) was scented with a drop of lavender oil (Botanics Aromatherapy Pure Essential Oil, Boots, UK) placed onto a tissue in the pan, and contained an infrared (IR) light bar to allow filming as this box was always dark. In the second box (context two, C2), the pan was filled with wood shavings and the walls and ceiling decorated with black stars on a white background, and this box was always light (via houselight in the test chamber). Boxes were cleaned with ethanol wipes between animals, and sawdust/lavender scent replaced. Half of the animals from each group and sex were trained in C1 and the other half in C2, and all trials were video recorded for later analysis. The conditioned stimulus (CS) was 15 s, 75 dB white noise and the unconditioned stimulus (US) was 0.5 s, 0.5 mA scrambled footshock in all protocols.
Behavior
Training. Animals were placed individually into chambers for 120 s. Animals were then trained in one of three protocols: trace protocol (TP)—rats received 10 CS-US pairings, with a 30 s stimulus free trace interval between the offset of the CS and the onset of the US, delay protocol (DP)—10 CS-US pairings were presented, here the CS was followed immediately by the US and control protocol (CP)—animals received 10 presentations of the CS and 10 presentations of the US, which were explicitly unpaired. Intertrial intervals (ITI) were 312s (+/– 62 s) for trace and delay protocols, 156s (+/– 31s) for the control protocol. ITI length is positively correlated with freezing to later presentations of the CS in trace conditioning, and negatively associated with freezing to a context39. This length of ITI was selected as optimal for assessing freezing to cue and context in both trace and delay protocols39.
Context recall 1. Animals were returned to their training chamber 24 h later for 10 min to assess contextual fear responses.
Cue recall. Forty-eight hours after training, animals trained in C1 were placed into C2 and vice versa to reduce contextual associations and maximise responding to the CS. A plastic insert was placed over the bars of the floor to aid the discrimination. Animals received a 120 s acclimation period, followed by 360 s of CS the 240 s post -CS.
Context recall 2. Seventy-two hours after training, animals were returned to their training chamber for 10 min to assess contextual fear responses.
Freezing during training (baseline, post -shock and trace interval) and recall was analysed from video recordings by an observer blind to group. Freezing was defined as immobility with the exception of movement required for respiration, and was sampled every 10 s.
Statistical analysis
All data were checked for homogeneity of variance and normality, and transformed when these conditions were not met. Data were analysed in JMP (statistical software, SAS Institute, Cary, NC, USA) using generalised linear models, with experimental treatment (control or PPS), sex, separation distance (pattern separation only), protocol (trace, delay or control, fear conditioning only) fitted as factors. Litter was nested within group and added as a random factor to account for litter of origin and animal was nested within litter and added as a random factor to account for multiple measurements on the same animal.
Results
Neurogenesis
PPS resulted in a 46% decrease in cell proliferation in the suprapyramidal blade of the ventral dentate gyrus in males only (F1,8.81 = 4.9, p = 0.05, Fig. 2a), as measured by the number of co-labelled BrdU and DCX cells. There were no changes in the infrapyramidal blade or in females (F1,7.541 = 0.28, p = 0.62). Cell survival (as measured by DCX-labelled neurons) was increased by 65% in supra- and infrapyramidal blades of the ventral dentate gyrus after PPS in males (F1,9.29 = 14.21, p = 0.004, Fig. 2b) but not females (F1,8.38 = 1.25, p = 0.29). Volume of the dentate gyrus did not vary as a result of PPS (males: F1,8.82 = 0.01, p = 0.91; females: F1,8.31 = 0.09, p = 0.77).
Behaviour
Pattern separation
Animals from both groups spent an equal amount of time exploring all three objects in the sample phase (F1,8.5 = 0.01, p = 0.91). During the choice phase for LS, all animals spent more time exploring the novel object (A5) than the familiar one (A4) (one-sample t test: control males: t = 3.62, p = 0.005; PPS males: t = 3.05, p = 0.01; control females: t = 3.12, p = 0.01; PPS females: t = 3.18, p = 0.01), and both sexes were able to discriminate between objects equally well during LS (F1,4.46 = 0.07, p = 0.81, Fig. 3a), demonstrating that PPS did not affect pattern separation in the easy condition. During the choice phase for SS, only control males spent more time exploring the novel object than the familiar one (one-sample t test: control males: t = 4.38, = 0.001), and control males were the only group able to discriminate between novel and familiar objects in SS (F1,37.55 = 9.3, p = 0.006, Fig. 3b). Thus, control and stressed females were equally unable to distinguish between the novel and familiar objects in the SS condition, and stressed males showed an impairment in spatial pattern separation in the more difficult SS condition when compared to control males.
Fear conditioning: context
Results are shown from the first 90 s of each period only. Twenty-four hours after conditioning, male and female control animals experiencing CP and TP demonstrated robust levels of contextual freezing (Fig. 4a, data shown for males only). As expected, contextual freezing was markedly decreased in control DP animals where stronger associations to the CS are formed, resulting in decreased contextual freezing39. However, PPS resulted in increased contextual freezing in DP males (F2,198.3 = 3.24, p = 0.04, Fig. 4a) but not females (F2,24.49 = 0.67, p = 0.52). There were no differences between protocols or stressed and control animals in contextual freezing responses 72 h post conditioning (males: F2,24.76 = 0.75, p = 0.48; females: F2,30.65 = 2.12, p = 0.14).
Fear conditioning: cue recall
Forty-eight hours after conditioning cue recall was performed. In males, PPS reduced conditioned freezing during CS presentation in the TP (F2,198.9 = 3.6, p = 0.03, Fig. 4b). In females, PPS increased freezing during CS presentation in the DP (F2,185 = 3.37, p = 0.04, Fig. 4c). Overall, DP animals showed a greater freezing response to the CS than CP and TP animals, and TP froze more than CP animals (males: F2,198.9= = 3.6, p < 0.0001; females: F2,24.24 = 25, p < 0.0001). This is expected as DP animals make stronger CS-US associations than TP animals, who make stronger associations than controls. There was no effect of PPS on post CS freezing, but overall, TP froze more than DP animals (males: F2,197.1 = 3.48, p = 0.03; females: F2,27.08 = 4.2, p = 0.03).
To investigate whether differences between the groups were present during encoding, we analysed freezing responses for 60 s after presentation of the 10 US (shocks) during conditioning, and during the 30 s ‘trace’ gap for TP animals (after presentation of CS, sound). All animals, males and females, froze more after US presentation than at baseline (F10,631.1 = 28.2, p < 0.0001), and TP animals froze more after CS presentation from CS 2 (F10,222 = 17.44, p < 0.0001). PPS resulted in lower levels of freezing in male TP animals both after US (F1,11.14 = 7.06, p = 0.02) and CS presentation (F1,10.93 = 10.74, p = 0.007, Fig. 4d).
GABA and GABAergic interneurons
PPS increased the expression of GABAergic (PV+) interneurons in the infrapyramidal but not suprapyramidal blade of the ventral hippocampus of males only (F1,8.27 = 6.83, p = 0.03, Fig. 5a). There were no differences in females (F1,7.948 = 3.2, p = 0.1). GAD67 was increased in the ventral but not dorsal hilus in stressed males (F1,311 = 18.3, p < 0.0001, Fig. 5b). Furthermore, following PPS, there was a significant positive correlation between the number of DCX+ and PV+ cells in the suprapyramidal blade of the dentate gyrus only (r = 0.64, p = 0.04).
Discussion
Summary
The present study demonstrates that PPS has a profound effect on the structure and function of the adult male hippocampus. Specifically, we found that PPS decreased production but enhanced survival of newborn neurons in the ventral dentate gyrus, and impaired pattern separation and trace fear conditioning—behaviours that depend on neurogenesis. We also found evidence of altered GABA signalling following PPS—expression of PV+ interneurons was increased in the ventral dentate gyrus and GAD67 increased in the ventral hilus. Due to GABA’s role in maintaining quiescence of neural progenitor cells and promoting survival of newborn neurons, this provides a potential molecular mechanism underlying alterations in AHN after PPS.
Neurogenesis
We predicted that PPS would reduce proliferation (BrdU/DCX co-expressing cells) and survival (DCX only) of newborn neurons, as prenatal and early postnatal stressors (first 1–3 weeks of life) robustly decrease AHN in adult males40,41. In the present study, PPS decreased proliferation, but resulted in increased survival of newborn neurons, and these changes were specific to the ventral dentate gyrus in males. Thus, similar to prenatal and early postnatal stressors, PPS results in decreased cell proliferation in the male dentate gyrus42,43. However, the importance of timing is highlighted, as in contrast to other early-life stress paradigms, PPS increases cell survival. Delineating the contrasting mechanisms of dysfunction produced by stress at different early-life time points is crucial to producing effective interventions.
Altered neurogenesis is robustly associated with impairments in hippocampal-dependent behaviours, especially those with a temporal or spatial component44. Therefore, the second aim of the present study was to determine the consequences of PPS on two hippocampal-dependent behaviours, trace conditioning and pattern separation.
Trace and delay fear conditioning
We previously demonstrated that PPS results in impaired hippocampal-dependent contextual fear conditioning responses in male animals16. Although an intact hippocampus is necessary for this task (at least in one trial studies)45, other brain regions, such as the amygdala, are also required46,47. Therefore, we used a trace/delay fear protocol to more precisely dissect the effects of PPS on the trace manipulation which is thought to rely on AHN.
Males subjected to PPS displayed lower conditioned fear responses to the CS but only after training with the trace protocol. This is likely due to deficits on encoding, as lower levels of freezing were observed in PPS trace protocol males after the second presentation of CS and US during training. Thus, PPS selectively impacts hippocampal function in males but not females. Hippocampal lesions impair responses to the CS in trace but not delay protocols, whereas other regions are equally important in both47–49. Furthermore, the acquisition of trace fear is impaired following ablation of AHN using opotogenetic or infusion methods, yet delay fear responses remain intact28,45,50. The relative contributions of dorsal and ventral hippocampus to trace conditioning are not entirely clear. One study found that bilateral infusions of the AMPA/kainate glutamate receptor antagonist CNQX into the dorsal dentate gyrus disrupts expression of trace fear conditioning (ventral inactivation was not assessed)50, whereas others reveal inactivation of the ventral, but not dorsal, hippocampus impairs acquisition and expression of trace fear51.
The effects of increased newborn neuronal survival on trace memories are unknown, but increased neurogenesis disrupts contextual fear memories52,53. Together, our data suggest that PPS-induced alterations in neurogenesis could underlie the impairments in trace fear conditioning observed in males in the present study.
Males who had experienced PPS froze more to contextual cues 24 h after training in the delay condition. The impact of PPS on contextual fear conditioning selectively in males is consistent with our previous findings, although here the direction of effect is different16. There are methodological differences between the studies which likely account for the disparity in results—a single presentation of the US in our previous work compared to the present study which used 10 CS-US pairings. Ablation of AHN results in impaired contextual fear responses in single but not multiple trial conditioning54. The enhanced contextual freezing in delay conditioning we measured may be modulated by other neural substrates including the amygdala, which is known to be overactive after multiple CS-US pairings in this model34. Females froze more to representation of the cue during the delay protocol only after PPS, another behaviour which relies more heavily on the amygdala55.
Pattern separation
AHN is necessary for successful spatial pattern separation, particularly when stimuli are of increasing similarity56. We showed that animals were able to discriminate between stimuli when they were presented at large separation distances, but only control males successfully completed the task at the smaller, more difficult, separation distances. Deficits in pattern separation are particularly evident in psychiatric disorders with a strong hippocampal phenotype, such as post-traumatic stress disorder, schizophrenia and major depressive disorder57. To date, no studies have considered the impact of early-life stress on pattern separation. This study provides novel evidence for the role of PPS in disrupted pattern separation ability in adult life, and again points to a role between disrupted AHN and impaired hippocampal-dependent behaviour.
Typically, the dorsal hippocampus has been associated with impairments in cognitive and spatial behaviour (such as pattern separation) and the ventral hippocampus with emotional responses (such as those associated with fear conditioning)44. However, the situation is likely more complex, and the dorsal hippocampus has been shown to play a role in trace fear responses, especially with long trace intervals58. Presently, the relative contributions of dorsal and ventral hippocampus to the pattern separation task used in the present study are unknown. However, studies using pattern separation for reward value suggest a crucial role for the ventral dentate gyrus59, and one study found expression of the immediate-early gene zif268 in both ventral and dorsal dentate gyrus when rats were required to change between different navigational strategies in a plus maze60. Further work is required to delineate the relative contributions of dorsal and ventral hippocampus to the pattern separation task used in the current study. Here, we see impairments in both emotional (trace fear) and spatial (pattern separation) tasks correlated with altered neurogenesis in the ventral hippocampus.
GABAergic interneurons
Neural stem cells, the substate for neurogenesis, are largely quiescent, a state maintained through tonic GABA current from PV (PV+) interneurons61. We found that PPS increased PV+ interneurons in the ventral dentate gyrus, and increased GAD67, the rate-limiting enzyme in GABA production, in the ventral hilus in males specifically. This suggests a mechanism whereby GABA could be responsible for supressing generation of adult-born neurons in the ventral hippocampus in males following PPS.
GABA promotes survival of newborn neurons61,62. This may explain why although generation of new neurons was decreased, survival of DCX-expressing neurons, a population aged 24 h–2 weeks, was markedly increased after PPS in the male ventral dentate gyrus, especially as we find a positive correlation between the number of PV+ and DCX+ cells in the ventral dentate gyrus. Taken together, altered GABA signalling through increased numbers of PV+ interneurons may underlie aberrant neurogenesis and behaviour observed after PPS. Altered GABA signalling has been previously found in models of PPS, with levels of α1–3 subunits of the ionotropic GABA A receptor altered in a region-specific manner63.
Sex differences
Sex-based differences in the development of neuropsychiatric disorders abound, and have been found previously in our PPS model16,64,65. In the present study, PPS produces fewer changes in females than males. Specifically, PPS enhanced freezing to representation of the CS after a delay protocol only in females, with no other changes evident. This complements research by Toledo-Rodriguez and Sandi66, who found that PPS on PND 28–30 impaired extinction to a tone CS in adult males but not females. Interestingly, this study found increased fear responses to a tone in adolescent males, and reduced contextual freezing in adolescent females after PPS, highlighting the importance of timing when assessing the long-term outcomes of early-life stress.
PPS decreased the production yet increased the survival of newborn neurons in the infrapyramidal blade of the ventral dentate gyrus of males, with no effect in females. This mirrors the adult situation, where males demonstrate greater sensitivity to the effects of stress hormones, showing more extensive suppression of neural stem cell proliferation in the dentate gyrus67. However, adults also show decreased survival of newborn neurons in response to stress, which is in contrast to the increased survival observed in the present study. Prenatal stressors almost universally result in decreased generation and survival of newborn neurons in both males and females, whereas postnatal stress in the first 1–2 weeks of life produces more variable results40. Age of assessment is also vital: males experiencing early postnatal stress tend to have enhanced neurogenesis at PND 21, which switches to suppression as the animals age (as measured by DCX-positive cells). In contrast, females display decreased neurogenesis at PND 21, and this effect disappears in adulthood (DCX-positive cells). Less is known about the age- and sex-specific effects of this early prenatal stress on cell proliferation. Our study demonstrates that PPS produces a decrease in proliferation (measured by BrdU) with an increase in survival (DCX-positive cells) in adult males, with no effects apparent in females, marking the pre-pubertal phase as a time-point with unique consequences for AHN.
Sex differences were also seen in the pattern separation task, male and female rats performed equally well when the task was easy, but only control males were able to complete the difficult phase. We previously found male animals were superior in a spatial navigation task (water maze)16, and a number of other studies reveal a male advantage in spatial reference; however, others find no difference68. The fact that control females could not complete the more difficult pattern separation means assessing the effects of PPS on females in this task was not possible, and alternative tasks are needed to probe further female responses to PPS.
Expression of PV+, GABAergic interneurons and GAD67 were altered in males experiencing PPS, but remained unchanged in females. Prenatal stress decreases PV+ interneurons and GAD67 expression in the frontal cortex and hippocampus in early and adult life in males69,70, and alters the expression of GABA receptor subunits in a region- and sex-dependent manner71. PPS has previously been shown to decrease the expression of GABAA receptor α1 subunit in the hippocampus of adult males, and elevation is associated with stress resilience72,73, but little is known of the effects in females. Exposure to a more extended PPS paradigm (spanning PND 28–42) resulted in reduced messenger RNA levels of GAD67 in the central nucleus of the amygdala in male rats74, whereas a shorter protocol (PND 28–30) was insufficient to elicit such changes. This suggests that the exact timing and duration of early-life stress are likely crucial in determining later outcomes. Our results add to the literature demonstrating sex differences in response to PPS. In the present study, males display greater responses to PPS, but previous work demonstrates that females are sensitive in different ways. For example, females display learning impairments, exhibit greater compulsivity in a delay discounting task and have reduced preference for sweet solutions (anhedonia)75,76. Furthermore, males and females react similarly regarding certain behaviours: both sexes display heightened anxiety and altered cognitive bias after PPS17,33,77. In humans sex differences are also observed: associations between childhood maltreatment and incidents of anxiety and depression are higher in women than men, whereas risk for substance use disorder show the opposite pattern78,79. This suggests that males and females are distinct in their vulnerabilities to the experience of stress during development. Understanding these differences is crucial in the advancement of appropriate interventions and treatments for psychiatric illness associated with experience of childhood maltreatment.
Acknowledgements
We wish to acknowledge Neuroscience and Mental Health Research Institute and Jane Hodge Foundation who provided NB with fellowship funding for this research.
Conflict of interest
The authors declare that they have no conflict of interest.
Footnotes
Publisher’s note: Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
References
- 1.Teicher MH, Samson JA, Anderson CM, Ohashi K. The effects of childhood maltreatment on brain structure, function and connectivity. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2016;17:652. doi: 10.1038/nrn.2016.111. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Herman JP. Regulation of adrenocorticosteroid receptor messenger-RNA expression in the central-nervous-system. Cell. Mol. Neurobiol. 1993;13:349–372. doi: 10.1007/BF00711577. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Goddings, A. G. & Jay, N. Structural Brain Development During Childhood and Adolescence (The MIT Press: Cambridge, MA, London, England, 2014).
- 4.Dannlowski U, et al. Limbic scars: long-term consequences of childhood maltreatment revealed by functional and structural magnetic resonance imaging. Biol. Psychiatry. 2012;71:286–293. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2011.10.021. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Opel N, et al. Hippocampal atrophy in major depression: a function of childhood maltreatment rather than diagnosis? Neuropsychopharmacology. 2014;39:2723–2731. doi: 10.1038/npp.2014.145. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Teicher MH, Anderson CM, Polcari A. Childhood maltreatment is associated with reduced volume in the hippocampal subfields CA3, dentate gyrus, and subiculum. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA. 2012;109:E563–E572. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1115396109. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Cao B, et al. Hippocampal subfield volumes in mood disorder. Mol. Psychiatry. 2017;22:1352–1358. doi: 10.1038/mp.2016.262. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Majer M, Nater UM, Lin JMS, Capuron LReeves. W. C. Association of childhood trauma with cognitive function in healthy adults: a pilot study. BMC Neurol. 2010;10:1–10. doi: 10.1186/1471-2377-10-61. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Syal S, et al. The effect of childhood trauma on spatial cognition in adults: a possible role of sex. Metab. Brain Dis. 2014;29:301–310. doi: 10.1007/s11011-014-9497-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Bremner JD, et al. Neural correlates of declarative memory for emotionally valenced words in women with posttraumatic stress disorder related to early childhood sexual abuse. Biol. Psychiatry. 2003;53:879–889. doi: 10.1016/S0006-3223(02)01891-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Weinstock M. Prenatal stressors in rodents: effects on behavior. Neurobiol. Stress. 2016;6:3–13. doi: 10.1016/j.ynstr.2016.08.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Grigoryan, G., Segal, M. Lasting differential effects on plasticity induced by prenatal stress in dorsal and ventral hippocampus. Neural Plasticity (2016). 10.1155/2016/2540462. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 13.Luoni A, Richetto J, Racagni G, Molteni R. The long-term impact of early adversities on psychiatric disorders: focus on neuronal plasticity. Curr. Pharm. Des. 2015;21:1388–1395. doi: 10.2174/1381612821666150105142824. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Brydges, N. M. Pre-pubertal stress and brain development in rodents. Curr. Opin. Behav. Sci. 8–14 (2016).
- 15.Grigoryan G, Ardi Z, Albrecht A, Richter-Levin G, Segal M. Juvenile stress alters LTP in ventral hippocampal slices: involvement of noradrenergic mechanisms. Behav. Brain Res. 2015;278:559–562. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2014.09.047. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Brydges NM, Wood ER, Holmes MC, Hall J. Prepubertal stress and hippocampal function: sex-specific effects. Hippocampus. 2014;24:684–692. doi: 10.1002/hipo.22259. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Brydges NM, et al. Juvenile stress enhances anxiety and alters corticosteroid receptor expression in adulthood. Brain Behav. 2014;4:4–13. doi: 10.1002/brb3.182. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Goncalves JT, Schafer ST, Gage FH. Adult neurogenesis in the hippocampus: from stem cells to behavior. Cell. 2016;167:897–914. doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2016.10.021. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Opendak M, Gould E. Adult neurogenesis: a substrate for experience-dependent change. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2015;19:151–161. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2015.01.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Bekinschtein P, et al. BDNF in the dentate gyrus is required for consolidation of “pattern-separated” memories. Cell Rep. 2013;5:759–768. doi: 10.1016/j.celrep.2013.09.027. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Bekinschtein P, et al. Brain-derived neurotrophic factor interacts with adult-born immature cells in the dentate gyrus during consolidation of overlapping memories. Hippocampus. 2014;24:905–911. doi: 10.1002/hipo.22304. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.McAvoy K, Besnard A, Sahay A. Adult hippocampal neurogenesis and pattern separation in DG: a role for feedback in hibition in modulating sparseness to govern population-based coding. Front. Syst. Neurosci. 2015;9:120. doi: 10.3389/fnsys.2015.00120. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Oomen CA, et al. The touchscreen operant platform for testing working memory and pattern separation in rats and mice. Nat. Protoc. 2013;8:2006–2021. doi: 10.1038/nprot.2013.124. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Nakashiba T, et al. Young dentate granule cells mediate pattern separation, whereas old granule cells facilitate pattern completion. Cell. 2012;149:188–201. doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2012.01.046. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Berron D, et al. Strong evidence for pattern separation in human dentate gyrus. J. Neurosci. 2016;36:7569–7579. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0518-16.2016. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Shors TJ, et al. Neurogenesis in the adult is involved in the formation of trace memories. Nature. 2001;410:372–376. doi: 10.1038/35066584. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Shors TJ, Townsend DA, Zhao MR, Kozorovitskiy Y, Gould E. Neurogenesis may relate to some but not all types of hippocampal-dependent learning. Hippocampus. 2002;12:578–584. doi: 10.1002/hipo.10103. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Seo DO, Carillo MA, Lim SCH, Tanaka KF, Drew MR. Adult hippocampal neurogenesis modulates fear learning through associative and nonassociative mechanisms. J. Neurosci. 2015;35:11330–11345. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0483-15.2015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Zhao CM, Deng W, Gage FH. Mechanisms and functional implications of adult neurogenesis. Cell. 2008;132:645–660. doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2008.01.033. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Urban NampGuillemo. F. Neurogenesis in the embryonic and adult brain: same regulators, different roles. Front. Cell. Neurosci. 2014;8:396. doi: 10.3389/fncel.2014.00396. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Catavero C, Bao HampSongJ. Neural mechanisms underlying GABAergic regulation of adult hippocampal neurogenesis. Cell Tissue Res. 2017;371:33–46. doi: 10.1007/s00441-017-2668-y. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Tozuka Y, Fukuda S, Namba T, Seki T, Hisatsune T. GABAergic excitation promotes neuronal differentiation in adult hippocampal progenitor cells. Neuron. 2005;47:803–815. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2005.08.023. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Brydges NM, Hall L, Nicolson R, Holmes MC, Hall J. The effects of juvenile stress on anxiety, cognitive bias and decision making in adulthood: a rat model. PLoS ONE. 2012;7:e48143. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0048143. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Brydges NM, et al. Imaging conditioned fear circuitry using awake rodent FMRI. PLoS ONE. 2013;8:e54197–e54197. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0054197. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Jacobson-Pick S, Audet MC, Nathoo N, Anisman H. Stressor experiences during the juvenile period increase stressor responsivity in adulthood: transmission of stressor experiences. Behav. Brain Res. 2011;216:365–374. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2010.08.016. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Lazic SEampEssiouxL. Improving basic and translational science by accounting for litter-to-litter variation in animal models. BMC Neurosci. 2014;14:37. doi: 10.1186/1471-2202-14-37. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Noori, H. R. & Fornal, C. A. The appropriateness of unbiased optical fractionators to assess cell proliferation in the adult hippocampus. Front. Neurosci.5, 140 (2011). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 38.Warburton EC, Baird AL, Morgan A, Muir JL, Aggleton JP. Disconnecting hippocampal projections to the anterior thalamus produces deficits on tests of spatial memory in rats. Eur. J. Neurosci. 2000;12:1714–1726. doi: 10.1046/j.1460-9568.2000.00039.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Detert JA, Kampa ND, Moyer JR. Differential effects of training intertrial interval on acquisition of trace and long-delay fear conditioning in rats. Behav. Neurosci. 2008;122:1318–1327. doi: 10.1037/a0013512. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Loi M, Koricka S, Lucassen PJ, Joels M. Age- and sex-dependent effects of early life stress on hippocampal neurogenesis. Front. Endocrinol. 2014;5:11. doi: 10.3389/fendo.2014.00013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Ortega-Martinez S. Influences of prenatal and postnatal stress on adult hippocampal neurogenesis: the double neurogenic niche hypothesis. Behav. Brain. Res. 2015;281:309–317. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2014.12.036. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Mirescu C, Peters JD, Gould E. Early life experience alters response of adult neurogenesis to stress. Nat. Neurosci. 2004;7:841–846. doi: 10.1038/nn1290. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Oomen CA, et al. Severe early life stress hampers spatial learning and neurogenesis, but improves hippocampal synaptic plasticity and emotional learning under high-stress conditions in adulthood. J. Neurosci. 2010;30:6635–6645. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0247-10.2010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Anacker C, Hen R. Adult hippocampal neurogenesis and cognitive flexibility—linking memory and mood. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2017;18:335–346. doi: 10.1038/nrn.2017.45. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Drew MR, Huckleberry KA. Modulation of aversive memory by adult hippocampal neurogenesis. Neurotherapeutics. 2017;14:646–661. doi: 10.1007/s13311-017-0528-9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Phillips RG, Ledoux JE. Differential contribution of amygdala and hippocampus to cued and contextual fear conditioning. Behav. Neurosci. 1992;106:274–285. doi: 10.1037/0735-7044.106.2.274. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Kochli DE, Thompson EC, Fricke EA, Postle AF, Quinn JJ. The amygdala is critical for trace, delay, and contextual fear conditioning. Learn. Mem. 2015;22:92–100. doi: 10.1101/lm.034918.114. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Burman MA, Simmons CA, Hughes M, Lei L. Developing and validating trace fear conditioning protocols in C57BL/6 mice. J. Neurosci. Methods. 2014;222:111–117. doi: 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2013.11.005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Burman MA, Starr MJ, Gewirtz JC. Dissociable effects of hippocampus lesions on expression of fear and trace fear conditioning memories in rats. Hippocampus. 2006;16:103–113. doi: 10.1002/hipo.20137. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Pierson JL, Pullins SE, Quinn JJ. Dorsal hippocampus infusions of CNQX into the dentate gyrus disrupt expression of trace fear conditioning. Hippocampus. 2015;25:779–785. doi: 10.1002/hipo.22413. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Czerniawski J, Yoon T, Otto T. Dissociating space and trace in dorsal and ventral hippocampus. Hippocampus. 2009;19:20–32. doi: 10.1002/hipo.20469. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Ishikawa R, Fukushima H, Frankland PW, Kida S. Hippocampal neurogenesis enhancers promote forgetting of remote fear memory after hippocampal reactivation by retrieval. eLife. 2016;5:e17464. doi: 10.7554/eLife.17464. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Gao AJ, et al. Elevation of hippocampal neurogenesis induces a temporally graded pattern of forgetting of contextual fear memories. J. Neurosci. 2018;38:3190–3198. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3126-17.2018. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Drew MR, Denny CA, Hen R. Arrest of adult hippocampal neurogenesis in mice impairs single—but not multiple-trial contextual fear conditioning. Behav. Neurosci. 2010;124:446–454. doi: 10.1037/a0020081. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Raybuck JD, Lattal KM. Bridging the interval: theory and neurobiology of trace conditioning. Behav. Process. 2014;101:103–111. doi: 10.1016/j.beproc.2013.08.016. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56.Clelland CD, et al. A functional role for adult hippocampal neurogenesis in spatial pattern separation. Science. 2009;325:210–213. doi: 10.1126/science.1173215. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57.Yun S, Reynolds RP, Masiulis I, Eisch AJ. Re-evaluating the link between neuropsychiatric disorders and dysregulated adult neurogenesis. Nat. Med. 2016;22:1239–1247. doi: 10.1038/nm.4218. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58.Chowdhury N, Quinn JJ, Fanselow MS. Dorsal hippocampus involvement in trace fear conditioning with long, but not short, trace intervals in mice. Behav. Neurosci. 2005;119:1396–1402. doi: 10.1037/0735-7044.119.5.1396. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59.Kirk RA, Redmon SN, Kesner RP. The ventral dentate gyrus mediates pattern separation for reward value. Behav. Neurosci. 2017;131:42–45. doi: 10.1037/bne0000172. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 60.Satvat E, Schmidt B, Argraves M, Marrone DF, Markus EJ. Changes in task demands alter the pattern of zif268 expression in the dentate gyrus. J. Neurosci. 2011;31:7163–7167. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0094-11.2011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 61.Song J, et al. Neuronal circuitry mechanism regulating adult quiescent neural stem-cell fate decision. Nature. 2012;489:150–U216. doi: 10.1038/nature11306. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 62.Song J, et al. Parvalbumin interneurons mediate neuronal circuitry-neurogenesis coupling in the adult hippocampus. Nat. Neurosci. 2013;16:1728–1730. doi: 10.1038/nn.3572. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 63.Albrecht A, et al. Neurobiological consequences of juvenile stress: a GABAergic perspective on risk and resilience. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2017;74:21–43. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2017.01.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 64.Eiland L, Ramroop J, Hill MN, Manley J, McEwen BS. Chronic juvenile stress produces corticolimbic dendritic architectural remodeling and modulates emotional behavior in male and female rats. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2012;37:39–47. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2011.04.015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 65.Cahill L. Why sex matters for neuroscience. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2006;7:477–484. doi: 10.1038/nrn1909. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 66.Toledo-Rodriguez, M. & Sandi, C. Stress before puberty exerts a sex- and age-related impact on auditory and contextual fear conditioning in the rat. Neural Plasticity 1–12 (2007). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 67.Brummelte S, Galea LAM. Chronic high corticosterone reduces neurogenesis in the dentate gyrus of adult male and female rats. Neuroscience. 2010;168:680–690. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2010.04.023. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 68.Clint EK, Sober E, Garland T, Rhodes JS. Male superiority in spatial navigation: adaptation or side effect? Q. Rev. Biol. 2012;87:289–313. doi: 10.1086/668168. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 69.Matrisciano F, et al. Epigenetic modifications of GABAergic interneurons are associated with the schizophrenia-like phenotype induced by prenatal stress in mice. Neuropharmacology. 2013;68:184–194. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropharm.2012.04.013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 70.Uchida T, Furukawa T, Iwata S, Yanagawa Y, Fukuda A. Selective loss of parvalbumin-positive GABAergic interneurons in the cerebral cortex of maternally stressed Gad1-heterozygous mouse offspring. Transl. Psychiatry. 2014;4:e371.. doi: 10.1038/tp.2014.13. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 71.Van den Hove DLA, et al. Vulnerability versus resilience to prenatal stress in male and female rats: implications from gene expression profiles in the hippocampus and frontal cortex. Eur. Neuropsychopharmacol. 2013;23:1226–1246. doi: 10.1016/j.euroneuro.2012.09.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 72.Jacobson-Pick S, Richter-Levin G. Short- and long-term effects of juvenile stressor exposure on the expression of GABA(A) receptor subunits in rats. Stress Int. J. Biol. Stress. 2012;15:416–424. doi: 10.3109/10253890.2011.634036. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 73.Ardi Z, Albrecht A, Richter-Levin A, Saha R, Richter-Levin G. Behavioral profiling as a translational approach in an animal model of posttraumatic stress disorder. Neurobiol. Dis. 2016;88:139–147. doi: 10.1016/j.nbd.2016.01.012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 74.Tzanoulinou S, Riccio O, de Boer MW, Sandi C. Peripubertal stress-induced behavioral changes are associated with altered expression of genes involved in excitation and inhibition in the amygdala. Transl. Psychiatry. 2014;4:e410. doi: 10.1038/tp.2014.54. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 75.Brydges NM, Holmes MC, Harris AP, Cardinal RN, Hall J. Early life stress produces compulsive-like, but not impulsive, behavior in females. Behav. Neurosci. 2015;129:300–308. doi: 10.1037/bne0000059. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 76.Horovitz O, Tsoory MM, Yovell Y, Richter-Levin G. A rat model of pre-puberty (Juvenile) stress-induced predisposition to stress-related disorders: sex similarities and sex differences in effects and symptoms. World J. Biol. Psychiatry. 2014;15:36–48. doi: 10.3109/15622975.2012.745604. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 77.Jacobson-Pick S, Richter-Levin G. Differential impact of juvenile stress and corticosterone in juvenility and in adulthood, in male and female rats. Behav. Brain Res. 2010;214:268–276. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2010.05.036. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 78.Gallo E, Munhoz TN, Loret de Mola C, Murray J. Gender differences in the effects of childhood maltreatment on adult depression and anxiety: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Child Abus. Negl. 2018;79:107–114. doi: 10.1016/j.chiabu.2018.01.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 79.Evans EA, Grella CE, Upchurch DM. Gender differences in the effects of childhood adversity on alcohol, drug, and polysubstance-related disorders. Soc. Psychiatry Psychiatr. Epidemiol. 2017;52:901–912. doi: 10.1007/s00127-017-1355-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]