Abstract
Cognitive flexibility refers to the ability to quickly reconfigure our mind, like when we switch between different tasks. This review highlights recent evidence showing that cognitive flexibility can be conditioned by simple incentives typically known to drive lower-level learning, such as stimulus-response associations. Cognitive flexibility can also become associated with, and triggered by, bottom-up contextual cues in our environment, including subliminal cues. Therefore, we suggest that the control functions that mediate cognitive flexibility are grounded in, and guided by, basic associative learning mechanisms, and abide by the same learning principles as more low-level forms of behavior. Such a learning perspective on cognitive flexibility offers new directions and important implications for further research, theory, and applications.
Keywords: Cognitive flexibility, Cognitive control, Associative learning, Reinforcement learning, Task switching
Much of human behavior is characterized by the extraordinary ability to quickly reconfigure our mind, and switch between different tasks: We can swiftly shift our focus from color and fabric, when sorting dirty clothes for laundry, to shape, when searching for socks in a pile of clothes fresh from the dryer. This ability, often referred to as cognitive flexibility, has been widely recognized as a core function of cognitive control (Diamond, 2013), is of increasing importance in this digital age of multi-tasking (Eshet-Alkalai, 2004), and anomalies in flexibility are thought to characterize various clinical disorders (Geurts et al., 2009; Meiran et al., 2011). Cognitive flexibility has been studied on many different levels, including individual differences (Hommel & Colzato, 2017) and developmental changes (Dajani & Uddin, 2015). However, while most psychologists agree on the kind of behaviors that require cognitive flexibility, we know little about how this control function is regulated: how do we know when to be flexible, and how much?
Here, we will highlight recent work from the task switching literature that offers important new insights into how cognitive flexibility might be controlled. Specifically, after a brief introduction on cognitive flexibility and task switching, we will review evidence showing that the high-level ability to reconfigure the mind can be conditioned by simple incentives, and triggered by contextual features in our environment, possibly even outside awareness. Finally, building on these findings, we will promote a learning perspective on cognitive flexibility.
Cognitive flexibility: the pinnacle of cognitive control?
According to Diamond (2013), cognitive flexibility is one of the three core cognitive control (or executive) functions, next to inhibition and working memory.
Cognitive control mechanisms allow us to use internal goals and current context to guide information processing “top down” (e.g., Miller & Cohen, 2001). For example, we can combine the contextual information of seeing a traffic agent with our goal of personal safety to impose a new set of rules on how we link stimuli to actions (i.e., focus on the agent’s hands rather than the malfunctioning traffic lights). Imposing control in this manner involves overriding well-learned, habitual actions (e.g., braking when the traffic light turns red) and, accordingly, cognitive control has traditionally been seen as diametrically opposed to basic associative learning mechanisms that mediate the binding of stimuli to responses in routine behavior (Norman & Shallice, 1986). While associative learning is generally thought to produce fast, automatized stimulus-response links that can run unsupervised (and possibly unconsciously), cognitive control is thought to require volition and attention to produce slow but strategic action (e.g., Norman & Shallice, 1986;Diamond, 2013).
In this conceptualization, cognitive flexibility may possibly be considered the pinnacle of cognitive control: Other control processes are important to maintain and protect our current goals and task sets (e.g., by selectively attending to goal-relevant stimuli and inhibiting habitual responses), but it is one’s overarching ability to flexibly change these goals and task sets that produces adaptive behavior. Cognitive flexibility can thus be seen as a form of “meta-control” (Goschke, 2003; Hommel, 2015). However, casting cognitive flexibility as a higher-order control process naturally invites the question of how this ability to change task sets is regulated: Absent the assumption of a homunculus, what controls cognitive flexibility? Intriguingly, recent work suggests that flexibility can in fact be guided by “low-level” associative learning processes.
Task switching as a marker of cognitive flexibility
Our brief review will focus on regulation of cognitive flexibility in the context of studies investigating task switching (for reviews, see Kiesel et al., 2010; Vandierendonck et al., 2010). Cognitive flexibility has also been studied using creative problem solving, or rule reversal learning paradigms, like the Wisconscin Card Sorting Test. However, these paradigms provide less experimenter control over when the actual change in task sets occurred (see also, Geurts et al., 2009). There is also conceptual overlap between task-switching research and the study of working memory updating, though the latter tends to focus primarily on changing “items” in (declarative) working memory than on changing (procedural) task rules (Hazy et al., 2006).
The task switching literature investigates switching between task sets. Task sets can be considered a configuration of context-dependent production (“if, then”) rules that are actively maintained in order to guide our current behavior. For example, when we want to call our friend, we use a given set of rules to navigate through our phone, which define our task set. While certain components are often shared across task sets, it is their associations with the different rules and goals that make task sets unique (e.g., pushing a number to dial a phone number versus pushing a number to change floors in an elevator).
Using paradigms in which participants have to switch between two or more tasks, task switching studies typically focus on the switch cost: slower and less accurate performance on task switches than task repetitions. The switch cost has been interpreted as an index of cognitive control processes required for reconfiguring the task set (Rogers & Monsell, 1995) and/or resolving interference from the previously active task set (Allport et al., 1994). As a more tonic and voluntary marker of cognitive flexibility, recent studies have also begun emphasizing the switch rate: how much people choose to switch tasks in a free choice environment (Arrington & Logan, 2004).
A possible role for associative learning in task-switching was initially only investigated at the level of task sets: studies showed that task sets can be bound to, and be primed by, task-relevant (e.g., Waszak, Hommel, & Allport, 2003) and -irrelevant stimuli (e.g.,Mayr & Bryck, 2007), and can be reinforced following reward feedback (e.g., Schiffer et al., 2014), similar to stimulus-response associations (for a review, see Abrahamse et al., 2016). More recently though, there has been a realization that learning may not only promote the retrieval of one task set over another, but could also modulate the preparedness to switch sets per se. For instance,Dreisbach and Haider (2006) observed that a higher switch-likelihood (a higher proportion of task switches vs. repetitions in a block of trials) resulted in reduced switch costs. This opened the door to asking whether “low-level” learning mechanisms can shape cognitive flexibility.
Cognitive flexibility can be conditioned
Cognitive control functions are assumed crucial for overriding habitual behavior, like strongly conditioned responses, but can control functions themselves be subject to conditioning by reward? Although recent research has begun to investigate interactions between cognitive control and reward processing (for reviews, Botvinick & Braver, 2015; Notebaert & Braem, 2015), most of these studies presented explicit reward motivation cues before task execution, thus focusing on the effects of anticipating reward on cognitive control. Possibly, this focus on explicit reward cues was motivated by the idea that top-down, strategic control processes can only be up-regulated proactively by explicit, preparatory cues. In contrast, the reinforcement learning literature usually focuses on the (automatic) strengthening of behavior following reward feedback (Sutton & Barto, 1998).
As a first step towards connecting these disparate literatures (see also, Umemoto & Holroyd, 2015), we recently demonstrated that the act of task switching can be conditioned by reward (Braem, 2017). In a first phase of the experiment, cues told participants which task had to be performed on each trial (i.e., cued task phase), and people were rewarded more when performing a task switch than a task repetition (Figure 1A). In a second phase, participants were free to choose which task to perform, and no more rewards were delivered. Interestingly, despite the fact that participants were unaware of the biased reward allocation in the first phase, they now showed more voluntary task switching behavior (Figure 1B), suggesting that cognitive flexibility can be conditioned. In a similar vein, another recent study showed that presenting participants with more task switches than repetitions during a cued task phase, influenced subjects’ choice to be more flexible in a subsequent voluntary task choice environment (i.e., performing more voluntary task switches; Fröber & Dreisbach, 2017).
Together, these studies suggest that the choice to be cognitively flexible is very susceptible to its recent (reinforcement) learning history. These studies are also congruent with a much older line of research in behavioral psychology, where (animal) psychologists demonstrated that variability in behavior (i.e., responding in a less predictable manner) is a behavior that in itself can be selectively reinforced (for a review, see Neuringer, 2002). Future studies should address whether this type of reinforced behavioral variability relies on the same mechanisms as those underlying task switching.
Cognitive flexibility can be triggered by contextual cues
A traditional assumption of cognitive flexibility (and cognitive control more broadly) is that it is generalizable. Thus, the processes responsible for task switching are not thought to be specific to particular tasks but to be shared among all possible task switching conditions. Consequently, many scholars have hypothesized that the effects of training people on being more cognitively flexible in one task context should transfer to other tasks measuring cognitive flexibility. However, recent meta-analyses have demonstrated that cognitive training studies rarely find transfer (e.g.,Simons et al., 2016).
In contrast, associative learning processes are thought to be trigger-specific in nature, as learned associations are known to bind to the context in which they occur (Pearce & Bouton, 2001). In behavioral psychology, this is often referred to as stimulus control, but we will speak of the context-specificity of learned behavior. For example, the habit of smoking can be very context-specific: environments that have been more frequently associated with smoking in the past will induce a higher urge to smoke, independent of the availability of cigarettes (Dols, van den Hout, Kindt, & Willems, 2002). Intriguingly, recent studies have documented that the same class of phenomena can be observed in relation to cognitive control settings. For instance, if a spatial context (like screen location) is predictive of more challenging task demands, over time this high-demand context comes to implicitly cue the retrieval of the appropriate attentional set, thus making participants better at meeting high task demands in that spatial context (for reviews, see Bugg & Crump, 2012; Egner, 2014).
Importantly, recent studies have extended these findings of “context-control learning” to the case of cognitive flexibility. For example, it has been shown that switch costs can be reduced for stimuli that are presented at a screen location associated with a higher likelihood of task switches (relative to repetitions), even when people are unaware of this contingency (Crump & Logan, 2010; for a similar observation in attention shifting, see Sali, Anderson, & Yantis, 2015). In a similar vein, Farooqui and Manly (2015) demonstrated that subliminally presented (i.e., not consciously perceived) cues signaling a higher likelihood of task switches were followed by smaller task switch costs.
If the readiness to switch between different tasks can be triggered by contextual cues, like location, it should also be possible to bind switch-readiness to specific task stimuli. We tested this hypothesis by linking particular stimuli to the need to update tasks more or less frequently (Chiu & Egner, 2017; see also Leboe et al., 2008). By employing three different task sets (see Figure 1C), we could demonstrate that stimuli (here, specific individuals’ faces) associated with task switches did indeed facilitate task switching, and that they did so irrespective of which task was being switched to (Figure 1D). This suggests that what participants learned was to associate specific cues with a general readiness to switch between tasks rather than to switch to one particular alternative task. This finding emphasizes a key distinction in the effects of learned stimulus-control vs. stimulus-response associations: while the latter are specific (e.g., promoting a particular motor response), the former are generalizable (here, aiding the switch to any other task) (Egner, 2014). The extent of this generalizability (e.g., to other measures of cognitive flexibility), however, remains an interesting avenue for future research.
Taken together, these findings show that, through learning, stimuli in our environment can be bound to the processes underlying cognitive flexibility (e.g., to an “updating threshold”, cf. Goschke, 2003), and eventually help triggering cognitive flexibility bottom-up, even subliminally. By relying on these fast associative learning processes, the contextual triggering of cognitive flexibility may allow for a more efficient and less effortful allocation of control strategies.
A learning perspective on cognitive flexibility
In trying to answer what controls cognitive flexibility, the above studies demonstrate that, much like simple motor responses, cognitive flexibility is highly sensitive to the environment it operates in, and rewards that follow it. However, in our view, the impact of these findings has remained underappreciated in the broader literature, likely because they do not fit with more traditional notions of cognitive control as being in competition with bottom-up associative behavior. Many psychologists still ascribe cognitive flexibility to independent, supervisory, or “executive” control systems that correct low-level behavior, without specifying regulatory mechanisms for employing these functions in an adaptive manner (e.g., Diamond, 2013).
Instead, we believe that these findings call for an alternative perspective where the functions which allow us to be flexible are guided by basic (associative) learning, and abide by the same learning principles as more low-level forms of behavior do. This view is consistent with recent theoretical perspectives on the regulation of other control functions, like conflict-control (Abrahamse, Braem, Notebaert, & Verguts, 2016; Egner, 2014), which has been effectively modeled using basic reinforcement learning rules (e.g., Botvinick et al., 2001). The basic premise of this perspective is that, rather than seeing cognitive flexibility as originating from a standalone module (or brain region) that intervenes - deus ex machina – to solve problems in lower-level associative processing, the processes underlying cognitive flexibility are grounded in the learning framework (and associative network) as simple stimulus-response associations (Figure 2). Thus, while cognitive control processes are “higher-level” in that they can produce generalizable benefits, their regulation must be understood in terms of basic associative learning processes.
Conclusion
In sum, we aimed to illustrate how recent observations break with traditional ideas on cognitive flexibility, by showing how cognitive flexibility can be conditioned and bound to contextual cues. We believe the literature is in need of a paradigmatic shift in how psychologists understand cognitive flexibility, and cognitive control more broadly.
A learning perspective on cognitive flexibility could provide new challenges for computational models of task switching (e.g., Holroyd & McClure, 2015), and theorizing about impairments in cognitive flexibility in certain neurocognitive disorders (e.g., autism, Geurts et al., 2009; OCD or depression, Meiran et al., 2011). Moreover, the conditioning and contextual cuing of cognitive flexibility could also offer promising applications for facilitating behavioral change, as other forms of conditioning have (e.g., De Houwer et al., 2001). For example, in training people to be more cognitively flexible, one could take advantage of its context-sensitivity by training people in the environments where flexibility is most required. Last, we only focused on learning via experience, but recent studies have shown that learning via instructions can also result in automatic stimulus-response associations (e.g., Meiran, Liefooghe, & De Houwer, 2017). Therefore, an interesting hypothesis to test would be whether instructed stimulus-control assocations would also result in the kind of automaticity reviewed here.
Recommended Reading.
Abrahamse, E., Braem, S., Notebaert, W., & Verguts, T. (See References). Provides a more comprehensive review of the empirical literature, and outlines the broader implications of an associative learning perspective on cognitive control.
Egner, T. (See References). Gives a more detailed description of the explanatory value of a learning perspective in the conflict adaptation literature, where more studies already investigated the context-specificity of cognitive control.
Geurts, H. M., Corbett, B., & Solomon, M. (See References). Reviews the literature on cognitive flexibility from a clinical perspective (i.e., in autism), and, in doing so, critically evaluates the concept of cognitive flexibility and how to best study it.
Ionescu, T. (2012). Exploring the nature of cognitive flexibility. New ideas in psychology, 30(2), 190–200. While outside the scope of the current brief review, this theoretical review offers an interesting discussion on the different uses of the term cognitive flexibility (e.g., as a skill versus property of the cognitive system).
Neuringer, A. (See References). Reviews an interesting, related line of research from behavioral psychology that studied the conditioning of variability in behavior.
Acknowledgements
S.B. is supported by FWO - Research Foundation Flanders (12K6316N). T.E. is supported in part by NIMH R01MH087610. The authors declare no competing financial interests.
References
- Abrahamse E, Braem S, Notebaert W, & Verguts T (2016). Grounding cognitive control in associative learning. Psychological Bulletin, 142, 693–728. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Allport DA, Styles EA & Hsieh S (1994) Shifting intentional set: Exploring the dynamic control of tasks In: Attention and performance XV, ed. Umiltà C& Moscovitch M. MIT Press. [Google Scholar]
- Arrington CM, & Logan GD (2004). The cost of a voluntary task switch.Psychological science, 15(9), 610–615. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Botvinick M, & Braver T (2015). Motivation and cognitive control: from behavior to neural mechanism. Annual review of psychology, 66. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Botvinick MM, Braver TS, Barch DM, Carter CS, & Cohen JD (2001). [Google Scholar]
- Conflict monitoring and cognitive control. Psychological review, 108(3), 624. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Braem S (2017). Conditioning task switching behavior. Cognition, 166, 272–276. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bugg JM, & Crump MJ (2012). In support of a distinction between voluntary and stimulus-driven control: A review of the literature on proportion congruent effects. Frontiers in psychology, 3, 367. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chiu YC, & Egner T (2017). Cueing Cognitive Flexibility: Item-Specific Learning of Switch Readiness. Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance. In press. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Crump MJ, & Logan GD (2010). Contextual control over task-set retrieval.Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 72(8), 2047–2053. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dajani DR, & Uddin LQ (2015). Demystifying cognitive flexibility: Implications for clinical and developmental neuroscience. Trends in neurosciences, 38(9), 571– 578. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- De Houwer J, Thomas S, & Baeyens F (2001). Association learning of likes and dislikes: A review of 25 years of research on human evaluative conditioning. Psychological bulletin, 127(6), 853. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Diamond A (2013). Executive functions. Annual review of psychology, 64, 135–168. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dols M, Hout MVD, Kindt M, & Willems B (2002). The urge to smoke depends on the expectation of smoking. Addiction, 97(1), 87–93. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dreisbach G, & Haider H (2006). Preparatory adjustment of cognitive control in the task switching paradigm. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 13(2), 334–338. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Egner T (2014). Creatures of habit (and control): a multi-level learning perspective on the modulation of congruency effects. Frontiers in psychology, 5: 1247. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Eisenreich BR, Akaishi R, & Hayden BY (2017). Control without controllers: toward a distributed neuroscience of executive control. Journal of cognitive neuroscience, 29(10), 1684–1698. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Eshet-Alkalai Y (2004). Digital literacy: A conceptual framework for survival skills in the digital era. Journal of Educational Multimedia and Hypermedia, 13(1), 93–106. [Google Scholar]
- Farooqui AA, & Manly T (2015). Anticipatory control through associative learning of subliminal relations: invisible may be better than visible. Psychological science, 26(3), 325–334. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fröber K, & Dreisbach G (2017). Keep flexible–Keep switching! The influence of forced task switching on voluntary task switching. Cognition, 162, 48–53. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Geurts HM, Corbett B, & Solomon M (2009). The paradox of cognitive flexibility in autism. Trends in cognitive sciences, 13(2), 74–82. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Goschke T (2003). Voluntary action and cognitive control from a cognitive neuroscience perspective Voluntary action: Brains, minds, and sociality, 49-85. Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Hazy TE, Frank MJ, & O’Reilly RC (2006). Banishing the homunculus: making working memory work. Neuroscience, 139(1), 105–118. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Holroyd CB, & McClure SM (2015). Hierarchical control over effortful behavior by rodent medial frontal cortex: A computational model. Psychological Review, 122(1), 54–83. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hommel B (2015). Between persistence and flexibility: The Yin and Yang of action control In Advances in motivation science, 2, 33–67. Elsevier. [Google Scholar]
- Hommel B, & Colzato LS (2017). The social transmission of metacontrol policies: Mechanisms underlying the interpersonal transfer of persistence and flexibility. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 81, 43–58. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kiesel A, Steinhauser M, Wendt M, Falkenstein M, Jost K, Philipp AM, & Koch I (2010). Control and interference in task switching—A review. Psychological bulletin, 136(5), 849. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Leboe JP, Wong J, Crump M, & Stobbe K (2008). Probe-specific proportion task repetition effects on switching costs. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 70(6), 935–945. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mayr U, & Bryck RL (2007). Outsourcing control to the environment: effects of stimulus/response locations on task selection. Psychological Research, 71(1), 107– 116. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Meiran N, Diamond GM, Toder D, & Nemets B (2011). Cognitive rigidity in unipolar depression and obsessive compulsive disorder: Examination of task switching, Stroop, working memory updating and post-conflict adaptation. Psychiatry research, 185(1), 149–156. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Meiran N, Liefooghe B, & De Houwer J (2017). Powerful instructions: Automaticity without practice. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 26(6), 509–514. [Google Scholar]
- Miller EK, & Cohen JD (2001). An integrative theory of prefrontal cortex function.Annual review of neuroscience, 24(1), 167–202. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Neuringer A (2002). Operant variability: Evidence, functions, and theory.Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 9(4), 672–705. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Norman DA, & Shallice T (1986). Attention to action In Consciousness and self-regulation, 1–18. Springer; US. [Google Scholar]
- Notebaert W, & Braem S (2015). Parsing the effects of reward on cognitive control In Braver T (Ed.), Motivation and cognitive control, 105–122, Taylor & Francis. [Google Scholar]
- Pearce JM, & Bouton ME (2001). Theories of associative learning in animals.Annual review of psychology, 52(1), 111–139. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rogers RD, & Monsell S (1995). Costs of a predictible switch between simple cognitive tasks. Journal of experimental psychology: General, 124(2), 207. [Google Scholar]
- Sali AW, Anderson BA, & Yantis S (2015). Learned states of preparatory attentional control. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 41(6), 1790. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schiffer AM, Muller T, Yeung N, & Waszak F (2014). Reward activates stimulus-specific and task-dependent representations in visual association cortices. Journal of Neuroscience, 34(47), 15610–15620. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Simons DJ, Boot WR, Charness N, Gathercole SE, Chabris CF, Hambrick DZ, & Stine-Morrow EA (2016). Do “brain-training” programs work?. Psychological Science in the Public Interest, 17(3), 103–186. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sutton RS, & Barto AG (1998). Reinforcement learning: An introduction (Vol. 1, No. 1). Cambridge: MIT press. [Google Scholar]
- Umemoto A, & Holroyd CB (2015). Task-specific effects of reward on task switching. Psychological research, 79(4), 698–707. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vandierendonck A, Liefooghe B, & Verbruggen F (2010). Task switching: interplay of reconfiguration and interference control. Psychological bulletin, 136(4), 601. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Waszak F, Hommel B, & Allport A (2003). Task-switching and long-term priming: Role of episodic stimulus–task bindings in task-shift costs. Cognitive psychology,46(4), 361–413. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]