Abstract
The integrated model of insight in schizophrenia suggests that poor insight is the result of multiple factors which compromise persons’ abilities to integrate streams of information into a personal awareness of psychiatric challenges, and make adaptive responses. This model hypothesizes that metacognitive deficits, or difficulties forming a complex and integrated understanding of the self and others, influence insight, regardless of other proximal causes including clinical profile. To explore this possibility, we performed a latent class analysis on 324 adults with schizophrenia or schizoaffective disorder. This analysis produced 4 groups on the basis of assessment of insight and Positive and Negative Syndrome Scale (PANSS) positive, negative, cognitive, and hostility symptoms. The resultant groups were characterized as: Good Insight/Low Symptoms (n = 71), Impaired Insight/High Negative Symptoms, (n = 43), Impaired Insight/High Positive Symptoms (n = 50) and Impaired Insight/Diffuse Symptoms (n = 160). When we compared metacognitive function as assessed with the Metacognition Assessment Scale - Abbreviated (MAS-A) across groups, we found that the good insight group had better overall metacognition as well as higher levels of self-reflectivity, awareness of the other and mastery as compared to all 3 groups with impaired insight. When controlling for total symptoms, all differences in metacognitive function between the good insight and the impaired insight groups remained significant. These results are consistent with the view that, independent of symptoms, impaired metacognition contributes to difficulties integrating information and hence impedes insight, or awareness of psychiatric challenges. Consistent with extant literature, results suggest that interventions focusing on metacognition as the target may lead to improved insight.
Keywords: insight, metacognition, positive symptoms, recovery, negative symptoms
Many diagnosed with schizophrenia have been reported to lack clinical insight or to deny that they have symptoms of a mental illness or need treatment.1 Among the first to explicitly define this, in 1882 Pick2 described insight into psychiatric disorders as conscious reflection about “pathological aspect(s) of … mental processes,” (p. 519), which could vary in its lucidity from an “illness-feeling,” to an “illness-insight” (p. 530). As insight began to be broadly used to refer to multiple facets of self-knowledge, 50 years after Pick, Lewis3 clarified that insight into psychiatric challenges refers to awareness of “morbid change” (p. 333).
In more recent years, variations in degree of insight have been found among adults with schizophrenia across cultures,4,5 in both early and later phases of illness,6–9 and during and following periods of acute disturbance.10 Poor insight has been linked to noncompliance in participation in pharmacological treatment,11–14 duration of untreated psychosis,15,16 and low therapeutic alliance with mental health professionals.17–20 It has also been associated with additional negative outcomes including more frequent relapses,21 worsening symptoms,22,23 and poorer interpersonal and community functioning.24–27 On the other side of the coin, in what has been called the insight paradox,28 good insight has been shown to lead to depression, hopelessness, low self-esteem, low quality of life, low sense of meaning in life, and suicidality, especially when coupled with acceptance of stigma.29–34
To make sense of this range of findings, contemporary research has sought to delineate the processes underlying insight, or those which enable persons to form ideas about their experiences of what others perceive as psychiatric challenges. It has become apparent that insight is more than the acceptance of a singular fact and instead requires the integration of multiple streams of information. These include the piecing together of knowledge of changes in internal states, external circumstances, the views of others, and the trajectory of a life, along with reflections about the causes and consequences of these changes.35–37
Seeking to operationalize these processes, the integrated model of insight35 suggests that metacognitive deficits are among the most proximal causes of poor insight. Metacognition refers to the ability to form an integrated sense of self and others and to use that knowledge to respond to emergent challenges in life.38–41 According to this model, metacognitive deficits should limit insight when they interfere with abilities to form and connect ideas about: (1) changes in thoughts, emotions, and behaviors that have occurred as a result of mental illness; (2) what are the pertinent historical events related to those changes; and (3) how different historical and psychological events are and are not related. Indeed, without the capacity to hold an integrated sense of self and others it is difficult to imagine how anyone could name and understand the emergence of mental illness and formulate an adaptive response.35 In this model, metacognition may also have a particularly unique connection to insight in that its influence may be independent of other potential causes including alterations in basic brain functions, symptoms, neurocognition, and social cognition. It may also moderate the effects of these phenomena, given the influence of metacognitive capacity on the extent to which information is integrated.1,42,43 One important implication of this model is that it may help science understand how and why poor clinical insight emerges from the lived experience of the person diagnosed with schizophrenia; namely it is a simple expression of the experience of fragmentation which limits the construction of any larger sense of the challenges that life presents.
Supporting the link between insight and metacognitive capacity is evidence that metacognitive deficits commonly occur in schizophrenia38,44 and that metacognitive deficits predict poor insight in both early and later phases of illness.9,45–48 Lesser levels of constructs related to metacognition such as organizational skills pertaining to self-reflection and complexity of personal narratives have also been linked with poorer insight in schizophrenia.49,50 Indirect evidence of the link between insight and metacognition can also be found in studies suggesting that treatments that target metacognition have a positive effect on insight.51–53 Other evidence may be found in imaging studies that suggest impaired insight is associated with alterations in the activities of cortical regions and circuits that may support processes involved in metacognition, including self-consciousness and the distinction between one’s own experience and that of another.54
Importantly, one limitation of the existing research on insight and metacognition is that due to modest sample sizes, studies have yet to explore whether metacognitive deficits are ubiquitous among persons with impaired insight or whether they are linked to specific clinical features. In other words, are deficits in metacognition related to impaired insight beyond specific clinical profiles? One set of clinical features which appears to be differentially related to insight are the positive and negative symptoms which define the condition. Positive symptoms have been suggested to compromise insight when they present anomalous experiences which defy interpretation, while negative symptoms may compromise insight when they interfere with the detection and expression of emotional experience and social connection.1 Several studies have suggested that higher levels of symptoms predict more stable deficits in insight over time in psychosis55 and schizophrenia.56 However, given that links between insight and positive and/or negative symptoms have been found in some studies, but not in others,42,43,57–60 it is likely that there are subgroups of patients that may all demonstrate impaired insight but may have uniquely high levels of positive and/or negative symptoms. The likelihood that there are distinct groups with varying levels of insight and symptom profiles is supported by cluster analyses which found groups with schizophrenia that differed in terms of insight and depression.61
To address the associations between metacognition and insight among persons with distinct clinical profiles, the present study examined the relationships of metacognition, symptoms, and insight among a relatively larger independent sample of adults with schizophrenia, excluding participants included in our previous cluster analysis that addressed the relationship of metacognition, insight, and depression.61 The first aim was to identify different configurations of symptoms and insight using latent class analysis (LCA). We chose this approach in order to use statistical rather than rational methods (eg, based on predetermine cut scores) to avoid any pre-existing biases we might have had regarding the underlying symptom and insight profiles that exist naturally. The second aim was to explore whether those with impaired insight experienced graver impairments in metacognition regardless of symptom profile. We predicted that we would identify a group with good insight and relatively fewer symptoms, and groups with impaired insight with varying symptoms presentations: predominantly positive symptoms, predominantly negative symptoms, and mixed positive and negative symptoms. We predicted that all impaired insight groups would show greater levels of metacognitive deficit than the good insight group, regardless of symptom severity and presentation.
Method
Participants
Participants were 324 adults in outpatient mental health treatment with a confirmed diagnosis of schizophrenia (219) or schizoaffective disorder (105) using the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV.62 Participants were recruited for studies of the effects of psychosocial rehabilitation at a Veterans’ Affairs Medical Center and community mental health center in Indianapolis Indiana as well as 2 partial hospitalization programs and 1 outpatient clinic in Newark and Piscataway, New Jersey. All participants were in a non-acute phase of illness, defined by no hospitalizations or changes in housing or medication within 30 days of study enrollment. As noted above participants were excluded if they were included in the cluster analysis we have previously presented.60 The mean age of the participants was 43.39 years (range: 18–71; SD = 12.32). The sample was 74% male (n = 241), 25% female (n = 80) and 1% transgender (n = 3) and the mean years of education was 11.44 years (range: 2–24; SD = 3.31).
Instruments
Indiana Psychiatric Illness Interview.63
The Indiana Psychiatric Illness Interview (IPII) is a semi-structured interview designed to elicit a sample of how individuals’ think about their psychiatric and related challenges. The interview typically lasts 30 to 60 minutes. The interview asks participants to talk about: (1) the story of their life; (2) whether they think they have a mental illness and, if so, how that has affected and not affected their thoughts, emotions, and behaviors; (3) the influence of their condition and their influence over their condition; (4) the influence of their condition on others and other’s influence over their condition; and (5) the future. The IPII elicits a nuanced account of psychiatric challenges which goes far beyond the acceptance of psychiatric labels and which can be the basis for assessments of metacognitive capacity.
Metacognition Assessment Scale - Abbreviated.46
The Metacognition Assessment Scale - Abbreviated (MAS-A) was used to rate metacognition on the basis of a typed transcript of the IPII. The MAS-A is an adaptation of the MAS which was originally designed to study metacognition within psychotherapy transcripts.40 The MAS-A transformed the original items of the MAS into 4 ordinal scales. These scales represent the domains of self-reflectivity (S) which assesses the degree to which a person has an integrated vs fragmented sense of self; awareness of others (O) which assesses the degree to which a person has an integrated vs fragmented sense of other people, decentration (D) which assesses the degree to which a person has an integrated or fragmented sense of their position within the larger community; and mastery (M) which assesses the degree to which a person can use metacognitive knowledge to respond to psychosocial challenges. The MAS-A frames metacognition as a series of increasingly complex and hierarchical processes such that once a given level is not attained, no further levels can be meaningfully achieved thus higher scores indicating a greater higher ability to form integrated sense of self and other. Good inter-rater reliability and validity have been presented elsewhere with intraclass correlations ranging from 0.80 to 0.92 for the MAS-A subscales.45–47,64
Positive and Negative Syndrome Scale.65
The Positive and Negative Syndrome Scale (PANSS) is a 30-item rating scale used for measuring symptom severity of patients with schizophrenia and other mental disorders. In this study, we used the Bell et al66 factor analytically-derived model which produces the following 5 component scores: positive, negative, cognitive, emotional discomfort, and hostility. We measured insight using the PANSS insight and judgment item and thus calculated the cognitive component excluding this item. Good inter-rater reliability was found in prior studies.61,65 Acceptable levels of interrater reliability have been previously found with intraclass correlations ranging from 0.83 to 0.94.
Procedure
Procedures were approved by the local Institutional Review Boards. After written informed consent was obtained from participants, clinical psychologists met with participants to confirm diagnoses with the SCID-IV. Trained master’s-level research assistants then administered the instruments as part of a baseline assessment. All measures were collected prior to randomization into a trial of psychosocial rehabilitation. Raters of the MAS-A were blind to PANSS scores and PANSS raters were blind to IPII content or MAS-A scores. There were a total of 4 PANSS raters deployed in the New Jersey Settings and 5 in the Indianapolis settings. All MAS-A ratings were conducted by raters in Indianapolis on the basis of typed IPII transcripts.
Analytical Strategy
Statistical analyses were conducted using SPSS version 24 and Mplus version 7.67,68 Analyses were conducted in 1 preliminary phase, 3 primary phases and 1 exploratory stage. As a preliminary analysis, we examine the potential effects of settings by comparing MAS-A scores from participants in New Jersey with those in Indianapolis using an intraclass correlation. Turning to the primary analyses, we first conducted a LCA in order to identify the potential presence of homogenous groups of individuals based on symptomatology. Given that previous studies have suggested that higher levels of each are associated with poorer insight, we included 4 of the PANSS primary symptom components: Positive, Negative, Cognitive and Hostility. In the second phase, we conducted a series of analyses of variance or covariance (ANOVAs) to compare demographics and MAS-A scores between groups. In the third phase, we conducted another set of ANCOVA repeated comparisons of a good insight group with impaired insight groups controlling for total symptoms. Finally, in order to characterize the groups in a more nuanced manner, we conducted exploratory ANOVA comparing the 4 groups on emotional discomfort symptoms as well as the individual positive and negative symptoms which make up the component scores.
LCA is a useful method to statistically identify latent homogenous groups (classes) of individuals from categorical or continuous multivariate data. It is based on probabilistic models of subgroup membership, which differs from other clustering methods that instead rely on finding clusters with distance measures that are arbitrary or theoretical.67,68 In the present study, LCA was used to identify latent classes of individuals with schizophrenia spectrum disorders based on PANSS symptom subscales and the insight and judgment item. Differences among classes for demographic variables and metacognitive ability were calculated using ANOVA and chi-square tests (significance level of P < .05).
The number of classes were not hypothesized a priori but were determined from an examination of model fit statistics, including entropy values, Akaike’s Information Criteria69 (AIC), Bayesian Information Criteria70 (BIC), and sample size-adjusted BIC71 (ssaBIC) (lower AIC, BIC, ssaBIC and higher entropy values indicating better fit). Bootstrapped likelihood ratio tests72,73 (BLRT) and Lo-Mendell-Rubin74 (LMR) tests, where n and n−1 number of classes are compared, were also conducted.
Results
First, to compare the potential effects of setting we conducted an intraclass correlation comparing the MAS-A total scores from participants in Indianapolis Indiana and those from Northern New Jersey. This revealed an intraclass correlation coefficient (ICC) = 0.01 suggesting that only 1% of the variance in scores was accounted for by setting and thus there was no need to control for study setting. Five LCA models were estimated specifying between 1 and 5 latent classes. The AIC and ssaBIC values decreased with each successive class addition and thus did not readily discriminate a model of best fit. BIC values decreased for k = 2–4 classes, but increased for the k = 5 model, suggesting that the 5-class model is a poorer fit to the data. Entropy values were adequate for most models (k = 3–5) but decreased slightly for the k = 5 model. Bootstrapped likelihood ratio tests (BLRT) remained significant (P < .0001) with each successive class addition to the model, thus not clearly discriminating a model of best fit. Lo-Mendell-Rubin (LMR) likelihood ratio tests were non-significant when comparing k = 2 to k = 3 classes, suggesting that the 3-class model did not significantly improve the fit of the model. However, the LMR test was significant when comparing the 3-class to the 4-class model, suggesting that the 4-class model was a significantly better fit to the data than the 3-class model. Taken together with the entropy value of the 4-class model, as well as model interpretability and consistency with the extant literature reviewed above, the 4-class model was determined to be the best fit to the data.
Descriptive statistics including group size, background characteristics, PANSS component and insight scores and MAS-A scores are presented in Table 1. Groups with mean PANSS insight item scores of “3” were classified as having impaired insight, while groups of scores with “2” or less were classified as having good insight, since the former scores reflect at least mild levels of unawareness of illness and the latter scores suggest less than minimal levels of unawareness. Based upon these scores and the PANSS component scores, the groups were classified as follows: Good Insight/Low Symptoms (n = 71), Impaired Insight/High Negative Symptoms (n = 43), Impaired Insight/High Positive Symptoms (n = 50), and Impaired Insight/Diffuse Symptoms (n = 160). By the label “Diffuse,” we sought to describe a group with a broad range of symptoms in which no one was class was especially prominent compared to the others. Concerning demographics, groups differed significantly only on gender, with the Impaired Insight/High Positive Symptoms having a greater proportion of women than the other 3 groups.
Table 1.
Class 1 | Class 2 | Class 3 | Class 4 | Test | P | Post hoc (0.05) | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
LCA items (m, SD) | n = 71 | n = 43 | n = 50 | n = 160 | |||
PANSS positive component | 2.10 (0.65) | 2.72 (0.73) | 3.42 (0.75) | 2.61 (0.71) | F(3,317) = 33.649 | <.001*** | 1<2,3,4; 2,4<3 |
PANSS negative component | 1.61 (0.40) | 3.55 (0.40) | 1.93 (0.41) | 2.35 (0.43) | F(3,317) = 206.833 | <.001*** | 1<2,3,4; 1,3,4<2; 3<4 |
PANSS hostility component | 1.57 (0.45) | 2.00 (0.66) | 2.94 (0.44) | 1.55 (0.41) | F(3,317) = 122.167 | <.001*** | 1<2,3; 2,4<3; 4<2; |
PANSS cognitive componenta | 1.68 (0.39) | 3.07 (0.58) | 2.55 (0.62) | 2.39 (0.54) | F(3,317) = 65.021 | <.001*** | 1<2,3,4; 1,3,4<2 |
PANSS insight and judgment item | 1.87 (0.92) | 4.40 (1.03) | 3.4 (0.90) | 3.54 (0.90) | F(3,317) = 77.818 | <.001*** | 1<2,3,4; 3,4<2 |
Covariates | |||||||
Gender (n, % male) | 47 (66.2) | 38 (88.4) | 24 (48.0) | 132 (82.5) | X2 = 32.595 | <.001*** | |
Diagnosis (n, % schizophrenia) | 43 (60.6) | 29 (67.4) | 30 (60.0) | 115 (71.9) | X2 = 4.564 | .207 | |
Age | 41.63 (12.81) | 42.67 (14.20) | 43.86 (11.02) | 44.21 (11.95) | F(3,320) = 0.790 | .5 | |
Education (y) | 11.55 (3.39) | 10.37 (3.86) | 11.65 (3.32) | 11.62 (3.09) | F(3,320) = 1.749 | .157 |
Note: PANSS, Positive and Negative Syndrome Scale.
aInsight and judgment item excluded from the cognitive component.
***P < .001.
As revealed in Table 2, when we compared groups on metacognitive ability with ANOVA and post hoc analyses using the Bonferroni correction for multiple comparisons, we found that the Good Insight group had better overall metacognitive functioning that the impaired insight groups. They also had significantly higher scores on self-reflectivity, awareness of the other and mastery than the 3 impaired insight groups. The metacognition scores of the Impaired Insight/High Positive Symptoms group was generally poorer than that of the Impaired Insight/Diffuse Symptoms group. When the MAS-A scores of the Good Insight group were compared with the 3 impaired insight groups, covarying for PANSS total score, the overall metacognition and self-reflectivity MAS-A sub-scales of the Good Insight group continued to be significantly higher than any of the 3 impaired insight groups. In addition, when PANSS total was covaried for, the Good Insight group was found to have better mastery scores than the Impaired Insight/High Negative Symptoms and the Impaired Insight/ Diffuse Symptoms groups, but not the Impaired Insight/High Positive Symptoms group. The Good Insight group continued to have higher awareness of the other scores, but only in comparison with the Impaired Insight/High Negative Symptoms group.
Table 2.
Class 1 | Class 2 | Class 3 | Class 4 | Test | P | Post hoc (0.05) | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
MAS-A self-reflectivity | 5.04 (1.42) | 3.45 (1.17) | 4.06 (1.17) | 4.25 (1.36) | F(3,320) = 13.915 | <.001*** | 1>2,3,4***; 4>2** |
MAS-A awareness of the other | 3.10 (0.70) | 2.43 (0.95) | 2.85 (0.96) | 2.94 (0.96) | F(3,320) = 5.177 | .002** | 1>2***; 4>2** |
MAS-A decentration | 0.64 (0.68) | 0.58 (0.75) | 0.35 (0.61) | 0.60 (0.77) | F(3,320) = 1.864 | .136 | |
MAS-A mastery | 4.25 (1.62) | 2.71 (1.60) | 3.11 (1.65) | 3.30 (1.74) | F(3,320) = 9.192 | <.001*** | 1>2,4***,3** |
MAS-A total | 13.04 (3.34) | 9.17 (3.43) | 10.37 (3.58) | 11.10 (4.06) | F(3,320) = 10.653 | <.001*** | 1>2,3***,4**; 4>2* |
PANSS emotional discomfort component | 2.92 (0.97) | 3.22 (1.17) | 3.78 (0.90) | 2.97 (1.04) | F(3,320) = 9.053 | <.001*** | 1<3***; 4<3***; 2<3+ |
Note: MAS-A, Metacognition Assessment Scale – Abbreviated; PANSS, Positive and Negative Syndrome Scale.
*P < .05, **P < .01, ****P < .001, +P = .059.
Next, to characterize the clinical features of the groups, we compared groups on the PANSS emotional discomfort scale and the individual items that comprise the positive and negative components. As revealed in Table 2, the Impaired Insight/High Positive Symptom group had higher levels of emotional distress compared to the other groups. ANOVA and post hoc analyses using the Bonferroni correction for multiple comparisons comparing the PANSS positive and negative sub-domain items revealed that the Impaired Insight/High Negative Symptoms group had significantly higher levels of each of the respective negative symptom PANSS items compared to the other 3 groups. The Impaired Insight/High Positive Symptoms group had significantly higher levels of 4 of the positive symptom sub-domain items: delusions, grandiosity, unusual thought content, and somatic concern (multiple comparisons using the Bonferroni correction P < .05). The Impaired Insight/High Positive Symptoms group did not have significantly higher levels of hallucinations or suspiciousness than the other groups.
Of note given the unexpected gender differences, we conducted a final set of ad hoc analyses comparing the PANSS insight and MAS-A total scores across gender. Here we found that men and women do not differ significantly on MAS total (means (SD): 11.10 (4.11) and 11.30 (3.37)) but did differ on PANSS insight and judgment item (F(1,320) = 8.123 P = .005), and that difference persisted after controlling for emotional distress (F(1,319) = 6.547 P = .011; means (SD): 3.39 (1.21) for men and 2.95 (1.1.8) for men and women, respectively.
Discussion
In this study, we empirically derived 4 groups of patients with schizophrenia on the basis of level of insight and symptoms. One group was found to have good insight and 3 groups were found to have impaired insight but different symptom presentations: one with predominantly negative symptoms, one with predominant positive symptoms and one with diffuse levels of most symptoms. As expected, all of the impaired insight groups had lower levels of overall metacognition than the group with good insight. Specifically, the impaired insight groups all had lower levels of overall metacognition and more impairment in domains of self-reflectivity, awareness of other people and mastery, or integrating information about the self and others to effectively face psychological challenges. When severity of psychopathology was held constant, the impaired insight group with high negative symptoms continued to have the same forms of poorer metacognition relative to the good insight group, while both the diffuse symptom group and the positive symptom group continued to have poor overall metacognition and self-reflectivity relative to the good insight group; the diffuse symptom group also continued to demonstrate worse mastery.
Findings are thus consistent with the integrated model of insight that suggests metacognition is associated with insight independent of clinical profile.35 Poor insight in the presence of uniquely high negative and/or positive symptoms all appear linked to dysfunctional abilities to form an integrated sense of self and others. Examination of mean scores suggest clinically significant differences, with all impaired insight groups struggling to see their mental states as changing and fallible and to use more than gross avoidance when facing psychosocial stressors.
There were unexpected findings. Decentration scores did not differ between groups. One explanation is that this reflects a restriction of range, both with limited variation in this score and less sensitivity to change in general. Further, we found that the positive symptoms and impaired insight group contained significantly more women. Paradoxically, we also found this group had higher levels of emotional discomfort whereas previous studies have found that good insight is more often related to emotional distress. This group also had higher levels of grandiosity, which may suggest a unique subgroup in which gender-specific factors play a role. One interpretation of the profile of this group is that in the face of emotional distress, grandiosity and impaired insight potentially play a self-protective role resulting in a cycle in which distress leads to denial and reality distortion which in turn leads to more distress. This interpretation is consistent with cognitive models that suggest that emotional concerns trigger or contribute to the maintenance of psychotic symptoms.75,76
Beyond this, however, results suggest clinical profile may not be irrelevant. The Impaired Insight/High Negative Symptoms group had poorer self-reflectivity and awareness of the other than the Impaired Insight/Diffuse symptoms group. This may suggest that this group has particularly significant metacognitive challenges. This is consistent with previous findings linking deficits in metacognitive capacity with future levels of negative symptoms.77,78 As with all unexpected findings, all interpretations should be regarded as speculative at best and needing further study.
While the cross-sectional nature of these findings precludes drawing causal conclusions, there are several potential interpretations of the results that could guide future research. First, with lesser metacognitive capacity, persons, regardless of symptom levels, are less able to integrate streams of information into a complex account of psychiatric challenges. For example, with poorer metacognitive capacities, persons may be less able to track changes in their own mental states and then consider alternative explanations for those changes. Further, deficits in the ability to adaptively use metacognitive knowledge may leave patients with higher levels of negative symptoms and little sense that they can affect their fate; as such, they may invest less effort in forming an understanding of the challenges they are facing. Alternative interpretations also cannot be ruled out, including that impaired insight leads to difficulties integrating experience and reduced metacognitive capacity. Future longitudinal studies are needed to track the relationships of these variables over time in order to confirm these possibilities.
There are limitations to the current study. The sample included only persons enrolled in treatment. Thus, it is unknown if these or similar relationships would be observed in samples of individuals who refuse treatment, as many with impaired insight may. We also only included persons with schizophrenia spectrum disorders. It is consequently unknown if the relationships found here are specific to schizophrenia or whether they may also apply to other disorders where lesser metacognitive deficits have been observed, including Bipolar Disorder,79 Borderline Personality Disorder,80 PTSD,81 and Major Depression.82 Assessments were limited to symptoms and metacognition and we used one assessment of insight. Groups also tended to have mild to moderate impairments in insight; research is needed to explore groups with more severe levels of insight impairment. We also focused on clinical insight and future research is needed to explore the interface of metacognition with other forms of insight. Future research should also examine other factors that may influence insight, including social cognition and neurocognition, as well as different dimensions of insight including cognitive insight, or awareness of and attitudes towards one’s general thought processes in longitudinal designs. A more nuanced study of the links of clinical insight and metacognition with subjective recovery is also needed. We also did not assess medication dosage or adherence and hence it is unknown to what extent pharmacological treatment may have influenced results.
Finally, concerning clinical practice, results emphasize the need to see insight as a matter of meaning making and not the acceptance of specific labels. As metacognition is fundamentally an intersubjective act,83 insight is something evolved with or between persons and not something that happens in isolation or somehow “within” the identified patient alone. This is consistent with observations that insight can readily become maladaptive when generated in interactions that are oppressive or laden with stigma.84 Thus, interventions that affect metacognitive capacity, are free from stigma, and empower persons to manage their own lives may lead to improvements in adaptive insight. In other words, it may be that by assisting persons in forming more complex and integrated ideas about themselves and others, it may, in turn, enable them to form a personally meaningful and non-destructive account of their illness, thus leading to effective self-management and a quicker return to recovery. Such interventions include Metacognition Training,85 which encourages persons to see how they draw conclusions and then reflect upon how they think about and respond to life, and Metacognition Reflection and Insight Therapy (MERIT),86 which seeks to promote a more integrated sense of self and others that are needed to develop an enhanced experience of personal agency and self-management.87
Funding
Research was supported in part from two grants: “Randomized Controlled Trial of Treatment for Internalized Stigma in Schizophrenia.” NIMH RO1 1R01MH094310-01A1 and “Effects of CBT and Cognitive Remediation on Work in Schizophrenia.” VA Rehabilitation Research and Development. D 6629R.
Acknowledgment
The authors have declared that there are no conflicts of interest in relation to the subject of this study.
References
- 1. Lysaker PH, Pattison ML, Leonhardt BL, Phelps S, Vohs JL. Insight in schizophrenia spectrum disorders: relationship with behavior, mood and perceived quality of life, underlying causes and emerging treatments. World Psychiatry. 2018;17:12–23. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2. Pick A. Über Krankheitsbewusstsein in psychischen Krankheiten. Archiv für Psychiatrie und Nervenkrankheiten. 1882;13:518–581. [Google Scholar]
- 3. Lewis A. The psychopathology of insight. Br J Med Psychol. 1934;14:332–348. [Google Scholar]
- 4. Schennach R, Meyer S, Seemüller F, et al. . Insight in schizophrenia-course and predictors during the acute treatment phase of patients suffering from a schizophrenia spectrum disorder. Eur Psychiatry. 2012;27:625–633. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5. Wang Y, Xiang YT, Wang CY, et al. . Insight in Chinese schizophrenia patients: a 12-month follow-up. J Psychiatr Ment Health Nurs. 2011;18:751–757. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6. Cuesta MJ, Peralta V, Campos MS, Garcia-Jalon E. Can insight be predicted in first-episode psychosis patients? A longitudinal and hierarchical analysis of predictors in a drug-naïve sample. Schizophr Res. 2011;130:148–156. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7. Koren D, Viksman P, Giuliano AJ, Seidman LJ. The nature and evolution of insight in schizophrenia: a multi-informant longitudinal study of first-episode versus chronic patients. Schizophr Res. 2013;151:245–251. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8. Osatuke K, Ciesla J, Kasckow JW, Zisook S, Mohamed S. Insight in schizophrenia: a review of etiological models and supporting research. Compr Psychiatry. 2008;49:70–77. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9. Vohs JL, Lysaker PH, Liffick E, et al. . Metacognitive capacity as a predictor of insight in first-episode psychosis. J Nerv Ment Dis. 2015;203:372–378. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10. Braw Y, Sitman R, Sela T, Erez G, Bloch Y, Levkovitz Y. Comparison of insight among schizophrenia and bipolar disorder patients in remission of affective and positive symptoms: analysis and critique. Eur Psychiatry. 2012;27:612–618. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11. Misdrahi D, Tessier A, Swendesen J, et al. . Determination of adherence profiles in schizophrenia using self-reported adherence: results from the FACE-SZ dataset. J Clin Psychiatry. 2016;77:1130–1136. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12. Lincoln TM, Jung E, Wiesjahn M, Wendt H, Bock T, Schlier B. The impact of negative treatment experiences on persistent refusal of antipsychotics. Compr Psychiatry. 2016;70:165–173. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13. Chan KW, Hui LM, Wong HY, Lee HM, Chang WC, Chen YH. Medication adherence, knowledge about psychosis, and insight among patients with a schizophrenia-spectrum disorder. J Nerv Ment Dis. 2014;202:25–29. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14. Chandra IS, Kumar KL, Reddy MP, Reddy CM. Attitudes toward medication and reasons for non-compliance in patients with schizophrenia. Indian J Psychol Med. 2014;36:294–298. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15. Hui CL, Lau WW, Leung CM, et al. . Clinical and social correlates of duration of untreated psychosis among adult-onset psychosis in Hong Kong Chinese: the JCEP study. Early Interv Psychiatry. 2015;9:118–125. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16. O’Donoghue B, Lyne J, Kinsella A, Turner N, O’Callaghan E, Clarke M. Detection and characteristics of individuals with a very long duration of untreated psychosis in an early intervention for psychosis service. Early Interv Psychiatry. 2014;8:332–339. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17. Lincoln TM, Rief W, Westermann S, et al. . Who stays, who benefits? Predicting dropout and change in cognitive behaviour therapy for psychosis. Psychiatry Res. 2014;216:198–205. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18. Berry K, Gregg L, Lobban F, Barrowclough C. Therapeutic alliance in psychological therapy for people with recent onset psychosis who use cannabis. Compr Psychiatry. 2016;67:73–80. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19. Cavelti M, Rüsch N, Vauth R. Is living with psychosis demoralizing? Insight, self-stigma, and clinical outcome among people with schizophrenia across 1 year. J Nerv Ment Dis. 2014;202:521–529. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20. Ruchlewska A, Kamperman AM, van der Gaag M, Wierdsma AI, Mulder NC. Working alliance in patients with severe mental illness who need a crisis intervention plan. Community Ment Health J. 2016;52:102–108. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21. Bergé D, Mané A, Salgado P, et al. . Predictors of relapse and functioning in first-episode psychosis: a two-year follow-up study. Psychiatr Serv. 2016;67:227–233. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22. Gumley AI, Schwannauer M, Macbeth A, et al. . Insight, duration of untreated psychosis and attachment in first-episode psychosis: prospective study of psychiatric recovery over 12-month follow-up. Br J Psychiatry. 2014;205:60–67. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23. Misiak B, Frydecka D, Beszłej JA, et al. . Effects of antipsychotics on insight in schizophrenia: results from independent samples of first-episode and acutely relapsed patients. Int Clin Psychopharmacol. 2016;31:185–191. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24. Erol A, Delibas H, Bora O, Mete L. The impact of insight on social functioning in patients with schizophrenia. Int J Soc Psychiatry. 2015;61:379–385. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25. Firmin RL, Luther L, Lysaker PH, Salyers MP. Self-initiated helping behaviors and recovery in severe mental illness: implications for work, volunteerism, and peer support. Psychiatr Rehabil J. 2015;38:336–341. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26. Montemagni C, Castagna F, Crivelli B, et al. . Relative contributions of negative symptoms, insight, and coping strategies to quality of life in stable schizophrenia. Psychiatry Res. 2014;220:102–111. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27. Tastet H, Verdoux H, Bouisson J, Destaillats J-M, Prouteau A. Impact of interpersonal factors on insight in schizophrenia. Schizophr Res. 2014;159:527–532. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28. Lysaker PH, Roe D, Yanos PT. Toward understanding the insight paradox: internalized stigma moderates the association between insight and social functioning, hope, and self-esteem among people with schizophrenia spectrum disorders. Schizophr Bull. 2007;33:192–199. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29. Adan A, Capella MD, Prat G, Forero DA, López-Vera S, Navarro JF. Executive functioning in men with schizophrenia and substance use disorders. influence of lifetime suicide attempts. PLoS One. 2017;12:e0169943. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30. López-Moríñigo JD, Wiffen B, O’Connor J, et al. . Insight and suicidality in first-episode psychosis: understanding the influence of suicidal history on insight dimensions at first presentation. Early Interv Psychiatry. 2014;8:113–121. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31. Massons C, Lopez-Morinigo JD, Pousa E, et al. . Insight and suicidality in psychosis: a cross-sectional study. Psychiatry Res. 2017;252:147–153. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32. Hasson-Ohayon I, Kravetz S, Roe D, David AS, Weiser M. Insight into psychosis and quality of life. Compr Psychiatry. 2006;47:265–269. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33. Hasson-Ohayon I, Kravetz S, Meir T, Rozencwaig S. Insight into severe mental illness, hope, and quality of life of persons with schizophrenia and schizoaffective disorders. Psychiatry Res. 2009;167:231–238. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34. Ehrlich-Ben Or S, Hasson-Ohayon I, Feingold D, et al. . Meaning in life, insight and self-stigma among people with severe mental illness. Compr Psychiatry. 2013;54:195–200. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35. Vohs JL, George S, Leonhardt BL, Lysaker PH. An integrative model of the impairments in insight in schizophrenia: emerging research on causal factors and treatments. Expert Rev Neurother. 2016;16:1193–1204. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36. Lysaker PH, Clements CA, Plascak-Hallberg CD, Knipscheer SJ, Wright DE. Insight and personal narratives of illness in schizophrenia. Psychiatry. 2002;65:197–206. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37. Roe D, Hasson-Ohayon I, Kravetz S, Yanos PT, Lysaker PH. Call it a monster for lack of anything else: narrative insight in psychosis. J Nerv Ment Dis. 2008;196:859–865. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38. Lysaker PH, Hamm JA, Hasson-Ohayon I, Pattison ML, Leonhardt BL. Promoting recovery from severe mental illness: implications from research on metacognition and metacognitive reflection and insight therapy. World J Psychiatry. 2018;8:1–11. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39. Moritz S, Lysaker PH. Metacognition – what did James H. Flavell really say and the implications for the conceptualization and design of metacognitive interventions. Schizophr Res. In press. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40. Semerari A, Carcione A, Dimaggio G, et al. . How to evaluate metacognitive functioning in psychotherapy? The metacognition assessment scale and its applications. Clin Psychol Psychother. 2003;10:238–261. [Google Scholar]
- 41. Lysaker PH, Hasson-Ohayon I. Metacognition in schizophrenia: introduction to the special issue. Isr J Psychiatry Relat Sci. 2014;51:4–7. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42. Bianchini O, Porcelli S, Nespeca C, et al. . Effects of antipsychotic drugs on insight in schizophrenia. Psychiatry Res. 2014;218:20–24. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43. Zhou Y, Rosenheck R, Mohamed S, et al. . Insight in inpatients with schizophrenia: relationship to symptoms and neuropsychological functioning. Schizophr Res. 2015;161:376–381. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44. Lysaker PH, Vohs J, Minor KS, et al. . Metacognitive deficits in schizophrenia: presence and associations with psychosocial outcomes. J Nerv Ment Dis. 2015;203:530–536. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45. Lysaker PH, Carcione A, Dimaggio G, et al. . Metacognition amidst narratives of self and illness in schizophrenia: associations with insight, neurocognition, symptom and function. Acta Psychiatr Scand. 2005;112:64–71. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46. Lysaker PH, Dimaggio G, Buck KD, et al. . Poor insight in schizophrenia: links between different forms of metacognition with awareness of symptoms, treatment need, and consequences of illness. Compr Psychiatry. 2011;52:253–260. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47. Lysaker PH, Vohs JL, Ballard R, et al. . Metacognition, self-reflection and recovery in schizophrenia. Future Neurol. 2013;8:103–115. [Google Scholar]
- 48. Nicolò G, Dimaggio G, Popolo R, et al. . Associations of metacognition with symptoms, insight, and neurocognition in clinically stable outpatients with schizophrenia. J Nerv Ment Dis. 2012;200:644–647. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49. Bedford NJ, David AS. Denial of illness in schizophrenia as a disturbance of self-reflection, self-perception and insight. Schizophr Res. 2014;152:89–96. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50. Moe AM, Breitborde NJ, Shakeel MK, Gallagher CJ, Docherty NM. Idea density in the life-stories of people with schizophrenia: associations with narrative qualities and psychiatric symptoms. Schizophr Res. 2016;172:201–205. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51. Gawęda Ł, Krężołek M, Olbryś J, Turska A, Kokoszka A. Decreasing self-reported cognitive biases and increasing clinical insight through meta-cognitive training in patients with chronic schizophrenia. J Behav Ther Exp Psychiatry. 2015;48:98–104. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52. Balzan RP, Galletly C. Metacognitive therapy (MCT+) in patients with psychosis not receiving antipsychotic medication: a case study. Front Psychol. 2015;6:967. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53. Vohs JL, Leonhardt BL, James AV, et al. . Metacognitive reflection and insight therapy for early psychosis: a preliminary study of a novel integrative psychotherapy. Schizophr Res. In press. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54. Gerretsen P, Menon M, Mamo DC, et al. . Impaired insight into illness and cognitive insight in schizophrenia spectrum disorders: resting state functional connectivity. Schizophr Res. 2014;160:43–50. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55. Quee PJ, van der Meer L, Krabbendam L, et al. . Insight change in psychosis: relationship with neurocognition, social cognition, clinical symptoms and phase of illness. Acta Psychiatr Scand. 2014;129:126–133. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56. Chan SK, Chan KK, Hui CL, et al. . Correlates of insight with symptomatology and executive function in patients with first-episode schizophrenia-spectrum disorder: a longitudinal perspective. Psychiatry Res. 2014;216:177–184. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57. Pousa E, Ochoa S, Cobo J, et al. ; Insight Barcelona Research Group. A deeper view of insight in schizophrenia: insight dimensions, unawareness and misattribution of particular symptoms and its relation with psychopathological factors. Schizophr Res. 2017;189:61–68. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58. Chan KK. Associations of symptoms, neurocognition, and metacognition with insight in schizophrenia spectrum disorders. Compr Psychiatry. 2016;65:63–69. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59. Chang WC, Lau CFC, Chan SSI, et al. . Premorbid, clinical and cognitive correlates of primary negative symptoms in first-episode psychosis. Psychiatry Res. 2016;242:144–149. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 60. Volavka J, Van Dorn RA, Citrome L, Kahn RS, Fleischhacker WW, Czobor P. Hostility in schizophrenia: an integrated analysis of the combined Clinical Antipsychotic Trials of Intervention Effectiveness (CATIE) and the European First Episode Schizophrenia Trial (EUFEST) studies. Eur Psychiatry. 2016;31:13–19. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 61. Lysaker PH, Vohs J, Hasson-Ohayon I, Kukla M, Wierwille J, Dimaggio G. Depression and insight in schizophrenia: comparisons of levels of deficits in social cognition and metacognition and internalized stigma across three profiles. Schizophr Res. 2013;148:18–23. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 62. First MB, Spitzer RL, Gibbon M, Williams JB.. Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV-TR Axis I Disorders, Research Version, Patient Edition. New York, NY: SCID-I/P; 2002. [Google Scholar]
- 63. Lysaker PH, Clements CA, Plascak-Hallberg CD, Knipscheer SJ, Wright DE. Insight and personal narratives of illness in schizophrenia. Psychiatry. 2002;65:197–206. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 64. Lysaker PH, Dimaggio G, Buck KD, Carcione A, Nicolò G. Metacognition within narratives of schizophrenia: associations with multiple domains of neurocognition. Schizophr Res. 2007;93:278–287. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 65. Kay SR, Fiszbein A, Opler LA. The positive and negative syndrome scale (PANSS) for schizophrenia. Schizophr Bull. 1987;13:261–276. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 66. Bell MD, Lysaker PH, Beam-Goulet JL, Milstein RM, Lindenmayer JP. Five-component model of schizophrenia: assessing the factorial invariance of the positive and negative syndrome scale. Psychiatry Res. 1994;52:295–303. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 67. Muthén LK, Muthén BO.. Mplus: The Comprehensive Modelling Program for Applied Researchers; User’s Guide, [Version 7.0]. Los Angeles, CA: Muthén & Muthén; 2012. [Google Scholar]
- 68. Hagenaars JA, McCutcheon AL.. Applied Latent Class Analysis. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press; 2002. [Google Scholar]
- 69. Akaike H. Factor analysis and AIC. Psychometrika. 1987;52:317–332. [Google Scholar]
- 70. Schwarz G. Estimating the dimension of a model. The Annals of Statistics. 1978;6:461–464. [Google Scholar]
- 71. Sclove SL. Application of model-selection criteria to some problems in multivariate analysis. Psychometrika. 1987;52:333–343. [Google Scholar]
- 72. McLachlan GJ. On bootstrapping the likelihood ratio test statistic for the number of components in a normal mixture. Appl Stat. 1987;36:318–324. [Google Scholar]
- 73. Nylund KL, Asparouhov T, Muthén BO. Deciding on the number of classes in latent class analysis and growth mixture modeling: a Monte Carlo simulation study. Struct Equ Modeling. 2007;14:535–569. [Google Scholar]
- 74. Lo Y, Mendell NR, Rubin DB. Testing the number of components in a normal mixture. Biometrika. 2001;88:767–778. [Google Scholar]
- 75. Freeman D, Garety PA. Connecting neurosis and psychosis: the direct influence of emotion on delusions and hallucinations. Behav Res Ther. 2003;41:923–947. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 76. Garety PA, Kuipers E, Fowler D, Freeman D, Bebbington PE. A cognitive model of the positive symptoms of psychosis. Psychol Med. 2001;31:189–195. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 77. Hamm JA, Renard SB, Fogley RL, et al. . Metacognition and social cognition in schizophrenia: stability and relationship to concurrent and prospective symptom assessments. J Clin Psychol. 2012;68:1303–1312. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 78. McLeod HJ, Gumley AI, Macbeth A, Schwannauer M, Lysaker PH. Metacognitive functioning predicts positive and negative symptoms over 12 months in first episode psychosis. J Psychiatr Res. 2014;54:109–115. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 79. Popolo R, Smith E, Lysaker PH, et al. . Metacognitive profiles in schizophrenia and bipolar disorder: comparisons with healthy controls and correlations with negative symptoms. Psychiatry Res. 2017;257:45–50. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 80. Lysaker PH, George S, Chaudoin-Patzoldt KA, et al. . Contrasting metacognitive, social cognitive and alexithymia profiles in adults with borderline personality disorder, schizophrenia and substance use disorder. Psychiatry Res. 2017;257:393–399. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 81. Lysaker PH, Dimaggio G, Wickett-Curtis A, et al. . Deficits in metacognitive capacity are related to subjective distress and heightened levels of hyperarousal symptoms in adults with posttraumatic stress disorder. J Trauma Dissociation. 2015;26:1–15. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 82. Ladegaard N, Larsen ER, Videbech P, Lysaker PH. Higher-order social cognition in first-episode major depression. Psychiatry Res. 2014;216:37–43. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 83. Hasson-Ohayon I, Kravetz S, Lysaker PH. The special challenges of psychotherapy with persons with psychosis: intersubjective metacognitive model of agreement and shared meaning. Clin Psychol Psychother. 2017;24:428–440. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 84. Hasson-Ohayon I. Overlap and distinction between measures of insight and self-stigma. Psychiatry Res. 2018;266:47–64. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 85. Moritz S, Woodward TS. Metacognitive training in schizophrenia: from basic research to knowledge translation and intervention. Curr Opin Psychiatry. 2007;20:619–625. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 86. Lysaker PH, Klion R.. Recovery, Meaning-Making, and Severe Mental Illness: A Comprehensive Guide to Metacognitive Reflection and Insight Therapy. New York, NY: Routledge; 2017. [Google Scholar]
- 87. Hasson-Ohayon I, Arnon-Ribenfeld N, Hamm JA, Lysaker PH. Agency before action: the application of behavioral activation in psychotherapy with persons with psychosis. Psychotherapy (Chic). 2017;54:245–251. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]